You are on page 1of 8

Intern. J.

of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268 – 275


www.elsevier.com/locate/ijresmar

The effect of emotional provider support on angry versus anxious consumers


Kalyani Menon a,⁎, Laurette Dubé b
a
School of Business and Economics,Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Avenue, Waterloo, ON, Canada N2L 3C5
b
Desautels Faculty of Management McGill University, 1001001 Sherbrooke Street West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1G5

Abstract

This paper examines the trend in service firms to integrate an emotional dimension into provider responses to consumer emotions. An
empirical study showed that such a strategy is effective only if the consumer coping demonstrates an emotional engagement with the service
episode. Because both angry and anxious consumers cope primarily by planful problem solving (i.e., attempting to resolve the situation and attain
their consumption goal), provider responses that offered solutions (instrumental support) were more effective than responses that offered
emotional support. Further, angry consumers were emotionally engaged with the situation while anxious consumers emotionally disengaged from
it. Consequently, provider response that included emotional support and instrumental support was the most effective for angry consumers, but
there was no benefit to also including emotional support for anxious consumers.
© 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Consumer emotions; Coping; Provider response strategies; Services

1. Introduction tions with consumers may detract from consumer evaluation of


the service (McBane, 1995; Menon & Dubé, 2004, 2000;
The “People Notice” and “Be Hospitable” campaigns by Suprenant & Solomon, 1987; Sutton & Rafaeli, 1990).
Holiday Inn and Hilton Hotels respectively, the employee training Secondly, integrating an emotional dimension into service
programs of United, Jet Blue, and SouthWest airlines, and provider responses entails a cost to the provider and the service
Commerce Bank's business model, all require service providers to firm. Providers have to frequently engage in surface and deep
demonstrate an emotional engagement (e.g., caring, compassion, acting which extracts considerable psychic toll (Grandey,
humor) with consumers while delivering the core, instrumental Dickter, & Sin, 2004; Hochschild, 1983), and firms have to
aspects of the service. While a large number of service firms have invest in training and support programs to enable effective
subscribed to this approach, little research has examined the emotional engagement by service providers. It is prudent to
effectiveness of such a service strategy, both in terms of the examine the effectiveness of integrating an emotional dimen-
magnitude of its impact and its underlying mechanisms. This sion into provider responses prior to incurring such costs.
paper examines whether the practice of integrating emotional This paper focuses on the effectiveness of integrating an
engagement into instrumental service delivery truly optimizes emotional dimension into provider response to consumers
consumer satisfaction with the service. experiencing two key consumption emotions: anger and anxiety.
Two factors motivated this research. Firstly, while emotional If consumers do indeed value emotional engagement, then they
engagement by providers may be effective in affectively are likely to value it particularly when they themselves are
charged, long-term, intimate service encounters (e.g., Arnould experiencing specific emotions. Further, negative emotions such
& Price,1993), its effectiveness is unclear in the transaction- as anger are highly contagious and responding to such emotions
oriented as opposed to relationship-oriented consumer-service presents a special challenge to providers (Grandey et al., 2004;
provider interactions that characterize most commercial ser- Menon & Dubé, 2000, 2004).
vices. In fact, there is evidence that non-task-oriented interac- We reason that the effectiveness of emotional engagement
with consumers will depend on the consumer's specific response
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 519 884 0710x2704; fax: +1 519 884 0201. (coping strategy) to the event that triggers anger or anxiety. We
E-mail address: kmenon@wlu.ca (K. Menon). derive hypotheses for differentiated coping responses to
0167-8116/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ijresmar.2007.04.001
K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275 269

