You are on page 1of 83

The Educational Values of Trees and Forests

Terry L. Sharik
Departments of Wildland Resources and Environment and Society College of Natural Resources Utah State University Logan, UT 84322-5215 February, 2009
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 1

Context
When we think about the benefits provided by trees and forests, we tend not to think about how they contribute to enhanced learning ability, despite the mounting evidence (MEA 2005).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Linkages between ecosystem services and human well-being (MEA 2005).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Conceptual framework of interactions between biodiversity, ecosystem services, human well-being, and drivers of change (MEA 2005).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Provisioning Services
Products obtained Food Fiber Fuel Genetic resources Biochemicals, natural medicines, pharmaceuticals Ornamental resources Fresh water

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Regulating Services
Regulation of ecosystem processes Air quality regulation Climate regulation Water regulation Erosion regulation Water purification Disease regulation Pest regulation Pollination Natural Hazard Regulation

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Supporting Services
Necessary for the production of all other services Soil formation Photosynthesis Primary production Nutrient cycling Water cycling

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Cultural Services
Non-material benefits
Cultural diversity Spiritual and religious values Knowledge systems Educational values Inspiration Aesthetic values Social relations Sense of place Cultural heritage values Recreation and ecotourism

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Objective
To review the literature on this topic with the intent of stimulating further research on the educational values of trees and forests, and fostering the application of this knowledge in the learning environment relative to natural resource/ecosystem management.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

Approach
Complexities of the learning process Links of this process to nature, including ways of interacting with nature Role of trees and forests Conclusions

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

10

COMPLEXITIES OF THE LEARNING PROCESS


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 11

Mediating Systems in Human Behavior (modified from Rue 2005)


Molecular Neural Reflex Perceptual Physiological Learning/Memory Emotional Cognitive Symbolic
E nv i r o n m e n t E c o s ys t e m s
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

12

Learning/Memory Systems
(Rue 2005)
Experiential promotion of changes in neural structures that conserve the effects of experience across time. Virtually all higher behavioral functions depend on neural systems modulated by learning.
Object recognition Concept formation Emotional experience Anticipation Planning Problem-solving Comparing and contrasting Decision-making Language use
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 13

Three Major Modes of Learning


(Kellert 2002)
1. Affective or Emotional
Focuses on the formation of emotional and feeling capacities. Typically precedes cognition or intellect as a basis for maturation and learning.

2. Cognitive or Intellectual
Stresses the formation of thinking and problem-solving skills.

3. Evaluative
Emphasizes the creation of values, beliefs, and moral perspectives. Emerges from a synthesis of affective and cognitive perceptions and understandings.

Additional References: Iozzi 1989a, 1989b


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 14

Three Major Modes of Learning


(Kellert 2002)

1. Affective or Emotional 2. Cognitive or Intellectual 3. Evaluative

Additional References: Iozzi 1989a, 1989b


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 15

Emotional Systems
(Rue 2005)
A temporary subjective feeling that arises during the process of determining the narrative meaning of an event, i.e. during a period of cognitive appraisal.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

16

Emotional Systems
(Rue 2005)
PRIMARY SECONDARY TERTIARY

Presuppose minimal self-concept


interest anxiety frustration/ consternation affection gratitude sympathy resentment contempt

Presuppose explicit self-concept


love? agape "aesthetic emotions" hope nostalgia fago (Ifalukia) Laiya (India and Nepal) "religious emotions"
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 17

fear disgust anger desire happiness sadness

hatred outrage shame guilt envy jealousy pride grief

resignation admiration wonder compassion alienation humility amusement

Affective or Emotional Mode


Kellert (2002)
There is some evidence that contact with the natural world, especially during middle childhood (ages 6-12), is important in a persons emotional responsiveness and receptivity. Seems to be due to its dynamic, varied, often unique, surprising, and adventurous character. Elicits such responses as satisfaction, delight, joy, excitement, and curiosity.

References: Cornell 1977, Cobb 1977, Kellert 1985, Kellert 1996, Derr 2001, Ratanopojnard 2001, Sabal 1993
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 18

Affective or Emotional Mode


Kellert (2002)

Interactions with natural settings reduce stress or elicit positive feelings and thereby enhance cognitive functioning, creativity, and performance, especially regarding higher order tasks.

