You are on page 1of 8

Chapter 1 .

2 Psychology' s Premises, Methods and Values


Brian R. Clifford University of East London On the surface it would seem as if law and psychology share common concerns in that they are both trying to understand and predict human behaviour. In short, both law and psychology take human nature as their subject matter. However, beyond this commonality of focus, closer inspection would seem to reveal that the two disciplines appear to diverge at the levels of value, basic premises, their models, their approaches, their criteria of explanation and their methods. Thus, while both psychology and substantive law, legal processes and jurisprudence all address assumptions about the causes and modifications of behaviour, about the process of perception, memory, recognition and decision making, and about how people think and feel, in the past mutual facilitation has been marked by its absence. Why is this? Arguments have been made (e.g. Tapp, 1969) that while law is value-laden and subjective, relying upon tradition and precedent, psychology is value-free and objective, relying upon empirical research. Again, it has been asserted that law is a practical art, a system of rules, a means of social control, concerned with solving practical problems. Psychology, on the other hand, is a science, concerned with the description, explanation, understanding, prediction and control of human behaviour. Haward (1979) talks of law as being an 'abattoir of sacred cows' for psychology, in so far as all the beliefs that psychologists are thought to hold dear are disputed by lawyers. Philosophically, the psychologist believes in some degree of determinism (or causality), whereas the lawyer believes in free will. Thus, while the psychologist tends to talk in terms of causes o/behaviour the lawyer talks in terms of reasonsfor behaviour. The law conceives of people as freely and consciously controlling their own behaviour, choosing their actions and thus taking responsibility for them. As Bentley (1979) points out,
Handbook of Psycho/ogy in Legal Contexts Edited by R. Bull and D. Carson. 1995 John Wiley & Sons Ltd

-'

.;,-.

. ^,

14

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

15

consciousness and free will are axiomatic in legal theory. This does not sit well with psychology, which has a different explanatory framework. ' . :' At the level of theory construction and everyday practical activity, as Meehl (1977) points out, lawyers rely on common-sense generalisations about human behaviours, based upon speculation, introspection, intuition, reflection, culturally transmitted beliefs and personal anecdotal observation. Psychology, on the other hand, favours empirical research and, where feasible and ethically practicable, experimental (or at least systematic) testing. Thus, the proper place of common sense in the 'scheme of things' is a major battle ground: if psychology agrees with common sense, lawyers feel that nothing has been gained by their interaction with psychologists; if psychology disagrees, or produces counter-intuitive findings, lawyers prefer to run with common sense and intuition, perhaps because they believe that psychology's counter-factual assertions or propositions, masquerading as truth, may be nothing more than controversial or speculative theory, poorly corroborated by available evidence. Like all caricatures, there is more than a grain of truth in this polarised stereotyping of law and psychology. But they are stereotypes none the less, and their truth values are called into question by the fact that in this century . psychology has provided us with models, concepts and findings which have radically changed the way we think about people, frequently with implications for law, since law, legal procedures and discussions of law and jurisprudence . all involve assumptions about the nature of human beings, their capacities and their behaviour. Psychological theory and research have influenced, inter alia, laws concerning discrimination, capital punishment, pornography, sexual behaviour, child abuse and the conditions under which individuals may not be held responsible for their actions (e.g. Tapp and Levine, 1977). In other words, a case can certainly be mounted that scientific psychology has added to, clarified and, I would like to argue, possibly improved upon the common sense or naive psychology on which law has proceeded in the past. If this positive fertilisation is to continue and accelerate into mutually beneficial reciprocity, a clear understanding by lawyers of what psychology is, and is not, is required. It is the purpose of this chapter to begin this process by outlining the premises, methods and values of psychology.