consumer anxiety and anger and propose that incorporating an 1.1.1. Consumer coping with anger
emotional dimension into provider responses will be effective Anger episodes tend to be initiated by appraisals of an event
only if consumer coping involves a specific need for emotional as being significant to but incongruent with the consumption
support. goals. There are appraisals of a well-identified cause for the
negative event which frequently involves blaming others. In the
1.1. Conceptual framework and research hypotheses context of services, since it is the service provider's responsi-
bility to ensure a smooth service transaction, blame for any
An emotion experience can be thought of as a script negative event will thus be directed towards the service
composed of interconnected, organized patterns of cognitive provider, and the service provider, as the causal agent, will be
appraisals, affective responses, physiological changes, and expected to alleviate the problem (Keltner, Ellsworth, &
action tendencies in response to personally relevant events Edwards, 1993; Lerner & Keltner, 2000; Smith & Ellsworth,
(Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986; 1985).
Keltner & Gross, 1999; Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, & O'Connor, Consumer researchers have found evidence linking anger to a
1987; Yi & Baumgartner, 2004). These components of an form of planful problem solving, i.e., coping that focuses on goal
emotion script convey to an individual how best to adapt to the achievement (Duhachek, 2005), as well as to confrontive coping
particular event, and this adaptation is seen in the coping that (i.e., showing antagonistic behavior towards the perceived cause of
occurs during emotion experiences. Coping responses aim either the event) (Menon & Dubé, 2000; Yi & Baumgartner, 2004).
to eliminate or modify the conditions that produce stress Coping responses are highly context specific (Folkman et al.,
(problem-focused coping) or to keep emotional consequences 1986), and individuals typically utilize a variety of coping tech-
within manageable bounds (emotion-focused coping) (Folkman niques with distinct functions to respond to the adverse event.
et al., 1986; Moos & Schaefer, 1993; Zeidner & Hammer, 1990). Therefore, we suggest that the service consumer's coping with
Coping reflects a consumer's preferred form of interacting anger will be a combination of coping strategies dominated by
with the service situation and signals consumer priorities and planful problem solving and followed by confrontive coping.
requirements in the context of that particular emotion (Thoits, Service transactions are purposeful exchanges where the consumer
1995). Based on this, we suggest that provider responses that has invested time and money to achieve specific goals (Bagozzi &
complement consumer coping will be the most effective, and Dholakia, 1999). Therefore the dominant coping response for a
that integrating emotional support into provider response will be consumer experiencing a high power/energy emotion such as anger
effective only if consumer coping reflects a need for such an will be planful problem solving, i.e., to see if a solution can be
emotional engagement. Fig. 1 describes our conceptual model. found, or alternative routes explored, to achieve that consumption

Fig. 1. Conceptual model.


270 K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275

goal. Angry consumers may also engage in confrontive coping. context, this instrumental support will relate to adapting the core
Cognitive appraisals of blame for the negative event attached to a service offering because this is crucial to the service
service provider along with the physiological and behavioral consumption. Emotional support extends empathy and under-
changes that ready an angry consumer for attack (e.g., Shaver et al., standing while showing affiliation and reassurance and aiding in
1987) will lead to an intense form of negative emotional an individual's emotion management. It acknowledges the
engagement i.e., confrontive coping towards the provider. emotional impact of a situation on an individual and focuses on
helping the individual manage the emotions rather than on
H1a. For angry consumers, the primary coping tendencies will
resolving the situation directly (Thoits, 1995). Such support
be planful problem solving which will be greater than the
relates to the more interpersonal and communal aspects of the
secondary coping tendency of confrontive coping.
service delivery (Clark & Mills, 1993).
Since planful problem solving is the primary coping tendency
1.1.2. Consumer coping with anxiety for both anxiety and anger, a response strategy that focuses on
Anxiety episodes contain attributions of uncontrollable instrumental support and focuses on the core service issue will
