You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221 – 227

Resilience, misfortune, and mortality: evidence that sense of coherence is a


marker of social stress adaptive capacity
Paul G. Surteesa,b,4, Nicholas W.J. Wainwrighta,b, Kay-Tee Khawc
a
Strangeways Research Laboratory, Worts Causeway, Cambridge, UK
b
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, University of Cambridge, Worts Causeway, Cambridge, UK
c
Clinical Gerontology Unit, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK

Received 16 November 2005; received in revised form 6 February 2006; accepted 16 February 2006

Abstract
Objective: The purpose of this study is to test the hypothesis that follow-up of 6.7 years, 1617 deaths were recorded. A one standard
sense of coherence (SOC) distinguishes adaptive capacity to adverse deviation increase in mean adaptation score (representing slower
event experience. Methods: A population-based cohort of 20,921 adaptation) was associated with a 6% increase in mortality rate
men and women completed a postal assessment of their lifetime ( P=.03) after adjusting for age and sex. Measures of event occur-
experience of specific adverse events and a measure of their SOC. rence and impact were less strongly associated with SOC and were
Reports of 111,857 events allowed construction of measures of event not significantly associated with mortality. Conclusion: These
impact and adaptation. Results: Those with a weak SOC reported results suggest that SOC is a potential marker of an individual’s
significantly slower adaptation to the adverse effects of their event social stress adaptive capacity, which is predictive of mortality.
experiences than those with a strong SOC ( Pb.0001). During mean D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Adaptation; Life events; Mortality; Resilience; Sense of coherence

Introduction of health status following adverse experience have been


associated with a diversity of outcomes, they have
The suggestion that nonspecific physiological systems provided strong evidence of the existence of individual
are activated by adverse agents that can be both health differences in resilience [5–10]. Such individual differences
protective and restorative but can also promote patho- have perhaps been most evident in studies of the health
genesis was expressed in the early work of Hans Selye [1], outcomes of people subsequent to their exposure to
represented through his concept of the bGeneral Adaptation profoundly stressful circumstances, including (for example)
Syndrome.Q Selye [2] considered that while no disease was concentration camp experience [11], shipwreck [12], and
bpurely a disease of adaptation . . . conversely, there is no terrorist attacks [13 – 16]. Not all individuals exposed to
disease in which adaptive phenomena play no partQ adverse or even traumatic circumstance experience health
(p. 630). Subsequent work has built upon this insight, for change [17,18]. In consequence, identification of individual
example, through the concepts of ballostasisQ (defined as an differences in adaptive capacity to adversity exposure
adaptive process to achieve stability through change) and could aid understanding of disease susceptibility and
ballostatic loadQ (representative of the cumulative physio- advance coping research.
logical cost of adaptation) [3,4]. In addition, while studies Sense of coherence (SOC) is a theoretical construct
founded upon the observations of Holocaust survivors.
Antonovsky et al [19] defined SOC to represent the
4 Corresponding author. Strangeways Research Laboratory, Worts
Causeway, CB1 8RN Cambridge, UK. Tel.: +44 1223 740651; fax: +44
salutogenic resources available to an individual through
1223 740147. the belief that what happens in their life is comprehensible
E-mail address: paul.surtees@srl.cam.ac.uk (P.G. Surtees). (that is rational, predictable, structured, and understand-

0022-3999/06/$ – see front matter D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.02.014
222 P.G. Surtees et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221–227

