You are on page 1of 8

In: Kaivo Thomson, Timo Jaakkola & Jarmo Liukkonen (Eds.

) Promotion of
Motor Skills in Sports and Physical Education University of Jyväskylä,
Finland Department of Sport Sciences,Research Unit of Motor
Skills.Publications 2/2006. Pp. 9-18.

WHAT IS MOTOR LEARNING?

Timo Järvilehto
Faculty of Education and Kajaani University Consortium, University of Oulu, Finland
timo.jarvilehto@oulu.fi

SUMMARY
It is argued, on the basis of organism-environment systems theory, that motor learning
doesn’t exist in the sense that it would be learning confined to the motor system only, and
that it could be contrasted to other types of learning. Motor learning means learning of
new acts, and each act consists of both motor and sensory constituents. Learning is a
process that involves always the whole organism-environment system, leading to a new
organization making new results of behavior possible. It is also stressed that all human
learning has essentially a social character, developing in relations to other people, which
forms the basis of motivation and self-esteem necessary for high-level performance.

INTRODUCTION

“Motor learning” is a term widely used in relation of learning of new skills, and gives the
impression that motor learning is a specific form of learning. This may be seen in usual
definitions of motor learning such as “Motor learning is the process of improving the
smoothness and accuracy of movements” or “A motor skill is a skill that regards the
ability of an organism to utilize skeletal muscles effectively”.

Such definitions imply that human behavior may be divided in sensory and motor parts
the latter one being the object of motor learning. However, we may ask what really is
trained in motor learning situations. In fact, the object in the training a motor skill is not
the ability to perform specific contractions of the muscles with associated movements of
the limbs, but rather the accomplishment of certain behaviors of acts. A trainee trying to
learn how to master the motor skill of jumping as far as possible is not really training the
muscles of the body for specific movements, but he is rather trying to develop the ability
to achieve a certain result, the maximal distance in jumping. This result may be achieved
under different circumstances by different movements, and the same movement may have
different significance depending on the situation.

Thus, motor learning is not really “motor”, but the process of learning new acts for the
achievement of specific results. An act, furthermore, always consists of a constellation of
many components, only part of them being “motor”. In order to accomplish any act many
other constituents are needed, belonging also to the sensory part of the nervous system.
From the point of view of the accomplishment of an act “motor” and “sensory” are only
anatomical divisions that cannot be functionally separated. It is also interesting to note
that both muscles and senses have, in fact, both afferent and efferent innervation, afferent
for muscles in the form of Ia afferents, for example, and efferent for senses in the form of
efferent fibers controlling the sensitivity of receptors (see e.g. Järvilehto, 1999).

TWO SYSTEMS OR ONE?

The concept of motor learning is based on a functional ”motor- sensory” dichotomy.


Such a dichotomy is understandable if the theorizing starts with a primary organism-
environment dichotomy, in which organism and environment are regarded as two
different systems in interaction (Fig.1).

System 1 System 2
Organism  Environment
 Subject Object
 Mental Physical
 Representation Stimuli
 Social Other people
Fig. 1. Organism and environment as two different systems.

In this view “motor” means traffic from the organism to environment, whereas “sensory”
means action from the environment to the organism. Such a division is based on our
common sense intuition: here am I and outside me is located the environment. However,
if we interested in learning of new acts we may ask if this kind of abstraction is useful
after all.

The theory of the organism-environment system (Järvilehto, 1998a,b; 1999; 2000) starts
with a postulate that most conceptual problems in psychology are due to the common
sense abstraction of organism and environment as two separate systems. This primary
dichotomy leads to many other dichotomies and controversies that can be solved only by
changing this two systems starting point. No organism can act without an environment,
and every act consists of elements belonging both to the organism and the environment.
Thus, an act as a system should be, just from the beginning, treated and analyzed as an
undivided whole. We don’t have two systems in interaction, but just one system that is
organized in relation to the result of behavior (Fig.2).
Fig. 2. The theory of the organism-environment system. Any act is accomplished by a
system consisting of both organismic and environmental elements.