circumstances (e.g., poor weather) for the negative event over be more effective for both these emotions rather than one that
which a consumer perceives low certainty, low perceptions of focuses only on emotional support. The hypothesized secondary
power and higher perceptions of helplessness (Ruth, Brunel, & coping tendency for anger indicates aggressive, confrontive, and
Otnes, 2002; Taylor, 1994). continuous emotional engagement with the situation (Buss &
Anxiety tends to be characterized by approach-avoidance Perry, 1992; Harmon-Jones & Sigelman, 2001). It will be
type of coping with anxious consumers likely to approach necessary for the service provider to acknowledge this
certain controllable aspects of the negative event and avoid confrontation and the ego-damage to the angry consumers, and
other more uncontrollable aspects. In the approach form of provide emotional support. Providing emotional support and
coping, anxious consumers may focus their resources on trying attempting to dilute the confrontive nature of angry consumers
to alleviate or gain greater control over the more controllable may also make it easier for providers to deliver instrumental
aspects of it. They may do this on their own account or by support. Hence, provider response combining emotional with
relying on the service provider to help them gain greater control instrumental support in a hybrid response strategy (instead of an
(Beck & Clark, 1997; Laux & Weber, 1991; Rachman, 1998). instrumental support only provider response) should lead to
Thus, anxious consumers too will show an instrumental higher service evaluation for angry consumers (because of their
engagement with the negative event through planful problem specific combination of coping strategies).
solving forms of coping. But in addition, anxious consumers are In contrast, the secondary coping tendency for anxiety is
likely to show emotional disengagement or distancing as a distancing and reflects emotional disengagement. Consequent-
secondary coping tendency that would include trying to control ly, any addition of emotional support is unlikely to have a
emotions and distracting oneself to emotionally distance from significant incremental impact on the perceived value of
the event (e.g., Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992; Yi & supportive provider responses. In fact, providing emotional
Baumgartner, 2004). Due to the absence of a well-defined cause support to consumers who are trying to distance themselves
for the negative event, as well as the absence of physiological from the situation may have a negative impact because it
changes and action tendencies that engender aggression, contradicts their own instinctive coping and forces them to
anxious consumers have no particular target or motivation for deal with an aspect of the issue, i.e., the emotional aspect,
the kind of venting and confrontive coping we see in anger. when in fact they would rather distance themselves from it.
Therefore, Therefore,
H1b. For anxiety, the primary coping tendencies will be planful H2A. Instrumental support strategy will lead to higher service
problem solving which will be greater than the secondary evaluation than emotional support in both anxiety and anger
coping tendency of distancing. episodes.
H2B. A hybrid support strategy that adds emotional support to
1.1.3. Complementary provider response to consumer coping instrumental support will lead to higher service evaluation for
The most efficient and effective use of provider resources angry consumers. A hybrid support strategy will not increase
will require providers to adapt their responses in keeping with service evaluation for anxious consumers.
the needs conveyed by consumer coping. The social support
literature argues that coping serves to marshal not just an 2. Method
individual's own resources but also resources from the social
environment to respond to the negative event in a manner that We used an experimental setting manipulating cognitive
the individual is most comfortable with (Holahan, Moos, & appraisals to evoke episodes of consumer anger and anxiety and
Schaefer, 1996; Thoits, 1995). then manipulating provider responses to test our hypotheses.
The social support literature groups supportive strategies into The subjects were regular users of airline services and they were
two categories: instrumental support and emotional support. required to role-play scenarios of episodes of anger and anxiety,
Instrumental support actively attempts to alter the situation and indicate their coping tendencies, and then evaluate the provider
enable movement towards the particular goal. In a service response strategies they were exposed to. Waiting in line at the
K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275 271

