You are on page 1of 8

Review

Effects of increasing cannabis potency on adolescent health


Jack Wilson, Tom P Freeman, Clare J Mackie

Cannabis is the most prevalent illicit drug used by adolescents worldwide. Over the past 40 years, changes in cannabis Lancet Child Adolesc Health 2018
potency through rising concentrations of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabiol (THC), decreases in cannabidiol, or both, have Published Online
occurred. Epidemiological and experimental evidence demonstrates that cannabis with high THC concentrations and December 17, 2018
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
negligible cannabidiol concentrations is associated with an increased risk of psychotic outcomes, an effect on spatial
S2352-4642(18)30342-0
working memory and prose recall, and increased reports of the severity of cannabis dependence. However, many
Addictions Department,
studies have failed to address cannabis use in adolescence, the peak age at which individuals typically try cannabis Institute of Psychiatry,
and probably the most vulnerable age to experience its harmful effects. In this Review, we highlight the influence that Psychology and Neuroscience,
changing cannabis products have on adolescent health and the implications they carry for policy and prevention King’s College London, London,
UK (J Wilson, T P Freeman PhD,
measures as legal cannabis markets continue to emerge worldwide.
C J Mackie PhD); Department of
Psychology, University of Bath,
Introduction people and discusses the importance of shifts in social Bath, UK (T P Freeman); and
Cannabis is the most widely used illicit drug worldwide, determinants on late adolescent development.9 South London and Maudsley
NHS Foundation Trust,
with approximately 183·3 million users who make up First, this Review focuses on the role of cannabis in the
Bethlem Royal Hospital,
nearly 4% of the global population.1 Despite a relatively endocannabinoid system, commonly discussed cannabis Beckenham, UK (C J Mackie)
stable prevalence of cannabis use globally, the drug is constituents, and global trends in cannabis potency. Correspondence to:
being used by individuals with greater frequency, for Second, we examine whether adolescents appear to be Dr Clare Mackie, National
instance in the USA where one in 17 adolescents aged more susceptible to rising concentrations of THC (and Addiction Centre, Institute of
Psychiatry, Psychology
17–18 years reported daily cannabis use, a rate that has lower concentrations of cannabidiol) in cannabis. Third,
and Neuroscience, King’s College
risen since 2007.2 It is estimated that 13 million people we review evidence concerning the possible effect of London, London SE5 8AF, UK
worldwide meet the clinical criteria for cannabis use increasing cannabis potency on adolescent neuro­ clare.mackie@kcl.ac.uk
disorder (ie, a problematic pattern of persistent use cognition and mental health. Last, the Review highlights
causing clinically significant impairment or distress) the importance of cannabis potency for clinical and
accounting for a global burden of disease of 2 million educational policy and practice, as well as making
disability-adjusted life-years.3 This burden peaks in late recommendations for future research.
adolescence (ages 20–24 years) and is highest in the USA,
Canada, Australia, New Zealand, and western European Global changes in cannabis potency and markets
countries such as the UK.3 In Europe, the number of first- The effects of cannabis and its exogenous cannabinoids
time clients starting specialist drug treatment for cannabis (including THC and cannabidiol) occur primarily through
increased from 43 000 in 2005, to 76 000 in 2015,4 with
rising trends in 16 of the 22 European countries that
provided eligible data.5 Even though the reason for this Key messages
trend is unclear, it could be because of factors such as • Problematic cannabis use typically peaks in adolescence—
greater detection rates of disorders, improved pathways an age group that could be particularly vulnerable to its
for referral, and changes in stigma towards mental health harmful effects
and treatment. An alternative explanation, however, • Cannabis markets are dominated by high-potency
suggests that this might result from an increase in cannabis (high in Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol [THC] and
cannabis potency through rising concentrations of low in cannabidiol), with THC content steadily increasing
Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and decreasing canna­ worldwide
bidiol concentrations.6 Widespread policy changes in parts • Compared with low-potency cannabis, high-potency
of the USA and Canada led to the legalisation of medicinal cannabis appears to be associated with a greater risk of
and recreational cannabis, potentially changing the psychotic symptoms, depression, anxiety, and cannabis
availability of cannabis products to millions of young dependence
people, and to marked increases in the potency of cannabis • Adolescents only partially titrate their use of high-potency
products.7,8 As such, understanding the effects of variation cannabis, which can result in the consumption of high
in cannabis potency on adolescent mental health, concentrations of THC
cognition, and development is of paramount importance. • Alongside more accurate measures of cannabis potency,
This knowledge will not only inform the causation models further research must adopt longitudinal, cognitive, and
of cannabis use and psychiatric comor­bidity but will also neuroimaging measures to gain a better understanding of
allow for the design of evidence-based prevention the health effects of cannabis use in adolescence
programmes targeting adolescent cannabis use. WHO • With cannabis policy rapidly changing, up-to-date evidence
defines adolescence as the age range of 10–19 years, and should inform decisions on potency taxes or potency
young people as those aged 10–24 years. This Review thresholds, as well as define the legal age of purchase
includes research referring to both adolescents and young

