You are on page 1of 23

Risk and Protective Factors Associated

With Speech and Language Impairment


in a Nationally Representative Sample
of 4- to 5-Year-Old Children

Linda J. Harrison
Sharynne McLeod
Purpose: To determine risk and protective factors for speech and language
Charles Sturt University, Bathurst, Australia
impairment in early childhood.
Method: Data are presented for a nationally representative sample of 4,983 children
participating in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (described in McLeod
& Harrison, 2009). Thirty-one child, parent, family, and community factors
previously reported as being predictors of speech and language impairment were
tested as predictors of (a) parent-rated expressive speech/language concern and
(b) receptive language concern, (c) use of speech-language pathology services, and
(d) low receptive vocabulary.
Results: Bivariate logistic regression analyses confirmed 29 of the identified factors.
However, when tested concurrently with other predictors in multivariate analyses,
only 19 remained significant: 9 for 2–4 outcomes and 10 for 1 outcome. Consistent
risk factors were being male, having ongoing hearing problems, and having a
more reactive temperament. Protective factors were having a more persistent and
sociable temperament and higher levels of maternal well-being. Results differed
by outcome for having an older sibling, parents speaking a language other than
English, and parental support for children’s learning at home.
Conclusion: Identification of children requiring speech and language assessment
requires consideration of the context of family life as well as biological and
psychosocial factors intrinsic to the child.
KEY WORDS: risk factor, protective factor, epidemiology, speech, language,
communication

S
peech and language acquisition in early childhood is a powerful
indicator of the developmental and cognitive abilities that under-
pin children’s successful transition to school (Nelson, Nygren, Walker,
& Panoscha, 2006). Longitudinal results from the U.S. National Institute
of Child Health and Human Development [NICHD] Study of Early Child
Care and Youth Development have demonstrated that “multiple path-
ways all funnel through one final common pathway, namely the child’s
language skills, just before entering school I to define the child’s ‘read-
iness’ for school” (NICHD, 2004, p. 28). These findings, particularly when
viewed in combination with prevalence studies (e.g., King et al., 2005;
Law, Boyle, Harris, Harkness, & Nye, 2000; McLeod & Harrison, 2009;
McLeod & McKinnon, 2007) showing that a significant proportion of chil-
dren do not successfully acquire speech and language prior to school, are
compelling. They point to the need to identify and provide support for

508 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010 • D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


children at risk of speech and language impairment describing parental, family, and neighborhood attributes,
in their early childhood years, which is when they are along with documentation of child health and psycho-
most likely to benefit from early intervention (Almost & social characteristics (e.g., Reilly et al., 2007; Zubrick,
Rosenbaum, 1998; Gibbard, Coglan, & MacDonald, 2004). Taylor, Rice, & Slegers, 2007). Such studies are able
Early detection and early intervention can reduce the to examine concurrently a large number of possible pre-
severity and longevity of speech and language difficul- dictors of speech and language delay or impairment and
ties (Gibbard et al., 2004; Schwarz & Nippold, 2002); use complex statistical analyses to identify the best set
however, many children who are eventually identified of predictors. Multivariable designs of this type, how-
as having speech and language impairment display no ever, are relatively rare in studies of speech and language
organic basis and few obvious indicators in the first years impairment. A systematic review of risk and protective
of life (Dollaghan & Campbell, 2009; Roulstone, Miller, factors associated with screening for speech and language
Wren, & Peters, 2009). Consequently, not all children who impairment in early childhood undertaken by the U.S.
may benefit from and be eligible for speech-language Preventative Services Task Force (Nelson et al., 2006;
pathology services are identified or referred prior to com- U.S. Preventative Services Task Force, 2006) showed
mencing school. that no studies encompassed all the potential predictor
Primary care professionals, such as doctors, nurses, domains (such as family history, child gender, socioeco-
and early childhood teachers, are often expected to iden- nomic status (SES), birth order, perinatal factors, paren-
tify children who may be at risk and may require a speech tal education, medical conditions, and other). Most of the
and language assessment to diagnose a speech and 16 studies reviewed had addressed only one or two of
language impairment. The methods that primary care these domains. Nelson et al. (2006) concluded “The most
professionals use to diagnose impairment tend to be consistently reported risk factors include a family history
(a) comparison with other children of a similar age, of speech and language delay, male gender, and perina-
(b) acknowledgment of parental concern, and (c) comple- tal factors; however, their role in screening is unclear”
tion of checklists of speech and language milestones such (p. e302).
as having fewer than 50 words or not combining words For the present study, articles included in the U.S.
at 24 months (e.g., Coplan, Gleason, Ryan, Bourke, & Preventative Services Task Force study (2006) have been
Williams, 1982; Luinge, Post, Wit, & Goorhuis-Brouwer, reviewed again, along with additional literature, to sum-
2006). Recognition and identification of known risk and marize evidence that confirms or disconfirms an associa-
protective factors is another method that can be employed, tion between each risk factor and childhood speech and
particularly when the above three screening methods are language impairment. Risk/protective factors are re-
either unavailable, inappropriate for a particular context, viewed within the domains identified by Nelson et al.
or lack sensitivity. Tomblin, Hardy, and Hein (1991) have (2006) plus additional domains: child hearing status, oral
recommended that “programs of preschool identification sucking habits, temperament, parent language spoken at
should consider the inclusion of a registry of children who home, minority status/race, maternal mental health and
are at risk for a communication disorder” (p. 1096). How- maternal age, family support for learning, family smok-
ever, as Nelson et al. (2006) concluded, “A list of spe- ing habits, and neighborhood disadvantage (see Table 1).
cific risk factors to guide primary care physicians in These domains are grouped within three broad categories:
selective screening has not been developed or tested” child, parent, and family/community. A brief summary of
(p. e302). the literature for each domain is presented, followed by
Current bioecological theories (Bronfenbrenner, 2005) consideration of design issues that may influence or ex-
provide a useful framework for addressing risk and pro- plain any differences in the reported findings.
tective factors for children’s health and development.
Bioecological models elucidate the interacting influ-
ences of proximal social and psychological contexts (e.g.,
Child Factors
parental and family characteristics) and distal social con- Sex. Sex of child was examined in 14 studies, as
texts (e.g., community characteristics and supports) with shown in Table 1. A significant association between be-
the inherited and biological characteristics of the indi- ing male and having an increased risk for speech and/or
vidual. This approach accords with the International language impairment was found in 11 of these studies
Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (World (Campbell et al., 2003; Chevrie-Muller, Watier, Arabia,
Health Organization, 2007), which recognizes the com- Arabia, & Dellatolas, 2005; Choudhury & Benasich, 2003;
plex interrelationships that exist between biological, indi- Prior et al., 2008; Reilly et al., 2006, 2007; Stanton-
vidual, and societal factors that influence child functioning. Chapman, Chapman, Bainbridge, & Scott, 2002; Tomblin
Research investigations of the predictors of speech and et al., 1991; Yliherva, Olsen, Maki-Torkko, Koiranen, &
language impairment that are consistent with these bio- Jarvelin, 2001; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, & Mehl,
ecological models seek to include a wide range of variables 1998; Zubrick et al., 2007).

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 509

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Perinatal factors. Perinatal factors were examined age, twin status was not a significant risk factor (Reilly
in 11 studies, and a significant association with speech et al., 2007).
and language impairment was found in 5. Prenatal fac- Medical conditions. Mixed findings have been re-
tors were associated with risk for speech and language ported for the impact of child illness and infection on
impairment in Fox, Dodd, and Howard (2002), who iden- speech and language impairment. Medical conditions
tified significant effects for extreme stress, maternal in- were examined in seven studies and a significant associ-
fections, and medications that could cause damage to the ation was found in five. Singer et al. (2001) found that
fetus during pregnancy, and in a population-based inves- patent ductus arteriosus and bronchopulmonary dyspla-
tigation of birth risk factors for specific language impair- sia were significant risk factors associated with speech
ment (SLI) by Stanton-Chapman et al. (2002), who noted and language impairment. Choudhury and Benasich (2003)
that late or no prenatal care was a significant predictor reported that autoimmune diseases presented a signif-
of SLI 6 years later, at school. icant risk, but asthma did not. The U.S. Preventative
Less consistent results have been reported for peri- Services Task Force review (2006) excluded studies of
natal difficulties. For example, Fox et al. (2002) found otitis media (OME) as a risk factor because it is “a com-
that “forceps or ventouse delivery, induced delivery, com- plex and controversial area” (Nelson et al., 2006, p. e302).
plications such as umbilical cord prolapse, infections, Two studies have reported a significant association be-
preterm birth, and post-partum resuscitation” (p. 122) tween OME and speech impairment in bivariate anal-
were significant predictors of speech impairment. Stud- yses but a nonsignificant association when multivariate
ies by Weindrich, Jennen-Steinmetz, Laucht, Esser, and analyses were applied (Campbell et al., 2003; Fox et al.,
Schmidt (2000) and Yliherva et al. (2001) have also linked 2002). Peters et al. (1997) indicated that “OME even when
perinatal factors to speech and language problems. In combined with a number of other risk factors produces
contrast, Peters, Grievink, van Bon, van den Bercken, only minor effects on later language” (p. 31). In contrast,
and Schilder (1997); Reilly et al. (2006, 2007); Tomblin Shriberg, Friel-Patti, Flipsen Jnr, and Brown (2000),
et al. (1991); and Tomblin, Smith, and Zhang (1997) found using structural equation modeling, reported a signif-
that postnatal factors did not present significant risks icant relationship between otitis media and speech /
for speech and language impairment. For example, Tomblin language outcomes.
et al. (1991) found that birth events such as “infections, Hearing status. Impaired hearing was found to be a
low birth weight, breathing difficulty, ototoxic drugs, feed- significant risk factor for difficulties with speech, lan-
ing problems, transfusions, and birth defects” (p. 1101) guage, and learning in a large study of 8,370 Finnish
did not predict poor communication status. Tomblin et al. children (Yliherva et al., 2001) but not a significant risk
(1997) also found that birth events including type of factor in the study by Singer et al. (2001) of over 200 chil-
delivery, induction of labor, duration of labor, and labor dren. In a study of 150 children with hearing impairment,
and birth complications were not significant risk factors. Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (1998) reported that children had
Existing findings are also inconsistent for prema- greater difficulties with speech and language develop-
turity and low birth weight. Reilly et al. (2006, 2007) ment if they were identified with a hearing impairment
found that being born preterm (< 36 weeks) was not a after the age of 6 months; identification prior to 6 months
significant risk factor for early language delay. Simi- coupled with subsequent early intervention was associ-
larly, Tomblin et al. (1997), who used < 2,500 g as the cut- ated with increased language scores.
off, and Reilly et al. (2007), who used a continuous scale of Oral sucking habits. Oral sucking habits, including
birth weight in kilograms, have reported that low birth breast-feeding, have been found to be both a risk and a
weight was not a significant predictor. In contrast, Zubrick protective factor in studies that have examined this
et al. (2007) found that low birth weight and premature factor. Fox et al. (2002) found that excessive sucking of
birth were independently significant for late language pacifiers, or thumb or bottle usage as a pacifier, was a
emergence. Similar results were noted by Stanton- moderate predictor of speech impairment. On the other
Chapman et al. (2002): low birth weight (< 2,500 g), very hand, Tomblin et al. (1997) demonstrated that breast-
low birth weight (< 1,500 g), and a low 5-min Apgar score feeding for less than 9 months was associated with an
were significant risk factors for school-age specific lan- increased risk of speech and language impairment.
guage impairment. Temperament. Three studies have examined child
Multiple birth. Two studies based on the same co- temperament characteristics as possible risk or protec-
hort have examined the association between multiple tive factors for speech and language impairment. Hauner,
birth and the risk for language impairment. Reilly et al. Shriberg, Kwiatkowski, and Allen (2005) considered the
(2006) found twin birth to be a significant risk factor for effect of different aspects of child temperament as risk
communication impairment in 8- and 12-month-old in- factors for “increased severity of expression of speech
fants; however, by the time this cohort reached 2 years of delay” (p. 635). Increased severity was related to negative

510 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Table 1. Summary of significant and nonsignificant risk factors for speech and language impairment for children based on the systematic review by the U.S. Preventative Task Force (2006) and additional studies.