References: Olmsted 1865, Kaplan and Talbot 1983, Isen et al. 1985, Kaplan and Kaplan 1989, Isen 1990, Hartig et al. 1991, Ulrich et al. 1991a, Ulrich 1993, Kaplan 1995, Wells 2000, Wells and Evans 2003, Am. Institutes for Research 2005
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 19

Affective or Emotional Mode


Kellert (2002)
Attention Restoration Theory has been utilized to explain the restorative aspects of contact with nature following a period of directed or forced, focused attention and associated mental fatigue (Kaplan 1995). There is both psychological and physiological evidence for the restorative value of nature. Results in cognitive clarity and reflection, thereby enhancing creativity.
Additional References: Ulrich 1981, Ulrich and Simmons 1986, Ulrich et al. 1991b, Chang and Pergn 1998, Chang and Uan 1999, Hartig et al. 2003, Hammitt 2007, Chang et al. 2008. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

20

Three Major Modes of Learning


(Kellert 2002)

1. Affective or Emotional 2. Cognitive or Intellectual 3. Evaluative

Additional References: Iozzi 1989a, 1989b


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 21

Cognitive Systems
(Rue 2005)
Internal representations of external affairs Ability to assimilate a diversity of information and process it in new ways Performing a wide range of operations on a wide range of mental objects (including perceptions, memory images, and concepts) Process of:
Encoding information representing the external world and the organisms vital interests Integrating these two forms of information to devise appropriate behaviors

Operators on mental objects include:


Reality (facts) Valance (values) Executive (outcomes)
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 22

Cognitive or Intellectual Mode


Kellert (2002)

Evidence that experiential contact with nature can have a positive impact on cognitive development comes from a number of sources. Bloom et al.s (1956) taxonomy of cognition has been used to frame this relationship (Table 2).
Additional References: Altman and Wohwill 1978, Ulrich 1993, Kahn 1999, Ratanapojnard 2001, Kellert 1997, Kellert 2002, Burdette and Whitaker 2005, Taylor and Kew 2006
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 23

Cognitive or Intellectual Mode


Name of Cognitive Ability Knowledge Acquisition Definition Enhancing Activity in Nature Identifying and classifying various aspects of the natural world Translating specific knowledge of nature into categories of related functions and processes

Assembling facts and figures

Comprehension

Interpreting and understanding empirical realities

Application

Putting knowledge to use Distinguishing one in various situations environmental feature from another

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

24

Cognitive or Intellectual Mode


Name of Cognitive Ability Analysis Definition Teasing apart elements or patterns nested within an overall structure Integrating distinctive elements into an overall whole Discerning worth and importance Enhancing Activity in Nature Dissecting elements in nature

Synthesis

Integrating elements and processes in nature

Evaluation

Judging the relative significance of particular aspects of the natural world

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

25

Cognitive or Intellectual Mode


Borrowing from the work of Mednick (1962), it has been argued that higher order cognitive functioning, which involves integration or association of diverse and seemingly unrelated information or concepts in novel ways, is likely enhanced by exposure to nature (Ulrich 1993).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

26

Three Major Modes of Learning


(Kellert 2002)

1. Affective or Emotional 2. Cognitive or Intellectual 3. Evaluative

Additional References: Iozzi 1989a, 1989b


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 27

Evaluative Mode
Kellert (1996) formulated nine values of the natural world that contribute significantly to human well-being (Table 3).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

28

Name of Value Dominionistic

Definition of Value Urge to master and control nature

Benefits Safety and protection; independence and autonomy; the urge to explore and confront the unknown; and willingness to take risks, be resourceful, and show courage Avoiding harm and injury; minimizing risk and uncertainty; and respect and awe for nature through recognizing its power to humble and destroy

Stage of Prominent Development (yrs.)* 3-6

Negativistic

Avoidance, fear, and rejection of nature

3-6

*The three age classes correspond to early, middle, and late (adolescent) childhood, respectively.
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 29

Name of Value Utilitarian

Definition of Value Material and commodity attraction of the natural world

Benefits

Stage of Prominent Development (yrs.)*

Physical and material 3-6 security; self-confidence and self-esteem through demonstrating craft and skill in nature; and recognition of human physical dependence on natural systems and processes Perceiving order and organization; developing ideas of harmony, balance, and symmetry; and evoking and stimulating curiosity, imagination, and discovery 6-12

Aesthetic

Physical attraction and appeal of nature

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

30

Name of Value
Humanistic

Definition of Value
Strong affection and emotional attachment to nature

Benefits
Developing intimacy, companionship, trust, and capacities for social relationship and affiliation; and enhancing self-confidence and self-esteem through giving, receiving, and sharing affection Classifying and labeling abilities; language acquisition and counting; resolution of difficult aspects of psychosocial development through story and fantasy; and enhanced communication and discourse through the use of imagery and symbol

Stage of Prominent Development (yrs.)*


6-12

Symbolic

Natures role in shaping and assisting human communication and thought

6-12

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

31

Name of Value
Scientific

Definition of Value
Empirical and systematic study and understanding of nature

Benefits
Intellectual competence; critical thinking; problem-solving abilities; enhanced capacities for empirical observation and analysis; and respect and appreciation for natural process and diversity