able), manageable (in that adequate and sufficient resources Adverse events in adulthood
are perceived to be available to aid difficulty resolution as
they arise), and meaningful (such that the demands created Evidence of lifetime adverse event experience was
by adversity exposure are seen as challenges and are worthy limited to circumstances considered most likely to be
of engagement). Sense of coherence was hypothesized, remembered reliably over an extended period. These were
therefore, as a flexible and adaptive dispositional orientation based upon those included in the questionnaire version of
enabling successful coping with adverse experience [20]. the List of Threatening Experiences [25,26]. A total of 16
Based upon data collected from participants in the Norfolk specific adverse events were assessed, including serious
(UK) European Prospective Investigation into Cancer illnesses (injuries or assaults) experienced by the participant
(EPIC-Norfolk) study [21], we have previously demonstra- (or a first-degree relative), relationship events (concerning
ted SOC to be strongly associated with mortality [22]. We separation, divorce, or termination of pregnancy), work
now test the hypothesis that SOC distinguishes adaptive events (retirement, redundancy, or being fired), and loss
capacity to adverse event experience, specifically, that a experiences through death (of first-degree relatives). Partic-
strong SOC will be associated with the report of more rapid ipants were requested to report up to two most recent
adaptation than a weak SOC and, secondly, that social stress experiences of each specific event. Opportunity was also
adaptive capacity, indexed through reports of actual provided to report (up to two) other particularly unpleasant
experience of stressful life events, will be associated with or disappointing lifetime event experiences. In addition to
all-cause mortality. event timing (within one year of occurrence), participants
were asked to rate the impact of each event (on a four-point
scale: not at all, a little, moderately, extremely) through
Method answering the question, bHow much did this upset you at
the time?Q and to rate their degree of adaptation to each
Participants and measures event experience by the time of assessment (also on a four-
point scale: completely, mostly, a little, not at all) through
During 1993 to 1997, participants were recruited to the answering the question bDo you feel that you have got over
EPIC-Norfolk study through general practice age–sex this now?Q
registers. The study was approved by the Norwich District
Health Authority Ethics Committee, and all participants Statistical analysis
gave signed informed consent (see Ref. [21] for further
details of study design and participant assessments). The For each participant, the total number of events and mean
social and psychological status of 20,921 participants (9101 impact and adaptation scores of all events reported were
men and 11,820 women) was assessed during 1996 to 2000 calculated. Events involving participant’s own illness were
through their completion of the Health and Life Experiences excluded from analysis. For each event, responses to the
Questionnaire (HLEQ), with a response rate of 73.2% of the impact and adaptation questions were assigned numerical
total eligible sample of 28,582 (see Ref. [23] for further values on a scale from 1 to 4 (for impact: not at all=1, a
details). All deaths among EPIC-Norfolk HLEQ study little=2, moderately=3, extremely=4; and for adaptation:
participants to 31st October 2004 were recorded through completely=1, mostly=2, a little=3, not at all=4). Where no
linkage with data from the United Kingdom Office for events were reported, impact and adaptation scores could not
National Statistics. be assigned. Analysis was repeated according to event
subsets of divorce/separation, loss (death of first-degree
Sense of coherence relatives), retirement, nonspecific (unpleasant or disappoint-
ing) lifetime event experiences, and other events (not
The HLEQ included a three-item SOC questionnaire [24] otherwise listed). Differences in the mean number of events
designed to assess the component constructs of comprehen- reported, mean impact, and mean adaptation scores were
sibility, manageability, and meaningfulness by single ques- evaluated through ANOVA and are presented according to
tions: (a) bDo you usually feel that the things that happen to weak and strong SOC (scored previously as strong=0, 1, and
you in your daily life are hard to understand?Q (comprehen- weak=2 to 6 [22]). Effect sizes are presented as differences in
sibility); (b) bDo you usually see a solution to problems and means divided by the standard deviation (S.D.) (of scale
difficulties that other people find hopeless?Q (manageability), scores). Progressive adaptation to events with increasing
and (c) bDo you usually feel that your daily life is a source of elapsed time is presented graphically as the mean reported
personal satisfaction?Q (meaningfulness). Response choice adaptation score for all events experienced in each year
was yes, usually (scored 0); yes, sometimes (scored 1); and relative to the time of questionnaire completion. Means and
no (scored 2). Comprehensibility was reverse scored. The 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the total number of events
three items were summed to provide a total SOC scale score reported and their impact and adaptation are presented
within the range 0 to 6 with a higher score representing a graphically according to increasing SOC scale score
weaker SOC. (increasingly weak SOC). For ease of comparison, the scale
P.G. Surtees et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221–227 223