www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0 1


Review

verbal fluency, as well as contributing to transient


A B paranoid-like symptoms in laboratory studies.13 By
contrast, cannabidiol is non-intoxicating and has been
found to offset the harmful effects of THC on memory
impairment and psychotic symptoms.14,15 Concentrations
of THC and cannabidiol are known to vary across
cannabis plants, due to variations in genetics, growing
conditions, preparation, and extraction.16 For instance,
unfertilised female plants yield a more potent product, as
the plant converts its energy to cannabinoid synthesis
C D rather than into seed production.16 Referred to as
sinsemilla (Spanish for without seeds; figure), and
commonly called skunk in the UK or nederwiet in the
Netherlands, this highly potent type of cannabis has been
found to contain THC ranging from 1·9% to 22·5%,
with negligible cannabidiol.17 Seeded herbal cannabis is a
less potent type and its THC can range between 1·8%
and 5·7%, whereas resin (compressed preparations of
plant matter) can vary greatly in THC content (0–29·3%).
Figure: Examples of cannabis products An emerging cannabis product that is less common but
(A) Sinsemilla or skunk (14% THC, <1% cannabidiol). (B) Herbal cannabis (3%
THC, <1% cannabidiol). (C) Hash or resin (4% THC, 2% cannabidiol). (D) Cannabis often extremely potent is cannabis concentrate (figure).
concentrates (69% THC, 1% cannabidiol). Data shown are medians from the UK Concentrates are produced through a range of extraction
(A–C)and USA (D).17,18 THC=Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol. © Reproduced with techniques (including use of butane, super-critical
permission from David Potter and Tom Freeman carbon dioxide, and combined heat and pressure) and as
a result differ in texture and appearance. These
interaction with the endocannabinoid system.10 The endo­ preparations have also been found to vary greatly in THC
cannabinoid system includes cannabinoid receptors 1 and and cannabidiol content, depending on the extraction
2 (CB1R and CB2R), their endogenous ligands including technique. One US study18 assessed the cannabidiol and
anandamide and 2-arachidonoyl­ glycerol, and enzymes THC content of 57 concentrate samples at a medical
such as fatty acid amide hydrolase, which breaks down cannabis market and found that they contained between
these ligands. The endocannabinoid system regulates 23·7% and 75·9% THC, with all but five samples having
numerous biological processes involved in development low levels (<5%) of cannabidiol.18
and neuroplasticity early in life and it plays a critical role In addition to recent advances in cannabis production
in regulating synaptic plasticity.10 CB1Rs are densely and extraction techniques, new psychoactive substances
located in key brain regions involved in cognition, reward, have also entered the drug market.19 Synthetic cannabinoids
and adolescent neurodevelopment, such as the hippo­ elicit cannabimimetic effects similar to natural cannabis.
campus, baso­lateral amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and However, whereas THC acts as a partial agonist, synthetic
the prefrontal cortex.11 The endo­ cannabinoid system cannabinoids typically act as full agonists to cannabinoid
occupies a broad spatial area of the developing brain and receptors.20 As a result, synthetic cannabinoids induce
plays a key role in age-related brain changes throughout physiological (eg, nausea) and psychiatric (eg, anxiety and
life.10 During the important time of neuromaturation, psychosis) effects that are considerably more intense than
the brain might be more vulnerable to disturbances those induced by cannabis20 and can cause more serious
from exogenous cannabinoids, which could have a supra­ adverse events, such as seizures and even death.21 A
physiological effect on endocannabinoid receptors, and thorough discussion of synthetic cannabinoids is beyond
thus alter normal brain functioning.12 the scope of this Review, which focuses on cannabis and its
Although cannabis contains many cannabinoids, the constituent cannabinoids.
most commonly discussed in research, media, and The cannabis markets in the USA,8 Australia,22 and
among consumers are THC and cannabidiol. THC acts parts of Europe17,23 have been shown to be dominated by
as a partial agonist to CB1 receptors, whereas cannabidiol highly potent cannabis with high concentrations of THC
has low affinity for CB1R but can attenuate CB1R agonist and little, if any, cannabidiol. Over the past 40 years,
effects and inhibit the reuptake and hydrolysis of endo­ THC content in cannabis have steadily increased
cannabinoids.11 These effects of cannabidiol have been worldwide, with average THC in 2009 being over nine
proposed as potential pathways to be targeted for the times greater than in 1970.24 This increase is consistent
treatment of disorders such as psychosis. THC is the with data from cannabis confiscations in the UK, where
main psychoactive component responsible for the feeling high-potency sinsemilla cannabis made up 15% of all
of getting high that users seek, and has been found to cannabis seized by police for the period 1999–2002,25
have dose-dependent effects on memory, attention, and 50·6% for 2004–05, 84·5% for 2007–08, and 93·6%