check-in counter became the core event for the written line at the check-in counter at an airport. To enable subjects to
scenarios. After eliciting either anxiety or anger in the partic- vividly visualize themselves in that situation, details were
ipants through the use of pre-tested scenarios and measuring provided about the service setting, about other consumers in the
their distinctive pattern of coping tendencies, participants were setting, and about the provider(s) in the setting. Scenarios were
exposed to one of four provider response strategies: instrumen- designed to manipulate anxiety and anger experiences inde-
tal, emotional, hybrid (instrumental plus emotional support), or pendently. Negative emotion experiences arise when events are
no support. Service evaluation was then measured in terms of perceived to be relevant to, but incongruent with a goal. The
perceived provider performance. Such a methodology where specific negative emotion experience i.e., anger or anxiety, then
written scenarios describing negative service events and service depends on appraisals like the causal attribution for the event
recovery are used to measure consumer evaluations of the (Keltner et al., 1993; Lerner & Keltner, 2000). Accordingly, the
response strategy has been utilized by Smith, Bolton, and emotion experience manipulation was achieved by making the
Wagner (1999) and Smith and Bolton (2002). As Smith et al. core event – waiting in a long line at the check-in counter at an
suggest, this method reduces biases such as memory lapses and airport – relevant and incongruent with the participant's goal of
rationalization tendencies that may be common when using getting on the flight, and by varying the attribution for the event
retrospective self-reports. In addition, the use of written across the two emotion conditions. In the anxiety condition, the
scenarios or recall instructions that manipulate cognitive ap- participant was told that he/she attributed the long line to it
praisals to elicit emotions and then study the effect of these being the holiday season (uncontrollable circumstances). The
emotions on a range of perceptual and behavioral responses has description ensured that no one, including the provider, was
been used extensively by emotion researchers (e.g., Lerner & directly responsible for the long line. In the anger condition, the
Keltner, 2000; Smith, Haynes, Lazarus, & Pope, 1993) as well participant was told that the long line at the check-in counter
as consumer researchers (e.g., Yi & Baumgartner, 2004). seemed to be because the airline had not opened enough check-
in counters and that the staff operating the few open counters
2.1. Experimental stimuli appeared to be inefficient and unconcerned about the long line
of passengers waiting to check-in (attribution of provider
The scenarios for the experimental study were developed blame).
according to a full factorial design combining, on a between-
participants basis, 2 emotion experiences (anxiety, anger) × 2 2.1.2. Provider response manipulation
emotional support provider response (present, absent) × 2 Emotional support was manipulated by describing a
instrumental support provider response (present, absent). The situation where airline personnel approached the participant
scenarios were divided into three sections with the first waiting in line at the check-in counter and reassured the
containing the emotion experience manipulation; the second, participant. The personnel is described as showing understand-
the provider response manipulation; and the third and final ing for the situation and trying to comfort the subject by saying
section, describing the conclusion to the air travel experience. that everything would turn out all right. Instrumental support
This last section described the passenger as making it to the manipulation was achieved by having the airline personnel
flight on time and arriving at the destination after an uneventful approach the subject and offer to try and change the negative
flight. The questionnaire began with a brief description of the situation. Specifically, the provider was described as offering to
study and then asked participants to provide demographic go to the supervisor and ask for more counters to be opened.
information. This was followed by instructions on how to Both the emotional support and the instrumental support
proceed through the questionnaire. Participants were informed manipulations appeared in the hybrid support condition. The
that the questionnaire described an event that may occur during no-support condition (emotional support absent, instrumental
air travel, and that this event was described in three sections. support absent) was achieved by replacing the description of the
They were to read each section carefully, imagine themselves in provider reaction with a neutral description of the passenger
the role of the passenger described in that section, answer the continuing to wait in line and the airline personnel (who in the
questions that followed each section, and only then move on to support providing conditions approached the subject with some
the next section. form of support) described as continuing to do some work
unrelated to the passenger.
2.1.1. Emotion manipulation
Emotion elicitation typically involves the use of written 2.2. Pre-test
scenarios that provide rich details of a specific event designed to
enable individuals to vividly visualize themselves in that A pre-test was conducted to test the effectiveness of
setting. Embedded in the details of the event are manipulations manipulations as well as to control for potential confounds. A
of appraisal dimensions of a situation (e.g., Bagozzi, Gopinath, pre-test (as opposed to a manipulation check in the main
& Nyer, 1999; Keltner et al., 1993; Lerner & Keltner, 2000; experiment) has been recommended to reduce demand
Smith & Bolton, 2002; Smith et al., 1993). characteristics (Keltner et al., 1993; Lerner & Keltner, 2000).
The written scenarios used for this research were closely One hundred and twenty eight MBA students (male = 88;
modeled on the scenarios used in the psychological approaches. female = 40) enrolled at a Canadian university participated in an
Written scenarios described an air travel event; waiting in a long in-class administration of the pre-test questionnaire. Subjects
272 K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275