range of the y-axis of each graph is set at two S.D.’s for such that those with a weak SOC reported that their event
each event variable. Because impact scores varied according experiences upset them more than for those with a strong
to event type, and adaptation scores varied according to both SOC (effect size=0.23). However, the largest difference was
event type and elapsed time since event occurrence, analysis observed for adaptation scores, whereby those with a weak
was repeated using indices of impact (I-Impact) and SOC reported that they had not got over the events
adaptation (I-Adapt) constructed to take this into account experienced to the same extent as those with a strong
(see Appendix for further details). Percentage variation in SOC (effect size=0.37). Participants with a weak SOC
SOC scale scores was calculated through the R 2 statistic reported less retirement events than those with a strong SOC
from linear regression. Finally, Poisson regression models, (a difference that persisted with adjustment for age) but
taking account of duration of follow-up, were used to more of all other events (with the biggest difference being
investigate the associations between the event variables and for nonspecific events). Reported impact scores were higher
all-cause mortality. Results are presented as rate ratios (95% for those with a weak SOC for all event types, with the
CIs) per S.D. increase in each event variable. largest difference being for retirement events. Adaptation
scores were consistently higher across all event types
(representing slower adaptation) for those with a weak as
Results compared to a strong SOC.
Fig. 1A shows mean adaptation scores plotted according
Completed SOC scores were available for 20,579 (of to the timing of (all) reported adverse event experiences
20,921) HLEQ participants (8974 men and 11,605 women, relative to questionnaire completion. The figure confirms
aged between 41 and 80 years). Internal consistency of the that those with a weak SOC report slower adaptation to the
three-item SOC scale, as measured by Chronbach’s a, was adverse effects of events than those with a strong SOC, and
.35. Of those participants who completed the scale, 8619 also reveals that differences persist for many years. Fig. 1B–
were classified as having a strong SOC and 11,960 as F shows that these differences in adaptation are consistent
having a weak SOC. The mean SOC scale score was 1.84 across all event types. For all events experienced (Fig. 1A),
(S.D.=1.15). Women reported a weaker SOC than men (with the mean difference in adaptation scores for those with a
mean scores 1.68 and 1.96 for men and women, respec- weak as compared to strong SOC was greatest for those
tively, Pb.0001). Sense of coherence was strongest for those events experienced within 10 years of HLEQ completion
aged 60 to 69 years (mean scores 1.88, 1.86, 1.78, 1.88, for (mean difference=0.28, effect size=0.38) and was stable
those aged 41 to 49, 50 to 59, 60 to 69, 70 to 80 years, thereafter [difference=0.17, effect size=0.29, for events
respectively, Pb.0001). experienced 10 to 19 years before questionnaire completion,
The total number of adverse events reported was 111,857 0.16 (0.26) for events between 20 and 29 years ago, and
(mean=5.35, S.D.=2.48, range=0 to 18 out of a possible 0.16 (0.25) for events experienced 30 years or more prior to
maximum of 27 including previous occurrences). Women questionnaire completion].
reported more events than men (with a mean of 5.27 events Indices both of impact (I-Impact) and adaptation (I-
reported by men and 5.41 by women, Pb.0001). Of these Adapt) were calculated to take account of variations by
events, 5513 were divorce/separation events, 19,783 retire- event type and by timing of event exposures (see
ment events, 9565 nonspecific events, 40,642 loss, and Appendix). I-Impact scores were higher (representing
36,354 other events. greater impact) for women than for men (mean
Table 1 shows the mean numbers of reported events scores= 0.28 for men, and 0.22 for women, effect
together with their mean impact and adaptation scores size=0.50) and were lower (representing lower impact) for
according to SOC scale score. While those with a weak participants who were older (mean scores 0.12, 0.11, 0.04,
SOC reported more events than those with a strong SOC, and 0.16 for those aged 41 to 49, 50 to 59, 60 to 69, and
the magnitude of this difference was small (effect 70 to 80 years, respectively; effect size across age
size=0.04). A more pronounced difference was observed range=0.29). I-Adapt scores were higher (representing

Table 1
Mean number of adverse life events in adulthood, mean impact, and mean adaptation according to a weak vs. a strong SOC
Mean number of events Mean impact Mean adaptation
a a
SOC Weak Strong ES P value Weak Strong ES P value Weak Strong ESa P value
Any event 5.41 5.32 0.04 .006 3.13 2.98 0.23 b.0001 1.57 1.37 0.37 b.0001
Divorce/separation event 0.27 0.26 0.03 .04 3.36 3.27 0.09 .002 1.44 1.24 0.34 b.0001
Retirement events 0.91 1.00 0.10 b.0001 1.61 1.43 0.23 b.0001 1.25 1.12 0.25 b.0001
Loss events 1.98 1.90 0.07 b.0001 3.53 3.44 0.14 b.0001 1.64 1.45 0.30 b.0001
Non specific event 0.50 0.44 0.09 b.0001 3.82 3.77 0.12 b.0001 2.05 1.73 0.35 b.0001
Other events 1.77 1.73 0.03 .04 3.23 3.12 0.13 b.0001 1.45 1.28 0.28 b.0001
a
ES=effect size=difference in means/S.D.
224 P.G. Surtees et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221–227