2 www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0


Review

for 2016.17 Trends towards high-potency sinsemilla been found to be associated with reduced awareness of
cannabis are also reflected in seizure data in the USA7 internal and external cues, such as the ability to recognise
and Australia,22 with average total THC content of 12% one’s own substance use as problematic. Research by
and 14%, respectively, along with reductions in Lopez-Larson and colleagues36 showed that abnormal
cannabidiol content in the USA.7 Furthermore, data activation of insular cortices plays a role in problematic
from Washington state, USA show that concentrated substance use.
cannabis extracts made up 21·2% of the market within Behan and colleagues31 also showed that adolescent
2 years of legal sales, suggesting a strong demand for cannabis users were less able to inhibit their responses
extremely potent forms of cannabis.8 Another notable in a go/no-go task compared with never-users.
change in legal cannabis markets has been the dramatic Furthermore, a positive correlation between self-reported
decrease in potency-adjusted price over time (both at the cannabis amount in the past week or month and parietal,
retail and supply level).26 As price decreases, the price bilateral cerebellar, and right frontal connectivity was
per unit of THC also drops and this drop might shown, suggesting that the cerebellum is compensating
encourage purchasing behaviour and raise exposure to when other task-related regions are not engaged. Although
THC. Therefore, increased levels of harm might be compensatory efforts have yielded similar results to
attributed to a decline in the potency-adjusted price per controls in other studies,34 worse performances by cannabis
unit of THC, the increase in potency, or both. users observed by Behan and colleagues31 are consistent
with the hypothesis that increased engage­ ment of the
Reasons for adolescent susceptibility to rising cerebellum during response inhibition is associated with
cannabis potency poorer task performance. Overall, the available literature
Adolescence is marked by a period of dramatic cognitive covered in this Review suggests that cannabis users require
development when the brain undergoes neuronal additional neural resources to do as well as non-users in
maturation and cortical restructuring via processes of cognitive inhibition tasks. In conclusion, and as described
cortical thinning, synaptic reorganisation, and myelination in the panel, adolescent developmental processes, such as
of white matter tracts.27 There are major changes in the neuromaturation, and predisposing factors, such as
prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, and the cognitive inhibition, impulsivity, and reward sensitivity,
nucleus accumbens, which are areas responsible for harm play a major role in the susceptibility of adolescents to the
avoidance, inhibition, decision making, learning and harmful effects of cannabis use. Whether these preceding
memory, emotion, and motivation and reward.27 While risk factors influence the type of cannabis used is a
cortical functions are still developing, already formed question that has yet to be investigated.
reward-related circuitry leads to the propensity of
adolescents to seek novelty and reward in the face of The effect of cannabis potency on adolescent
uncertainty or potential negative outcomes, such as health
through the use of alcohol and illicit drugs.27 Their Epidemiological studies have consistently demonstrated
impulsivity, stemming from their inability to control their that cannabis use in adolescence is associated with an
behaviour, is often implicated in early-onset adolescent increased risk of psychotic symptoms,40–44 anxiety,48 and
drug use.28 Behavioural inhibition tasks such as the stop- in some cases depression.49 The onset and magnitude of
signal task29 and the go/no-go30 task measure the ability (or the effects of cannabis use on neurological function
inability) to suppress a task-induced response to a go remains under debate. A review of longitudinal studies
stimulus. The results of neuroimaging and cognitive reported that early cannabis use was prospectively
studies with stop-signal and go/no-go tasks have shown associated with neurocognitive decline, particularly in
an association between impairment in neural responses IQ and episodic memory, with the greatest decline
to these tasks and the risk for adolescent substance use.31–33 occurring in daily users.50 However, almost all studies
One such study34 concluded that adolescent cannabis surveyed for this Review have categorised users
users exerted greater neurocognitive effort throughout according to frequency of cannabis use, and few studies
the tasks, despite similar performance to adolescent non- have used measures examining the effect of high
cannabis users. During the inhibition trials of a go/no-go potency versus low potency on either neurocognitive
task, cannabis users showed greater activation in the function or mental health outcomes. Morgan and
right occipital gyrus and the right dorsolateral prefrontal, colleagues15 compared psychotic-like symptoms in
bilateral medial frontal, bilateral inferior, and superior 54 recreational cannabis users with 66 daily cannabis
parietal lobules, compared with the control group. These users aged 16–23 years. The results showed lower
brain regions are implicated in sustained attention,35 psychotic symptoms in individuals with hair samples
suggesting that users had to recruit more attentional containing cannabidiol compared with those without;
resources to complete the tasks successfully. During the however, this effect was only noted in recreational users
non-inhibitory trials, cannabis users showed greater with high concentrations of THC in their hair. These
activity in right prefrontal, insular, and parietal cortices. findings suggest that cannabidiol modulates the
Interestingly, abnormal activation of insular cortices has psychotic-like effects of THC, but that frequent users

