You are on page 1of 16

American Journal of Primatology 379-24 (1995)

lnterbirth Interval Variation in Three Sympatric Species of


Neotropical Monkey
LINDA MARIE FEDIGAN' AND LISA M. ROSE'
'Department of Anthropology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada;
'Department of Anthropology, Washington Uniuersity, St. Louis, Missouri

Relatively few papers have focused on interbirth intervals in primates,


even though the spacing between births is one of the primary determi-
nants of female reproductive success in long-lived mammals. We present
life history data from a ten-year field study of Costa Rican capuchins
(Cebus capucinus),howlers (Alouattapalliata), and spider monkeys (Ateles
geoffroyi). Analyses of intraspecific variability found no significant differ-
ences attributable to individual variation in age, parity, weight, or ma-
ternal rank. Loss of an infant significantly shortened the interbirth inter-
val in all three species. There was no correlation between annual rainfall
and birth rates, but there was a significant clustering of births in the dry
season. Survival analyses demonstrated a significant difference between
the median interbirth intervals of the three species. Howlers have the
shortest intervals (19.9 months), capuchins exhibit longer intervals (26.36
months), and spider monkeys have the longest intervals (34.72 months).
This comparative pattern does not correspond to relative body weights of
the three species, but does correspond to relative brain weights. Compar-
isons to other primates with similar life history characteristics demon-
strate that interbirth intervals are best examined at the level of their
three component phases: gestation, lactation, and cycling to re-conception.
0 1995 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

Key words: interbirth intervals, monkeys, life history, reproduction,


gestation
INTRODUCTION
The reproductive success of a female can be measured as the product of her
reproductive life span, the pace of parturition, and the rate of offspring survivor-
ship [Clutton-Brock, 19881. In monkeys, most of whom give birth to one infant at
a time, age at first birth and the spacing between successive births are the primary
determinants of fecundity. Intraspecific variability in interbirth intervals has been
attributed to many factors, including age, dominance, and parity of the mother
[Clarke & Glander, 1984: Dunbar, 1987; Silk, 1990; Smuts & Nicholson, 19891,
survival of the previous infant [e.g., Altmann et al., 1978; Harley, 19851, social

Received for publication January 27, 1994; revision accepted July 19, 1994.
Address reprint requests to Linda Marie Fedigan, Department of Anthropology, University of Alberta,
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, T6G 254.

0 1995 Wiley-Liss, Inc.