"* ,

-' ' -

Premise 1: Acceptance of the Scientific Method Psychology accepts that science is but one approach to the discovery and ordering of knowledge. It is taken to be different from common sense, metaphysics, religion, magic, tradition, rationalism and phenomenology, not because of its subject matter, but because of its methodology. Psychology is unified and distinct because of its method or logic of discovery. Specific techniques vary between and within different sub-disciplines of psychology, but the basic method remains the samecareful, controlled observation, rational and constrained reasoning and the subjecting of theories to empirical test. Description is the empirical goal of psychology and explanation is the theoretical goal. Premise 2: Acceptance of the Basic Assumptions of the Scientific Enterprise The psychological approach is guided by assumptions that are unproven and unprovable. They are necessary prerequisites for the conduct of scientific discoursethey constitute the axiomatic substructure of psychology. Nature is orderly and regular. Events do not occur haphazardly. Change itself displays patterns that can be understood. This belief applies to all people, conditions and phenomena. We can know nature. This is an important axiom for psychology because it assumes that humans are just as much a part of nature as are other natural objects, and although they possess unique and distinctive characteristics they can yet be understood and explained by the same methods as all science. Individuals and groups exhibit sufficiently recurring, orderly and empirically demonstrable patterns as to be amenable to scientific study. Nothing is self-evident. Claims for truth must be demonstrated objectively. Tradition, subjective belief and common sense are not sufficient for verification. Herein lies the sceptical and critical attitude of psychology. Knowledge is acquired from the acquisition of experience. This emphasis on empirical knowledge is a counter to the belief that knowledge is innate in humans and that pure reason is sufficient to produce verifiable knowledge. Premise 3: Acceptance of the Rules of Scientific Enquiry The pre-theoretical axioms listed above break through to the surface when we examine the rules of science. These are many but the chief ones are as follows. Use operational definitions. Operationism means that terms must be defined by the steps or operations used to measure them.

,, ,

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES Psychology claims to be both a theoretical science and at the same time an applied discipline, able to serve in the practical affairs of the world (Clifford, 1981). It makes this claim because, as a discipline, it sees itself as objective, empirical, eclectic and humane. Underpinning this self-perception is the acceptance of a number of premises.

16

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

17

Generality. This means that discussion or statements need to be about abstract variables, not particular antecedent and consequent conditions. Thus, descriptions are stated in terms of variables, not the specific stimuli that exhibited these variables; for example, 'punishment', not a smack on the hand or a smack on the bottom. Controlled observation. This allows tentative identification of why something happened, what caused it, under what conditions. The stress on this method is predicated upon the concept of causality. The argument is that, assuming causality, the surest way to uncover such causality is to hold constant all variables not under test while varying the variable that is under investigation (the independent variable), because variables that do not vary cannot explain changes in behaviour. Controlling variables is not the only way to discount a variable as the cause of change, but it is the most direct and certain. To lack control in this sense is to court the dreaded confound: where two variables vary together, making interpretation of their separate effects impossible. Replication. To be replicable data must be reliable. If data are not reliable, then explanations are not either. Scientific statements are meant to apply to whole classes or populations of people, conditions or phenomena. Before generalisation can be accepted it must be shown to hold for an adequate sample drawn from the population of interest. Unless proper and sufficient replications are made, generalisations ought not to be made. Parsimony. Psychologists should never produce a complicated or more abstract explanation unless all other, simpler, explanations have been experimentally ruled out. Consistency. If two explanatory statements are contradictory then at least one of them must be false. The principle of consistency requires that an explanatory statement must not contradict any explanatory statement that has been confirmed. Confirmation. Explanatory statements must admit of predictive statements and these statements must be verifiable/falsifiable.

Problem Generalisation Hypothesis

Data analysis

Research design

Measuring instruments Figure 1.2.1

empirical verification are the criteria employed by science to evaluate claims to knowledge. So with psychology. Logic is concerned with valid reasoning but not empirical truth. Thus, while validity has to do with internal consistency, experience or empiricism has to do with approximations to truth. Both these ways of reasoning, and both aims or goals, are enshrined in the research process. This research process has seven basic stages, as can be seen in Figure 1.2.1. Problemformulation. The observation of a problem to be understood can come from anywhere or anyone. Problem formulation involves thinking about a problem and tentatively generating some possible explanations. These possible explanations are then converted into possible tests of their correctness. This is usually done in the form of hypotheses. Hypotheses. Hypotheses represent refinements of ideas into testable propositions that can be, at least in principle, confirmed or falsified. Research designs. The nature of the hypotheses generated and the embedding theory and experimentation that are known to be relevant to it will suggest how best to go about testing predictions. Different research methods and experimental designs each have their good and bad points. The selection of the appropriate design is as much an art as it is a science. Measuring instruments. This is partly a function of operationalisation referred to above but, again, different questions will require different solutions in terms of which measures will be decided upon.