Stage of Prominent Development (yrs.)*


6-12, 13-17

Moralistic

Ethical and spiritual affinity for nature

13-17 Sense of underlying meaning, order, and purpose; the inclination to protect and treat nature with kindness and respect; and enhanced sociability from shared moral and spiritual conviction

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

32

Name of Value Naturalistic

Definition of Value Desire for close contact and immersion in nature

Benefits

Stage of Prominent Development (yrs.)*

Inclination for exploration, 13-17 discovery, curiosity, inquisitiveness, and imagination; enhanced selfconfidence and self-esteem by demonstrating competence and adaptability in nature; and greater calm and coping capacities through heightened temporal awareness and spatial involvement

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

33

Most Important Values to Learning


Those that contribute most strongly to the development of learning skills include the aesthetic, naturalistic, and scientific, and to a lesser extent the symbolic the latter mostly through the development of language skills.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

34

Multiple Intelligences (Gardner 1999)


Linguistic Logical-mathematical Spatial Bodily-kinesthetic Musical intelligence Interpersonal Intrapersonal Naturalist
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 35

Importance of Naturalist Perspective


Gardners (1999) inclusion of naturalist intelligence among the eight multiple intelligences he formulated to represent the entire spectrum of human cognition lends support to the importance of the naturalist perspective as a fundamental way of learning.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

36

WAYS OF INTERACTING WITH NATURE CAN MAKE A DIFFERENCE


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 37

Three ways in which (young) peoples experience of nature occurs


(Kellert 1996, 2005) Direct
Actual physical contact with nature in a spontaneous and unstructured way, including play

Indirect
Actual physical contact with nature, but more structured and planned. e.g., zoos, arboreta, botanical gardens, science museums, and nature centers; contacts with domesticated plants and animals; [outdoor lab instruction ? (TLS)]

Vicarious or symbolic
Representations or depicted scenes of nature Principally through the mass media (classroom? TLS)
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 38

Direct Experience of Nature


Considerable conceptual and empirical support for the argument that direct experience of nature plays a significant role in affective, cognitive, and evaluative development in humans that is not replaced by indirect and vicarious experiences, which are on the rise. Attributed to diversity and variability in space and time of the natural world, together with its unpredictiveness and challenges.
References: Searles 1959, Kaplan and Talbot 1983, Moore 1986, Kaplan and Kaplan 1989, Sebba 1991, Pyle 1993, Sobel 1993, Nabhan and Trible 1994, Kellert 1996, Kellert 2005, Burdette and Walker 2005
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 39

Reduced Contact with Nature


Considerable evidence that children (and adults) are spending increasingly less and less time experiencing high quality natural environments.
Due to a number of factors, including habitat destruction, species loss, environmental contamination, natural resource depletion, urban sprawl, human population growth, videophilia, and fear of violence.

References: Barney et al. 1980, Wilcox et al. 1991, Groombridge 1992, Wilson 1992, Myers 1994, Heywood 1995, Savage 1995, Kellert 2002, Pergams and Zaradic 2008

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

40

Reduced Contact with Nature


Parental concern for the health and safety of their children, in turn related to increased violence in our society, real and imagined, is an increasingly important factor.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

41

Nature-Deficit Disorder
Louv (2005) labeled the health syndrome associated with this diminished experience of nature in children today as nature-deficit disorder as a way of creating heightened awareness of the problem. Has had a major impact on the development of impending federal legislation on no child left inside.

Additional References: Clements 2004, Karstan 2005, Farmer 2005, Veitch et al. 2006, Jackson and Tester 2008
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 42

Nearby Nature
Key point concerning childrens direct contact with nature is that it be ongoing and highly accessible, which in turn implies that it be nearby. Has important implications for regional and urban planning.

References: Quantz 1897, Kaplan and Kaplan 1989, Sobel 1993, Kaplan et al. 1998, Wells 2000, Kellert 2002, Taylor et al. 2002, Wells and Evans 2003, Bell and Dyment 2006
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 43

CONTRIBUTIONS OF TREES AND FORESTS TO LEARNING


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 44

Dendro-Psychoses
The first scholarly treatment of the origins of the fundamental relationship between trees and people was that of the psychologist J. O. Quantz (1897).
Created the term dendro-psychoses to represent this relationship. Provided several lines of biological and psychological evidence of an adaptive nature to support the argument that the earliest humans dwelled in trees. Examples of psychological evidence included the fear of wild animals, thunder and lightning, high winds, and falling; hide and seek games; rocking babies to sleep.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

45

A Central Role
There is considerable evidence that trees have played a central role in everyday life throughout human history, including: Source of food and shade Safe sleeping and eating places Vantage points for surveying the landscape Escape from predators Provision of shelter, weapons, tools, and medicine Inspiration (TLS)

References: Bourliere 1963, Lee 1979, Isaac 1983, Shipman 1986, Heerwagen and Orians (1993).
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 46