www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0 3


Review

Few studies have accounted for cannabis type when


Panel: Vulnerability pathways of adolescent cannabis use and cannabis-related harms assessing depression and anxiety in adolescent cannabis
Risk factors for cannabis use users. An anonymous global drug survey of young people
Genetic factors (>18 years) showed that those with a lifetime diagnosis of
The effects of cannabis on the presence and severity of mental health symptoms is depression and anxiety were significantly more likely to
stronger in those who are genetically vulnerable to mental health conditions.37 use high-potency cannabis, in particular butane hash
oil.52 Butane hash oil is a frequently sold cannabis extract
Impulsivity, novelty seeking, and inhibition
with high concentrations of THC and relatively little
There is a positive association between abnormal task-specific neural connectivity in
cannabidiol.18 However, it must be noted that this study is
inhibition tasks and cannabis amount used.31 Cannabis use is strongly associated with young
of cross-sectional design and uses a lifetime diagnosis of
people (10–24 year-olds) who are more impulsive and have poor response inhibition.32,33
anxiety and depression, which makes the existence and
Neuromaturation direction of causality difficult to establish. A second
The brain could be more vulnerable to disturbances from exogenous cannabinoids, which cross-sectional study15 had similar findings, with higher
alter its normal function and development.12 depression and anxiety scores reported in recreational
Cannabis use behaviour
and daily cannabis users aged 16–23 years, whose hair
Potency
samples contained high concentrations of THC.
Concentrations of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) rise and those of cannabinol fall in
However, it is unclear whether an increase in THC
international cannabis markets.8,16,18,23 Extremely potent cannabis concentrates form a
concentrates in hair samples can be attributed to an
major part of new legal cannabis markets in the USA.8
increase in the frequency of cannabis use or to the use of
products with higher potency.
Patterns of use Few studies have investigated the association between
Inexperienced cannabis users show poor knowledge of drug potency.38 Younger cannabis high-potency and low-potency cannabis and cannabis-
users could be less likely to titrate (adjust smoking behaviour) higher potency products.39 related problems. Freeman and Winstock53 used data
Cannabis-related harms from the anonymous global drug survey and found that
Psychosis frequent use of high-potency cannabis was associated
Frequent adolescent cannabis use is associated with a greater risk of psychotic symptoms.40–44 with a greater severity of cannabis dependence. A
Earlier age of onset of cannabis use is associated with greater symptom severity.40 Those 16-year study in the Netherlands6 found an association
using cannabis high in THC and low in cannabidiol are at a greater likelihood of being between changes in THC concentrations in cannabis
diagnosed with a psychotic disorder compared with adults who have never used cannabis.45 sold at national retail outlets and the number of people
entering specialist treatment for cannabis problems.
Neurocognitive performance
However, given that most studies were cross-sectional,
Adolescent cannabis users exhibit impairments in tasks that assess sequencing ability,
longitudinal studies are needed to investigate the
cognitive processing speed, inhibition, and sustained attention.34,46,47,
existence and direction of potential causal relationships
Other disorders between cannabinoids and mental health outcomes in
Adolescents might have an increased risk of developing disorders from cannabis use. young people.
Increases in treatment for cannabis use disorders coincide with increases in cannabis
potency.6 Clinical studies should further explore the relationship between dependence Behaviours of cannabis use in adolescents
and potency. Although high-potency cannabis is associated with
greater harms compared with low-potency cannabis in
equal quantities, what must be considered is whether
could be tolerant to these protective effects. In a case- cannabis users adjust (titrate) their consumption
control study of inpatients presenting with first-episode according to THC and cannabidiol levels. A handful of
psychosis and controls aged between 18 and 65 years, studies have explored such a possibility in cannabis
Di Forti and colleagues45 showed that individuals who users.38,39,54 Identified by a cluster analysis based on
used skunk-like cannabis (high THC, minimal canna­ demographics and cannabis user and consumption
bidiol) daily were more than five times as likely to be characteristics, Korf and colleagues39 found that the
diagnosed with a psychotic disorder compared with “strongest high” group consisted mostly of younger
cannabis non-users. Moreover, frequent use of high- participants (mean age 22·7 years) who were least likely
potency cannabis was associated with an increased risk to report titration (ie, reducing the number of grams
of relapse following first-episode psychosis.51 These used, depth of inhalation, or pace of smoking) in response
findings are consistent with experimental evidence to rising cannabis potency. Additionally, some members
suggesting that the psychotomimetic effects of THC are of this group actually reported using more cannabis as
dose-dependent13 and might be offset by cannabidiol.14 potency rose, further enhancing their exposure to THC.
Overall, these studies clearly show the importance of In a subsequent Dutch study assessing titration in an
highlighting the risk of high-potency cannabis products, ecological setting, van der Pol and colleagues54 requested
particularly for adol­escents who might be susceptible to that users bring their own externally-sourced cannabis
the development of psychotic symptoms. and discovered that its THC concentration was positively