The architecture of the organism-environment system is determined by the result of


behavior, not by such anatomical divisions as motor-sensory, for example. It is also this
system in which mental processes are realized, and in which learning takes place, not in
parts of the system. Thus, on this basis, the following definitions of the basic concepts
may be presented:
• ”Mental” is a process in the organism-environment system, in which ”inner” and
”outer” cannot be separated. Psychological concepts refer always to the
organization of the whole system involving both sensory and motor components.
Perception and action, for example, are two sides of the same coin.
• “Learning” is a process of differentiation and widening of the organism-
environment system making new results possible.
• “Sensory” is not related to information processing from the environment, but to
joining of the organism with environment
• “Motor” motion is not based on efferent signals, but on reorganization of the
whole organism-environment system

THERE IS NO SEPARATE “MOTOR” LEARNING

On the basis of the theory of the organism-environment system, there is no motor


learning that could be contrasted with “sensory” learning, for example. Learning is a
process that involves always the whole organism-environment system, leading to a new
organization making new results of behavior possible.

Thus, it is also futile to try to find motor learning in the brain. From the psychological
point of view, there is nothing “motor” in the motor cortex, as little as there is anything
“sensory” in the sensory cortices. The units in the brain do not carry out psychological
operations; the neurons are only parts of a larger system, in which psychological
operations are accomplished. During learning there are, of course, changes in the
structure of the nervous system and in the functioning of neural units, but these changes
are related to the achievement of new results, not to separate psychological “functions”.
In the learning process the neurons form new action systems making new results possible.
And in this process both motor and sensory components are always necessary.

On this basis we may have also a new look on learning of motor skills: This process is
not related to movements as such, but rather to formation of sensory-motor integration in
the form of action systems for specific results, being the target of the training process.
Such systems are formed when the trainee acts in the teaching program by carrying out
movements related to the result, but partially such systems may be formed also without
the movements. In the latter case we speak about “mental” training. This kind of training
is not at all more “mental” than the former one (i.e. when actual movements are
performed), but it is related to the formation of parts of the action systems necessary in
the final accomplishment of the object of training.

As all “motor” learning is directed towards the accomplishment of certain results, the
process is essentially prospective, i.e. it cannot be regarded as response to certain stimuli,
but rather as a self-organizing process for the expected ends. The subject is not waiting
for certain stimuli in order to act, but it is rather his action which renders certain
environment components useful in the achievement of the result. This also explains why
in many performances amazing speeds of movement are possible.

EXAMPLE: IS READING A SENSORY OR MOTOR PROCESS?

As an example of sensory-motor integration we may look at such a skilled process as


reading: Is ”reading” a motor or sensory process? One could call it motor, because of the
significance of eye movements and especially because the control of saccades is an
essential component. However, from the traditional point of view, reading is conceived
rather as a sensory process, as a process of transmission of information from the written
text. However, from the present point of view, neither description is accurate.

In our recent experiments (Holappa and Vierelä, 2006) we studied the reading process by
recording eye movement parameters, reading speed, and delay from fixation to uttered
word with three different types of text: 1) normal (“text written normally”), 2) scrambled
(“txet wrttien in scrabmeld from”, and 3) continuous (“textwrittentogether”). It was
assumed that the main differences in studied parameters should appear with the
scrambled text if reading is based on linear and hierarchical sensory processing of textual
information.
Number of fixations
Saccadeam plitude
asafunctionof text type
asafunctionoftexttype

NUMBER OF FIXATION S
SACCADE AMPLITU DE

125,00 
 

3.00

100,00


2.00 75,00

50,00

1.00

25,00

0.00
Normal Scrambled Continuous Normal Scrambled Continuous
Error BarsshowMean+/- 1,0SE

Fig. 3 shows some of the results. In contrast to the hypothesis, 1) and 2) did not
significantly differ in any reading parameters, whereas marked differences were found
with 3) in most parameters studied. It was also significant that the delay between fixation
to a word and its uttering was very short, on the average about 600 ms, indicating that
there is not even time for any complicated processing of textual information after the
fixation. Furthermore, this delay was maximal with the continuous text.