Demographic information Child variables Parent variables Family and community variables

Oral Minority Parental Maternal Family Home Smoking


Study and Age in Outcome Male Perinatal Multiple Medical Hearing sucking Temper- Family Languages status Mother’s Father’s mental age at size and learning in the Socioeconomic Neighborhood
country Number months measure sex factors birth condition status habits ament history spoken or race education education health birth birth order activities household status disadvantage

Brookhouser 24 cases 28–62 Language – – – y – – – n – – – – – – – – – – –


et al. (1979)
USA
Campbell et al. 398 cases and 36 Speech y – – n – – – y – n y – – – – – – n –
(2003) USA 241 controls
Chevrie–Muller 2059 in cohort 42 Language y – – – – – – – y – y y – – – – – – –
et al. (2005)*
France
Choudhury & 42 cases and 36 Language y n – y/n – – – y – – n n – y/n y – – n –
Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010

Benasich 92 controls
(2003) USA
Felsenfeld & 156 adopted 84 Speech n – – – – – – y – – – – – – – n – – –
Plomin (1997)* and
USA nonadopted
children
Fox et al. (2002) 65 cases and 32–86 Speech n y – n – y – y – – – – – – – – – – –
Germany 48 controls
Hauner et al. 29 cases and 36–72 Speech – – – – – – y – – – – – – – – – – – –
(2005)* USA 87 controls
Lyytinen et al. 107 with risk of 0–54 Speech and – – – – – – – y – – – – – – – – – – –
(2001) Finland dyslexia and language
93 without
Peters et al. (1997) 946 in cohort 84–96 Language n n – y – – – – y – y y – – – – – – –
Netherlands
Prior et al. (2008)* 1,911 in cohort 12 and 24 Language y – – – – – y – – – – – y – – – – – –
Australia
Reilly et al. (2006)* 1,911 in cohort 8 and 12 Language y n y – – – – y – – – – n – – – – – y
Australia
Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors

Reilly et al. (2007)* 1,720 in cohort 24 Language y/n n n – – – – y y/n – y/n – n y/n n – – – n
Australia
Singer et al. (2001) 98 cases and 36 Language – – – y n – – – – y n – – – – – – y
USA 70+95
controls
Stanton-Chapman 5,862 cases 72–84 Language y y – – – – – – n – y – – n y – n – –
et al. (2002) and
USA 201,834
not
identified
Tallal et al. (1989) 76 cases and 48–59 Language – – – – – – – y – – y y – – – – – – –
USA 54 controls
Tomblin et al. 662 in cohort 30–60 Speech and y/n n – – – – – y – – n y – – y – – – –
(1991) USA language
Tomblin et al. 177 cases and kindergarten Speech and – n – – – y – y/n – – y y – y – – y – –
(1997) USA 925 controls language

(Continued on the following page)


511
512
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010
Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010

Table 1 Continued. Summary of significant and nonsignificant risk factors for speech and language impairment for children based on the systematic review by the U.S. Preventative Task Force (2006) and additional studies.

Demographic information Child variables Parent variables Family and community variables

Oral Minority Parental Maternal Family Home Smoking


Study and Age in Outcome Male Perinatal Multiple Medical Hearing sucking Temper- Family Languages status Mother’s Father’s mental age at size and learning in the Socioeconomic Neighborhood
country Number months measure sex factors birth condition status habits ament history spoken or race education education health birth birth order activities household status disadvantage

Weindrich et al. 320 in cohort 54 and 96 Speech and – y – – – – – – – – y y y – – – – – –


(2000) language
Germany
Whitehurst et al. 62 cases and 24–38 Language – – – – – – – n – – – – – – – – – – –
(1991) 55 controls
Yliherva et al. 8,370 in cohort 96 Speech and y y – y/n y – – – – – y – – y y – – – –
(2001) Finland language
Yoshinaga–Itano 150 cases 13–36 Speech and y – – – y – – – – n n – – – – – – n –
et al. (1998) USA language
Zubrick et al. 1,766 in cohort 24 Language y y – – – – n y – y/n n n n n y – n n n
(2007)*
Australia

Note. y = yes, the variable was examined and there was a statistically significant association; n = no, the variable was examined and was not associated with speech and/or language delay; y/n = different findings for different outcomes; – = the variable was not examined.
Asterisk indicates studies that were not included in the review by the U.S. Preventative Services Task Force (2006).
affect associated with low approachability/sociability, as African-American was not a significant risk factor.
negative mood, and low task persistence. Prior et al. Minority status was not a significant factor in the study
(2008) found that having a shy temperament was nega- conducted by Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (1998).
tively related to vocabulary production and communi- Educational level of mother and father. Of 14 studies
cation and symbolic development in a large cohort of 1- to that have examined the association between parents’
2-year-old children. On the other hand, in Zubrick et al. educational level on children’s speech and language ac-
(2007), only one of nine dimensions of child temperament quisition, 10 reported a risk for speech and language
(negative mood) occurred more frequently in 2-year-old impairment at low parental educational level. These
children with late language emergence. included studies of only mother’s education (Campbell
et al., 2003; Peters et al., 1997; Reilly et al., 2007; Stanton-
Parent Factors Chapman et al., 2002; Yliherva et al., 2001), only father’s
education (Tomblin et al., 1991), and both mother’s and
Family history of speech and language problems. father’s education (Chevrie-Muller et al., 2005; Tallal
Thirteen studies recorded family history of speech, lan- et al., 1989; Tomblin et al., 1997; Weindrich et al., 2000).
guage, and/or learning difficulties, with 11 identifying In contrast, 4 studies have shown that parental educa-
this as a risk factor for childhood speech and language im- tion level was not a significant risk factor (Choudhury
pairment (Campbell et al., 2003; Choudhury & Benasich, & Benaisch, 2003; Singer et al., 2001; Yoshinaga-Itano
2003; Felsenfeld & Plomin, 1997; Fox et al., 2002; et al., 1998; Zubrick et al., 2007).
Lyytinen et al., 2001; Reilly et al., 2006, 2007; Tallal,
Parental mental health. Five studies that have ex-
Ross, & Curtiss, 1989; Tomblin et al., 1991, 1997; Zubrick
amined this domain reported mixed results. Three stud-
et al., 2007). Of these, Tomblin et al. (1997) found that
ies found that indicators of parental mental health were
paternal family history was significant, but maternal
not associated with speech and language impairment in
history was not. Three other studies have reported that
8- to 12-month-old infants (Reilly et al., 2006) or 2-year-
family history of speech / language impairment (Brookhouser,
olds (Reilly et al., 2007; Zubrick et al., 2007). In contrast,
Hixson, & Matkin, 1979; Whitehurst et al., 1991) and
Prior et al. (2008) reported that maternal psychosocial
family history of hearing loss (Tomblin et al., 1991) were
indices, specifically mothers’ rate of coping and partner
not significantly associated with language impairment
relationship satisfaction, were positively associated with
or poor communication status in children. The impact of
language development at 24 months, and Weindrich et al.
family history may be due to genetic or environmental
(2000) found that parental mental health was a risk fac-
influences or to a combination of both. This question has
tor for speech, language, reading, and spelling in children
been examined by Felsenfeld and Plomin (1997) in a study
aged 54 and 96 months.
of adopted and nonadopted children. Family history for
biological parents was a significant risk factor for speech Maternal age at birth of child. Studies that exam-
impairment, whereas for adoptive parents it was not. ined maternal age at the birth of the child have reported
Their results support the view that the biological basis of mixed findings for speech and/or language impairment.
family history has a stronger influence on children’s speech Younger mothers have been identified in risk groups
and language than the home learning environment. for children with specific language impairment (Tomblin
et al., 1997) and poor speech and language abilities
Languages spoken. Risk for speech and language
(Yliherva et al., 2001). Choudhury and Benasich (2003)
impairment in children with a nondominant language
noted that younger maternal age was a characteristic of
background has been demonstrated in the case of non-
families with a history of speech language impairment
English speakers in an English-dominant society (Reilly
but was not linked to children’s assessed receptive and
et al., 2007), non-French speakers in a French-dominant
expressive language at age 3 years. Similarly, Stanton-
society (Chevrie-Muller et al., 2005), and non-Dutch speak-
Chapman et al. (2002) reported no relationship between
ers in a Dutch-dominant society (Peters et al., 1997). In
maternal age and school-identified specific language im-
contrast, Stanton-Chapman et al. (2002), who studied
pairment, after accounting for the effects of other bio-
an English-dominant U.S. state with a large Spanish-
logical and environmental risks. Reilly et al. (2007) found
speaking population, reported that Spanish and other
that older maternal age was a significant risk factor for
non-English speakers were “less likely to be placed in SLI
communication and symbolic behavior at age 24 months
classrooms than native-English speakers” (p. 397).
but not for vocabulary production.
Minority status or race. Risk for speech and lan-
guage impairment has been studied in relation to minority
status or race. Singer et al. (2001) reported that children
Family and Community Factors
of a minority race were at greater risk than their peers; Family size. Findings linking speech and language
however, Campbell et al. (2003) found that identification acquisition with the number of siblings, the number of