4 www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0


Review

correlated with the amount of cannabis they rolled in Netherlands confirmed a strong increase in potency
their joints. However, THC concentration was negatively from 2000 to 2004 in cannabis randomly sampled directly
correlated with inhalation volume, reducing exposure to from retail outlets.56 Second, few cannabis potency
the compound. Therefore, those who used higher potency studies address the issue of price, despite its important
cannabis tended to roll larger joints but partially engaged role for purchasing behaviour and consumption, and the
in titration by lowering their inhalation volume. The possibility of contrasting trends in different regions or
concept of partial titration was also supported by an markets. Therefore, in future studies, combining
ecological study of adolescent cannabis users (aged information on potency and price will be more inform­
16–24 years) in the UK.38 That study found that as THC ative than information on potency alone.
concentrations rose, users added less cannabis in their Although clinical studies involving adult populations
joints, partially reducing the effects of increased potency. can be useful in drawing conclusions from the effects of
However, they did not adjust their behaviour according to cannabis use, it can be difficult to generalise these findings
concentrations of cannabidiol in their cannabis. Measures to adolescents in the community. Moreover, unmeasured
of titration might also be important for identifying risk of confounding variables are a limitation common to many
transition to problematic use. A follow-up of the above observational studies, and there is a paucity of evidence
Dutch study done by van der Pol and colleagues found from placebo-controlled, double-blind studies.55 For
that cannabis smoking topography (increased puff example, a major confounding factor that is not adequately
volume and duration) predicted the severity of cannabis addressed in many studies to date is tobacco, which is
dependence 1·5 years later, after adjusting for baseline frequently co-administered with cannabis, and has been
levels of dependence.54 Taken together, these findings found to be associated with later incidents of psychosis.57
suggest that cannabis users might partially (but not Another major limitation in studies that has been
completely) adapt to changes in potency by titrating either identified in this Review relates to the measurement of
the amount they smoke or their inhalation. The cannabis use. Most studies evaluate the harms of cannabis
contrasting effects of cannabis between adults and use by employing duration and frequency but neglect
adolescents is further highlighted by Mokrysz and measures of cannabis potency or quantification of THC
colleagues.55 When measuring a range of acute effects and cannabidiol concentrations. Self-reported data for
following the inhalation of vaporised active or placebo potency might be limited by the wide range of THC and
cannabis, it was found that adolescent participants (aged cannabidiol concentrations within cannabis products.
16–17 years) felt less stoned and experienced lower However, previous data have validated self-reported
psychotic-like symptoms and anxiety compared with cannabis type against actual THC and cannabidiol
adults (aged 24–28 years). Furthermore, adults demon­ concentrations measured in the laboratory.38 Even though
strated a greater impairment in reaction time on spatial laboratory tests are more precise, they are far less feasible
working memory and prose recall tasks. Where adults for estimating long-term patterns of use (eg, by repeatedly
expressed satiety, adolescents did not and instead wanted sampling an individual’s cannabis use across their
more cannabis, regardless of whether they were taking lifespan). We therefore recommend that the assessment
the active drug or placebo. It could therefore be suggested of cannabis potency should accompany questions about
that the increased drive for the rewarding properties of frequency and duration in healthcare and research
cannabis is a possible contributing factor to escalating settings. Pictorial aids (as illustrated in the figure) and
use among young people.55 In conclusion, cannabis use verbal descriptions might be helpful for identifying
behaviour, such as understanding cannabis potency, different cannabis products. Moreover, researchers should
titration, satiety, and acute cannabis effects, is an use laboratory tests to calculate precise concentrations of
important factor to consider when assessing the harms of THC and cannabidiol in cannabis where possible. Unlike
cannabis use in adolescents. Although future research the standard units of alcohol used in the alcohol literature,
must account for cannabis type, cannabis use behaviours there are currently no agreed standards for measuring
also contribute to determining the amount of THC cannabis.58 The implementation of standardised units of
consumed by young people, and thus the potential harms cannabis use could vastly improve our understanding of
they are exposed to. variation in cannabis use and its consequences on
adolescent health.
Limitations
Even though evidence from several US states and Future research
countries report increases in cannabis potency,7,8,17,24 there As a result of the limitations of current research strategies,
are several limitations in the data. First, most of the data there are several directions for future investigations. As
are based on police confiscations, which might result in there is a gap in the research focusing on adolescence,
sampling bias. However, there is no reason to believe there is also an absence of cognitive and neuroimaging
that this sampling bias varies with time, so this probably measures when focusing on potency.59 Whereas some
does not account for the increases in potency observed in studies have imaged the brains of cannabis users, they
global cannabis markets. Moreover, data collected in the have either been restricted to the limitations associated

www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0 5


Review

that although existing information and prevention


Search strategy and selection criteria programmes have resulted in small reductions in drug
This Review originated from a comprehensive search of the use, the most effective programmes have been those that
literature via PubMed, Google Scholar, and the authors’ own involve a combination of drug information, social skills
files. The search involved key terms such as “adolescence”, training (eg, goal setting and decision making), and
“cannabis”, “early-onset cannabis use”, “cannabis potency”, anti-drug resistance skills training.66
“cannabis harms”, and “delta-9-tetrahydrocannabiol, Programmes must also be evidence-based, yet despite
THC: Cannabidiol, cannabidiol”. As research focusing on this, those that include information on potency or
adolescent cannabis use is scarce, this Review included cannabis type are scarce. An internet-delivered pro­
articles written in English published in the past 15 years gramme, the Climate Schools, educated users on the
(January, 2003, to September, 2018), except for original importance of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol content in
citations for measurements (eg, stop-signal task). Finally, cannabis. This programme was efficacious in improving
the decision to include articles was made on the basis of their knowledge of the drug and reducing its frequency of
relevance within the scope of this Review. use.67 Future prevention programmes must allow for the
discussion of how cannabis types differ in constituents,
availability, risks, and harms, so that adolescents are
with hair analysis or have not included supplementary equipped with up-to-date evidence,68 allowing for the
cognitive measures.60,61 Studies including these measures, prevention or delay of cannabis use among those most
alongside an accurate measure of cannabis potency, would susceptible to its detrimental effects.
allow for a better understanding of the neurocognitive Contributors
effects across different cannabis products, both in the CJM had the idea for this Review. JW conducted the literature search and
long term and short term. wrote the initial draft. All authors contributed to the writing and editing
of the paper and agreed the final version.
With cannabis policy rapidly evolving, there is a
possibility that further countries and states will legalise Declaration of interests
All authors declare no competing interests.
recreational cannabis use alongside existing US states and
Canada. Although it is important to recommend that age- Acknowledgments
This research has been supported in part by the Parents’ and Carers’
related restrictions for ultra-high potency products be Leave Fund awarded to CJM by King’s College London. TPF was
guided by evidence-based public health research, it is supported by the Senior Academic Fellowship award from the Society for
acknowledged that the legal age of purchase of cannabis is the Study of Addiction. We would like to thank David Potter for giving us
often based on that for alcohol. Furthermore, although permission to use the photos in the figure.