Fig. 1. Mean adaptation scores by elapsed time: (A) all adverse events in adulthood; (B) divorce/separation events; (C) retirement events; (D) loss events;
(E) nonspecific events; and (F) other events, for those with a strong (solid line) and weak (dashed line) SOC.

slower adaptation) for women than for men (mean magnitude of differences was greater as compared to their
scores= 0.17 for men, and 0.13 for women; effect respective crude measures (data not shown). The percentage
size=0.30) and were lower (representing more rapid variation in SOC scale scores explained by these adverse
adaptation) for participants who were older (mean scores event measures was least for the total number of adverse
0.10, 0.08, 0.06, and 0.09 for those aged 41 to 49, 50 to events experienced (only 0.1% of variation explained),
59, 60 to 69, and 70 to 80 years, respectively; effect size greater for impact scores (1.9%), and greater still for
across age range=0.19). adaptation (5.6%). Percentage variation explained by I-
Fig. 2 shows the mean number of events reported, mean Impact was 2.0% and by I-Adapt was 5.8%.
impact, and mean adaptation according to increasing SOC During mean follow-up of 6.7 years, 1617 deaths were
score (progressively weaker SOC). This reveals that recorded (including 939 men and 678 women). Table 2
increasingly weak SOC was associated with progressive shows that neither adverse event experience nor impact was
increases in all of these adverse event measures. In addition, associated with mortality, but that stress adaptive capacity
it shows that the magnitude of these differences was again was associated with mortality. One S.D. increase in
least for the total number of events reported, greater for adaptation was associated with a 6% increase in mortality
impact scores, and greater still for adaptation. The pattern of rate ( P=.03) after adjustment for age and sex. Similarly, I-
results was the same for I-Impact and I-Adapt, and the Impact was not associated with mortality, whereas a 1 S.D.

Fig. 2. Association (means and 95% CIs) between measures of adverse event experience in adulthood and SOC score (higher score represents progressively
weaker SOC). (A) Number of events; (B) impact scores; (C) adaptation scores. Plotted range on y-axis is standardized at two S.D.’s for each adversity variable.
P.G. Surtees et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221–227 225