Table 1 2.3. Main study


Pre-test results: Mean (S.D.) values for emotion manipulation
Variable Emotion condition 2.3.1. Participants
Anger (n = 38) Anxiety (n = 54) No emotion (n = 36) To enhance generalizability of the study, we used a sample of
Self-reported anger 4.141a (.81) 3.401b (.82) 3.011b(.84) actual consumers of air travel drawn from different parts of
Self-reported anxiety 3.542a (.98) 4.022b (.76) 3.561a (.95) USA and Canada working in a variety of economic sectors. The
Relevance 5.76a (1.51) 5.52a (1.36) 4.83b (1.53) most cost-efficient way of collecting such data was to use a
Incongruence 4.68a (2.02) 4.22a (1.76) 3.11b (1.38) snowball sampling technique whereby individuals from a range
Provider blame 5.571a (1.05) 3.971b (1.36) 4.33b (1.25)
of corporate and governmental organizations across North
Uncontrollable 2.612a (1.3) 4.072b (1.23) 3.84b (1.05)
circumstances America were contacted and the study explained to them. If they
agreed to participate, they were sent a random mix of survey
Note: Means with different numeric superscripts in a column are significantly
different at p b .01. Means with different alphabetic superscripts in a row are questionnaires reflecting the various experimental conditions.
significantly different at p b .01. They were asked to respond to the study and to recruit others
from their organizations for data collection purposes. Respon-
dents were told that the study concerned service experiences
were randomly assigned to the different conditions. The modal during air travel. Beyond this all were blind to the conditions
class for age was 26–35 years (64.8%) and for frequency of air and to the more specific objectives of the research. The sample
travel, “at least once every six months” (47.7%). While the main consisted of 175 employed adults (54.7% male, 45.3% female).
experimental study was designed as a 2 emotion experiences The modal age was 20 to 30 years (43.7%), and frequency of air
(anxiety, anger) × 2 emotional support provider response travel “at least once every 6 months” (51.7%). Subjects were not
(present, absent) × 2 instrumental support provider response asked to indicate the purpose of these trips (i.e., the trips could
(present, absent), the pre-test was designed as a 3 emotion have been for business or personal reasons).
experiences (anger, anxiety, no emotion) × 2 emotional support
provider response (present, absent) × 2 instrumental support 2.3.2. Procedure
provider response (present, absent). A control no-emotion Participants were randomly assigned to one cell of a 2-emotion
condition was included in the pre-test to ensure that our stimuli (anger, anxiety)× 2-emotional support (present, absent) × 2-in-
are indeed eliciting emotions. The manipulations of anxiety and strumental support (present, absent) experimental design. Section
anger were tested in terms of the intensity of the subjective 1 of the questionnaire contained the air travel scenario with the
experience as well as the extent to which consumers used emotion eliciting manipulation followed by measures of coping
specific appraisals in response to the emotion manipulation. responses. The second section of the questionnaire contained the
Following Richins (1997), self-reported anxiety was measured provider response manipulation: emotional support, instrumental
by anxious, worried, nervous, tense, (α = 0.86); 4 items were support, mixed support, or no support. Participants were then
used to measure anger: anger, irritation, hostility, outrage provided with the same description of the remaining part of the
(α = 0.80). Single item 7-point scales (not at all–very much so) trip as being relatively eventless and all getting to the destination
were used to measure appraisals of perceived relevance and under the same condition. The final segment of the questionnaire
perceived incongruence, a 2-item 7-point scale measured consisted of measures of service evaluation on the trip.
uncontrollability of event and a 3-item 7-point scale measured
provider blame for the event. ANOVA was conducted and 2.3.3. Measures
means are presented in Table 1. We adapted the well established Ways of Coping scale
The provider response manipulation was followed by 7-point (Folkman et al., 1986) to measure the three theoretically-relevant
scales measuring perceived emotional support provider showed
empathy, reassured you, comforted you, (α = 0.85) – and
instrumental support – provider tried to change the situation, Table 2
and gave concrete assistance (α = 0.85) (Dunkel-Schetter, Pre-test results mean (S.D.) values for provider response manipulation
Folkman, & Lazarus, 1987). The last section describing the Dependent Provider response condition
conclusion of the air travel experience was followed by an variables
Emotional Instrumental Hybrid No support
assessment of the ecological validity of the scenario in 2 items, support support support (n = 44)
(very unlikely/very likely; unrealistic/realistic), which was (n = 35) (n = 26) (n = 23)
found to be satisfactory (M = 5.19, on 7-point scale) and equal Perceived 4.51a (1.21) 3.641b (1.29) 4.661a (1.22) 2.711c (1.14)
across experimental conditions. We first conducted an overall emotional
MANOVA to test for the emotion × provider response interac- support
Perceived 2.852b (1.37) 4.662a (1.42) 4.431a (1.49) 2.881b (1.14)
tion on respondents' perceptions of emotional support and
instrumental
instrumental support. Results showed there was no interaction support
effect (F b 1). ANOVA tests were then conducted and the
Note: Means with different numeric superscripts in a column are significantly
relevant means are presented in Table 2. different at p b .01. Means with different alphabetic superscripts in a row are
The pre-test results indicate that the manipulations were significantly different at p b .01.
effective. Scales increasing from 1–7.
K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275 273

