Welcome to Scribd, the world's digital library. Read, publish, and share books and documents. See more
Download
Standard view
Full view
of .
Look up keyword
Like this
23Activity
0 of .
Results for:
No results containing your search query
P. 1
Cognitive and Affective Development in Adolescence - Laurence Steinberg

Cognitive and Affective Development in Adolescence - Laurence Steinberg

Ratings:

4.5

(10)
|Views: 12,157|Likes:
Published by Tlecoz Huitzil

More info:

Published by: Tlecoz Huitzil on Feb 15, 2008
Copyright:Attribution Non-commercial

Availability:

Read on Scribd mobile: iPhone, iPad and Android.
download as PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
See more
See less

12/27/2012

pdf

text

original

 
Cognitive and affective developmentin adolescence
Laurence Steinberg
Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
Questions about the nature of normative and atypicaldevelopment in adolescence have taken on specialsignificance in the last few years, as scientists havebegun to recast old portraits of adolescent behavior inthe light of new knowledge about brain development.Adolescence is often a period of especially heightenedvulnerability as a consequence of potential disjunctionsbetween developing brain, behavioral and cognitivesystems that mature along different timetables andunder the control of both common and independentbiological processes. Taken together, these develop-ments reinforce the emerging understanding of adoles-cenceasacriticalorsensitiveperiodforareorganizationof regulatory systems, a reorganization that is fraughtwith both risks and opportunities.Introduction
 Adolescence is characterized by an increased need toregulate affect and behavior in accordance with long-termgoals and consequences, often at a distance from theadults who provided regulatory structure and guidanceduring childhood. Because developing brain, behavioraland cognitive systems mature at different rates and underthe control of both common and independent biologicalprocesses, this period is often one of increased vulner-ability and adjustment. Accordingly, normative develop-ment in adolescence can profitably be understood withrespect to the coordination of emotional, intellectual andbehavioral proclivities and capabilities, and psychopathol-ogy in adolescence may be reflective of difficulties in thiscoordination process.The notion that adolescence is a heightened period of  vulnerability specifically because of gaps betweenemotion, cognition and behavior has important impli-cations for our understanding of many aspects of bothnormative and atypical development during this period of the life-span. With respect to normative development, forinstance, this framework is helpful in understanding agedifferences in judgment and decision-making, in risk-taking, and in sensation-seeking[1]. With respect toatypical development, the framework helps us to under-stand why adolescence can be a time of increased risk forthe onset of a wide range of emotional and behavioralproblems, including depression, violent delinquency andsubstance abuse[2].Questions about the nature of normative and atypicaldevelopment in adolescence have taken on special signifi-cance in the last few years as scientists have begun torecast old portraits of adolescent psychological develop-ment in light of new knowledge about adolescent braindevelopment. Recent discoveries in the area of develop-mental neuroscience have stimulated widespread scien-tific and popular interest in the study of braindevelopment during adolescence, as well as substantialspeculation about the connections between brain matu-ration and adolescents’ behavioral and emotional devel-opment. Indeed, the topic has garnered such widespreadpublic interest that it was the subject of a recent coverstory in
Time
magazine aimed at parents of teenagers[3],and was raised in arguments submitted in late 2004 to theUnited States Supreme Court in connection withthe Court’s consideration of the constitutionality of the juvenile death penalty[4].
Brain development in adolescence
 As reviewed in the accompanying article by Paus[5]thereis growing evidence that maturational brain processes arecontinuing well through adolescence. Even relativelysimple structural measures, such as the ratio of white-to-gray matter in the brain, demonstrate large-scalechanges into the late teen-age years[6–8]. The impact of this continued maturation on emotional, intellectual andbehavioral development has yet to be thoroughly studied,but there is considerable evidence that the second decadeof life is a period of great activity with respect to changesin brain structure and function, especially in regions andsystems associated with response inhibition, the cali-bration of risk and reward, and emotion regulation.Contrary to earlier beliefs about brain maturation inadolescence, this activity is not limited to the earlyadolescent period, nor is it invariably linked to processesof pubertal maturation (Figure 1).Two particular observations about brain developmentin adolescence are especially pertinent to our under-standing of psychological development during this period.First, much brain development during adolescence is inthe particular brain regions and systems that are key tothe regulation of behavior and emotion and to theperception and evaluation of risk and reward. Second, itappears that changes in arousal and motivation broughton by pubertal maturation precede the development of regulatory competence in a manner that creates adisjunction between the adolescent’s affective experience
Corresponding author:
Steinberg, L. (lds@temple.edu). Available online 23 December 2004
Review
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 
Vol.9 No.2 February 2005
www.sciencedirect.com1364-6613/$ - see front matter
Q
2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005
 