current legal frameworks in the USA allow for legal References


1 United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. World Drug Report
cannabis potency and price to be set by the market, policy 2015. Vienna: United Nations publication, 2015.
makers should consider the implementation of THC unit 2 Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Miech RA, Bachman JG,
taxes or THC thresholds.8 For example, if harm increases Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national survey results on
drug use, 1975–2016: overview, key findings on adolescent drug use.
as the price per unit of THC decreases, setting an Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of
acceptable level of tax per THC unit could help minimise Michigan, 2017.
harm. By contrast, if the potency of cannabis products is 3 Degenhardt L, Ferrari AJ, Calabria B, et al. The global epidemiology
more important (irrespective of price), then setting an and contribution of cannabis use and dependence to the global
burden of disease: results from the GBD 2010 study. PLOS One
upper limit on THC concentration could be more effective. 2013; 8: e76635.
Furthermore, to fully evaluate the health consequences of 4 European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction.
changes in cannabis use, it will be essential to determine European drug report 2017: trends and developments. Luxembourg:
Publications Office of the European Union, 2017.
the extent to which cannabis can act as a substitute or a 5 Montanari L, Guarita B, Mounteney J, Zipfel N, Simon R.
complement to other drugs, such as tobacco or alcohol.62 Cannabis use among people entering drug treatment in Europe:
Given the growing body of research on cannabis potency a growing phenomenon? Eur Addict Res 2017; 23: 113–21.
6 Freeman TP, van der Pol P, Kuijpers W, et al. Changes in cannabis
and cannabis-related harms, there is now a pressing need potency and first-time admissions to drug treatment: a 16-year study
to understand how different types of cannabis products in the Netherlands. Psychol Med 2018; 48: 2346–52.
affect adolescent health. 7 ElSohly MA, Mehmedic Z, Foster S, Gon C, Chandra S, Church JC.
Changes in cannabis potency over the last 2 decades (1995–2014):
With changes in policy potentially making cannabis analysis of current data in the United States. Biol Psych 2016;
increasingly accessible to adolescents in several states 79: 613–19.
and countries worldwide, effective prevention and 8 Smart R, Caulkins JP, Kilmer B, Davenport S, Midgette G.
information is critical. Approximately half of all first-time Variation in cannabis potency and prices in a newly legal market:
evidence from 30 million cannabis sales in Washington state.
cannabis use occurs before the age of 18 years.63 With Addiction 2017; 112: 2167–77.
evidence suggesting that risk perception is more difficult 9 Sawyer SM, Azzopardi PS, Wickremarathne D, Patton GC.
to alter after first-time use rather than before,64 it is The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc Health 2018;
2: 223–28.
important that cannabis use is targeted for prevention at 10 Chadwick B, Miller ML, Hurd YL. Cannabis use during adolescent
an early age, regardless of the differential risk of use in development: susceptibility to psychiatric illness. Front Psychiatry
adolescents.65 Furthermore, a Cochrane review reported 2013; 4: 129.