forms of coping: planful problem solving, confrontive coping, p b .001). For both anger and anxiety, the presence of support-
and distancing. Planful problem solving was measured by a providing reaction from the provider (whether it be instrumental,
seven-item scale: asking provider for advice, finding out what emotional, or hybrid support) led to higher perceived performance,
can be done, trying to follow a plan of action, trying to change (anxiety, M = 5.15, anger, M = 4.85) than the absence of a
part of the scenario, coming up with different solutions, drawing supportive reaction (anxiety: M = 2.36, F(1,167) = 105.34,
on past experiences to decide what to do, and trying to get the p b .01; anger: M = 1.60, F(1,167) = 103.55, p b .01). Pairwise
provider to do something about the situation (α = .87). comparisons similarly revealed that each type of support led to
Distancing was measured by a 4-item scale: trying not to let higher evaluation than the absence of support.
this event get to you, wishing the situation would end, thinking H2a predicted that the provision of instrumental support
you have bad luck, and controlling your feelings, (α = .73). alone would lead to higher service evaluation for both angry and
Confrontive coping was measured by a three-item scale: anxious consumers than emotional support alone. Consistent
expressing anger to the provider, showing antagonism towards with expectations, across both emotions, an instrumental
the provider, standing your ground and fighting for what you support strategy induced higher service evaluation (anger,
want (α = .73). All items were presented in random order. M = 4.92; anxiety, M = 5.56) than an emotional support strategy
Participants were asked to indicate on 7-point scales (not at all– alone (anger M = 4.03, F(1, 167) = 6.26, p b .05, anxiety,
very much so) the extent to which they would use each of the M = 4.88, F(1, 167) = 3.79, p b .05). The predictions of H2b
listed coping strategies. Average scores were created for each were also supported. In episodes of anger, hybrid support
dimension. Service evaluation was measured in terms of the (M = 5.60) led to higher perceived performance than either an
participants' perception of the provider performance, using a 7- emotional support, (M = 4.03, F(1, 167) = 18.96, p b .01) or an
point (not at all–very much so) 3-item scale: response met needs instrumental support strategy (M = 4.92, F(1, 167) = 3.62,
and wants, showed concern, and was in keeping with specific p b .05). In anxiety episodes, however, there was no significant
requirements, (α = .94). Subjects were asked to respond by benefit to adding emotional support to instrumental support to
taking into consideration the entire air travel experience scenario create a hybrid support strategy (M = 5.15, F b 1). A hybrid
from arriving at the airport to landing at the destination. An support strategy in fact tended to reduce perceived performance
average score was created as a measure of service evaluation. below the levels achieved by an instrumental support strategy
alone, though this difference was not statistically significant.
3. Results We had proposed that these results would occur because of
different coping responses to anger (where confrontive coping
3.1. Coping responses occurs more than distancing) and anxiety (where distancing
occurs more than confrontive coping). To further explore the
Comparisons between coping responses to consumer anxiety central argument that the effectiveness of provider response is
and consumer anger were conducted by within-participants contingent on the degree of match with consumer coping, a
contrasts. Confirming H1a, for anger episodes, planful problem mean split was conducted, aggregating across both emotions, to
solving (M = 5.6) was greater than confrontive coping (M = 4.17, form groups with different levels of distancing and confrontive
t(80) = 9.39, p b .01) and both these were greater than distancing coping. That is, groups that were high in distancing and
(M = 3.45, t(80) = 11.7, p b .01 and t(80) = 3.74, p b .01, respec- confrontive coping were isolated and an ANOVA with provider
tively). Confirming H1b, anxiety episodes were dominated by response as a between-subjects function was conducted.
planful problem solving (M = 5.55) which was greater than Analyses revealed that while subjects with high levels of
distancing (M = 3.90, t(93) = 10.74, p b .01), and both these were distancing had higher service evaluation when they received some
greater than confrontive coping (3.51, t(93) = 13.23, p b .01 and form of support (F(1, 167) = 93.04, p b .01), the various support-
t(93) = 2.13 p b .05, respectively). Correlation analysis revealed providing strategies did not significantly impact evaluation (all
that distancing and confrontive coping were inversely, but not ps N .15). On the other hand, high confrontive coping subjects
significantly, related to each other. Thus, both angry and were very sensitive to the type of provider response. A hybrid
anxious consumers used planful problem solving as the dom-
inant form of coping. However, angry consumers used con-
frontive coping and anxious consumers used distancing as their Table 3
secondary coping strategies. Results for main study: mean (S.D.) values for service evaluation
Anger Anxiety
3.2. Effect of provider response strategies No support 4
1.60 (.63) 2.363 (.77)
n = 17 n = 24
Table 3 presents the mean service evaluations. We conducted a Instrumental support 4.922 (.82) 5.562 (1.27)
n = 22 n = 22
2 emotion (anxiety/anger) × 2 emotional support (present, ab-
Emotional support 4.033 (1.76) 4.881 (1.27)
sent) × 2 instrumental support (present, absent) ANOVA. The main n = 21 n = 24
effect of emotion was not significant ( p b .10). However, the main Hybrid support 5.601 (.76) 5.151, 2 (1.09)
effects of emotional support ( p b .001) and instrumental support n = 21 n = 24
(p b .001) were both significant, as was the emotion× emotional Note: Means in a column with different superscripts are significantly different at
support × instrumental support interaction (F[4, 167] = 13.31, p b .05.
274 K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275