Perception
Process of attaining awareness or understanding of sensory information (Wickipedia 2008) Information enhancers integrate active sense data with memory to produce a coherent perception (Rue 2005)

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

47

Tree Forms
A B

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

48

Tree Forms

Reference: Sommer and Summit 1996


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 49

Tree Forms
Orians (1980) hypothesized that tree forms that were most important in the survival of early humans in African savannas should be most preferred by humans today from the standpoint of aesthetics.
Referred specifically to the acacia form, with canopies broader than tall, trunks terminating and branching considerably below half the height of the tree, small leaves, and layered branching. Argued that such trees are easy to climb and their canopies offered greater protection from sun or rain. Hypothesis was tested on preferences of Americans for trees cultivated in Japanese gardens and for various dimensions of the dominant East African tree, Acacia tortilis.
Hypothesis was not rejected.

Additional References: Orians 1986, Heerwagen and Orians 1993

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

50

Acacia Tree Form


Preference for the acacia tree form over other tree forms was subsequently investigated by utilizing college students from all major continents except Antarctica.

References: Sommer and Summit 1996, Sommer 1997


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 51

Tree Forms

eucalyptus

oak

conifer

palm

acacia

Reference: Sommer and Summit 1996


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 52

Tree Forms

Sommer and Summint (1996)

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

53

Acacia Tree Form


In all cases, students preferred the acacia form over other forms (eucalyptus, oak, conifer, and palm). Moreover, they tended to prefer the most common tree experienced in their respective childhoods more so than students who grew up with other tree forms as most common, indicating that experience also shapes ones preferences of tree form.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

54

LANDSCAPE-LEVEL PREFERENCES
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 55

Landscape-Level Preferences

D
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

E
56

Landscape-Level Preferences
At the stand or landscape level, the evidence suggests that people prefer natural environments over built environments and savanna-type natural environments over other natural environments (Ulrich 1986, Kahn 1999).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

57

Landscape-Level Preferences

Balling, Falk. Mean Preference Scores for Each Biome as a Function of Age Group (1982).

Balling, Falk. Mean Preference Scores for Each Biome Under Both Live and Visit Instructions (1982).

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

58

Landscape-Level Preferences
(Balling and Falk 1982)
Young children (age 8-11 years) from the eastern U.S. preferred East African savannas over mixed hardwood forest (their home environment), boreal forest, rain forest, and desert, while older children showed an equal preference for savannas and mixed hardwood forest. Suggests the influence of both genetics and environment (familiarity) in such preferences.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

59

Savanna-like Environments
Preference for the savanna-like environment seems to be the result of being able to acquire new information (mystery) without becoming disoriented or lost (legibility).

References: Kaplan and Kaplan 1989, Kaplan 1992


T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 60

Savanna-like Environments
May help explain the appeal for closed-canopy forest stands that have been thinned or for urban or suburban open spaces that are planted with scattered trees in a grassy matrix, i.e., are park-like.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

61

Savanna-like Environments
May also help explain the tendency of European settlers in North America to open up forested landscapes and plant trees in prairie landscapes. (Orians 1980)

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

62

Ecological vs. Scenic Aesthetics


(Gobster 1999)
ECOLOGICAL preference for maintaining and restoring the ecological structure and function of ecosystems and for preserving and enhancing the health and diversity of native species and ecological communities. SCENIC a perceptual, affective reaction to the landscape: preference for scenic beauty or visual quality.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

63

Ecological vs. Scenic Aesthetics


(Gobster 1999)

Fire Dead and down wood Forest fragmentation

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

64

CONCLUSIONS

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

65

Conclusions
The development of learning skills, especially in children, and the realization of creativity and productivity in adults, seem to be enhanced significantly by direct and informal contact on a regular basis with natural settings, especially those that are savanna- or park-like. These findings may have important implications for the teaching and learning process in our society. In particular, the notion of less structured and more emotions- or value-laden environments for learning seem to run counter to traditional institutional approaches.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

66

Conclusions
Visual preferences for trees and forests may reflect adaptive behaviors in the early evolution of humans and conflict with the development of an ecological aesthetic, which may seem counter-instinctive or counter-intuitive.
Can learning/education remove this disconnect? (education of the emotions) The theory of visible stewardship (Sheppard 2001), which emphasizes caring for and attachment to a particular landscape, suggests it will be challenging.
T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 67

Conclusions
It is natural that we should learn from nature.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