Table 2 this sample (Chronbach’s a=.35), this is likely to be partially


Association between the number of adverse events reported, mean impact
due to the small number of scale items. In addition, the
and mean adaptation (of all events reported), and all-cause mortality
(adjusted for age and sex) original developers of the scale reported satisfactory short-
term test–retest reliability and validity for the three-item
Deaths Rate ratio 95% CI
measure (see Ref. [24] for further details). However, other
1617
studies using longer scales have questioned the stability of
Events reported 0.98 (0.93–1.03)
Mean impact 1.03 (0.98–1.09) SOC over time periods of up to 5 years [29,30].
Mean adaptation 1.06 (1.01–1.12) Previous analysis of this cohort revealed a strong
Rate ratios (95% CIs) per S.D. increase in each measure. relationship between SOC and all-cause mortality, such that
a one S.D. increase in SOC scale score (representing weaker
increase in I-Adapt score was associated with a 7% increase SOC) was associated with a 19% increased rate of mortality
in mortality rate after adjustment for age and sex (rate (see Ref. [22,31] for further details), and with some support
ratio=1.07; 95% CI, 1.02–1.13; P=.008). from other work [32]. While the association observed here
between adaptation to the adverse effects of life events and
all-cause mortality was not of the same magnitude, together,
Discussion these results provide evidence that the association between
SOC and mortality may be partially mediated by an
This study has provided strong evidence in support of the individual’s capacity to deal with stressful situations.
hypothesis that SOC distinguishes adaptive capacity to While recent evidence has been published demonstrating
adverse event experience. Specifically, study participants that SOC moderates the impact of negative life events
with a strong SOC reported more rapid adaptation to their (experienced during the previous 12 months) on self-reported
experience of social adversity than those with a weak SOC, health status [33], we are unaware of any other study that has
whether social adversity was defined by all types of events reported a comprehensive test of the hypothesis that SOC is a
or just when restricted to specific circumstances. In addition, marker of social stress adaptive capacity with which to
the results suggested that a relatively slower capacity to compare findings. However, our results may be seen to
adapt to the consequences of adverse experience was contribute to recent debates concerning individual differ-
associated with increased mortality, whether adaptive ences in the human capacity to cope with adverse experience.
capacity was defined by either a crude (mean) measure of In particular, these include issues arising from resilience
adaptation or the I-Adapt index, that included consideration research [9,18,34], from the study of positive (change or)
of adverse event type and event timing. In contrast, adaptation to trauma [35,36], from wisdom research
measures of event occurrence and impact were less strongly [35,37,38], and from tests of set-point theory [39]. With
associated with SOC and were not associated with mortality. differing degrees of emphasis, these interrelated research
These results may be seen to provide support to some of the areas have focused on identifying and extending under-
fundamental ideas underlying Antonovsky’s concept of standing of individual differences (or contexts) that differ-
salutogenesis, including a focus on bactive adaptation to entiate the process of human adaptation to adversity.
an inevitably stressor-rich environmentQ (see Ref. [20], p. 9), For example, research has suggested that higher mental
recognition that bthe greater the stressor load, the more ability assessed in childhood is inversely associated with
important the role of salutary factorsQ (see Ref. [27], p. 159), adult mortality [40,41], with speculation that the association
and that adverse experiences bare open-ended in their may, at least in part, implicate psychometric intelligence as a
proximal as well as their distal consequences for healthQ significant influence on effective health self-care [42]. Other
(see Ref. [28], p. 970). recent work has shown a strong SOC to be associated with
While the current study has major design strengths, improved health biomarkers [43,44] and with the adoption of
including the cohort size (with nearly 21,000 participants health-promoting dietary habits [45]. The results of this study
and over 100,000 adverse life events assessed), and may therefore stimulate investigation of the joint association
prospective ascertainment of mortality (with 1617 deaths between SOC and wisdom-related measures. Future evalua-
from all-causes over a 7-year follow-up), a number of tion of set-point theory may also benefit through consid-
potential limitations may restrict the generalizability of eration of interrelationships with SOC. Recent tests, based
findings. The study cohort is an older population (age range, upon assessment of progressive adaptation following marital
41 to 80 years). The study design includes a retrospective status changes, including death of partner [46] and following
account of lifetime experience of stressful life events, the unemployment [39], noted substantial individual differences
design relies on a single assessment of SOC contempora- in the capacity of individuals to adapt to these experiences,
neous with the assessment of adversity, and SOC was and that these were probably influenced by personality.
operationalized by Antonovsky [20] initially as a 29-item Collectively, therefore, our findings strengthen the rationale
questionnaire, whereas the assessment in this study was underlying these research areas, underpin other evidence
based upon a simplified three-item measure. While the concerning the human capacity for resilience, show SOC
internal consistency of the three-item measure was low in powerfully to distinguish social stress adaptive capacity, and
226 P.G. Surtees et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221–227