: -.,'.,!-

Premise 4: Acceptance of the Generic Research Process So far we have outlined the unwritten assumptions of the psychological method and we have detailed the discipline's operating ground rules for membership of the scientific club. Let us now see how psychologists actually progress, irrespective of their particular sub-discipline, at a fairly abstract level. Scientific knowledge is knowledge provable by both reason and experience. That is, science operates in two distinct but interrelated worldsthe conceptual/theoretical and the observational/empirical. Logical validation and

18

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

19

Data collection. Once again, decisions have to be made concerning the timing, duration, amount and nature of data to be collected. A basic issue, because of the reliance of experimental psychology on statistical analysis, is that the data should be quantifiable in some way, either initially or at least eventually. This is one of the areas where the psychologist and the psychiatrist can most clearly be differentiated. Unlike the psychologist, the psychiatrist is concerned to listen to the utterances of a person and then engage in interpretation of these utterances. However, when psychiatrists employ tests to aid their interpretation they are coming closer to the psychologist, because these tests will have involved standardisation on large numbers of people initially. Data analyses. A huge number of statistical tests are potentially available to the psychologist, and a lawyer coming to psychology is likely to be mystified by this range. This is not a real issue, however, because the choice of appropriate statistical test is dictated by such things as the number of groups in the study, whether the conditions are related in some way or not, and the level of measurement of the dependent variable (see below). The number of types of different measurement one takes of any person in an experiment is also a deciding factor in what statistical tests should be used. Generalisation. As was said above all psychologists wish to generalise their particular findings beyond the actual study conducted. Whether they can legitimately do so depends on a number of issues. Statistical analysis allows generalisation to other samples of the population from which the experimental sample was drawn, and the population of that sample per se. To the extent that the study reproduced the conditions of the situation to which the person wishes to generalise tentative generalisation is defensible. In general, the issue of extrapolation from known situations to unknown situations is a question of internal and external validity (see below). Premise 5: Acceptance of the Aims of Science The aims of science in general and psychology in particular are many and varied and shift at different stages in the maturity of the various sub-disciplines, and within a research programme' s life cycle. However, with few exceptions, ' all psychologists would accept that they are concerned with describing, explaining, understanding, predicting and controlling particular processes and general behaviour repertoires. These, then, are the premises upon which psychology operates. It believes that the scientific method is the most powerful calculus yet devised to produce verifiable and falsifiable knowledge. It has unwritten assumptions, unspoken rules of evidence and evaluation, and clearly marked stages of data gathering, theory construction and knowledge generation. It also has areas of uncertainty and controversy. The scientific method is believed by psychologists to be the royal road, but it is not without its pot holesbut these pot holes are

troublesome, not terminal. Having looked at the premises, we can now move on to look at the methods that psychology employs.

METHODS OF PSYCHOLOGY
At the level of method all psychologists, of whatever persuasion, believe that observed behaviours and changes in behaviour are caused by something, whether they be external factors or internal factors such as limited processing capacity, attitudes, beliefs or values, or mere habit and conditioning. The application of research methods by psychology is concerned to establish just what causes what. The True Experiment The true experiment, defined as the random allocation of subjects (i.e. people) to experimental conditions and the manipulation of one or more independent variables, is believed to be the best way to disentangle cause and effect relationships. By randomly allocating people to experimental conditions any individual difference factors, due to the individuality of these different people, . are spread throughout the experimental design and thus, while these human differences may dilute any real effects that are present, they will not distort the true effects. By varying only one or a few variables while holding all other conditions constant this allows fairly unambiguous delineation of cause and effect relationships. .; .''. The key feature of the experimental method involves careful manipulation or change of some aspect of a situation and observing the effects this change has ;; on some behaviour or thought process of interest, in order to establish ' relationships. The event, condition or situation that is manipulated is called the independent variable (a variable being something that can assume different valuese.g. noise: high, medium or low), so called because it is under the ;. .; <: \ .' control of the experimenter and independent of the people taking part in the experiment. The physical, psychological or social changes which are measured are called the dependent variable (because the changes depend on the values of the manipulated variablee.g. increased, decreased or unchanged reasoning performance). Changes in the dependent variable are usually recorded as, or translated into, numbers to allow statistical procedures to be applied to them. The basic experimental design involves two groups of people (subjects): the experimental group, which has the independent variable applied to or withheld from it, and the control group, which does not experience any experimental manipulation at all. This basic design can be, and usually is, greatly increased in complexity.