68

Contact Information
Terry.Sharik@usu.edu

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

69

Pertinent References
Altman, I. and J. Wohlwill (eds.). 1978. Children and the environment. Plenum Press: NewYork. American Institutes for Research. 2005. Effects of outdoor education programs for children in California. American Institutes for Research: Palo Alto, CA. Appleton, J. 1990. The symbolism of habitat. University of Washington Press: Seattle. Balling, J. D. and J. H. Falk. 1982. The development of visual preference for natural environments. Environment and Behavior 14: 5-28. Barkow, J., L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (eds.). 1992. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press: Oxford. Barney, G. O. (ed.). Global 2000 report. Council on Environmental Quality: Washington, D. C. Bell, Anne C. and J. E. Dyment. 2006. Grounds for action: Promoting physical activity through school ground greening in Canada. Evergreen: Toronto, ON. Bettelheim, B. 1977. The uses of enchantment: The meaning and importance of fairy tales. Vintage Books: New York. Bloom, B. S., M. B., Engelhart, E. J. Furst, W. H. Hill, and D. R. Krathwohl. 1956. Taxonomy of educational objectives. Handbook I: The classification of educational goalscognitive domain. Longman: New York. Bourleire, F. 1963. Observations of the ecology of some large African mammals. pp. 43-54 In F. C. Howell and F. Bourliere (eds.). African ecology and human evolution. Aldine: Chicago. Burdette, H. L. and R. C. Whitaker. 2005. Resurrecting free play in young children: Looking beyond fitness and fatness to attention, affiliation, and affect. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine 159: 46-50.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

70

Chang, C. Y., W. E. Hammitt, P. K. Chen, L. Machnik, and W. C. Su. 2008. Psychophysiological responses and restorative values of natural environments in Taiwan. Landscape and Urban Planning 85: 79-84. Chang, C. Y. and Perng, J. L. 1998. Effect of landscape on psychological and physical responses. Journal of Therapeutic Horticulture 9: 783-76. Chang, C. Y. and Uan, L. L. 1999. Influences of landscape types on recovery of concentration and EMG. Journal of Landscape Planning 7: 1-22. Chawla, L. 1994. In the first century of places: Nature, poetry, and childhood memory. State University of New York Press: Albany, NY. Chawla, L. 2002. Spots of time: Manifold ways of being in nature in childhood. pp. 199-225 In P. Kahn, and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). 2002. Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Clements, R. 2004. An investigation of the state of outdoor play. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood 5: 68-80. Cobb, E. 1977. The ecology of imagination in childhood. Columbia University Press: New York. Coley, J. D., G. E. A. Solomon, and P. Shafto. 2002. The development of folk biology: A cognitive science perspective on childrens understanding of the biological world. pp. 65-91 In Kahn, P. and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

71

Cornell, J. 1979. Sharing nature with children. Dawn: Nevada City, CA. Derr, V. 2001. Growing up in the Hispano homeland: The interplay of nature, family, culture, and community in shaping childrens experiences and sense of place. Doctoral dissertation. School of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University: New Haven. Diamond, J. 1993. New Guineans and their natural world. Pp. 251-271 In S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (eds.). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press/Shearwater Books: Washington, D. C. Driver, B. L. 1996. Benefits driven management of natural areas. Natural Areas Journal, 16, 94-99. Driver, B. L., Nash, R., & Haas, G. (1987). Wilderness benefits: A state-of-knowledge review. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the National Wilderness Research Conference: Issues, state-of-knowledge, future directions, Ogden, UT. Eagles, P. J. and S. Muffitt. 1990. An analysis of childrens attitudes toward animals. J. Environmental Education 21: 41-44. Engel, S. 1995. The stories children tell: Making sense of the narratives of childhood. Freeman: New York. Erikson, E. 1968. Identity: Youth and crisis. Norton: New York. Ewert, A. 1989. Outdoor adventure pursuits: Foundations, models and theories. Publishing Horizons: Scottsdale, AZ. Farmer, C. 2005. Home Office citizenship survey: Top level findings from the children and young people survey. Home Office and the Department for Education and Skills: London, England. Fjrtoft, I. 2004. Landscape as playscape: The effects of natural environments on childrens play and motor development. Children, Youth and Environments 14: 21-44. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 72

Gardner, H. 1999. Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. Basic Books: New York. Gibson, J. J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton-Mifflin: Boston, MA. Gobster, P.H. 1999. An ecological aesthetic consideration for forest landscape management. Landscape Journal. 18(1):54-64. Goldsmity, E. 1993. The way: An ecology world-view. Shambala: Boston. Groombridge, B. (ed.). 1992. Global biodiversity. Chapman and Hall: London. Hammitt, W. E. 2007. The mental restorativeness of outdoor recreation environments. Annals of Leisure and Recreation Research. In review. Han, Ke-Tsung. 2003. A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Landscape and Urban Planning 64: 209-232. Hart, R. 1979. Childrens experience of place. Irvington Publishers: New York. Hartig, T., M. Mang, and G. W. Evans. 1991. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environment and behavior. 23: 3-26. Hartig, T., K. Korpela, G. W. Evans, T. Garling et al. 1997. A measure of restorative quality in environments. Scandanavian Housing and Planning research 14: 175-195. Hartig, T. and H. Staats. 2006. The need for psychological restoration as a determinant of environmental preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology 26: 215-226. Hartig, T., Evans, G. W., Jamner, L. D., Davis, D. S., & Garling, T. 2003. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. Journal of Environmental Psychology 23:109-123. Heerwagen, J. H. and G. H. Orians. 1993. Humans, habitats, and aesthetics. Pp. 138-172 In S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (eds.). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press/Shearwater Books: Washington, D. C. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 73