provide evidence to our knowledge for the first time that [18] Bonanno GA. Loss, trauma, and human resilience: have we under-
estimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive
individual differences in stress adaptability are predictive of
events? Am Psychol 2004;59:20 – 8.
all-cause mortality. [19] Antonovsky A, Maoz B, Dowty N, Wijsenbeek H. Twenty-five years
later: a limited study of the sequelae of the concentration camp
experience. Soc Psychiatr 1971;81:186 – 93.
Acknowledgments [20] Antonovsky A. Unraveling the mystery of health. San Francisco
(Calif)7 Jossey-Bass; 1987.
EPIC-Norfolk is supported by program grants from the [21] Day N, Oakes S, Luben R, Khaw KT, Bingham S, Welch A, Wareham
Medical Research Council UK (G9502233, G0300128) and N. EPIC-Norfolk: study design and characteristics of the cohort. Br J
Cancer 1999;80(Supplement 1);95 – 103.
Cancer Research UK (C865/A2883) with additional support
[22] Surtees PG, Wainwright NWJ, Luben R, Khaw KT, Day NE. Sense of
from the European Union, Stroke Association, British Heart coherence and mortality in men and women in the EPIC-Norfolk UK
Foundation, Department of Health, Food Standards Agency, prospective cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2003;158:1202 – 9.
and the Wellcome Trust. [23] Surtees PG, Wainwright NWJ, Khaw KT, Day NE. Functional health
status, chronic medical conditions and disorders of mood. Br J
Psychiatry 2003;183:299 – 303.
[24] Lundberg O, Nystrfm Peck M. A simplified way of measuring sense
References of coherence: experiences from a population survey in Sweden. Eur J
Public Health 1995;5:56 – 9.
[1] Selye H. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature [25] Brugha T, Bebbington P, Tennant C, Hurry J. The list of threatening
1936;138:. experiences—a subset of 12 life event categories with considerable
[2] Selye H. Stress and disease. Science 1955;122:625 – 31. long-term contextual threat. Psychol Med 1985;15:189 – 94.
[3] McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. [26] Brugha TS, Cragg D. The list of threatening experiences—the
N Engl J Med 1998;338:171 – 9. reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta
[4] McEwen BS. Neurobiology of interpreting and responding to stressful Psychiatr Scand 1990;82:77 – 81.
events: paradigmatic role of the hippocampus. In: McEwen BS, editor. [27] Antonovsky A. Personality and health: testing the sense of coherence
Handbook of physiology Section 7: The endocrine system Volume 4: model. In: Friedman HS, editor. Personality and disease. Chichester7
Coping with the environment: neural and endocrine mechanisms. Wiley; 1990. p. 155 – 77.
New York7 Oxford University Press; 2001. p. 155 – 78. [28] Antonovsky A. Complexity, conflict, chaos, coherence, coercion and
[5] Brown GW, Harris TO. Life events and illness. New York7 Guilford civility. Soc Sci Med 1993;37:969 – 74.
Press; 1989. [29] Nilsson B, Holmgren L, Stegmayr B, Westman G. Sense of
[6] Cohen S, Tyrrell DAJ, Smith AP. Psychological stress and suscept- coherence—stability over time and relation to health, disease, and
ibility to the common cold. N Engl J Med 1991;325:606 – 12. psychosocial changes in a general population: a longitudinal study.
[7] Hansen D, Lou HC, Olsen J. Serious life events and congenital Scand J Public Health 2003;31:297 – 304.
malformations: a national study with complete follow-up. Lancet [30] Smith PM, Breslin FC, Beaton DE. Questioning the stability of sense
2000;356:875 – 80. of coherence—the impact of socio-economic status and working
[8] Lillberg K, Verkasalo PK, Kaprio J, Teppo L, Helenius H, Koskenvuo conditions in the Canadian population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr
M. Stressful life events and risk of breast cancer in 10,808 women: a Epidemiol 2003;38:475 – 84.
cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2003;157:415 – 23. [31] Surtees PG, Wainwright NWJ, Luben R, Khaw KT, Day NE. Mastery,
[9] Bonanno GA. Clarifying and extending the construct of adult sense of coherence and mortality: evidence of independent associations
resilience. Am Psychol 2005;60:265 – 7. from the EPIC-Norfolk prospective cohort study. Health Psychol
[10] Li J, Precht DH, Laursen TM, Precht DH, Olsen J, Mortensen PB. 2006;25:102 – 10.
Hospitalization for mental illness among parents after the death of a [32] Poppius E, Tenkanen L, Hakama M, Kalimo R, Pitkanen T. The sense
child. N Engl J Med 2005;352:1190 – 6. of coherence, occupation and all-cause mortality in the Helsinki Heart
[11] Dimsdale JE. The coping behavior of Nazi concentration camp Study. Eur J Epidemiol 2003;18:389 – 93.
survivors. Am J Psychiatry 1974;131:792 – 7. [33] Richardson CG, Ratner PA. Sense of coherence as a moderator of the
[12] Henderson S, Bostock T. Coping behaviour after shipwreck. Br J effects of stressful life events on health. J Epidemiol Community
Psychiatry 1977;131:15 – 20. Health 2005;59:979 – 84.
[13] North CS, Nixon SJ, Shariat S, Mallonee S, McMillen JC, Spitznagel [34] Luthar SS, Zelazo LB. Research on resilience: an integrative review.
EM, Smith EM. Psychiatric disorders among survivors of the In: Luthar SS, editor. Resilience and vulnerability: adaptation in the
Oklahoma City bombing. JAMA 1999;282:755 – 62. context of childhood adversities. New York7 Cambridge University
[14] Galea S, Ahern J, Resnick H, Kilpatrick D, Bucuvalas M, Gold J, Press; 2003. p. 510 – 49.
Vlahov D. Psychological sequelae of the September 11 terrorist [35] Linley PA. Positive adaptation to trauma: wisdom as both process and
attacks in New York City. N Engl J Med 2002;346:982 – 7. outcome. J Trauma Stress 2003;16:601 – 10.
[15] Schlenger WE, Caddell JM, Ebert L, Jordan BK, Rourke KM, Wilson [36] Linley PA, Joseph S. Positive change following trauma and adversity:
L, Thalji L, Dennis JM, Fairbank JA, Kulka RA. Psychological a review. J Trauma Stress 2004;17:11 – 21.
reactions to terrorist attacks—findings from the national study of [37] Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. Wisdom—a metaheuristic (pragmatic) to
Americans’ reactions to September 11. JAMA 2002;288:581 – 8. orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. Am Psychol 2000;
[16] Bleich A, Gelkopf M, Solomon Z. Exposure to terrorism, stress- 55:122 – 35.
related mental health symptoms, and coping behaviors among a [38] Ardelt M. Wisdom as expert knowledge system: a critical review of a
nationally representative sample in Israel. JAMA 2003;290:612 – 20. contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept. Hum Dev
[17] Ryff CD, Singer B. Thriving in the face of challenge: the integrative 2004;47:257 – 85.
science of human resilience. In: Kessel F, Rosenfield P, Anderson N, [39] Lucas RE, Clark AE, Georgellis Y, Diener E. Unemployment alters
editors. Expanding the boundaries of health and social science: case the set point for life satisfaction. Psychol Sci 2004;15:8 – 13.
studies in interdisciplinary innovation. New York7 Oxford University [40] Whalley LJ, Deary IJ. Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and
Press; 2003. p. 181 – 205. survival up to age 76. BMJ 2001;322:819 – 22.
P.G. Surtees et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61 (2006) 221–227 227