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 513

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


children in the household, and birth order have been Methodological Considerations
largely consistent. Choudhury and Benasich (2003) dem-
onstrated that an increased number of children in the
When Examining Studies
household was a significant risk factor for language to Determine Risk Factors
impairment. Yliherva et al. (2001) found that having As can be seen from the collation of findings set out
more than four children in the household increased risk in Table 1, varying results have been reported as to
of speech, language, and learning difficulties. Tomblin whether or not each of the factors reviewed presents as a
et al. (1991) demonstrated that children who held later significant risk for childhood speech and language im-
birth positions in the family were more likely to have pairment. These differences make it difficult to provide
a poor communication status between 2.5 and 5 years a definitive list of specific risk factors to guide primary
of age, and, similarly, Stanton-Chapman et al. (2002) care professionals (cf. Nelson et al., 2006). There are
reported that children whose birth position was third three main reasons for the observed differences in study
or higher were more likely to be identified as having results: (a) differences in the size and nature of the sam-
specific language impairment at age 6–7 years. Zubrick ples, (b) differences in the speech and language outcome
et al. (2007) reported that the presence of two or more measures that have been used to identify impairment,
siblings was a risk for language delay in a sample of and (c) differences in the range and number of possible
2-year-olds, and Reilly et al. (2007), who studied the predictor variables included in the design and the anal-
same age group, reported that birth order was a risk for yses that have been applied to these variables.
vocabulary production but not for communication and
Size and nature of the sample. The reviewed studies
symbolic behavior.
have used two sampling techniques: clinical sampling
Home learning activities. In a study of adopted with or without a control group and cohort studies that
and nonadopted children, Felsenfeld and Plomin (1997) identify children with and without speech and language
used the HOME Scale of family environment (Caldwell impairment within the full population. Studies utilizing
& Bradley, 1984) and found that family environment the former approach tend to have relatively small sam-
was not significantly associated with speech outcome at ples (e.g., 24 children aged 28–62 months in Brookhouser
age 7. et al., 1979; 63 cases and 48 controls in Fox et al., 2002)
Smoking in the household. Tomblin et al. (1997) with some exceptions (e.g., 177 cases and 923 controls in
found that maternal smoking in the household increased Tomblin et al., 1997), whereas the latter includes larger
the risk of speech and language difficulties but that this samples (e.g., 8,370 Finnish children recruited by Yliherva
was mediated by maternal education levels. Zubrick et al. et al., 2001; 1,911 Australian children reported on by Prior
(2007), on the other hand, reported no effect of maternal et al., 2008, and Reilly et al., 2006; 207,693 children in the
smoking (current and during/before pregnancy) on late Florida cohort surveyed by Stanton-Chapman et al.,
language emergence, and Stanton-Chapman et al. (2002) 2002). Larger population-based samples have greater var-
found no effect of smoking during pregnancy on school- iability across predictors, which may mask or blur differ-
identified specific language impairment. ences observed in clinical-control samples.
Socioeconomic factors. Of the five studies that con- The samples also differ by the target age and the age
sidered family SES (i.e., combined yearly income, occu- range of the study children. In some studies, children were
pational prestige, education levels, and qualification for examined at a specific age (e.g., 36 months in Campbell
Medicaid health insurance), only one found a significant et al., 2003; 96 months in Yliherva et al., 2001), whereas
risk for language impairment (Singer et al., 2001). Low other studies examined children over a wide age range
SES was not identified as a risk factor by Campbell et al. (e.g., 30–60 months in Tomblin et al., 1991; birth to
(2003), Choudhury and Benasich (2003), Yoshinaga-Itano 54 months in Lyytinen et al., 2001). Sampling differences,
et al. (1998), or Zubrick et al. (2007). both in terms of the heterogeneity of the samples (cf.
Neighborhood disadvantage. Two studies included Nelson et al., 2006) and the likelihood of sampling con-
information provided by the census-based Socio-Economic founds (Zubrick et al., 2007), have constrained the eval-
Indexes for Areas (SEIFA) from the Australian Bureau uation of risk factors for speech and language impairment
of Statistics (ABS, 2003) as a measure of neighborhood in early childhood. Furthermore, it is likely that “predic-
disadvantage. Reilly et al. (2006, 2007) reported that tive relationships change over time” (Zubrick et al., 2007,
lower scores on the Index of Disadvantage (i.e., living in p. 1588) such that certain risk factors become more
a more disadvantaged area) was a significant predictor salient at younger or older ages (see Reilly et al., 2006,
for language difficulties at age 8–12 months but not at vs. Reilly et al., 2007; see Table 1).
age 24 months. Zubrick et al. (2007) noted no difference Identification of speech and language impairment.
for children with and without late language emergence Differences in results also reflect the type and specificity
by SEIFA scores. of the speech and language measures used to identify

514 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


the target group. The majority of the reviewed studies comprehensive range of previously identified risk and
considered both speech and language outcomes, but protective factors for speech /language acquisition. It
some considered speech outcomes only whereas others also sought to address some of the weaknesses of pre-
considered language outcomes only. Reilly et al. (2006, vious research by focusing on a large population-based
2007) found that different outcome measures identified sample of children of a similar age who were approach-
different risk and protective factors. The source of diag- ing the beginning of formal schooling and by including
nosis also varies, being based on parent or teacher report a range of sources and types of information to identify
or on direct assessment of speech and language skills. Al- speech/language impairment status. This article reports
though there is evidence of correspondence across these on the second of two studies examining speech and lan-
sources (see McLeod & Harrison, 2009, for a discussion), guage impairment in a nationally representative popula-
there has not, as yet, been a systematic comparison of tion sample of Australian children. The first study (McLeod
risk factors in relation to parent, teacher, and direct as- & Harrison, 2009) determined the prevalence of ex-
sessment sources of identification. pressive and receptive speech and language impairment
Range, number, and analysis of possible predictor based on four measures: two reported by parents, one
variables. Table 1 illustrates the range, number, and directly assessed by trained interviewers, and period
type of predictors that were included and analyzed as prevalence of attendance at speech-language pathology
risk or protective factors in the reviewed studies. Some services as reported by parents and teachers. Building
studies focused on the predictive strength of a small on these findings, the present study used the same four
number of domains (e.g., two in Brookhouser et al., 1979, measures of speech and language impairment as bi-
and Whitehurst et al., 1991); others have a broader cov- nomial outcomes to test the effects of a wide range of
erage (e.g., five to seven domains in Campbell et al., previously identified risk/protective factors. Bivariate
2003; Tomblin et al., 1991, 1997); and some have included effects for each of these factors were tested, followed by
most of the identified domains (e.g., 10 in Reilly et al., multivariate analyses to identify the best set of predic-
2007; 12 in Zubrick et al., 2007). The inclusion of mea- tors from a range of child, parent, family, and community
sures from a large number of domains enables the use of factors.
multivariate analysis techniques, which have been shown
to negate previous findings using bivariate analyses. For
example, using multivariate analyses, Reilly et al. (2007) Method
and Zubrick et al. (2007) examined a large number of
known risk factors for language delay in two samples The Study
of Australian 2-year-olds. In both studies, results con- As with the companion article (McLeod & Harrison,
firmed only three or four predictors: male sex, perinatal 2009), the current study examined data collected from
factors, family history, and presence of siblings in the the kindergarten cohort of children in the first wave (age
Zubrick et al. study; and family history, low maternal 4–5 years) of Growing Up in Australia—The Longitudi-
education, and non-English-speaking background in the nal Study of Australian Children (LSAC; Sanson et al.,
Reilly et al. study. Note, however, that a large sample 2002). LSAC is the first comprehensive national study of
size is required when seeking to test predictors from Australian children, funded by the Australian Govern-
multiple domains. The discrepancies between the small ment to examine children’s health and development over
number of significant predictors identified in multivar- time and within the social, economic, and cultural envi-
iate analyses versus the more diverse range of predictors ronments of the families and communities in which they
identified in bivariate analyses warrant further inves- are growing up. Recruitment of the sample using the
tigation. Such work is needed to investigate possible con- most comprehensive database of Australia’s population
founding relationships between child biological factors was facilitated by the Australian Government and the
and parental or family factors. Health Insurance Commission. An overview of LSAC and
additional details about sample recruitment and data
weighting are provided in McLeod and Harrison (2009).
Aim of the Present Study LSAC data were weighted to allow for unequal probabil-
Several groups of researchers (Campbell et al., 2003; ities of inclusion in the study and to ensure that the LSAC
Nelson et al., 2006; Reilly et al., 2006, 2007; Tomblin sample matched families in the Australian population
et al., 1991; Zubrick et al., 2007) have sought to identify a with a 4- to 5-year-old child on a wide range of parental
set of predictors that would provide a means of identi- and family characteristics, including parents’ ethnicity,
fying children for speech and language assessment, us- country of birth, education, and income; family size and
ing different analytical techniques and with differing structure; and whether the mother spoke a language other
levels of success. The present study sought to extend this than English (LOTE) at home. Weighted sample data
work by assessing the unique and collective effects of a were used in all the analyses reported in this article.

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 515

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Participants Risk and protective factors. Risk and protective fac-
tors identified by the U.S. Preventative Services Task
A total of 4,983 children (2,537 boys; 2,446 girls) and Force (Nelson et al., 2006; U.S. Preventative Services
their parents participated in the kindergarten cohort of Task Force, 2006) and our review of these and additional
the LSAC study. Children ranged in age from 4;3 (years; studies (see Table 1) were matched to relevant items in
months) to 5;7, but the majority (80%) were within a the LSAC dataset. All but the family history of speech
6-month age span: 4;6 to 5;0 with a mean age of 56.91 and language impairment were available. Television view-
months (SD = 2.64). Parents reported that over 96% ing was a risk factor that had not been considered in
of children were attending an early childhood service previous studies but was added to the present study due
such as a child care center, preschool, or school (Harrison to its currency in public debate. A total of 31 potential
& Ungerer, 2005; Harrison, Ungerer, et al., 2009). With risk/protective factors were identified from parent report.
permission from parents, each child’s teacher was ap-
Child factors. Postnatal factors were described by
proached and asked to complete a brief questionnaire. A
prematurity (defined as < 36 weeks of pregnancy), birth
total of 3,276 children’s teachers participated.
weight (low birth weight defined as < 2500 g), whether
the child received neonatal intensive care, and the occur-
rence of multiple birth versus single birth. Medical con-
Measures ditions identified by parents included ongoing problems
of asthma, bronchiolitis, and ear infections. Hearing sta-
Outcome measures. Four outcome measures, devel-
tus was identified by the occurrence of ongoing hearing
oped and described in McLeod and Harrison (2009),
problems. Oral sucking habits were described by whether
were used to determine childhood risk status for speech
the child was breastfed for > 9 months. Child temper-
and language impairment. These measures drew on mul-
ament was assessed using the 12-item Short Tempera-
tiple informants, including the child’s parent and teacher,
ment Scale for Children (STSC; Sanson, Prior, Garino,
and direct assessment by a trained interviewer.
Oberklaid, & Sewell, 1987). The STSC provides ratings
1. Parent report of expressive speech and language for three subscales: sociability (e.g., “This child is shy
concern based on the Parents’ Evaluation of Devel- when first meeting new children”), persistence (e.g.,
opmental Status (PEDS; Glascoe, 2000) question “Do “This child stays with an activity [e.g., puzzle, construc-
you have any concerns about how your child talks and tion kit, reading] for a long time”), and reactivity (e.g.,
makes speech sounds?” (25.2% of the sample was “When shopping together, if I do not buy what this child
identified as impaired). wants [e.g., sweets, clothing], he/she cries and yells”).
2. Parent report of receptive language concern based Items are scored on a scale from 1 = almost never to 6 =
on the PEDS question “Do you have any con- almost always and combined to generate a mean score
cerns about how your child understands what you for each subscale. Children’s proficiency in an LOTE
say to him / her?” (9.5% of the sample was identified was described by two factors: regularly spoken to in a
as impaired). language other than English and speaks a language other
than English in the home.
3. Parent and teacher report of use of speech-language
pathology services in the past 12 months (14.5% of Parent factors. Demographic characteristics included
the sample was identified as impaired). mother’s age at child’s birth and mother’s and father’s
years of education. Parental minority status or race was
4. Assessed scores of vocabulary comprehension on
recorded if either parent self-identified as being of Aborig-
the Adapted Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III
inal or Torres Strait Islander background (indigenous
(PPVT–III; Rothman, 2003). Children were identi-
status). Parents’ language status was also self-identified
fied as having difficulty if they scored more than or
as “speaks a language other than English” (parents’
equal to 1 SD below the mean (14.7% of the sample
LOTE status). There were 40 different home languages
was identified as impaired).
spoken by the parents in this study. English (86%) was
There was a low-to-moderate overlap across these four the main language spoken at home, followed by Arabic
groups. For example, of the children whose parents were (1.6%), Cantonese (1.3%), Vietnamese (1.0%), Mandarin
concerned about their expressive speech and language (0.8%), Greek (0.8%), Italian (0.7%), Samoan (0.5%),
(Group 1), 27.2% had parents who were concerned about Spanish (0.5%), Hindi (0.4%), and other languages. Such
receptive language, 43% were attending speech-language cultural diversity is typical of the Australian population.
pathology services, and 22.9% were in the low vocabulary Maternal mental health was measured using the 6-item
group. The use of four different outcome measures en- screening version of the Kessler scale of nonspecific psy-
abled examination of distinct contributions of risk and chological distress (K6; Kessler et al., 2002). The K6 is an
protective factors to explain the disparate findings sum- effective self-report measure for probing symptoms of
marized in Table 1. anxiety and depression and is a good predictor of mood