20

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

21

Usually an experiment involves taking measurements from each individual under each of the conditions of manipulation or treatment that they appear in. All these scores are then converted to a mean score (average) for the particular condition or group and then interpreted for correlation, difference or trend. Basic to this task is statistics: the discipline that deals with sampling data from a (theoretical) population of individuals and then drawing inferences about the population from the sample. Statistical tests are applied to the means of the different manipulated groups, and if numerical differences translate into statistically significant differences then this declares that the observed differences between the different groups are trustworthy and not due to chance factors. As such, they should generalise to other samples of the same population and the population as a whole. There are a number of intrinsic but not insuperable problems in the true experiment as far as the law is concerned. First, in experimental analyses it is the means of the groups that are compared and, because of individual differences, it is possible that any one person in a particular group will be better or worse than the mean score would suggest. That is, group means may not represent the performance of any one individual in that group. This has the implication that, for example, a mean difference between adult and child witness groups, in favour of adults, may mask the fact that some children actually outperformed some adults. A second problem is that significance is evaluated in terms of presence or absence, not size. But 'significance' does not equal 'importance'; it simply indicates that it is unlikely that the effect came about by chance. Most psychological research has not been analysed by the power statistics recommended by Cohen (1977; Cohen and Cohen, 1975) despite these being readily available and computationally simple. Thus we need to know not only that an effect is present, but whether it is large enough to matter for practical purposes. Yet another issue raised concerning the experimental method is its artificiality. Rabbitt (1981) calls it a tenuous abstraction of real-life situations. This raises questions of internal and external validity. Internal validity refers to the degree to which we can be sure that variation in the dependent variable is due to manipulation of the independent variable. External validity refers to the extent to which the results of an experiment can be generalised to other situations, other subjects and other tasks. The problem is that, in general, as you increase one type of validity so you reduce the validity of the other. External validity is often talked about in terms of ecological validity, but the two are somewhat different. External validity refers to subject representativeness (can we extrapolate from rats to humans?), variable representativeness (do findings with 'white noise', which is known to cause arousal, generalise to the arousal present in rape situations?), and the setting representativeness of the study (do findings obtained from people watching a rape scene on a video recorder correspond to findings obtained from victims or witnesses actually involved

in a real rape crime?). Only this last area of representativeness is concerned with ecological validity. However, when we consider problems of ecological validity we need to distinguish between realism and generalisability (Berkowitz and Donnerstein, 1982). Realism refers to whether the experimental setting bears a resemblance to the real world. A key question is the importance of the mundane realism (Clifford, 1978). If we can be sure that the psychological processes are the same in the contrived situation as in the real world, then the degree of realism is not a real threat to external validity. The ability of an experiment's results to generalise to other situations is more important than their superficial resemblance to the real world. Additionally, surface realism does not guarantee an increase in generalisability (Banaji and Crowder, 1989; Crowder, 1993). Thus it is generalisability rather than surface realism that determines whether a study is ecologically valid (Elmes, Kantowitz and Roediger HI, 1992). Despite all these objections the experimental method is still the linchpin of the psychological endeavour. This is so because, despite its acknowledged weaknesses, the true experiment is still the best method of ascertaining knowledge in situations where it is practical and ethical to conduct such a study. All the other research methods available, and used by the psychologist, have their own problems which, on balance, are more problematic than the experimental method. However, in certain situations the true experiment is not available as an option and other methods must be adopted, as described below.

Case Studies
Case study research can be of two types: archival case histories or case studies proper. In the first, a researcher surveys the case records of many people, trying to discern trends or patterns. Such study can provide important descriptive information but not much else. The second type of case study involves longitudinal studies of single subjects over time with many observations being made. There are several problems with this type of research: there is no adequate baseline against which to compare change; because they are idiographic (based upon one person), generalisation is problematic; because variables are hopelessly confounded, causality cannot be unambiguously ascribed. Thus, the most we can have are interesting hypotheses.