6 www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0


Review

11 Curran HV, Freeman TP, Mokrysz C, Lewis DA, Morgan CJA, 34 Tapert SF, Schweinsburg AD, Drummond SPA, et al.
Parsons LH. Keep off the grass? Cannabis, cognition and addiction. Functional MRI of inhibitory processing in abstinent adolescent
Nat Rev Neurosci 2016; 17: 293–306. marijuana users. Psychopharmacology 2007; 194: 173–83.
12 Keimpema E, Mackie K, Harkany T. Molecular model of cannabis 35 Drummond SP, Bischoff-Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L,
sensitivity in developing neuronal circuits. Trends Pharmacol Sci Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor
2011; 32: 551–61. vigilance task. Sleep 2005; 28: 1059–68.
13 D’Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, et al. The psychotomimetic 36 Lopez-Larson MP, Bogorodzki P, Rogowska J, et al. Altered
effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy prefrontal and insular cortical thickness in adolescent marijuana
individuals: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology users. Behav Brain Res 2011; 220: 164–72.
2004; 29: 1558–72. 37 Henquet C, Di Forti M, Morrison P, Kuepper R, Murray RM.
14 Englund A, Morrison PD, Nottage J, et al. Cannabidiol inhibits Gene-environment interplay between cannabis and psychosis.
THC-elicited paranoid symptoms and hippocampal-dependent Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 1111–21.
memory impairment. J Psychopharmacol 2013; 27: 19–27. 38 Freeman TP, Morgan CJA, Hindocha C, Schafer G, Das RK,
15 Morgan CJA, Gardener C, Schafer G, et al. Sub-chronic impact of Curran HV. Just say ‘know’: how do cannabinoid concentrations
cannabinoids in street cannabis on cognition, psychotic-like influence users’ estimates of cannabis potency and the amount they
symptoms and psychological well-being. Psychol Med 2012; roll in joints? Addiction 2014; 109: 1686–94.
42: 391–400. 39 Korf DJ, Benschop A, Wouters M. Differential responses to
16 Potter DJ. A review of the cultivation and processing of cannabis cannabis potency: a typology of users based on self-reported
(Cannabis sativa L.) for production of prescription medicines in the consumption behaviour. Int J Drug Policy 2007; 18: 168–76.
UK. Drug Test Anal 2013; 6: 31–38. 40 Large M, Sharma S, Compton MT, Slade T, Nielssen O. Cannabis
17 Potter DJ, Hammond K, Tuffnell S, Walker C, Di Forti M. use and earlier onset of psychosis: a systematic meta-analysis.
Potency of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol and other cannabinoids in Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 555–61.
cannabis in England in 2016: implications for public health and 41 Mackie CJ, O’Leary-Barrett M, Al-Khudhairy N, et al.
pharmacology. Drug Test Anal 2018; 10: 628–35. Adolescent bullying, cannabis use and emerging psychotic
18 Raber JC, Elzinga S, Kaplan C. Understanding dabs: contamination experiences: a longitudinal general population study. Psychol Med
concerns of cannabis concentrates and cannabinoid transfer during 2012; 43: 1033–44.
the act of dabbing. J Toxicol Sci 2015; 40: 797–803. 42 Mackie CJ, Castellanos-Ryan N, Conrod PJ. Developmental
19 European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction. trajectories of psychotic-like experiences across adolescence:
European drug report 2016: trends and developments. 2016. impact of victimization and substance use. Psychol Med 2010;
http://www.emcdda.europa.eu/publications/edr/trends- 41: 47–58.
developments/2016_en (accessed Nov 30, 2018). Luxembourg: 43 Kuepper R, van Os J, Lieb R, Wittchen H-U, Höfler M, Henquet C.
Publications Office of the European Union. Continued cannabis use and risk of incidence and persistence of
20 Castaneto MS, Gorelick DA, Desrosiers NA, Hartman RL, Pirard S, psychotic symptoms: 10 year follow-up cohort study. BMJ 2011;
Huestis MA. Synthetic cannabinoids: Epidemiology, 342: d738.
pharmacodynamics, and clinical implications. Drug Alcohol Depend 44 Henquet C, Krabbendam L, Spauwen J, et al. Prospective cohort
2014; 144: 12–41. study of cannabis use, predisposition for psychosis, and psychotic
21 Trecki J, Gerona RR, Schwartz MD. Synthetic cannabinoid-related symptoms in young people. BMJ 2004; 330: 11.
illnesses and deaths. N Engl J Med 2015; 373: 103–07. 45 Di Forti M, Marconi A, Carra E, et al. Proportion of patients in
22 Swift W, Wong A, Li KM, Arnold JC, McGregor IS. Analysis of south London with first-episode psychosis attributable to use of
cannabis seizures in NSW, Australia: cannabis potency and high potency cannabis: a case-control study. Lancet Psychiatry 2015;
cannabinoid profile. PLoS One 2013; 8: e70052. 2: 233–38.
23 Niesink RJ, Rigter S, Koeter MW, Brunt TM. Potency trends of 46 Medina KL, Hanson KL, Schweinsburg AD, Cohen-Zion M,
delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabidiol and cannabinol in Nagel BJ, Tapert SF. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescent
cannabis in the Netherlands: 2005–15. Addiction 2015; marijuana users: subtle deficits detectable after a month of
110: 1941–50. abstinence. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007 13: 807–20.
24 Cascini F, Aiello C, Di Tanna G. Increasing delta-9- 47 Lisdahl KM, Price JS. Increased marijuana use and gender predict
tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ-9-THC) content in herbal cannabis over poorer cognitive functioning in adolescents and emerging adults.
time: systematic review and meta-analysis. Curr Drug Abuse Rev J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012; 18: 678–88.
2012; 5: 32–40. 48 Alexandre CJ, Waldo ZA, Rocio MS, et al. Cannabis and anxiety:
25 King LA, Carpentier C, Griffiths P. EMCDDA insights— a critical review of the evidence. Hum Psychopharmacol 2009;
an overview of cannabis potency in Europe. Luxembourg: Office for 24: 515–23.
Official Publications of the European Communities, 2015. 49 Lev-Ran S, Roerecke M, Le Foll B, George TP, McKenzie K, Rehm J.
26 Caulkins JP, Bao Y, Davenport S, et al. Big data on a big new The association between cannabis use and depression: a systematic
market: insights from Washington State’s legal cannabis market. review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Med 2013;
Int J Drug Policy 2018; 57: 86–94. 44: 797–810.
27 Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. 50 Gonzalez R, Pacheco-Colón I, Duperrouzel JC, Hawes SW.
Ann NY Acad Sci 2008; 1124: 111–26. Does cannabis use cause declines in neuropsychological
28 de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug functioning? A review of longitudinal studies. J Int Neuropsychol Soc
use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol 2009; 14: 22–31. 2017; 23: 893–902.
29 Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple 51 Schoeler T, Petros N, Di Forti M, et al. Effects of continuation,
and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. frequency, and type of cannabis use on relapse in the first 2 years
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1984; 10: 276–91. after onset of psychosis: an observational study. Lancet Psychiatry
30 Verbuggen F, Logan GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal 2016; 3: 947–53.
paradigm. Trends Cogn Sci 2008; 12: 418–24. 52 Chan GCK, Hall W, Freeman TP, Ferris J, Kelly AB, Winstock A.
31 Behan B, Connolly CG, Datwani S, et al. Response inhibition and User characteristics and effect profile of butane hash oil:
elevated parietal-cerebellar correlations in chronic adolescent an extremely high-potency cannabis concentrate.
cannabis users. Neuropharmacology 2014; 84: 131–37. Drug Alcohol Depend 2017; 178: 32–38.
32 Norman AL, Pulido C, Squeglia LM, Spadoni AD, Paulus MP, 53 Freeman TP, Winstock AR. Examining the profile of high-potency
Tapert SF. Neural activation during inhibition predicts initiation of cannabis and its association with severity of cannabis dependence.
substance use in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011; 119: 216–23. Psychol Med 2015; 45: 3181–89.
33 Mahmood OM, Goldenberg D, Thayer R, Migliorini R, 54 van der Pol P, Liebregts N, Brunt T, et al. Cross-sectional and
Simmons AN, Tapert SF. Adolescents’ fMRI activation to a response prospective relation of cannabis potency, dosing and smoking
inhibition task predicts future substance use. Addict Behav 2013; behaviour with cannabis dependence: an ecological study. Addiction
38: 1435–41. 2014; 109: 1101–09.