[41] Martin LT, Kubzansky LD. Childhood cognitive performance and risk and for adaptation: completely=1, mostly=2, a little=3, not
of mortality: a prospective cohort study of gifted individuals. Am J
at all=4). Impact scores were centered according to event
Epidemiol 2005;162:887 – 90.
[42] Gottfredson LS, Deary IJ. Intelligence predicts health and longevity, type (through subtraction of the sample mean impact score
but why? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2004;13:1 – 4. for all reported events of the same event type), and
[43] Lutgendorf SK, Vitaliano PP, Tripp-Reimer T, Harvey JH, Lubaroff adaptation scores were centered according to event type
DM. Sense of coherence moderates the relationship between life stress and the elapsed time since event occurrence (through
and natural killer cell activity in healthy older adults. Psychol Aging subtraction of the sample mean adaptation score for all
1999;14:552 – 63.
[44] Lindfors P, Lundberg O, Lundberg U. Sense of coherence and events of the same type reported in the same year relative to
biomarkers of health in 43-year-old women. Int J Behav Med questionnaire completion). For each individual, I-Impact
2005;12:98 – 102. and I-Adapt were derived as the mean centered impact and
[45] Lindmark U, Stegmayr B, Nilsson B, Lindahl B, Johansson I. Food adaptation scores for all events reported by that individual.
selection associated with sense of coherence in adults. Nutr J 2005;4:
Where no events were reported (for 412 and 969 partic-
[doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-4-9].
[46] Lucas RE, Clark AE, Georgellis Y, Diener E. Reexamining adaptation
ipants, respectively), these indices could not be calculated.
and the set point model of happiness: reactions to changes in marital In addition, the S.D.’s of I-Impact and I-Adapt were greater
status. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003;84:527 – 39. for participants who reported fewer events (where the
number of event experiences on which I-Impact and I-
Appendix. Derivation of indices of event impact Adapt were based varied from 1 to 18 and 1 to 15,
(I-Impact) and adaptation (I-Adapt) respectively). Finally, therefore, I-Impact and I-Adapt were
standardized, first, such that their S.D.’s were constant
Indices of event impact (I-Impact) and adaptation (I- according to the number of events experienced, and second,
Adapt) were derived to take account of variations by event to have mean=0 and S.D.=1. A positive I-Impact score
type and (for adaptation) by event timing. For each event represents a higher impact of adverse events experienced
reported, responses to the impact and adaptation questions than the sample mean, and a positive I-Adapt score
were assigned numerical values on a scale from 1 to 4 (for represents slower adaptation to the adverse effects of the
impact: not at all=1, a little=2, moderately=3, extremely=4; adverse events experienced than the sample mean.

You might also like