Such results support the interpretation of reading as process of anticipation of meaning, in


which fixation to a word is not a start, but rather the end of the prospective process of
generation of meaning. Thus, reading is not a sensory or motor process, but an integrated
process for the achievement of results

SOME GENERAL ASPECTS OF THE LEARNING PROCESS

If learning is defined as differentiation and widening of the organism-environment


system it is clear that learning cannot be sensory or motor, only, but the process of
learning involves always many systemic constituents. Learning to master of a skill
consists essentially of development of prospective organization of the organism-
environment system for skilled sequences of results, in which sensory and motor
components are integrated.
It is also important to stress that from the point of view of the organism-environment
theory human learning has essentially a social character and presupposes the existence of
consciousness (see Järvilehto, 2000). According to the organism-environment theory,
consciousness develops in cooperation with other people; thus, human learning is a
process exceeding the borders of the individual organism-environment systems. It is this
larger organization in which the human learning is realized, and therefore all efficient
learning presupposes the participation of both the teacher and the pupil (or the trainer and
the trainee).

Fig. 4. The development of consciousness. Consciousness develops in a system that


consists of several organism-environment systems directed towards achievement of
common results. Communication is the tool for creating the system.

From this it follows that the task of the trainer is not that of information transmission
(“teaching” in the classical sense), but rather creation of the cooperative organization in
which the learning resources of the trainee may be realized. An essential characteristic of
this kind of process is the determination of the developmental possibilities of the trainee,
and finding out his personal style in the process of achievement of the desired results in
cooperation with the trainer.

Coaching is not a process of transmission; it is rather a process of development of the


pre-existing skills of the trainee in a larger organization consisting, in the beginning, of
the trainer (and all other relevant people) and the trainee, but getting with continuing
training more differentiated and narrow, until the trainee is able to achieve the results also
without the immediate support of the associated people. Such a process was coined by
Vygotski already in the beginning of the last century with the term “zone of proximal
development”. However, according to the present view this development doesn’t mean a
shift from external to internal control of behavior, as implied by Vygotski, but rather as a
process of development of the personal aspect of performance.
In conclusion, the development of mastering of motor skills is not a process of “motor”
learning going on in the motor cortex of the trainee, for example, but rather a deeply
social process directed towards creation of an integrative organization consisting of many
participants. The brain is, of course, also an important aspect of the necessary conditions
of such an organization, but the learning process is not confined to the brain only, but it
presupposes also many other necessary components, such as the body, environmental
possibilities, social relations, etc. It is also this larger organization, in which the
motivational factors and the self-esteem develop --- mastering of the skill as a
consequence of result-oriented training, and motivation and self-esteem on the basis of
relations to other people --- making eventually the high-level performance possible.

POSSIBLE RESEARCH PROBLEMS

On the basis of the present theoretical account, the following central research problems
may be formulated:

 What are the critical components in formation of prospective organization for


results?
 Differences in prospective organization with different levels of mastering the
skill.
 Developmental differences: skill development.
 Role of emotions (motivation) in creating the prospective organization.
 Prospective and relational organization in teams of players.

REFERENCES

Jarvilehto T (1998a) The theory of the organism-environment


system: I. Description of the theory. Integrative
Physiological and Behavioral Science, 33, 321-334.
URL:
http://wwwedu.oulu.fi/homepage/tjarvile/orgenv1.pdf
Järvilehto T (1998b). The theory of the organism-environment
system: II. Significance of nervous activity in the
organism-environment system. Integrative
Physiological and Behavioral Science, 33, 335-343.
URL:
http://wwwedu.oulu.fi/homepage/tjarvile/orgenv2.pdf
Jarvilehto, T. (1999). The theory of the organism-environment
system: III. Role of efferent influences on receptors in
the formation of knowledge. Integrative Physiological
and Behavioral Science, 34, 90-100.
URL:
http://wwwedu.oulu.fi/homepage/tjarvile/orgenv3.pdf
Jarvilehto, T. (2000). The theory of the organism-environment
system: IV. The problem of mental activity and
consciousness. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral
Science, 35, 35-57.
URL:
http://wwwedu.oulu.fi/homepage/tjarvile/orgenv4.pdf
Holappa, E. and Vierelä, H. (2006) Miten lukeminen tapahtuu. Vertaileva
tutkimus eri tekstityyppien yhteydestä
lukijan silmänliikkeisiin ja lukemisen kestoon. University of Oulu: Master’s thesis.

You might also like