Naturalistic Observation Studies These are frequently referred to as field studies. Here humans are observed in their natural habitats. The major problems are what and how to observe. Choice of observational units may entail theory that has not been made explicit. How to measure will involve sampling of behaviours, and decisions over whether ' to measure frequency, rate, magnitude, duration or latency (i.e. period before

22

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

23

onset) of behaviour will also have to be made. The biggest drawback to this form of research is that you are at the mercy of events. If the event of interest does happen then other things may be happening at the same time, either seen or not seen by the researcher. If this is the case, then again we have major problems of ascribing causality. To the extent that these methods cannot ascribe causality, then this type of research is merely illustrative, descriptive, interesting or suggestive. Correlational Studies These types of study look for relationships between two or more factors or variables, at least one of which is not controlled by the researcher. The correlational approach has been a valuable tool in the history of psychology, especially in the fields of personality, social and intellectual development. It is mainly used when manipulation is impossible or impractical. Multiple regression or factor analysis, in which many variables are inter-correlated, have frequently been employed with this type of method to determine the presence or importance of relevant clusters of common factors or correlations. The problem with correlational studies is that correlation does not mean causation. All that a correlation study can show is one of two things: if there is no correlation then there can be no causal relationship between or among the variables being investigated; if, however, there is a statistically significant correlation between or among variables, causation cannot be deduced. This is because of the possibility of a mediating, but undetected, truly causal variable being present. Thus, a significant correlation can only be suggestive of causality, never definitively so. Quasi Experimental Designs Here we can directly manipulate the independent variable but we cannot randomly allocate subjects to experimental conditions. Because such designs are always dealing with intact groups (i.e. pre-existing different groups of people) within specific experimental conditions, causality is always problematical because other factors could be acting as confounds between these intact groups, falsely'suggesting'that the independent variable is having an effect. Questionnaire Research This is a popular methodology of social psychologists. It can produce a huge amount of data on a wide range of issues in a short space of time. However, it has several difficulties and disadvantages. The questions asked have to be very carefully composed, the responses have to be considered in terms of the level of measurement that they will yield, and not everyone returns their questionnaire. This immediately causes problems of sampling and the possible

distorted results that may eventuate from a self-selected sample. There is no check possible that the respondent filled out the questionnaire in the way it was hoped they wouldor even that they, rather than someone else, filled it out. As such, this methodology is suggestive but never definitive.

Qualitative Research
This approach is used by social psychologists and by developmental!sts who explain behaviour by underlying changes in structures and mechanisms that are related to age. The data sought are qualitative rather than quantitative, and are concerned with the feelings, concepts and imagery that people have about the issue under investigation. This type of research is being strengthened by the development of discourse analysis, and other quasi-quantifiable techniques such as Q sort, which gets people to sort a large number of statements into a rank order and then proceeds to interpret the groupings or cluster of respondents giving similar rank orderings. Thus, Q sort scales people, not items, and this analysis is aided by computerised statistical programs. Statistical Methods All the above research methodologies try to exercise some sort of experimental control over the responses of interest. It is, however, also possible to exercise statistical control. The major statistical technique is multiple regression. This technique is employed by those who accept that all behaviour is multidetermined and complex. Multiple regression assumes independent variables are correlated but that it is possible to separate out the individual and joint '" ' * contribution of any factor to any dependent variable of interest. Thus, the contribution of age, sex, birth position, economic status, amount of television watched, previous offending, racial type, and so on, could all be looked at together in multiple regression analysis and the individual and collective contribution of each of these variables to, for example, delinquent behaviour could be assessed. There are other equally powerful statistical techniques that psychologists are increasingly employing as an alternative to tight experimental control over variables, especially where such control is difficult or unethical. These then are the many and varied techniques that psychologists have at their disposal. The technique chosen will always be that which gives greatest control over variance (variability within subjects and between experimental conditions) and sources of error. Thus the methodology eventually chosen for use will be that which maximises induced variance, minimises error variance and controls extraneous variance, as far as the situation allows. Having said this, however, a sensible approach that should be adopted by any psychologist who knows that his or her findings are going to be applied, or may be applied at some future date, is to validate initial findings by convergent