Heywood, V. 1995. Global biodiversity assessment. United Nations Environment Program/Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. Iozzi, L. A. 1989a. What research says to the educator. Part I. Environmental education and the affective domain. J. Environmental Education20: 3-9. Iozzi., L. A. 1989b. What research says to the educator. Part 2. Environmental education and the affective domain. J. Environmental Education 20: 6-13. Isaac, G. 1983. Bones in contention: Competing explanations for the juxtaposition of Early Pleistocene artifacts and faunal remains. In J. Clutton-Brock and C. Grigson. Animals and archeology, Part I: Hunters and their prey. B.A.R. International Series: Oxford. Isen, A. M., T. E. Nygren, and F. G. Ashby. 1985. The influence of positive affect on perceived utility of gains and losses. Bulletin Psychonomic Society 23: 301. Isen. A. M. 1990. Adaptation sociobiology, evolutionary psychology, and the variety of lessons to be learned. Motivation and Emotion 14: 295-302. Jackson, R. J. J. and J. Tester. 2008. Environment shapes health, including childrens mental health. Journal American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 47: 129-131. Kahn, P. H., Jr. 1999. The human relationship with nature: Development and culture. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Kahn, P. H., Jr. 2002. Childrens affiliations with nature: Structure, development, and the problem of environmental generational amnesia. pp. 93-116 In Kahn, P. and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). 2002. Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

74

Kahn, P. and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). 2002. Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations.. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Kanner, A. D., T. Roszak, and M. E. Gomes. 1995. Ecopsychology: Restoring the earth, healing the mind. Sierra Club Books: San Francisco Kaplan, R. and S. Kaplan. 1989. The experience of nature: a psychological perspective. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Kaplan, R. and S. Kaplan. 2002. Adolescents and the natural environment: A time out? pp. 227-257 In Kahn, P. and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). 2002. Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations.. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Kaplan, R., S. Kaplan, and R. L. Ryan. 1998. With people in mind: Design and management for everyday nature. Island Press: Washington, D. C. Kaplan, S. 1987. Aesthetics, affect, and cognition: Environmental preference from an evolutionary perspective. Environment and Behavior. 19:1(3-32). Kaplan, S. 1992. Environmental preference in a knowledge-seeking, knowledge-using organism. pp. 581-598 In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (eds.). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press: New York. Kaplan, S. 1995. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169-182. Kaplan, S. 2002. Some hidden benefits of the urban forest. p. 29 In: C. C. Konijnenkijk, N. E. Koch, K.. H. Hoyer, and J. Schipperijn J. (eds.). Forestry Serving Urbanized Societies. Proceedings of the IUFRO European Regional Conference, 27-30 August 2002, Copenhagen. Hoerscholm, Denmark: Skov & Landskab. (Abstract)

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

75

Kaplan, S. and R. Kaplan. 1982. Cognition and environment: Functioning in an uncertain world. Praeger Publishers: New York. Kaplan, S. and J. Talbot. 1983. Psychological benefits of a wilderness experience. In I. Altman and J. Wohlwill (eds.). Behavior and the natural environment. Plenum Press: New York. Karstan, L. 2005. It all used to be better? Different generations on continuity and change in urban childrens use of space. Childrens Geographies 3: 275-290. Kellert, S. R. 1985. Attitudes toward animals: Age-related development among children. J. Environmental Education 16: 29-39. Kellert, S. R. 1996. The value of life: Biological diversity and human society. Island Press: Washington, D.C. Kellert, S. R. 1997. Kinship to mastery: Biophilia in human evolution and development. Island Press/Shearwater Books: Washington, D. C. Kellert, S. R. 2002. Experiencing nature: Affective, cognitive, and evaluative development in children. pp. 117-151 In P. Kahn and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Kellert, S. R. 2005. Building for life: Designing and understanding the human-nature connection. Island Press: Washington, D. C. Kellert, S. R. and V. Derr. 1998. National study of outdoor wilderness experience. School of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University: New Haven. Kellert, S. R. and M. O. Westervelt. 1983. Childrens attitudes, knowledge and behaviors toward animals (Phase 5). U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service: Washington, D. C. T. L. Sharik Kellert, S. R. and E. O. Wilson (eds.). 1999. The human relationship with nature. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Mar 2009 76