516 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


or anxiety disorders measuring concurrent mental state Logistic regression analysis (Hosmer & Lemeshow,
(Furukawa, Kessler, Slade, & Andrews, 2003; Kessler 1989) was used to examine the unique contribution of
et al., 2003). Items (e.g., “In the past 4 weeks, how often each of these 31 risk/protective factors to the four mea-
did you feel nervous?”) are rated on a 5-point Likert scale sures of speech/language impairment, in which impair-
(1 = none of the time, 5 = all of the time) and combined to ment was coded as a binary variable (impaired = 1;
provide an overall mean score. nonimpaired = 0). Results report the odds ratio (OR) for
Family factors. SES was described by weekly house- each equation. ORs that are above 1.00 indicate that an
hold income, which was defined as combined yearly in- increase in the predictor increases the odds of impair-
come before tax. In addition, a specific index of family ment, whereas ORs that are below 1.00 indicate that an
financial hardship was determined by asking the pri- increase in the predictor decreases the odds of impair-
mary parent whether he or she had experienced any ment. The closer the OR is to 1.00, the smaller the effect
of seven different types of financial hardship in the past of the predictor. The criteria for significance ( p < .05) of
12 months, such as “being unable to pay gas, electricity, the OR are based on the ORs for the 95% confidence
or telephone bills on time” or “going without meals.” interval (CI) not including 1.00; for example, OR = 1.24,
Summary categories of financial hardship were defined CI [1.05, 1.47] is significant, whereas OR = 1.22, CI
by the total number of indices endorsed by the parent [0.83, 1.79] is not significant. A predictor with a sig-
(none, one, two, three, or more). Family size was described nificant OR > 1.00 is considered to be a risk factor for
by three related variables: the number of children in the being in the impaired group, and a predictor with a sig-
household and whether the LSAC child had older siblings nificant OR < 1.00 is considered to be a protective fac-
or had younger siblings. Home learning activities were tor. See Tomblin et al. (1997) and Zubrick et al. (2007)
determined by asking the primary parent whether he for further description of the application of ORs to the
or someone in the household had provided any of seven field of speech-language pathology.
different types of learning support, such as reading to Multivariate logistic regression was then used to test
the child, drawing or doing craft activities with the child, the effects of individual predictor variables after adjust-
or playing with the child (Australian Institute of Family ing multivariately for the effects of all other child-related,
Studies, 2007). A weekly score was recorded for each in- parent-related, family, and community predictors. The
dex on a scale from 0 = none to 3 = every day and averaged full set of variables identified as being significant pre-
to generate an overall mean. Television watching were dictors in bivariate analyses was entered in four sepa-
reported by the primary parent as the number of hours rate regression equations, one for each of the outcome
on typical weekdays and weekend days that the child measures. These analyses assess the combined predic-
watches TV or videos. Scores ranged from 1 = does not tive effect of the full set of variables, as reported by the
watch TVor videos to 5 = 5 hours or more. Smoking in the Model c2 and the Nagelkerke pseudo R2. The Nagelkerke
household was recorded if either parent reported that measure is an analog to the R2 produced by multivariate
they smoked. linear regression and provides an approximation of the
Community factors. Neighborhood disadvantage was percent of variance explained (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).
described by SEIFA score (ABS, 2003), which provides a Effect size can be assessed by calculating the square-root
general indicator of neighborhood advantage or disadvan- of the R2 and using Cohen’s (1988) guidelines: small effect
tage based on information collected in the 2001 census size, R = .1; medium, R = .3; large, R = .5. As noted pre-
for each postcode (zip code). A bivariate measure of com- viously, the effect of each individual variable in the model
munity disadvantage was computed, with disadvantage is indicated by the OR. Results of the analyses also pro-
being equivalent to SEIFA scores > 1 SD below the mean. vide three additional statistics: the overall percent of
cases classified correctly; sensitivity or percent identifi-
cation of true positives (impaired group), being the num-
Analysis Plan ber of true positives divided by true positives plus false
Outcome measures were four binomial indicators of negatives; and specificity or percent identification of true
speech/language risk: parent expressive language concern, negatives (nonimpaired group), being the number of true
parent receptive language concern, use of speech-language negatives divided by true negatives plus false positives.
pathology services, and low vocabulary comprehension
score on the Adapted PPVT–III, each of which identified
a speech/language impairment group (impaired) and a non- Results
speech/language impairment group (nonimpaired). De-
scriptive analyses determined the frequency (n and % for
Bivariate Analyses
binomial variables) or mean scores (and SD for continuous Table 2 presents the results of descriptive and logis-
variables) for the 31 risk/protective factors for impaired tic regression analyses for each of the 31 risk and protective
and nonimpaired groups on each of the four outcomes. factors for the four outcome measures: (a) parent-reported

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 517

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Table 2. Bivariate descriptive statistics and logistic regression results for child, parent, family, and community predictors of children being in the speech and language impaired (impaired) and
518

non-speech and language impaired (nonimpaired) groups.


Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Outcome 1: Outcome 2: Outcome 3: Outcome 4:


Parent-reported expressive Parent-reported receptive Use of speech-language Low receptive vocabulary
speech and language concern language concern pathology services PPVT–III score < 1 SD
n = 4,980 to 3,197 n = 4,980 to 3,196 n = 4,179 to 3,183 n = 4,375 to 2,916

Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired


Risk/protective LSAC n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR
factors variable M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI)

Child factors
Sex Male 808 1,743 2.06 307 2,244 1.84 391 1,760 1.89 357 1,873 1.24
(64.4%) (46.8%) (1.80–2.35) (64.5%) (49.8%) (1.51–2.24) (64.6%) (49.2%) (1.58–2.56) (55.6%) (50.2%) (1.05–1.47)
Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010

Postnatal Prematurity 77 152 1.55 37 193 1.88 47 150 1.95 33 162 1.22
factors (6.4%) (4.2%) (1.17–2.05) (8.0%) (4.4%) (1.31–2.71) (8.0%) (4.3%) (1.39–2.74) (5.4%) (4.5%) (0.83–1.79)
Birth weight 109 223 1.50 50 282 1.75 56 223 1.52 56 222 1.54
(< 2500 g) (8.9%) (6.1%) (1.18–1.91) (10.7%) (6.4%) (1.27–2.40) (9.3%) (6.3%) (1.12–2.07) (8.9%) (6.0%) (1.14–2.09)
Neonatal 231 513 1.41 109 635 1.81 114 517 1.38 103 543 1.12
intensive (18.5%) (13.8%) (1.19–1.68) (22.9%) (14.1%) (1.44–2.28) (18.9%) (14.5%) (1.11–1.72) (16.0%) (14.6%) (0.89–1.41)
care
Single vs. Multiple birth 39 101 1.14 13 127 0.94 20 103 1.16 23 96 1.40
multiple (3.1%) (2.7%) (0.78–1.66) (2.7%) (2.8%) (0.52–1.69) (3.3%) (2.9%) (0.72–1.89) (3.6%) (2.6%) (0.88–2.23)
births
Medical Asthma 296 754 1.22 127 924 1.41 169 733 1.49 145 787 1.10
conditions (23.6%) (20.3%) (1.04–1.42) (26.7%) (20.6%) (1.14–1.75) (27.9%) (20.6%) (1.23–1.82) (22.6%) (21.1%) (0.90–1.34)
Bronchiolitis 217 572 1.15 99 689 1.47 122 543 1.43 94 619 0.86
(17.5%) (15.5%) (0.97–1.37) (21.1%) (15.4%) (1.16–1.86) (20.4%) (15.3%) (1.15–1.77) (14.8%) (16.7%) (0.68–1.09)
Ear infections 145 254 1.78 76 323 2.46 74 263 1.75 57 287 1.17
(11.6%) (6.8%) (1.44–2.21) (16.0%) (7.2%) (1.88–3.22) (12.2%) (7.4%) (1.33–2.30) (8.9%) (7.7%) (0.87–1.57)
Hearing status Ongoing 96 71 4.27 64 102 6.67 55 86 4.09 28 105 1.60
hearing (7.6%) (1.9%) (3.12–5.84) (13.4%) (2.3%) (4.80–9.26) (9.1%) (2.4%) (2.88–5.80) (4.4%) (2.8%) (1.05–2.43)
problems
Oral sucking Breastfed for 446 1,533 0.79 122 1,855 0.49 208 1,487 0.74 208 1,570 0.65
habits/ > 9 months (35.9%) (41.4%) (0.69–0.90) (25.8%) (41.4%) (0.40–0.61) (34.7%) (41.8%) (0.61–0.89) (32.0%) (42.3%) (0.54–0.77)
breastfeeding
Temperament Persistence 3.64 3.99 0.69 3.29 3.97 0.50 3.67 3.94 0.76 3.61 3.97 0.68
(1.02) (0.93) (0.64–0.75) (1.10) (0.93) (0.45–0.56) (1.06) (0.94) (0.69–0.83) (1.01) (0.94) (0.62–0.75)
Reactivity 2.89 2.66 1.29 3.19 2.67 1.73 2.86 2.69 1.21 3.02 2.65 1.51
(1.00) (0.90) (1.20–1.39) (1.04) (0.91) (1.56–1.92) (1.01) (0.92) (1.10–1.32) (1.00) (0.90) (1.37–1.66)
Social 3.77 3.84 0.95 3.74 3.83 0.94 3.78 3.83 0.97 3.64 3.86 0.87
(1.20) (1.23) (0.90–1.01) (1.27) (1.22) (0.87–1.02) (1.25) (1.22) (0.90–1.04) (1.22) (1.22) (0.81–0.93)

(Continued on the following page)


Table 2 Continued. Bivariate descriptive statistics and logistic regression results for child, parent, family, and community predictors of children being in the speech and language impaired
(impaired) and non-speech and language impaired (nonimpaired) groups.