24

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

25

operations. That is, attempt to obtain the same findings despite using several different methodologies, tasks and experimental populations. In addition, while it is true that the research method is a question of logic and not of location, ecological validity dictates that different situations of mundane realism be sampled also. Replication is the backbone of science and unless and until replications, replications with refinements, and conceptual replications have been undertaken no psychologists worth their salt would allow their results to go forward as solid and thus pertinent to practical issues.

methods will be experimental and learning paradigms. Behavioural psychologists could be relevant to legal concerns by explaining the causes of crime and the roots of particular behaviours, to the concept of mens rea and to issues of punishment and rehabilitation programmes. Cognitive psychologists are concerned with the higher mental processes of perception, memory, thinking and decision making. Thus, they will explain overt behaviour by recourse to cognitive mechanisms and processes. A key explanatory framework will involve information processing whereby incoming information is processed in a variety of waysit is selected, transformed, translated, compared and contrasted with information and every-day world knowledge already in memory. Their methods will be almost exclusively experimental. These psychologists have already had a great deal of contact with the criminal justice system in the fields of witnessing, confessions and jury decision making. A psychoanalytic ally orientated psychologist will have imbibed the views of the psychoanalytic school of Freud and his followers and will argue that much of our behaviour stems from unconscious processes. Unconscious motives rather than rational reasons are sought for all behaviours under this perspective. Their methods will involve case histories and talking-through. This involves allowing patients/clients to talk freely about their lives in order to facilitate the bringing to the surface of deep-seated blocks, and bound and free-floating anxieties. The analysis of dreams could also play a part in uncovering hidden motives for behaviour. These psychologists will be concerned with dangerousness, aggressive behaviour, the way trauma can have deep and lasting effects, and all cases involving mental or behavioural abnormality. All these behaviours, however, can also be explained outwith the psychoanalytic domain. Psychoanalytic psychologists are found within the developmental, clinical and educational specialisms to be discussed below. Developmentalists concern themselves mainly with the evolutionary 'staged' aspects of development from birth to old age. They will be interested in such things as separation, attachment and bonding issues, and cognitive abilities that impact on, for example, competency to testify. All these issues are central concerns of the law in so far as it relates to children. Their methods will be eclectic and involve observation, survey, experimental and case history. Personality psychologists are concerned with the uniqueness of persons, whether as a member of an identifiable type or group or as an individual. They continuously debate whether the nomothetic approachwhich tries to establish the major dimensions of personality on which people may differ but which are present to some degree in everyoneor the ideographic approachwhich seeks to establish the uniqueness of an individual's personalityis the better way fully to understand personality. That is, whether we should treat everyone as unique, thus ruling out the possibility of predicting

VALUES IN PSYCHOLOGY
I began by arguing that psychologists comprise a wide range of specialisms. It was not always so. As psychology evolved from the 1890s onwards and established itself as an empirical science being based on systematic observation rather than reason alone, it developed and separated into a small number of loosely coherent schools best characterised by busy confusions with occasional transient clear directions. Such schools declined after the 1940s and this way of looking at psychology is no longer profitable. Their influence lives on but the organisation of psychology is now along different lines, and may best be conceived of in terms of perspectives, fields of interest or domains of operations or specialisms. That is, different groups believe that their particular approach offers the best value in understanding human beings. It is with different perspectives, fields of interest or specialisms, rather than adherents to grand schools of psychology that lawyers will most likely interact. It is important to appreciate, however, that these different perspectives are not mutually exclusive and may represent merely a different focus on different aspects of the same complex phenomenon. Many psychologists classify themselves as biological psychologists, psychophysiologists, psychobiologists orneuropsychologists. Their basic assumption is that all psychological events correspond to some activity in the brain. The main concern is to explain psychological phenomena by means of brain sites or tissues, neurotransmitters, or hormonal activity stimulated by brain activity. Thus they will be interested, for example, in how hormones influence mood and behaviour, what areas of the brain control speech or reasoning and how brain damage or injury can cause the loss of these faculties, or how drugs can influence rational behaviour. Their major methods will be experimental, allied to invasive and increasingly non-invasive brain techniques. Behavioural psychologists will not invoke neurophysiological structures in the brain to account for behaviour; nor will they seek to recruit mentalistic concepts. They will be concerned to explain functional relations between stimulus and response by making recourse to rewards and punishment, and their scheduling (their consistent or inconsistent application). Their major