strategy received higher evaluation (M = 5.57) than an instrumen- providers often need to respond to angry and anxious con-
tal support strategy (M = 4.90, F(1, 167)= 4.19, p b .05), which in sumers in ways that are contrary to their natural tendency to
turn was evaluated more positively than emotional support respond. When faced with an angry consumer using confrontive
strategy alone (M = 4.18, F(1,167) = 4.75, p b .05). coping, the instinctive tendency of providers may be to try and
ignore this confrontation and focus solely on the core
4. Discussion instrumental support. However, when faced with anxious
consumers who are not confrontational, service providers may
This study highlights the need for provider responses to find it easier to be emotionally supportive. Our study shows that
complement consumer coping with negative emotions such as in fact it is the exact opposite response that consumers desire
anger and anxiety. Results show that because both angry and from providers. Designing such counter-intuitive provider
anxious consumers use planful problem solving strategies as the responses may be particularly challenging since research in
dominant form of coping, instrumental support is more critical psychology has consistently shown that anger is one of the more
than emotional support for both emotions. In addition to this contagious emotions (Averill, 1982). Provider recruitment
dominant form of coping, angry and anxious consumers also (Randall & Randall, 1990) and training should consider their
use secondary coping strategies of confrontive coping and ability to recognize their own natural anger response and replace
distancing respectively. Hybrid strategies paid off in episodes of it with a more effective response (Menon & Dubé, 2004). There
anger given its confrontive coping showing emotional engage- may be significant long-term rewards to managing consumer's
ment but not in anxiety episodes with its emotional disengage- episode-level anger, because research shows that anger also has
ment and distancing. an optimistic bias causing angry individuals to have a more
Anxious consumers choose to approach only one aspect of positive appraisal of future stressful events, thus making them
the event (the instrumental aspect) and avoid the emotional fall- more amenable to long-term relationships (Hemenover &
out while angry consumers approach both aspects of the event. Zhang, 2004).
While anxiety is often conceptualized as a flight emotion, we It is important to note that while emotional disengagement in
show that in a goal-oriented service situation with its implicit response to consumer coping strategies like distancing may
promise of service satisfaction, anxious consumers will try and achieve immediate consumer satisfaction, this may be at the
approach and resolve the anxiety-causing problem. The paper expense of a fruitful, long-term relationship between the service
also shows that consumers are actively participating in the and the consumer. Repeated consumer use of distancing coping,
service experience, using their coping to elicit specific and the matching of this by provider responses, may lead to a
responses from the service provider, and basing their service situation where consumers are increasingly distanced from all
evaluations on the extent to which provider response comple- aspects of the service experience. Provider support strategies
ments their coping. may have to cater to the anxious consumer's immediate
In keeping with the growing evidence indicating service tendency to use emotional distancing as well as help anxious
providers should tailor their responses to the specifics of the consumers develop alternative coping abilities such as seeking
consumer experience of failure (e.g., Patterson, Cowley, & emotional support even if the event appears uncontrollable.
Prasongsukarn, 2006; Smith & Bolton, 2002; Smith et al., Identifying critical points in service delivery that may be
1999), we show that service firms should not indiscriminately particularly likely to evoke anxiety, encouraging consumers to
incorporate both instrumental and emotional aspects into their seek emotional support at these stages, or providing illustrations
responses. Delivering “more” service requires greater use of of alternative coping strategies may go a long way to developing
resources and the pay-off is questionable. We found that when a sense of self-efficacy in anxious consumers. Similarly, while
more service than was desired by consumers were delivered – the confrontive coping of angry consumers increases their
as when anxious consumers received more support (both engagement with the service, it can also be disruptive to the
instrumental and emotional) than they needed (since their service setting and the other consumers. Service firms may need
primary coping strategy was planful problem solving) – they to consider campaigns to educate consumers about appropriate
actually had lower evaluation of provider performance than and effective forms of coping.
when they received instrumental support alone. This excess While the experimental setting with actual consumers of
utilization of provider resources thus backfired from both the airline services allowed for a controlled test of our hypotheses,
consumers' and providers' perspective. This study also high- and we elicited anger and anxiety using standard manipulations –
lights the primacy of instrumental support, and the need for attributing causality for the negative event to the provider or the
firms to consider such support as the essential core of any circumstances respectively – there is the possibility that the
response strategy. manipulations were confounded with locus of control over the
Providers need to be trained to identify consumer coping as it perceived negative event. The role-playing methodology also
occurs in a service context and to tailor their responses involves a self-conscious, cognitive effort to portray how one
accordingly. Past research (e.g., Menon & Dubé, 2000; Shaver would have behaved in an imagined situation while real-life
et al., 1987) has developed a list of expressive and behavioral experience of aversive events often involves spontaneous, non-
cues accompanying distinct emotion experiences that service rational responses (Mikolic, Parker, & Pruitt, 1997) and ideally
providers should familiarize with to help identify consumer these results should be replicated in a real-life context. Also, this
emotion and coping experiences. Further, this study shows that research was conducted in the specific context of air travel and
K. Menon, L. Dubé / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 24 (2007) 268–275 275