Outcome 1: Outcome 2: Outcome 3: Outcome 4:


Parent-reported expressive Parent-reported receptive Use of speech-language Low receptive vocabulary
speech and language concern language concern pathology services PPVT–III score < 1 SD
n = 4,980 to 3,197 n = 4,980 to 3,196 n = 4,179 to 3,183 n = 4,375 to 2,916

Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired


Risk/protective LSAC n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR
factors variable M (SD ) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD ) M (SD) (95% CI)
Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010

Child factors
Languages Regularly 219 870 0.69 112 979 1.11 84 753 0.60 278 634 3.73
spoken spoken to (17.5%) (23.4%) (0.59–0.82) (23.6%) (21.7%) (0.89–1.39) (13.9%) (21.1%) (0.47–0.77) (43.3%) (17.0%) (3.13–4.46)
in LOTE
Speaks an 216 867 0.69 112 973 1.12 84 747 0.61 278 628 3.78
LOTE (17.2%) (23.3%) (0.58–0.81) (23.6%) (21.6%) (0.90–1.40) (13.9%) (20.9%) (0.48–0.78) (43.3%) (16.8%) (3.16–4.52)
Parent factors
Maternal age Maternal age 29.60 29.85 0.99 28.63 29.91 0.96 30.10 29.94 1.01 28.81 30.02 0.96
at birth at birth (5.48) (5.39) (0.98–1.10) (5.77) (5.36) (0.94–0.97) (5.59) (5.26) (0.99–1.02) (5.63) (5.22) (0.94–0.97)
of child of child
Educational level Mother ’s years 13.92 14.17 0.96 13.57 14.17 0.92 13.92 14.25 0.95 13.19 14.32 0.84
of parents of education (2.51) (2.68) (0.94–0.99) (2.34) (2.66) (0.89–0.95) (2.43) (2.63) (0.92–0.98) (2.54) (2.57) (0.81–0.87)
Father’s years 14.34 14.63 0.95 14.09 14.61 0.92 14.29 14.66 0.94 13.74 14.71 0.85
of education (2.42) (2.51) (0.93–0.98) (2.49) (2.49) (0.88–0.96) (2.48) (2.48) (0.91–0.98) (2.58) (2.42) (0.81–0.88)
Maternal mental Maternal 4.18 4.34 0.70 3.94 4.34 0.47 4.23 4.31 0.83 4.02 4.36 0.50
Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors

health psychological (0.73) (0.61) (0.63–0.77) (0.81) (0.61) (0.41–0.53) (0.71) (0.63) (0.73–0.95) (0.84) (0.58) (0.44–0.57)
well-being
Minority status Parents’ 50 133 0.65 30 153 1.17 21 121 0.64 43 103 4.71
or race indigenous (4.0%) (3.6%) (0.54–0.79) (6.3%) (3.4%) (0.91–1.51) (3.5%) (3.4%) (0.48–0.85) (6.7%) (2.8%) (3.88–5.72)
status
Languages Parents’ LOTE 146 626 1.13 83 690 1.89 60 514 1.05 227 408 2.52
spoken and status (11.8%) (17.1%) (0.81–1.57) (17.8%) (15.6%) (1.26–2.85) (10.0%) (14.6%%) (0.66–1.67) (37.0%) (11.1%) (1.75–3.65)
proficiency
in English

(Continued on the following page)


519
520
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Table 2 Continued. Bivariate descriptive statistics and logistic regression results for child, parent, family, and community predictors of children being in the speech and language impaired
(impaired) and non-speech and language impaired (nonimpaired) groups.

Outcome 1: Outcome 2: Outcome 3: Outcome 4:


Parent-reported expressive Parent-reported receptive Use of speech-language Low receptive vocabulary
speech and language concern language concern pathology services PPVT–III score < 1 SD
n = 4,980 to 3,197 n = 4,980 to 3,196 n = 4,179 to 3,183 n = 4,375 to 2,916

Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired Impaired Nonimpaired


Risk/protective LSAC n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR n (%) or n (%) or OR
factors variable M (SD ) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD) M (SD) (95% CI) M (SD ) M (SD) (95% CI)
Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010

Family factors
Socioeconomic Household 9.95 10.33 0.95 9.27 10.34 0.87 10.11 10.39 0.96 9.00 10.53 0.82
factors income (2.88) (2.71) (0.93–0.98) (2.79) (2.73) (0.84–0.90) (2.62) (2.70) (0.93–0.995) (2.72) (2.64) (0.79–0.84)
Financial Financial 0.69 0.57 1.27 0.85 0.57 1.65 0.63 0.56 1.15 0.78 0.55 1.53
hardship hardship (0.74) (0.70) (1.16–1.38) (0.76) (0.70) (1.46–1.87) (0.71) (0.70) (1.02–1.30) (0.78) (0.69) (1.37–1.72)
Family size Number of 2.54 2.50 1.03 2.47 2.51 0.97 2.50 2.48 1.02 2.88 2.44 1.41
children (1.08) (1.06) (0.97–1.10) (1.11) (1.06) (0.88–1.06) (0.98) (1.03) (0.94–1.11) (1.34) (0.98) (1.31–1.51)
in the
household
Older siblings 773 2,154 1.17 249 2,679 0.75 378 2,066 1.22 418 2,154 1.36
(61.6%) (57.8%) (1.03–1.34) (52.4%) (59.5%) (0.62–0.91) (62.5%) (57.8%) (1.02–1.46) (65.0%) (57.7%) (1.15–1.62)
Younger 573 1,725 0.98 234 2,063 1.15 277 1,654 0.98 321 1,729 1.15
siblings (45.7%) (46.3%) (0.86–1.10) (49.3%) (45.8%) (0.95–1.39) (45.9%) (46.3%) (0.83–1.17) (49.9%) (46.3%) (0.98–1.36)
Home learning Support for 1.69 1.71 0.92 1.61 1.71 0.71 1.74 1.70 1.16 1.57 1.74 0.57
activities children’s (0.55) (0.55) (0.82–1.03) (0.57) (0.55) (0.60–0.84) (0.54) (0.55) (0.99–1.35) (0.55) (0.55) (0.49–0.67)
learning
Television TV watching 3.13 3.04 1.20 3.17 3.05 1.26 3.12 3.05 1.15 3.16 3.03 1.29
watching (weekdays) (0.73) (0.72) (1.09–1.31) (0.81) (0.71) (1.10–1.43) (0.71) (0.72) (1.02–1.29) (0.80) (0.70) (1.14–1.45)
TV watching 3.06 2.99 1.10 3.13 3.00 1.21 3.09 3.00 1.13 3.15 2.98 1.28
(weekends) (0.86) (0.83) (1.02–1.18) (0.93) (0.83) (1.08–1.35) (0.83) (0.82) (1.02–1.26) (0.95) (0.80) (1.15–1.41)
Smoking in the Smoking in the 154 440 1.05 84 508 1.76 88 496 1.12 133 372 2.73
household household (14.7%) (14.2%) (0.86–1.28) (21.5%) (13.5%) (1.36–2.28) (15.4%) (14.0%) (0.88–1.43) (26.2%) (11.5%) (2.18–3.42)
Community factors
Neighborhood SEIFA > 1 SD 186 528 1.05 88 626 1.41 79 512 0.90 127 476 1.69
disadvantage below mean (14.8%) (14.2%) (.88–1.26) (18.5%) (13.9%) (1.10–1.80) (13.1%) (14.3%) (0.70–1.16) (19.8%) (12.8%) (1.36–2.10)

Note. PPVT–III = Adapted Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III (Rothman, 2003); LSAC = The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (Sanson et al., 2002); LOTE = language other than English;
SEIFA = Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2003). Significant outcomes (p < .05) are indicated by bold type.
expressive speech and language concern, (b) parent-reported ongoing problems with ear infections (OR = 1.41) and
receptive language concern, (c) use of speech-language hearing problems (OR = 3.18), having a more reactive
pathology services, and (d) low vocabulary comprehen- temperament (OR = 1.13), and having older siblings
sion (score < 1 SD below the mean on the Adapted (OR = 1.26). Reduced odds for being in the impaired
PPVT–III). An examination of the ORs and CIs for these group were associated with the following protective
analyses showed that all but 2 of the 31 predictor vari- factors: having a more persistent temperament (OR =
ables were significant predictors of at least one measure 0.75), mothers having higher ratings for psychological
of speech/language impairment. Twenty-two variables well-being (OR = 0.74), and parents speaking an LOTE
were significant for three or four of the four outcome mea- (OR = 0.52). The full model accounted for 11.7% ( pseudo
sures. Risk factors for the child were being male, having R2 = .117) of the variance, which is equivalent to a me-
a history of perinatal complications (prematurity, low birth dium effect size (R = .34; Cohen, 1988). In comparison,
weight, use of neonatal intensive care), having past and Reilly et al. (2007), using multivariate linear regression
ongoing medical conditions (asthma, ear infections, hear- and a smaller set of significant predictors (4), reported
ing problems), having a more reactive temperament, and a lower proportion of the explained variance (6.4% and
having older siblings. Higher levels of financial hardship 7.0%) for mothers’ ratings of their 2-year-olds’ speech and
and more weekend and weekday television viewing were expressed vocabulary. The proportion of cases classified
family risk factors for all four outcomes. Protective factors correctly was relatively low (76.5%). Although the model
for the child were being breastfed for > 9 months and had high specificity (97.7% of nonimpaired cases were
having a more persistent temperament. Family protec- correctly identified), it had poor sensitivity (11.3% of im-
tive factors were mothers and fathers having completed paired cases were correctly identified).
more years of education, higher levels of maternal psy- Significant predictors for Outcome 2: Receptive lan-
chological well-being, and higher household income. Pre- guage concern. Seven variables were significant predic-
dictors that were identified as both a risk and a protective tors for parental report of receptive language concern.
factor for different outcomes were the child speaking or Increased odds were evident for the following risk
being spoken to in LOTE, and parents’ being of indig- factors: being male (OR = 1.39), having ongoing hearing
enous background or speaking LOTE. A further seven problems (OR = 4.43), and having a more reactive tem-
variables were identified as significant predictors for perament (OR = 1.47). Reduced odds were evident for the
one or two of the outcomes: childhood bronchiolitis (risk), following protective factors: being breastfed for > 9 months
greater temperamental sociability (protective), older ma- (OR = 0.70), having a more persistent temperament (OR =
ternal age at birth (protective), larger family size (risk), 0.54), mothers having higher ratings for psychological
greater support for children’s learning in the home (pro- well-being (OR = 0.66), and the presence of older siblings
tective), smoking in the household (risk), and living in a (OR = 0.67). The full model accounted for 21.5% of the
more disadvantaged neighborhood (risk). Two predic- variance. This is equivalent to a moderate-to-large effect
tors, being a multiple birth and having younger siblings, size (R2 = .215, R = .46; Cohen, 1988). The proportion of
were not significant for any of the four outcomes. cases classified correctly was relatively high (91.8%), but
this was due to the model’s high specificity (99.5% of
nonimpaired cases were correctly identified). The model
Multivariate Analyses had poor sensitivity (9.8% of impaired cases were cor-
Multivariate logistic regressions were used to test the rectly identified).
above predictor variables collectively. Variables that were Significant predictors for Outcome 3: Attendance at
found to be nonsignificant in bivariate tests were excluded. speech-language pathology. Ten predictors were associ-
Highly intercorrelated variables were either represented ated with children’s attendance at speech-language pa-
by a single variable (e.g., child speaking or being spoken to thology. Increased odds were noted for being male (OR =
in LOTE was dropped and parents’ LOTE status was re- 1.71), being premature (OR = 1.84), having asthma (OR =
tained) or combined (e.g., weekday and weekend television 1.33), having ongoing hearing problems (OR = 2.95),
viewing were summed). This process left 26 factors, which having older siblings (OR = 1.39), and spending more
were included in four separate multivariate logistic re- time watching television (OR = 1.10). Increased odds were
gression analyses. Results are presented in Table 3. also seen for home support for learning (OR = 1.36), with
Significant predictors for Outcome 1: Expressive speech more support being associated with greater likelihood of
and language concern. Of the 26 putative risk /protective being in the impaired group. This finding is difficult to
factors included in the model, 8 achieved significance as explain in that it may reflect the use of speech-language
a predictor of parent-reported expressive speech and pathology services by parents who are also more sup-
language concern (impaired group). Increased odds for portive of their children’s learning, but equally it may
being in the impaired group were associated with the reflect the additional support that parents are provid-
following risk factors: being male (OR = 1.97), having ing at home as a result of their children attending

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 521

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Table 3. Multivariate logistic regression: adjusted odds ratio and cumulative amount of variance (pseudo R2) for child, parent, family, and
community predictors of speech and language impairment.