and his or her ability to regulate arousal and motivation.To the extent that the changes in arousal and motivationprecede the development of regulatory competence areasonable speculation, but one that has yet to beconfirmed – the developments of early adolescence maywell create a situation in which one is starting an enginewithout yet having a skilled driver behind the wheel[9].
Cognitive development in adolescence
Until recently, much of the work on adolescent cognitivedevelopmentwasdevotedtoasearchforacoremechanismthat could account parsimoniously for broad changes inadolescent thinking[10]. After nearly 50 years of search-ing, what has emerged instead is the necessity of anintegrated account. What lies at the core of adolescentcognitive development is the attainment of a more fullyconscious,self-directedandself-regulatingmind[10].Thisis achieved principally through the assembly of anadvanced ‘executive suite’ of capabilities[11], ratherthan through specific advancement in any one of theconstituent elements. This represents a major shift inprevailing views of adolescent cognition, going beyond thesearch for underlying elements[12]that are formed andoperate largely outside awareness. As Keating[10]has noted, the plausibility of such anintegrative account has been substantially enhanced byrecent major advances in the neurosciences[6–8,13–18],in comparative neuroanatomy across closely relatedprimate species that illuminate core issues of humancognitive evolution[10,19], and in a deepened under-standing of the critical role of culture and context in theshaping of cognitive and brain development[11,20]. Muchof the underlying action is focused on specific develop-ments in the prefrontal cortex, but with an equallysignificant role for rapidly expanding linkages to thewhole brain[11,15,21].This complex process of assembly is supported by increasingly rapid connectivity(through continued myelination of nerve fibers), par-ticularly in communication among different brainregions, and by significant and localized synapticpruning, especially in frontal areas that are crucialto executive functioning[6–8,18].Whatever the underlying processes, during earlyadolescence, individuals show marked improvements inreasoning (especially deductive reasoning), informationprocessing (in both efficiency and capacity), and expertise.These conclusions derive from studies of age differences inlogical reasoning on tasks in which participants are askedto solve verbal analogies or analyze logical propositions;basic cognitive processes, such short- or long-termmemory; and in specialized knowledge[10].There has been broad consensus for more than 25 years that, as aresult of these gains, individuals become more capable of abstract, multidimensional, planned and hypotheticalthinking as they develop from late childhood into middleadolescence (less is known about cognitive changes duringlate adolescence). No research in the past several decadeshas challenge this conclusion.
Implications of new brain maturation research foradolescent cognitive development
 After a rather lengthy period during the late 1980s andearly 1990s, when the study of adolescent cognitivedevelopment was more or less moribund, interest inintellectual development during adolescence has beenrevitalized in recent years in two ways. First, researchersin the field of developmental neuroscience began to directattention to the study of structural and functional aspectsof brain development during early adolescence[6,8,13,22].These studies have pointed both to significant growth andsignificant change in multiple regions of the prefrontalcortex throughout the course of adolescence, especiallywith respect to processes of myelination and synapticpruning (both of which increase the efficiency of infor-mation processing)[8,17,23]. These changes are believedtoundergirdimprovementsinvariousaspectsof executivefunctioning,includinglong-termplanning,metacognition,self-evaluation, self-regulation and the coordination of affect and cognition[10]. Mostresearch has focused on agedifferences in skills known to be related to functioning inthe dorsolateral region of the prefrontal cortex, such asthose involving working memory, spatial working memoryand planning[24–26],but two recent studies[27,28] indicate as well that adolescence is a time of improvement
Puberty heightensemotional arousability,sensation-seeking,reward orientationPeriod of heightenedvulnerability torisk-taking and problemsin regulation ofaffect and behaviorMaturation of frontallobes facilitatesregulatory competenceEarly adolescenceMiddle adolescenceLate adolescence
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 
Figure 1.
It has been speculated that the impact of puberty on arousal and motivation occurs before the maturation of the frontal lobes is complete. This gap may create aperiod of heightened vulnerability to problems in the regulation of affect and behavior, which might help to explain the increased potential in adolescence for risk-taking,recklessness, and the onset of emotional and behavioral problems.
Review
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 
Vol.9 No.2 February 200570
 
