You are on page 1of 4

INFORMATION-PROCESSING THEORY

-Describes development as if it worked like a computer. According to this


view, a sensory register receives sensory information and holds it for a brief
time- only seconds. If any of that information processed, it is transferred to
short-term memory (STM). It then maybe further processed into a form where
it can be stored indefinitely in long- term memory (LTM). The most important
part of this model concerns STM and LTM, both of which alter or process
information in important ways.

An Example of Information Processing: Studying for a Test


To see how the information-processing viewpoint helps to explain a
more every day example of problem solving, imagine how high school
students might study for a midterm test in, say, history.

How are high school student are likely differ from elementary
school children in preparing for the test?

Applying expert skills-


first, adolescents have probably had more time to become experts in taking
multiple-choice tests. Younger children, in contrast, cannot afford to do this:
for them, the format of the test itself might be part of the problem they are
trying to solve.

Selective Allocation of Attention-


second, adolescents have probably read enough textbooks to know that
authors of such books usually organize material around headings, topic
sentences, key terms, and summary paragraphs.

Use of domain-specific Knowledge


Older students have usually taken more courses in history and in
subjects related to history, so they know more than younger children do.
Older students use their richer prior knowledge to help remember the new
material that they have to learn.

Informational-Processing Features of Adolescents


Thought
Information-processing theory tends not to emphasize qualitative
changes or transformations between major stages or period of life. Instead, it
treats each major stage as a continuation or extension of processes begun
earlier in childhood.

Improved Capacity to Process Information


Typically an adolescent can deal with, or process, more information
than a child can. A first-grader may remember three or four random digits(3 9
5 1), but a teenager can usually remember six or seven.

TWO SOURCES OF DIFFERENCES


Structural capacity—a person’s basic “mental power” or cognitive ability.
Differences in structural capacity are like the differences in physical strength
between adolescents and children.

Functional capacity—the ability to make efficient use of existing mental


abilities. Differences in functional capacity are more like the improvements in
physical performance that come to a gymnast who is already perfectly
conditioned; at a certain point in her training.

When it comes to cognitive development, it is usually hard to


discriminate between the relative influences of structural and functional
capacity—between how much of an adolescents improved thinking comes
from more greater efficiency in using existing talents

ENCOURAGING CRITICAL THINKING


Critical thinking is reflection or thought about complex issues, often for the
purpose of choosing actions related to the issues. In spite of the term critical,
it does not refer to thinking that is negative or full of complaints but to
thinking that is thoughtful, that yields new insights, and that gives a basis for
intelligent choice. Critical thinking is a broad, practical skill; it can help in
figuring out why an unfamiliar appliance has broken down, in composing a
term paper, in resolving a personal conflict with a friend, or in deciding what
kind of career to pursue.

Various ways to classify the Elements of


Critical Thinking
1. Basic operations of reasoning- to think critically, a person has to be
able to classify, generalize, deduce conclusions, and perform other logical
steps mentally.

2. Domain-specific knowledge-to deal with a problem, a person has to


know something about its topic or content.
3. Meta-cognitive knowledge-effective critical thinking requires a person
to monitor when she really understands an idea, to know when she needs
new information, and to predict how easily she can gather and learn that
information.

4. Values, beliefs, and dispositions- thinking critically means valuing


fairness and objectivity.

GENERAL PRINCIPLES THAT ENHANCE THE QUALITY OF


PROGRAMS THAT TEACH CRITICAL THINKING
1. Teaching thinking is best done directly and explicitly. Critical thinking does
not develop on its own by unconscious osmosis, so to speak.

2. Good programs for teaching thinking offer lots of practice at solving actual
solving problems. Just telling about the elements of critical thinking does not
turn students into skillful thinkers.

3. All successful programs try to create an environment explicitly conducive


to critical thinking.

EXPERTISE IN SPECIFIC DOMAINS OF


KNOWLEDGE
By adolescence, many individuals have become comparative experts in
specific domains of knowledge or skill. Much of such expertise may depend
not on generalized development of cognitive structures, as the Piagetians
would claim, but on the long, slow acquisition of large amounts of specific
knowledge. Experts also know more about problem solving in their particular
field of expertise. Expertise may often make creative problem solving
unnecessary. Instead of using valuable energy in thinking through a solution,
the expert can simply remember what to do. This fact as much as any other
makes his performance seem fast and effortless. This advantage is confined
to specific domains or types of problems, an experts performance contradicts
Piaget’s assertion that powerful, abstract thought develops across many
domains at once.

EVALUATION OF THE INFORMATION-PROCESSING


VIEWPOINT
By focusing on the detailed features of problem solving, the
information processing viewpoints provides a valuable complement to the
boarder approach of cognitive developmental theory.
Reported By;

MADEL SERNAL SABIDALAS


Beed- Eced 2-A

You might also like