Kirby, M. A. 1989. Nature as refuge in childrens environments. Childrens Environments Quarterly 6: 7-12. Lawrence, E. A. 1993. The sacred bee, the filth pig, and the bat out of hell: Animal symbolism as cognitive biophilia. pp. 301-341 In S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (eds.). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press: Washington, D.C. Lee, R. B. 1979. The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. Lickona, T. 1991. Educating for character. Bantam Books: New York. Lieberman, G. A. and L. L. Hoody. 1998. Closing the achievement gap: Using the environment as an integrating context for learning. SEER: Poway, CA. Louv, R. 2005. Last child in the woods: Saving our children from nature-deficit disorder. Algonquin Books of Chapel Hill: Chapel Hill, NC. Lumsden, D. and E. O. Wilson. 1983. The relation between biological and cultural evolution. J. Biological Structure 8: 343-359. Maker, C. J. 1982. Teaching models of the gifted. Pro-ed: Austin. Mednick, S. A. 1962. The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review 69: 220-232. Metzner, R. 1993. Spirit, self, and nature: Essays in green psychology. Green Earth: El Verno, CA. Millennium Ecosystem Assessment. 2005. Ecosystems and human well-being: Synthesis. Island Press: Washington, D.C. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 77

Moore, R. C. 1986. Childhoods domain: Play and space in child development. Croom Helm: London. Moore, R. C. 1993. Plants for play: A plant selection guide for childrens outdoor environments. MIG Communications: Berkeley, CA. Moore, R. C. 1997. The need for nature: A childhood right. Social Justice 24: 203. Moore, R. C. and H. H. Wong. 1997. Natural learning: The life history of an environmental schoolyard: Creating environments for rediscovering natures way of teaching. MIG Communications: Berkeley, CA. Myers, N. 1994. Global biodiversity II: Losses. In G. Meffe and C. Carrolls (eds.) . Principles of conservation biology. Sinauer: Sunderland, OH. Nabhan, G. P. and S. St. Antoine. 1993. The loss of floral and faunal story: The extinction of experience. pp. 229-250 In S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (eds.). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press: Washington, D.C. Nabhan, G. P. and S. A. Trimble. 1994. The geography of childhood: Why children need wild places. Beacon Press, Concord Library: Boston. Nicholson. S. 1971. How not to cheat children: The theory of loose parts. Landscape Architecture 62: 30-34. Olmsted, F. L. 1865. Preliminary report upon the Yosemite and Big Tree Grove. Report to the congress of the state of California. Reprinted in V. P. Ranney, G. J. Rauluk, and D., F. Hoffman (eds.). 1990. The Papers of Frederick Law Olmsted, vol. V: The California frontier, 1863-1865. John Hopkins Press: Baltimore. Orians, G. H. 1980. Habitat selection: General theory and applications to human behavior. In J. S. Lockard (ed.). The evolution of human social behavior. Elsevier: New York. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 78

Orians, G. H. 1986. An ecological and evolutionary approach to landscape aesthetics. In E. C. Penning-Rowsell and D. Lowenthal. Allen and Unwin: London. Orians, G. H. and J. H. Heerwagen. 1992. Evolved responses to landscapes. pp. 555-579 In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (eds.). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press: New York. Orr, D. W. 1992. Ecological literacy: Education and transition to a postmodern world. State University of New York Press: Albany, NY. Pergams, O. R. W. and P. A. Zaradic. 2008. Evidence for a fundamental and pervasive shift away from nature-based recreation. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.o709893105. Piaget, J. 1969. The childs conception of the world. Littlefield, Adams: Totowa, NJ. Pyle, R. M. 1993. The thunder tree: Lessons from an urban wildland. Houghton Mifflin: Boston. Quammen. D. 1985. Natural acts: A sidelong view of science and nature. Avon Books: New York. Quantz, J. O. 1897. Dendro-psychoses. Am. J. Psychology 9: 449-506. Ratanapojnard, S. 2001. Community-oriented biodiversity environmental education: Its effects on knowledge, values, and behavior among rural fifth-and sixth-grade students in northeastern Thailand. Doctoral dissertation, School of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University: New Haven. Roszak, T. 1992. The view of the earth: An exploration of ecopsychology. Simon and Schuster: New York. Rue, L. 2005. Religion is not about God: How spiritual traditions nurture our biological nature and what to expect when they fail. Rutgers University Press: New Brunswick, NJ. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 79