Outcome 1:
Parent-reported Outcome 2: Outcome 3: Outcome 4:
expressive speech Parent-reported Use of speech- Low receptive
and language receptive language language pathology vocabulary
concern concern services PPVT < 1 SD
Model Model Model Model
c2(26, N = 3,197) = c2(26, N = 3,196) = c2(26, N = 3,183) = c2(26, N = 2,916) =
256.22; 314.05; 164.93; 369.63;
pseudo R2 = .117 pseudo R2 = .215 pseudo R2 = .092 pseudo R2 = .239
LSAC identifier Adjusted OR (p) Adjusted OR (p) Adjusted OR (p) Adjusted OR (p)

Child factors
Male 1.97 (<.001) 1.39 (.025) 1.71 (<.001) 1.29 (.055)
Premature 1.17 (.549) 1.59 (.222) 1.84 (.035) .86 (.711)
Birth weight (<2500 g) 1.04 (.863) .87 (.687) 1.14 (.612) 1.61 (.127)
Neonatal intensive care 1.00 (.976) .96 (.824) .90 (.513) .94 (.767)
Breastfed for > 9 months .87 (.147) .70 (.022) .83 (.101) .89 (.387)
Asthma 1.01 (.930) 1.15 (.414) 1.33 (.024) 1.12 (.491)
Bronchiolitis .97 (.818) 1.21 (.279) 1.06 (.680) .96 (.825)
Ear infections 1.41 (.024) 1.38 (.136) 1.30 (.141) 1.27 (.319)
Ongoing hearing problems 3.18 (<.001) 4.43 (<.001) 2.95 (<.001) 1.32 (.423)
Temperament: Social .94 (.105) .98 (.676) .91 (.038) .86 (.007)
Temperament: Persistence .75 (<.001) .54 (<.001) .79 (<.001) .82 (.005)
Temperament: Reactivity 1.13 (.012) 1.47 (<.001) 1.11 (.084) 1.19 (.018)
Parent factors
Maternal age at birth of child 1.00 (.805) 1.00 (.852) 1.02 (.078) .96 (.004)
Mother’s years of education 1.00 (.979) .95 (.155) 1.00 (.935) .91 (.002)
Father’s years of education .99 (.559) 1.02 (.609) .98 (.320) .96 (.167)
Maternal psychological distress/well-being .74 (<.001) .66 (<.001) .92 (.348) .78 (.014)
Parents’ LOTE status .52 (<.001) 1.20 (.370) .55 (.002) 5.60 (<.001)
Parents’ indigenous status .88 (.615) .99 (.987) .81 (.502) 1.14 (.711)
Family factors
Household income .99 (.614) .98 (.636) .97 (.321) .91 (.004)
Financial hardship 1.03 (.670) 1.13 (.262) 1.06 (.547) 1.01 (.931)
Number of children in the household 1.01 (.864) .94 (.469) .93 (.276) 1.43 (<.001)
Older siblings 1.26 (.032) .67 (.017) 1.39 (.012) .88 (.411)
Support for children’s learning at home 1.15 (.100) .99 (.946) 1.36 (.004) .68 (.002)
TV watching (weekdays + weekends) 1.05 (.138) 1.02 (.719) 1.10 (.020) 1.03 (.516)
Smoking in the household .92 (.555) 1.35 (.118) 1.04 (.801) 1.69 (.002)
Community factors
SEIFA > 1 SD below mean .88 (.284) 1.19 (.332) .83 (.209) 0.94 (.719)
Proportion of cases classified correctly 76.5% 91.8% 85.8% 89.6%
Sensitivity 11.3% 9.8% 2.6% 15.8%
Specificity 97.7% 99.5% 99.7% 99.0%

Note. Pseudo R2 is calculated according to Nagelkerk, as provided by the SPSS output. Significant outcomes (p ≤ .05) are indicated by bold type.

speech-language pathology. Reduced odds were seen (although still equivalent to a medium effect size) is likely
for the following protective factors: possessing a more due to additional factors, such as the availability and
sociable and more persistent temperament (ORs = 0.91 affordability of speech-language pathology, which were
and 0.79) and parents speaking an LOTE (OR = 0.55). not assessed. The proportion of cases classified correctly
The combined set of predictors accounted for 9.2% of the was 85.8%, with high specificity (99.7% of nonimpaired
variance for attendance at speech-language pathology cases were correctly identified) but poor sensitivity (2.6%
(R2 = .092, R = .30). The lower figure for this outcome of impaired cases were correctly identified).

522 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Significant predictors for Outcome 4: Low score on
Adapted PPVT–III. The largest number of significant Discussion
predictors (12) was identified for having a low assess- Recent conceptualizations of child functioning
ment score (< 1 SD below the mean) on the Adapted (World Health Organization, 2007) acknowledge that
PPVT–III. Increased odds for being in the identified biological, psychosocial, and societal factors interactively
group were associated with five risk factors: being male influence development. Understanding and explicating
(OR = 1.29), having a more reactive temperament (OR = the underlying contributors to childhood speech and lan-
1.19), parents speaking an LOTE (OR = 5.60), having guage impairment, therefore, requires consideration of
more children in the household (OR = 1.43), and parental multiple attributes of the individual as well as of the
smoking in the household (OR = 1.69). Reduced odds were family and social contexts, each of which may expose the
associated with the following protective factors: the child to risk or protection. The present study has applied
child having a more sociable and persistent tempera- this model (Bronfenbrenner, 2005) to investigate child,
ment (ORs = 0.86 and 0.82); mothers being older at the parent, family, and community predictors of speech and
birth of child (OR = 0.96), having more years of edu- language impairment in a nationally representative sam-
cation (OR = 0.91), and reporting higher levels of ple of 4- to 5-year-old children. Speech and language
psychological well-being (OR = 0.78); a higher house- impaired versus nonimpaired status was described by
hold income (OR = 0.91); and parents providing more four distinct measures: parent-reported expressive and
home support for learning (OR = 0.68). The full model receptive language concern, teacher- and parent-reported
accounted for almost one-quarter (23.9%) of the var- use of speech-language pathology services, and assessed
iance (R2 = .239, R = .49), which was equivalent to receptive vocabulary. Results for these four outcomes
a moderate-to-large effect size. The proportion of cases identified a core set of significant predictors related to
classified correctly was 89.6%, with high specificity child factors (being male; having ongoing hearing prob-
(99.0% of nonimpaired cases were correctly identified) lems; and having a less persistent, less sociable, and
and low sensitivity (15.8% of impaired cases were cor- more reactive temperament), parent factors (mothers with
rectly identified). a lower sense of psychological well-being, parents speak-
ing languages other than English), and family factors
The variance explained was similar to that noted for
(support for children’s learning at home, the presence of
the association for parent-reported receptive language
older siblings). Table 4 provides a summary of these ef-
concern (R2 = .215, R = .46); however, the pattern of
fects and their direction (risk or protection). The first six
predictors was notably different. Low scores on assessed
rows indicate the factors that were consistently associ-
receptive vocabulary were strongly associated with envi-
ated with all four indicators of speech and language im-
ronmental indicators (parent, family, and community);
pairment; the remaining rows show where discrepancies
however, parent-rated concern was primarily associ-
were noted in the direction of the effects.
ated with child-related factors. In addition, the presence
of ongoing hearing problems was highly significant for These and other results presented in this study add
parent-rated but not assessed poor receptive vocabulary to existing knowledge that may be useful for primary
care professionals seeking to identify children at risk,
(OR = 4.43, p < .001 vs. OR = 1.32, p = ns), and parents’
who may require specialist assessment of their speech
LOTE status was highly significant for assessed but
and language development or who would benefit from
not parent-rated low receptive vocabulary (OR = 5.60,
early intervention. The findings from this investigation
p < .001 vs. OR = 1.20, p = ns).
confirmed that childhood speech and language impair-
The discrepancy in results for parents’ LOTE status ment is influenced by multiple factors. Each of these is
is likely due to the influence of the primary language discussed in turn.
being assessed in each outcome measure. The Adapted
PPVT–III assessed competency in English, whereas
parents were reporting on the child’s first and other Child Factors
languages in the outcome measures of receptive and Being male was a significant risk factor for all four
expressive language concern. This was most notable for outcome measures: parental concern regarding expres-
parent-reported expressive language concern, in which sive as well as receptive speech and language difficulties,
LOTE status was a protective factor (OR = 0.52). The having a low score for receptive vocabulary, and attending
discrepancy in results for ongoing hearing problems speech-language pathology services (ORs of 1.29–1.97).
may also be due to parents’ interpretation of the ques- These findings were not surprising considering previous
tion “Do you have any concerns about how your child research (reviewed earlier in this article), but they do
understands what you say to him?” which could be in- confirm an identifiable risk at an age when most boys are
terpreted as “hearing what you say” or “understanding attending an early childhood education program (Harrison
the language you use.” & Ungerer, 2005). A message that boys are at risk for

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 523

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Table 4. Consistent risk and protective factors relating to speech and language impairment in a nationally representative sample of 4- to
5-year-olds.