www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0 7


Review

55 Mokrysz C, Freeman TP, Korkki S, Griffiths K, Curran HV. 63 Degenhardt L, Stockings E, Patton G, Hall WD, Lynskey M.
Are adolescents more vulnerable to the harmful effects of cannabis The increasing global health priority of substance use in young
than adults? A placebo-controlled study in human males. people. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3: 251–64.
Transl Psychiatry 2016; 6: e961. 64 Salloum NC, Krauss MJ, Agrawal A, Bierut LJ, Grucza RA.
56 Pijlman FTA, Rigter SM, Hoek J, Goldschmidt HMJ, Niesink RJM. A reciprocal effects analysis of cannabis use and perceptions of risk.
Strong increase in total delta-THC in cannabis preparations sold in Addiction 2018; 113: 1077–85.
Dutch coffee shops. Addict Biology 2006; 10: 171–80. 65 Foxcroft D R, Tsertsvadze A. Universal school-based prevention
57 Gage SH, Hickman M, Heron J, et al. Associations of cannabis and programs for alcohol misuse in young people.
cigarette use with psychotic experiences at age 18: findings from the Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011; 5: CD009113.
Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Psychol Med 2014; 66 Faggiano F, Silvia M, Versino E, Daria B. Universal school-based
44: 3435–44. prevention for illicit drug use. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2014;
58 Hindocha C, Norberg MM, Tomko RL. Solving the problem of 12: CD003020.
cannabis quantification. Lancet Psychiatry 2018; 5: e8. 67 Newton NC, Andrews G, Teesson M, Vogl LE. Delivering prevention
59 Murray RM, Quigley H, Quattrone D, Englund A, Di Forti M. for alcohol and cannabis using the internet: a cluster randomised
Traditional marijuana, high-potency cannabis and synthetic controlled trial. Prev Med 2009; 48: 579–84.
cannabinoids: increasing risk for psychosis. World Psychiatry 2016; 68 Freeman TP, Wilson J, Mackie C. Commentary on Salloum et al.
15: 195–204. (2018): rethinking adolescent cannabis use and risk perception.
60 Yücel M, Lorenzetti V, Suo C, et al. Hippocampal harms, protection Addiction 2018; 113: 1086–87.
and recovery following regular cannabis use. Transl Psychiatry 2016;
6: e710. © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
61 Demirakca T, Sartorius A, Ende G, et al. Diminished gray matter in
the hippocampus of cannabis users: possible protective effects of
cannabidiol. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011; 114: 242–45.
62 Subbaraman MS. Substitution and complementarity of alcohol and
cannabis: a review of the literature. Subst Use Misuse 2016;
51: 1399–414.

8 www.thelancet.com/child-adolescent Published online December 17, 2018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30342-0

You might also like