Savage, J. 1995. Systematics and the biodiversity crisis. BioScience 45: 673-679. Searles, H. F. 1959. The nonhuman environment. International Universities Press: New York. Sebba, R. 1991. The landscapes of childhood: The reflections of childhoods environment in adult memories and in childrens attitudes. Environment and Behavior 23: 395-422. Shepard, P. 1978. Thinking animals: Animals and the development of human intelligence. Viking Press: New York. Shepard, P. 1982. Nature and madness. Sierra Book Clubs: San Francisco. Shepard, P. 1996. The others: How animals made us human. Island Press: Washington, D.C. Sheppard, S. R. J. 2001 Beyond visual resource management: Emerging theories of an ecological aesthetic and visible stewardship. pp. 149-172 In S. R. J. Sheppard and H. W. Harshaw (eds.). Forests and landscapes: linking ecology, sustainability, and aesthetics. CABI Publications: New York. Shipman, P. 1986. Scavenging or hunting in early hominids: Theoretical framework and tests. American Anthropologist 88: 27-40. Sobel, D. 1993. Childrens special places: Exploring the role of forts, dens, and bush houses in middle childhood. Zephyr Press: Tucson. Sobel, D. 1996. Place-based education: Connecting classrooms and communities. The Orion Society and the Myrin Institute: Great Barrington, MA. Sommer, R, F. Learey, J. Summit, and M. Tirrell. 1994. Social benefits of resident involvement in tree planting: Comparison with developer-planted trees. J. Arboriculture 20: 323-328. Sommer, R. 1997. Further cross-national studies of tree form preference. Ecol. Psychol. 9: 153-160. Sommer, R. 2001. The dendro-psychoses of J. O. Quantz. J. Arboriculture 27: 40-43. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 80

Sommer, R. and J. Summit. 1996. Cross-national rankings of tree shape. Ecol. Psychol. 8: 327-341. Staats, H. 2003. Guest editor's introduction: restorative environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 103107. Staats, H.and T. Hartig. 2004. Alone or with a friend: a social context for psychological restoration and environmental preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 199-211. State Education and Environment Roundtable (SEER). 2005. California Student Assessment Project Phase Two: The effects of environmental-based education on student achievement. SEER: Poway, CA. Summit, J. and R. Sommer. 1999. Further studies of preferred tree shapes. Environment and Behavior 31: 550-576. Taylor, A. F. and F. E. Kuo. 2006. Is contact with nature important for healthy child development? State of the evidence. pp. 124-140 In C. Spencer and M. Blades (eds.). Children and their environments: Learning, using, and designing space. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK. Taylor, A. F., F. E. Kuo, and W. C. Sullivan. 2002. View of nature and self-discipline: Evidence from inner city children. Journal of Environmental Psychology 22: 46-63. Thomashow, C. 2002. Adolescents and ecological integrity. pp. 259-278 In P. Kahn and S. R. Kellert. (eds.). Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. Ulrich, R. S. 1979. Visual landscapes and psychological well being. Landscape Research 4: 17-23. Ulrich, R. S. 1981. Natural versus urban scenes: Some psychophysiological effects. Environment and Behavior 13: 523-556. Ulrich, R. S. 1986. Human responses to vegetation and landscapes. Landscape and Urban Planning 13: 29-44. T. L. Sharik Mar 2009 81

Ulrich, R. S. 1993. Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes. pp. 73-137 In S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson. The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press/Sherwater Books: Washington, D. C. Ulrich, R. S. and Simons. 1986. Recovery from stress during exposure to everyday outdoor environments. pp. 115122 In J. W. Wineman and C. Zimring (eds.). Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Environmental Design Research Association, April, 1986. EDRA: Washington, DC. Ulrich, R. S., R. Simons, B. D. Losito, E. Fiorito, M. A. Miles, and M. Zelson. 1991a. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology 11: 201-230. Ulrich, R. S., U. Dimbeg, and B. L. Driver. 1991b. Psychophysiological indicators of leisure benefits. pp. 73-89 In B. L. Driver, P. J. Brown, and G. L. Peterson (eds.). Benefits of Leisure. Venture Publishing: State College, PA. Veitch, J., S. Bagley, K. Ball, and J. Solomon. 2006. Where do children usually play? A qualitative study of parents perceptions of influences on childrens active free play. Health and Place 12: 383-393. Wells, N. M. 2000. At home with nature: Effects of greenness on childrens cognitive functioning. Environment and Behavior 32: 775-795. Wells, N. M. and G. Evans. 2003. Nearby nature: A buffer of life stress among rural children. Environment and Behavior 35: 311-330. Whittaker, R. H. 1970. Communities and ecosystems. Macmillan: New York.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

82

Wilcov, D. S., D. Rothstein, J. Dubow, A. Phillips, and E. Losos. 1998. Quantifying threats to imperiled species in the United States. Bioscience 48: 607-615. Williams, G. C. and R. M. Nesse. 1991. The dawn of Darwinian medicine. Quarterly Review of Biology 66: 1-21. Wilson, E. O. 1984. Biophilia. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA. Wilson, E. O. 1992. The diversity of life. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA. Wilson, E. O. 1993. Biophilia and the conservation ethic. pp. 31-41 In S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (eds.). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press: Washington, D.C.

T. L. Sharik Mar 2009

83

You might also like