!f

26

INTRODUCTION: INTERDISCIPLINARY AND INTERPROFESSIONAL

PSYCHOLOGY'S PREMISES, METHODS AND VALUES

27

their behaviour, but gaining a richer insight into their unique traits, or view each person as a constellation of known traits and thus somewhat predictable in known situations. Their methods will involve personality questionnaires, repertory grids, observation and correlational studies. Clinical and counselling psychologists share the same focus but are differently trained. Clinical psychologists are not medically trained (as psychiatrists are), but they do deal with abnormal and psychiatric behaviour of all types. Counselling psychologists, while often concerned with abnormal behaviour, tend to be concerned with less severe forms of abnormality. However, because both are engaged in the application of psychological principles at the point of breakdown, and with the diagnosis and treatment of emotional and behavioural problems, they will be concerned with mental illness, drug addiction, marital and family conflicts and, especially counselling psychologists, less serious adjustment problems. As mentioned above, clinical psychologists may have a .psychoanalytic orientation but, especially in the UK, the behaviouristic, cognitive and personality approach is also widely present. Educational psychologists usually deal with individual children who have emotional or learning problems and thus could have an important role to play in several areas of the law as it relates to children. Their methods will involve psychometric testing, observation and case histories. From the above it is likely that when a lawyer comes to the discipline of psychology he or she may be excused for exclaiming 'what discipline?'. So what is it that binds these disparate activities together? What principle brings about coherence? The answer is that, whatever the nature of activity being investigated, there is always a level of analysis beyond the 'what' level. This deeper level asks the question 'how' and 'why'. These questions are the focus of all professionals who called themselves psychologists, and it is the nature of the answers to these questions which cause the 'bewildering confederacy' of psychology to cohere, despite their very different methods of enquiry concerning the addressing of these questions. This broad-stroke insight into what psychology is and is not should help you appreciate and understand what is to follow in subsequent chapters of this volume. The assumptions and working methods of a wide variety of psychologists have been outlined and the core relevance of psychology to law as a discipline, and as a body of professionals, has at least been hinted at.

D.P. Farrington, K. Hawkins and S. Lloyd-Bostock (Eds), Psychology, Law and Legal Processes. London: Macmillan. Berkowitz, L. and Donnerstein, E. (1982). External validity is more than skin deep. American Psychologist, 37, 245-57. Clifford, B.R. (1978). A critique of eyewitness research. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris and R.N. Sykes (Eds), Practical Aspects of Memory. London: Academic Press. Clifford, B.R. (1981). Towards a more realistic appraisal of the psychology of testimony. In S. Lloyd-Bostock (Ed.), Psychology in Legal Context: Applications and Limitations. London: Macmillan. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for Behavioural Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cohen, J. and Cohen, P. (1975). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Crowder, R.G. (1993). Commentry: faith and skepticism in memory research. In G.M. Davies and R.H. Logic (Eds), Memory in Everyday Life. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. Elmes, D.G., Kantowitz, B.H. and Roediger HI, H.L. (1992). Research Methods in Psychology (4th edn). St Pauls: West Publishing. Haward, L.R.C. (1979). The psychologist as expert witness. In D.P. Farrington, K. Hawkins, and S. Lloyd-Bostock (Eds), Psychology, Law and Legal Processes. London: Macmillan. Meehl, P.E. (1977). Law and the fireside inductions: some reflections of a clinical psychologist. In J.L. Tapp and F.J. Levine (Eds), Law, Justice and the Individual in Society. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston. Rabbitt, P. (1981). Applying human experimental psychology to legal questions about evidence. In S. Lloyd-Bostock (Ed.), Psychology in Legal Contexts: Applications and Limitations. London: Macmillan. Tapp, J.L. (1969). Psychology and the law: the dilemma. Psychology Today, 11,16-22. Tapp, J.L. and Levine, F.J. (1977). Law, Justice and the Individual in Society. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

REFERENCES
Banaji, M.R.. and Crowder, R.G. (1989). The bancruptcy of everyday memory. American Psychologist, 44, 1185-93. Bentley, D. (1979). The infant and the dream: psychology and the law. In

You might also like