in abilities that have been linked to functioning in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, such as the calibration of risk and reward (Box 1).In addition, adolescence appears to be a time of improved connectivity between regions of the prefrontalcortex and several areas of the limbic system, a restruc-turing that further affects the ways in which individualsevaluate and respond to risk and reward[22,29]. Whetherand to what extent these changes in brain structure andfunction are linked to processes of pubertal maturation isnot known. Some aspects of brain development arecoincident with, and likely linked to, neuroendocrinologi-cal changes occurring at the time of puberty, but othersappeartotakeplacealongadifferent,andlater,timetable.Disentanglingthefirstsetfromthesecondisanimportantchallenge for the field[9]. It is also important to note thatthere are relatively few studies of developmental changesin brain function (as opposed to structure) in adolescence,and that conclusions about the putative links betweenchanges in cognitive performance and changes in brainstructure during adolescence are suggestive, rather thanconclusive.
Cognitive development in context
 A second relatively new direction in research on adoles-cent cognitive development has involved the study of cognitive development as it plays out in its social contextand, in particular, as it affects the development of  judgment, decision-making and risk-taking[30–35]. Newperspectivesonadolescentcognition-in-contextemphasizethat adolescent thinking in the real world is a function of social and emotional, as well as cognitive, processes, andthat a full account of the ways in which the intellectualchanges of adolescence affect social and emotional devel-opment must examine the ways in which affect andcognition interact[10]. Studies of adolescents’ reasoningor problem-solving using laboratory-based measures of intellectual functioning might provide better understand-ing of adolescents’ potential competence than of theiractual performance in everyday settings, where judgmentand decision-making are likely affected by emotionalstates, social influences and expertise[1].Thus, whereas studies of people’s responses to hypothetical dilemmasinvolving the perception and appraisal of risk show fewreliable age differences after middle adolescence, studiesof actual risk-taking (e.g. risky driving, unprotectedsexual activity, etc.) indicate that adolescents are signifi-cantly morelikely tomakerisky decisions thanareadults.One reasonable hypothesis is that adults and adoles-cents 16 and older share the same logical competencies,but that age differences in social and emotional factors,such as susceptibility to peer influence or impulse control,lead to age differences in actual decision-making[36].Research that examines developmental changes in cogni-tive abilities assessed under varying social and emotionalconditions (e.g. the same task administered under con-ditions of high versus low affective arousal, or in thepresence versus absence of peers) would be potentiallyquite informative, as at least one study indicates thatadolescents’ risk taking is more influenced than that of adults by the presence of peers[37](Figure 2). Consistent with findings on the advances in reasoning,characteristic of the transition into adolescence, studies of social cognition demonstrate that the ways in whichadolescents think about others becomes more abstract,moredifferentiatedandmoremultidimensional[38].More recent studies of changes in social cognition duringadolescence have attempted to clarify the conditionsunder which relatively more advanced displays of socialcognition are likely to be seen; to describe gender andcultural differences in certain aspects of social cognition,such as prosocial reasoning[39,40]or impression for-mation[41];and to examine the links between social cognition and social behavior. These studies indicate thatpatterns of social cognitive development in adolescence,like patterns of cognitive development more generally, vary both as a function of the content under considerationand the emotional and social context in which thereasoning occurs[38].For example, although individuals’ thinking aboutmoral dilemmas becomes more principled over the course
Box 1. Age differences in ventromedial functioning
The most widely used task in the assessment of ventromedialfunctioning is the Iowa Gambling Task[56]. Subjects are asked todraw cards from one of four face-down decks; each card providesinformationonhowmuchhasbeenwonandlostonthatdraw.Cardsfromtwoofthedecksofferhighrewards,butinonedecktherewardsare paired either with occasional very high losses, and in the otherwith frequent modest losses, and continuing to draw from either of these decks results in a net loss over time. By contrast, cards fromthe other two decks offer low rewards, but these are paired eitherwith frequent small losses, or sporadic, but modest, losses, andthese decks lead to net gains over time.Researchers monitor individuals’ pattern of card selection overnumerous trials. Healthy adults typically begin by drawing from thedecks at random, then gradually increase their pulls from the ‘good’decks and decrease their pulls from the ‘bad’ decks. The notableexceptions to this pattern are patients with lesions to the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex, individuals with substance abuse pro-blems, and individuals who report high levels of risk-taking ineveryday life, all of whom persist in drawing from the ‘bad’ decksdespite the net losses that result.Only recently have studies examined age differences on the IowaGambling Task. In one study[27]offour age groups – 6–9 year-olds,10–12 year-olds, 13–15 year-olds and 18–25 year-olds – the youngestsubjects drew equally from the good and bad decks. The two middlegroups showed modest improvement over time; by the final trialblock,theyweredrawingfromthegood decksabout55%and 60%of the time, respectively. By the final block, however, the young adultswere drawing from the good deck nearly 75% of the time, and theybegan shifting towards the good decks much earlier than theyounger groups.Another study, of 9- to 17-year-olds, also found significantimprovement in performance on this task with age[28]. 14–17year-olds drew from the good decks more often than 9–10 year-olds(although not moreoften than 11–13 yearolds) and began shifting tothe good decks earlier than did either of the younger groups. Theresearchers also administered two tasks known to activate thedorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a go/no-go task, which assessesresponse inhibition, and a digit span test, which assesses workingmemory. As expected, performance on both dorsolateral tasksimproved with age. More importantly, however, there were nosignificant correlations between performance on the ventromedialtask and performance on either of the dorsolateral tasks, suggestingthat maturation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex may be adevelopmentally distinct process from the maturation of otherregions of the frontal lobe.
Review
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 
Vol.9 No.2 February 2005 71

Activity (23)

You've already reviewed this. Edit your review.
1 hundred reads
1 thousand reads
Fan Zhang liked this
Delisa Johnson liked this
Melina Ong liked this
AijaLivca liked this
Bhatnagar Shel liked this
Cresenthea Redi liked this
Zhay Vacunawa liked this
LittleMama714 liked this

You're Reading a Free Preview

Download
scribd
/*********** DO NOT ALTER ANYTHING BELOW THIS LINE ! ************/ var s_code=s.t();if(s_code)document.write(s_code)//-->