Outcome 1: Outcome 2: Outcome 3: Outcome 4:


Expressive speech Receptive Attend Low score
and language language speech-language on Adapted
Risk/Protective factor concern concern pathology PPVT–III

Male Risk Risk Risk Risk


Ongoing hearing problems Risk Risk Risk
Temperament: Social Protective Protective
Temperament: Persistence Protective Protective Protective Protective
Temperament: Reactivity Risk Risk Risk
Maternal psychological well-being Protective Protective Protective
Parents’ status regarding languages other than English spoken Protective Protective Risk
Older siblings Risk Protective Risk
Support for children’s learning at home Risk Protective

poorer speech and language should be strongly imparted impairment, being associated with all four outcomes.
to primary care professionals, including early childhood Children with a more persistent temperament (i.e., able
teachers, who are in a position to recommend referral for to stay on task, keep on trying and do not give up easily)
speech/language assessment. were less likely to have difficulties with speech and lan-
Having ongoing hearing problems was also an im- guage (ORs of 0.54–0.82). Previous work by Hauner et al.
portant risk factor, particularly for expressive and recep- (2005) has also shown that “decreased task persistence or
tive language as reported by parents and for receipt of attention” (p. 635) was a significant risk factor for speech
speech-language pathology support. Results showed that impairment. On the other hand, a more reactive temper-
children with ongoing hearing problems were three times ament was associated with greater risk of speech and
more likely to be identified with expressive speech and language impairment (ORs of 1.13–1.47). Similar find-
language difficulties and to be attending speech-language ings were noted by Hauner et al. (2005) who described
pathology (ORs = 3.19 and 2.96) and four times more “approach-related or withdrawal-related negative affect,
likely to have difficulties with receptive language ability negative emotionality or mood” (p. 635) as a significant
than their nonaffected peers (OR = 4.43). These results risk factor for increased severity of speech impairment.
confirm work by Yliherva et al. (2001) that included on- Reactivity in young children has been associated with
going hearing problems as a predictor for speech and inhibition and fearfulness in novel situations, impulsiv-
language impairment in a similarly large-scale study ity, and behavioral dysregulation (see Sanson, Hemphill,
of Finnish children. Both studies support the need for & Smart, 2004), all of which could tend to impede speech
infant hearing screening programs that can assist with and language acquisition. Temperamental sociability, on
early identification of children with hearing problems the other hand, was associated with a reduced risk of
and have the potential to reduce the observed risk to speech and language impairment (ORs = 0.91 and 0.86).
childhood speech and language. Sociable children tend to have more positive social rela-
tions and be more popular with friends (Sanson et al.,
Child medical conditions that predicted at least one
2004), characteristics that are likely to enhance speech
of the four speech/language outcomes were asthma, which
and language development. Prior et al. (2008) found that
was identified as increasing risk for attendance at speech-
shy children were at increased risk for communication
language pathology (OR = 1.33), and ongoing ear in-
difficulties; however, the authors stated it was unknown
fections, which were a risk for expressive speech and
whether shyness affected the child’s initiation and respon-
language concern (OR = 1.41).
siveness in conversation or whether poor communication
A protective factor for receptive language was being led to the child’s shyness.
breastfed for > 9 months (OR = 0.70). Although this was
included as an indicator of oral sucking habits, with
benefits for children’s oromuscular development, pro- Parent Factors
longed breastfeeding is also likely to be influenced by Among the risks and protective factors associated
and interact with parental and family factors, such as with the child’s home environment, maternal psycholog-
maternal well-being and socioeconomic status. ical well-being was consistently found to positively im-
Child temperament characteristics were an impor- pact children’s expressive and receptive language (ORs =
tant and consistent predictor of speech and language 0.66–0.78). Conversely, children did less well when their

524 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


mothers reported more symptoms of anxiety and de- Barr, McLeod, and Daniel (2008) found that typically
pression. The sociofamilial environment, particularly developing children who are a sibling of a child with a
language stimulation and responsivity, is an important communication impairment can be worried, protective,
context for the development of language skills (Desmarais, and act as an interpreter for their sibling. The benefit
Sylvestre, Meyer, Bairati, & Rouleau, 2008). It has been is that younger siblings hear rich language from their
hypothesized that maternal well-being facilitates language older siblings as well as their parents and therefore do
stimulation in the home and conversely that maternal not have receptive language difficulties. The finding that
psychological distress or depression reduces language having younger siblings was neither a significant risk
stimulation (Prior et al., 2008). nor a protective factor is further evidence to support these
The home language environment was a further pre- speculations.
dictor of speech and language impairment; however, as Children’s access to a supportive home-learning en-
discussed in the previous section, the direction of re- vironment was a protective factor for assessed receptive
sults differed by the outcome being measured. Parents who vocabulary (OR = 0.68). This result, along with maternal
spoke an LOTE reported less concern about their child’s age and years of education, family income, and house-
expressive speech and language ability (OR = 0.52), but hold size, suggest that children from more advantaged
on the other hand, these children were more likely to be families with parents who were more involved in their
in the low scoring group for English receptive vocabulary children’s learning were less likely to be classified as
(OR = 5.60), a discrepancy likely due to the parent having poor receptive vocabulary (on the Adapted
reporting on all of the languages spoken by the child, not PPVT–III). The result is consistent with other findings
just English. Children of parents who spoke an LOTE for the LSAC children linking sociodemographic vari-
were also less likely to be attending speech-language ables (education, income, occupation, SEIFA) and sup-
pathology (OR = 0.55), but the reduced use of speech- port for home learning with enhanced learning outcomes
language pathology could be due to cross-cultural dif- (Wake et al., 2008) and with recent findings from the
ferences in definitions of disability and differences in United States linking higher SES backgrounds with
accessing professional services (cf. Hwa-Froelich & Westby, greater vocabulary at school entry (Rowe & Goldin-
2003). When comparing these results to previous findings, Meadow, 2009).
it is important to note that several studies that have Smoking in the household was a significant risk fac-
examined risk factors have specifically excluded children tor for low scores on the Adapted PPVT–III (OR = 1.68).
with bilingualism (e.g., Fox et al., 2002; Tallal et al., A similar finding was noted for kindergarten children
1989). Three studies found increased risk for speech/ studied by Tomblin et al. (1997) but not for 2-year-olds
language impairment in nondominant language speakers (Zubrick et al., 2007).
within examined societies that were English-dominant
Television watching is a variable that had not been
(Reilly et al., 2007), French-dominant (Chevrie-Muller
assessed in previous studies. Bivariate analyses identi-
et al., 2005), and Dutch-dominant (Peters et al., 1997).
fied more time spent in television watching as a signif-
However, other research (summarized in Table 1) has
icant risk for all four outcomes; however, it was only
shown mixed results for the impact of proficiency in
retained for the use of speech-language pathology ser-
home language and minority status or race. In the pre-
vices (OR = 1.10) in the multivariate analyses. It is not
sent study, the inclusion of four distinct outcome mea-
clear whether the amount of TV watching affected the
sures has enabled the current research to examine some
child’s ability to engage in communication or whether
of the discrepancies in the results of these studies.
poor communication skills led to the increase in TV
watching.
Family Factors
The presence of siblings is a further feature of the
Strengths
home environment that was expected to impact child- To summarize, the strengths of the current study
hood speech and language impairment. In the present include the size of the Longitudinal Study of Australian
study, only having an older sibling had a significant Children (LSAC) sample, the use of population sampling
effect, being a risk for expressive speech and language to represent all 4- to 5-year-olds in Australia, the in-
concern and for attending speech-language pathology clusion of a broad range of potential risk and protective
(ORs = 1.26 and 1.39) and a protective factor for recep- factors, and the concurrent testing of the effects of these
tive language concern (OR = 0.67). These findings pro- factors using a multivariate design. The current study
vide some evidence to support the suspicion, often noted also included new and rarely studied variables such as
in clinical folklore, that older siblings are more verbal child temperament and features of the home environ-
(i.e., use more “air time”) than younger siblings and ment such as television watching and smoking in the
speak on behalf of their younger siblings. Relatedly, household. Furthermore, by using and comparing findings

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 525

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


for four speech and language outcome measures, this set (predicting other child outcomes) have reported sim-
study has enabled a deeper consideration of some of the ilar figures (10%–25%, Harrison, Ungerer, et al., 2009;
complexities involved in the processes influencing speech 7%–20%, Wake et al., 2008). The multivariate logistic
and language acquisition. regression results for proportion of cases classified cor-
This study adds to the growing number of large- rectly, sensitivity, and specificity also point to limita-
scale studies that are examining the ways that biological tions in the predictive power of this approach. The set of
and environmental factors explain or influence speech predictor variables did not correctly identify impaired
and language development in early childhood (Reilly et al., cases (low sensitivity), although there was a high level
2006, 2007; Zubrick et al., 2007). The results of recent of specificity (correctly identified nonimpaired cases).
research with children under 2 years of age have sug-
gested that biological trajectories are the primary drivers Conclusion
of early communication and vocabulary development
(Zubrick et al., 2007) and that social and environmental This study has extended previous work on identifi-
factors have a minimal effect (e.g., explaining less than cation of risk and protective factors for childhood speech
7% of the variance; Reilly et al., 2009). Findings presented and language impairment by using a large, nationally
here are consistent with this work, in that factors intrin- representative sample of 4- to 5-year-old children. A
sic to the child (sex, temperament) or related to neuro- comprehensive range of predictor variables was used,
biological mechanisms (prematurity, low birth weight, including previously unexplored or underexplored vari-
perinatal difficulties) were significant predictors of speech ables. The multivariate design enabled testing of the
and language impairment. However, ongoing medical con- collective contribution of these predictors. When the full
ditions, which are likely to be affected by social and envi- set of child, parent, family, and community variables was
ronmental conditions, were also important and consistent included in the model, many of the variables identified as
predictors, as were other factors related to the family en- significant predictors in bivariate analyses were elimi-
vironment. The overall impression from the present study nated due to overlapping of underlying constructs. Nine
is that psychosocial and socioeconomic factors do con- factors were consistently identified in multivariate anal-
tribute to childhood speech and language development, yses as having a unique effect on speech and language
at least at age 4–5 years. The variance explained for impairment: being male (risk), having ongoing hearing
expressive and receptive language (11.7%, 21.5%, and problems (risk), having a more reactive temperament
23.9%) was considerably larger than reported by Reilly (risk), having a more persistent temperament (protec-
et al. (2009) for 2-year-olds. This suggests that screening tive), having a more social temperament (protective), in-
for child and family risk factors in combination with ob- creased maternal well-being (protective), having an older
servations or informal testing of children’s speech and sibling (risk/protective), parental LOTE status (risk/
language ability and vocabulary growth in relation to de- protective), and support for children’s learning in the
velopmental norms is likely to provide the best means of home (risk/protective). These early risk and protective
identifying children who need referral to speech-language factors along with observations of children’s speech and
pathology or specialist early intervention. language milestones can be a useful guide for primary
care professionals seeking to identify children who ben-
efit from early intervention communication programs.
Limitations
The limitations of the current study relate to the
measures that were possible to collect in a large multi- Acknowledgments
dimensional study such as LSAC. Only one of the out- This research was supported by the following sources:
comes was based on direct assessment of the children Australian Research Council Discovery Grant DP0773978
(Adapted PPVT–III). Two of the outcomes were based and the Charles Sturt University Research Institute for
on parent report of concern, and although the validity Professional Practice, Learning, and Education. An earlier
of two outcomes has been confirmed (Harrison, McLeod, version of a portion of this article was presented at the 2008
Berthelsen, & Walker, 2009; McLeod & Harrison, 2009), Conference of the International Clinical Linguistics and
these should be considered a screening measure rather Phonetics Association in Istanbul, Turkey. The authors would
like to acknowledge the contribution of the Australian Rotary
than representing a comprehensive assessment. A further
Health Fund and Foundation for Children Research Grant
limitation is the lack of information pertaining to fam-
and the members of the Longitudinal Study of Australian
ily history of speech and language impairment. This is Children Research Consortium: John Ainley, Donna Berthelsen,
an important predictor, which would likely add to the Michael Bittman, Linda Harrison, Ilan Katz, Jan Nicholson,
variance explained. The large amount of unexplained Bryan Rodgers, Ann Sanson, Michael Sawyer, Sven Silburn,
variance is acknowledged as a limitation; however, we Lyndall Strazdins, Judy Ungerer, Graham Vimpani, Melissa
note that other multivariate analyses of the LSAC data Wake, and Stephen Zubrick.