this restricts its generalizability. Finally, while the study did McBane, D. (1995). Empathy and the salesperson: A multidimensional
control for certain individual level variables such as prior perspective. Psychology and Marketing, 12(4), 349−371.
Menon, K., & Dubé, L. (2000). Engineering effective interpersonal responses to
experience with flying, age, and gender, and found no effect for consumer emotions for higher satisfaction. Journal of Retailing, 76(3), 285−307.
these variables, it should be noted that other individual char- Menon, K., & Dubé, L. (2004). Service provider responses to anxious and angry
acteristics such as coping tendency as a personality trait, trait consumers: Different challenges, different payoffs. Journal of Retailing, 80,
anger/anxiety, and emotional expressiveness were not included 229−237.
Mikolic, J. M., Parker, J. C., & Pruitt, D. C. (1997). Escalation in response to
but might also impact the results.
persistent annoyance: Groups versus individuals and gender effects. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 151−163.
References Moos, R. H., & Schaefer, J. A. (1993). Coping resources and processes: Current
concepts and measures. In L. Goldberger, & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of
Arnould, E., & Price, L. (1993). River magic: Extraordinary experience and the stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (pp. 234−257)., 2nd ed. New York:
extended service encounter. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 24−45. Free Press.
Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Patterson, P., Cowley, E., & Prasongsukarn, K. (2006). Service failure recovery:
Springer-Verlag Inc. The moderating impact of individual-level cultural value orientation on
Bagozzi, R., & Dholakia, U. (1999). Goal setting and goal striving in consumer perceptions of justice. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 23(3),
behavior. Journal of Marketing, 63, 19−32. 263−277.
Bagozzi, R., Gopinath, M., & Nyer, P. (1999). The role of emotions in Rachman, S. (1998) Anxiety. Clinical psychology: A modular course hove,
marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 184−206. England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis.
Beck, A., & Clark, D. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Randall, J., & Randall, C. (1990). Review of salesperson selection techniques
Automatic and strategic processes. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35(1), and criteria: A managerial approach. International Journal of Research in
49−58. Marketing, 7(2,3), 81−95.
Buss, A., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Richins, M. (1997). Measuring emotions in the consumption experience.
Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452−459. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 127−146.
Clark, M., & Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange Ruth, J. A., Brunel, F., & Otnes, C. (2002). Linking thoughts to feelings:
relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Investigating cognitive appraisals and consumption emotions in a mixed-
Bulletin, 19(6), 684−691. emotion context. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30, 44−55.
Duhachek, A. (2005). Coping: A multi-dimensional, hierarchical framework of Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O'Connor, C. (1987). Emotion
responses to stressful consumption episodes. Journal of Consumer Research, knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of
32(1), 41−54. Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1061−1086.
Dunkel-Schetter, C., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1987). Correlates of social Simpson, J., Rholes, W., & Nelligan, J. (1992). Support seeking and support giving
support receipt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 71−80. within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attachment styles.
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & Gruen, R. J. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434−446.
(1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: Cognitive attribution, coping, Smith, A. K., & Bolton, R. N. (2002). The effect of customers' emotional responses
and encounter outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, to service failure on their recovery effort evaluations and satisfaction
992−1003. judgments. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30, 5−23.
Grandey, A., Dickter, D., & Sin, H. -P. (2004). The customer is not always right: Smith, A. K., Bolton, R. N., & Wagner, J. (1999). A model of consumer
Customer aggression and emotion regulation of service employees. Journal satisfaction with service encounters involving failure and recovery.
of Organizational Behavior, 25(3), 397−419. Journal of Marketing Research, 26, 356−372.
Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain Smith, C., & Ellsworth, P. (1985). Patterns of cognitive attribution in emotions.
activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813−838.
associated with experienced anger and aggression. Journal of Personality Smith, C., Haynes, K., Lazarus, R., & Pope, L. (1993). In search of “hot”
and Social Psychology, 80(5), 797−803. cognitions: Attributions, appraisals, and their relation to emotion. Journal of
Hemenover, S. H., & Zhang, S. (2004). Anger, personality and stress appraisals. Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 916−929.
Cognition and Emotion, 18, 363−382. Suprenant, C., & Solomon, M. (1987). Predictability and personalization in the
Hochschild, A. (1983). The managed heart. Berkeley: University of California service encounter. Journal of Marketing, 51(2), 86−97.
Press. Sutton, R., & Rafaeli, A. (1990). Busy stores and demanding customers: How do
Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. H., & Schaefer, J. A. (1996) Coping, stress resistance, they affect the display of positive emotion? Academy of Management
and growth: Conceptualizing adaptive functioning. In M. Zeidner & N.S. Journal, 3, 6230637.
Endler (Eds.) Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications. NY: Taylor, S. (1994, April). Waiting for service: The relationship between delays
John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and Schaefer. and evaluation of services. Journal of Marketing, 58, 56−69.
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Thoits, P. A. (1995). Stress, coping and social support processes: Where are we?
Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality What next?Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 53−79 [Extra Issue].
and Social Psychology, 64, 740−752. Yi, S., & Baumgartner, H. (2004). Coping with negative emotions in purchase
Keltner, D., & Gross, J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and related situations. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(3), 303.
Emotion, 13(5), 467−480. Zeidner, M., & Hammer, A. L. (1990). Life events and coping resources as
Laux, L., & Weber, H. (1991). Presentation of self in coping with anger and predictors of stress symptoms in adolescents. Personality and Individual
anxiety: An intentional approach. Anxiety Research, 3(4), 233−255. Differences, 11, 693−703.
Lerner, J., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-
specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4),
473−493.

You might also like