526 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


References Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G.
(2003). The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales
Almost, D., & Rosenbaum, P. (1998). Effectiveness of speech for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey
intervention for phonological disorders: A randomized con- of Mental Health and Well-Being. Psychological Medicine,
trolled trial. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 357–362.
40, 319–325.
Gibbard, D., Coglan, L., & MacDonald, J. (2004). Cost-
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2003). 2039.0 Information effectiveness analysis of current practice and parent interven-
paper: Census of population and housing – Socio-economic tion for children under 3 years presenting with expressive
indexes for areas (SEIFA). Canberra, Australia: Author. language delay. International Journal of Language and
Australian Institute of Family Studies. (2007). Growing Communication Disorders, 39, 229–244.
up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Glascoe, F. P. (2000). Parents’ Evaluation of Developmental
Children. Retrieved June 29, 2007 from www.aifs.gov.au/ Status: Authorised Australian version. Parkville, Australia:
growingup/home.html. Centre for Community Child Health.
Barr, J., McLeod, S., & Daniel, G. (2008). Siblings of chil- Harrison, L. J., McLeod, S., Berthelsen, D., & Walker, S.
dren with speech impairment: Cavalry on the hill. Language, (2009). Literacy, numeracy, and learning in school-aged chil-
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 21–32. dren identified as having speech and language impairment
Bronfenbrenner, U. (Ed.). (2005). Making human beings in early childhood. International Journal of Speech-Language
human. Bioecological perspectives on human development. Pathology, 11, 392–403.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Harrison, L., & Ungerer, J. (2005). What can the Longitu-
Brookhouser, P. E., Hixson, P. K., & Matkin, N. D. (1979). dinal Study of Australian Children tell us about infants’ and
Early childhood language delay: The otolaryngologist’s 4 to 5-year-olds’ experiences of early childhood education
perspective. Laryngoscope, 89, 1898–1913. and care? Family Matters, 72, 26–35.
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home observation Harrison, L. J., Ungerer, J. A., Smith, G. J., Zubrick, S. R.,
for measurement of the environment. Little Rock: University & Wise, S., with Press, F., Waniganayake, M., & the LSAC
of Arkansas Press. Research Consortium. (2009). Child care and early educa-
Campbell, T. F., Dollaghan, C. A., Rockette, H. E., Paradise, tion in Australia. The Longitudinal Study of Australian
J. L., Feldman, H. M., et al. (2003). Risk factors for speech Children. Social Policy Research Paper No. 40. Canberra,
delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children. Child Australia: Australian Government Department of Families,
Development, 74, 346–357. Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs.

Chevrie-Muller, C., Watier, L., Arabia, J., Arabia, C., & Hauner, K. K. Y., Shriberg, L. D., Kwiatkowski, J., &
Dellatolas, G. (2005). Teachers’ screening for language and Allen, C. T. (2005). A subtype of speech delay associated
behaviour difficulties in 2059 children aged 3,5 years. Revue with developmental psychosocial involvement. Journal of
d’Epidémiologie et de Santé Publique, 53, 645–657. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 635–650.
Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. A. (2003). A family aggrega- Hwa-Froelich, D. A., & Westby, C. E. (2003). Frameworks
tion study: The influence of family history and other risk of education: Perspectives of Southeast Asian parents and
factors on language development. Journal of Speech, Language, Head Start staff. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
and Hearing Research, 46, 261–272. Schools, 34, 299–319.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. regression. New York: Wiley.
Coplan, J. G., Gleason, J. R., Ryan, R., Bourke, M. G., & Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek,
Williams, M. L. (1982). Validation of an early language D. K., et al. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor pop-
milestone scale in high-risk population. Pediatrics, 70, ulation prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological
677–683. distress. Psychological Medicine, 32, 959–976.
Desmarais, C., Sylvestre, A., Meyer, F., Bairati, I., & Kessler, R. C., Barker, P. R., Colpe, L. J., Epstein, J. F.,
Rouleau, N. (2008). Systematic review of the literature Gfroerer, J. C., et al. (2003). Screening for serious mental
on characteristics of late-talking toddlers. International illness in the general population. Archives of General Psy-
Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, chiatry, 60, 184–189.
361–389.
King, T. M., Rosenberg, L. A., Fuddy, L., McFarlane, E.,
Dollaghan, C. A., & Campbell, T. F. (2009). How well do Sia, C., & Duggan, A. K. (2005). Prevalence and early
poor language scores at ages 3 and 4 predict poor language identification of language delays among at-risk three year
scores at age 6? International Journal of Speech-Language olds. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
Pathology, 11, 358–365. 26, 293.
Felsenfeld, S., & Plomin, R. (1997). Epidemiological and Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C.
offspring analyses of developmental speech disorders using (2000). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech
data from the Colorado Adoption Project. Journal of Speech, and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of
Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 778–791. the literature. International Journal of Language and Com-
munication Disorders, 35, 165–188.
Fox, A. V., Dodd, B., & Howard, D. (2002). Risk factors
for speech disorders in children. International Journal of Luinge, M. R., Post, W. J., Wit, H. P., & Goorhuis-Brouwer,
Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 117–132. S. M. (2006). The ordering of milestones in language

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 527

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


development for children from 1 to 6 years of age. Jour- Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connec-
nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, tions between temperament and social development: A
923–940. review. Social Development, 13, 142–170.
Lyytinen, H., Ahonen, T., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T. K., Sanson, A., Nicholson, J., Ungerer, J., Zubrick, S., Wilson,
Laakso, M.-L., et al. (2001). Developmental pathways of K., et al. (2002). Introducing the Longitudinal Study of
children with and without familial risk for dyslexia during Australian Children (LSAC Discussion Paper No. 1).
the first years of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies.
535–554.
Sanson, A., Prior, M., Garino, E., Oberklaid, F., & Sewell,
McLeod, S., & Harrison, L. J. (2009). Epidemiology of J. (1987). The structure of infant temperament: Factor
speech and language impairment in a nationally represen- analysis of the Revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire.
tative sample of 4- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 97–104.
Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 1213–1229.
Schwarz, I. E., & Nippold, M. A. (2002). The importance of
McLeod, S., & McKinnon, D. H. (2007). The prevalence early intervention. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology,
of communication disorders compared with other learning 4, 69–73.
needs in 14,500 primary and secondary school students.
International Journal of Language and Communication Shriberg, L. D., Friel-Patti, S., Flipsen, P. J., & Brown,
Disorders, 42(S1), 37–59. R. L. (2000). Otitis media, fluctuant hearing loss, and
speech-language outcomes: A preliminary structural equa-
National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop- tion model. Journal Speech, Language, and Hearing
ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2004). Research, 43, 100–120.
Multiple pathways to early academic achievement. Harvard
Educational Review, 74, 1–29. Singer, L. T., Siegel, A. C., Lewis, B., Hawkins, S.,
Yamashita, T., & Baley, J. (2001). Preschool language
Nelson, H. D., Nygren, P., Walker, M., & Panoscha, R.
outcomes of children with history of bronchopulmonary
(2006). Screening for speech and language delay in preschool
dysplasia and very low birth weight. Journal of Develop-
children: Systematic evidence review for the U.S. Preventive
mental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 19–26.
Services Task Force. Pediatrics, 117, e298–e319.
Stanton-Chapman, T. L., Chapman, D. A., Bainbridge,
Peters, S. A., Grievink, E. H., van Bon, W. H., van den
N. L., & Scott, K. G. (2002). Identification of early risk
Bercken, J. H., & Schilder, A. G. (1997). The contribution
factors for language impairment. Research in Developmental
of risk factors to the effect of early otitis media with effusion
Disabilities, 23, 390–405.
on later language, reading, and spelling. Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology, 39, 31–39. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate
statistics. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Prior, M., Bavin, E. L., Cini, E., Reilly, S., Bretherton, L.,
et al. (2008). Influences on communicative development Tallal, P., Ross, R., & Curtiss, S. (1989). Familial aggrega-
at 24 months of age: Child temperament, behaviour prob- tion in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech
lems, and maternal factors. Infant Behavior and Develop- and Hearing Disorders, 54, 167–173.
ment, 31, 270–279. Tomblin, J. B., Hardy, J. C., & Hein, H. A. (1991). Predict-
Reilly, S., Bavin, E. L., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., Eadie, ing poor-communication status in preschool children using
P., et al. (2009). The Early Language in Victoria Study risk factors present at birth. Journal of Speech and Hearing
(ELVS): A prospective, longitudinal study of communication Research, 34, 1096–1105.
skills and expressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and Tomblin, J. B., Smith, E., & Zhang, X. (1997). Epidemiol-
24 months. International Journal of Speech-Language ogy of specific language impairment: Prenatal and perinatal
Pathology, 11, 344–357. risk factors. Journal of Communication Disorders, 30,
Reilly, S., Eadie, P., Bavin, E. L., Wake, M., Prior, M., et al. 325–344.
(2006). Growth of infant communication between 8 and U.S. Preventative Services Task Force. (2006). Screen-
12 months: A population study. Journal of Paediatrics and ing for speech and language delay in preschool children:
Child Health, 42, 764–770. Recommendation statement. American Family Physician,
Reilly, S., Wake, M., Bavin, E. L., Prior, M., Williams, J. 73, 1605–1610.
et al. (2007). Predicting language at 2 years of age: A Wake, M., Sanson, A., Berthelsen, D., Hardy, P., Mission,
prospective community study. Pediatrics, 120, e1441–e1449. S., et al. (2008). How well are Australian infants and
Rothman, S. (2003, June). An Australian version of the children aged 4 to 5 doing? Findings from the Longitudinal
Adapted PPVT–III for use in research. Unpublished manu- Study of Australian Children Wave 1 (Social Policy Research
script, Australian Council for Educational Research. Paper No. 36). Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia.
Roulstone, S., Miller, L. L., Wren, Y., & Peters, T. J. (2009). Weindrich, D., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Laucht, M., Esser,
The natural history of speech impairment of 8-year-old G., & Schmidt, M. H. (2000). Epidemiology and prognosis
children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and of specific disorders of language and scholastic skills.
Children: Error rates at 2 and 5 years. International Journal European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 186–194.
of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 381–391. Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Smith, M., Fischel,
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009, February 13). J. E., Lonigan, C. J., & Valdez-Menchaca, M. C. (1991).
Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in Family history in developmental expressive language
child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323, delay. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34,
951–953. 1150–1157.

528 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 53 • 508–529 • April 2010

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


World Health Organization. (2007). International classifi-
Received April 24, 2008
cation of functioning, disability and health—Version for
children and youth: ICF-CY. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Revision received April 3, 2009
Yliherva, A., Olsen, P., Maki-Torkko, E., Koiranen, M., Accepted July 30, 2009
& Jarvelin, M. R. (2001). Linguistic and motor abilities DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0086)
of low-birthweight children as assessed by parents and
teachers at 8 years of age. Acta Paediatrica, 90, 1440–1449. Contact author: Linda Harrison, School of Teacher Education,
Charles Sturt University, Panorama Avenue, Bathurst,
Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A. L., Coulter, D. K., & Mehl,
NSW, 2795, Australia. E-mail: lharrison@csu.edu.au.
A. L. (1998). Language of early- and later-identified children
with hearing loss. Pediatrics, 102, 1161–1171.
Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Rice, M. L., & Slegers, D. W.
(2007). Late language emergence at 24 months: An epide-
miological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50,
1562–1592.

Harrison & McLeod: Risk and Protective Factors 529

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on June 11, 2010


Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like