10 I Fedigan a n d Rose
factors [Deputte, 19911, group size [Dunbar, 1980; Robinson, 1988a1, and resource
availability [Cheney et al., 1988; Milton, 1981; Strum & Western, 1982; Richard,
19851. Differences between species in the pattern of birth spacing has been ac-
counted for primarily by the general finding that life history characters scale to
body mass [e.g., Harvey et al., 1987; Stearns, 1983; Western, 19791. Once the
effects of different body sizes have been removed, the remaining variation in the
pace of reproduction has been ascribed to habitat differences [Cords & Rowell,
1987; Ross, 19881, brain size [Harvey et al., 1987; Martin, 1981, 19901, basal met-
abolic rate [Rasmussen & Izard, 1988; but cf. Ross, 19921, and the pattern of
parental investment [Galdikas & Wood, 19901.Thus, it has been argued that forest
primates generally develop and reproduce more slowly than those primates found
in a variety of open and secondary growth habitats; that the brain may act as a
pacemaker for life history traits, such that species with larger relative brain sizes
develop and age more slowly; and that the length of suckling-induced anovulation
and the degree of assistance from the male (paternal care) all affect the charac-
teristic rate at which females produce offspring.
The objective of this paper is to examine the variation in interbirth intervals
(IBI) for three sympatric species of cebid monkeys, Cebus capucinus, Alouattu
palliata, and Ateles geoffroyi, a t both the intra and interspecific levels, in the
context of several of the causative factors that have been previously proposed.
Relatively few papers have focused directly upon birth spacing patterns in pri-
mates, in part because of the many years of research required to collect such data
on long-lived, slowly reproducing species. Our knowledge of the birth spacing
patterns of neotropical primates is especially poor in comparison to t h a t of the
catarrhines. Furthermore, many of the large scale comparisons and theoretical
discussions of life history patterns in primates rely to various degrees upon mea-
sures taken from captive primates. At present we are limited in our ability to
assess the impact of provisioned foods, veterinary care, and cage environments on
the values we obtain for measures such as weights, growth rates, pace of repro-
duction, and survivorship. Analyses of data such a s those presented here on lesser-
known, free-ranging animals will augment our overall understanding of life his-
tory patterns in natural primate populations.
METHODS
Study Site and Subjects
Data come from three groups of white-faced capuchins, four groups of mantled
howlers and one community of black-handed spider monkeys inhabiting overlap-
ping home ranges in the forests of Area de Conservacion Guanacaste (formerly
Santa Rosa National Park), in Costa Rica. The main entrance of the park is located
approximately 35 kilometers northwest of Liberia on the Pan American highway,
and the original boundaries of SRNP encompass 10,800 ha of tropical dry forest in
a series of stepped plateaus from the foothills of volcanic mountains down to the
Pacific coastal plain. The area experiences distinct wet and dry seasons, with
almost all of the annual 900-2,500 mm of rainfall occurring between mid-May and
mid-December [Janzen, 19911.
The study began in 1983 with a census of the local primate populations [Fedi-
gan, 1986; Fedigan et al., 19851, and in 1984, the senior author (L.M.F.) and her
field assistants began to discriminate individual monkeys, either through natural
markings or by distinctive tags that we placed on the animals [Fedigan et al., 1988;
Glander et al., 19911. In 1985, we began to observe the known individuals on a
regular basis, in order to record births, deaths, disappearances, and group migra-
tions, as well as various foraging and social behaviors. At least 20 days each month
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys / 11
have been spent locating and watching the monkeys from 1985 to the present, with
only one 4 month hiatus in 1991. Birth and death dates for September-December
1991 are estimated, almost all others are exact to the week. Fifty-seven howlers
(19 males and 38 females) and 13 sexually mature spiders (2 males and 11females)
have been marked and released since 1985. Although 8 capuchins were originally
tagged in 1985, we have since come to rely on natural markings only for this
species. As of April, 1993, a total of 110 infants have been born to known mothers
(36 capuchin infants, 46 howler infants, and 28 spider infants).
Data Collection
At the time of capture, females were weighed, measured, and assessed for
parity and age. Age estimates were probably most accurate for howlers, because of
considerable comparative data on the biology of this species [Glander, 1980; and
unpublished]. In the case of howlers and spiders, we relied mainly on tooth wear to
estimate age; for capuchin females we used the assessment of brow length measure
that was originally developed by Oppenheimer [19691. Since few capuchin females
were caught, weights are not available for them; and because howlers and spiders
were not the subjects of our own behavioral studies [but see Chapman, 1988,1990;
Chapman & Chapman, 1986, 19901, dominance rank data are presented only for
capuchins.
Interbirth Intervals OBI)
Analyses of 71 interbirth intervals (23 for capuchins, 24 for howlers, and 24 for
spiders) are presented in this report. All IBISwere calculated to the nearest month.
The sample of mothers was comprised of those females for whom we had complete
records from the birth of the infant who opened the interval to the subsequent
birth: 9 capuchin mothers, 9 howlers, and 8 spiders. More than one interbirth
interval was recorded for some of the mothers. Not included in the interval anal-
yses were females giving birth to only one infant since being markedkecognized,
and intervals in which the first infant did not survive to the mother’s subsequent
parturition. Of the 71 intervals included, 26 were incomplete (censored), either
because no subsequent birth occurred to close the interval prior to the end of the
study period, or because the mother was lost due to death or migration. The ex-
clusion of censored intervals has been shown to result in underestimation of in-
terbirth intervals in the sample as a whole [Smuts & Nicholson, 1989; Galdikas &
Wood, 19901. The data were analyzed using the SPSS-PC program SURVIVAL,
which makes use of both censored and uncensored intervals, and corrects for cen-
soring. Survival analysis provides median IBIS for groups or subgroups and per-
forms a Wilcoxon-Gehan test of interval distributions [Lee, 19921. We compared
interbirth intervals across the three species (capuchin, howler, spider) and, within
each species, tested for effects of age, weight, parity, and rank. Rank, parity, and
age were assessed a t the beginning of each interval. Weights were collected at least
once during the period covered by the intervals. The level of significance was set at
P < 0.05. For spider and howler females, we used a Spearman’s rank order corre-
lation to test the relationship between IBI and maternal weight.
Birth Rates
We calculated annual birth rates for each study group, from a sample of 101
births (35 capuchins, 44 howlers, and 22 spider infants). Females giving birth to
only one infant during the study period were included in these calculations, but
births occurring in 1993 and prior to 1985 were not included. We used Mann-
Whitney tests [Siegel & Castellan, 19881 to compare birthrates in large and small
12 I Fedigan and Rose
capuchin and howler groups, and a binomial test [Sokal & Rohlf, 19811 to test
seasonal differences in birth rates in all three species. A Spearman rank-order test
was used to examine the relationship between annual rainfall and annual birth-
rates. Daily rainfall has been recorded a t a standard weather station in Santa Rosa
National Park since 1979 [Janzen, 19911.
RESULTS
Intraspecific Variation in Interbirth Intervals
Survival analyses resulted in no significant differences in IBI length attrib-
utable to age, parity, weight, or dominance rank. Specific test results are as fol-
lows. Capuchins: high rank (n = 12) IBI = 24.50, low rank (n = 11)IBI = 31.65,
Wilcoxon (Gehan) statistic = 1.056, df = 1,P = 0.3041; multiparous (n = 20) IBI
= 25.91, primiparous (n = 3) IBI = 27.50, W = 0.052, df = 1,P = 0.8192; old (n
= 11)IBI = 24.00, young (n = 12) IBI = 28.27, W = 0.264, df = 1,P = 0.6074.
Howlers: multiparous (n = 20) IBI = 23.68, primiparous (n = 2) IBI = 20.00, W
= 0.433, df = 1,P = 0.5058; old (n = 12) IBI = 21.28, young (n = 9) IBI = 20.31,
W = 0.514, df = 1,P = 0.4733; weight and IBI: Rho = -0.147 (n.s.1. Spiders: old
(n = 16) IBI = 35.57, young (n = 5) IBI = 45.83, W = 0.879, df = 1,P = 0.3485;
weight and IBI: Rho = -0.354 (n.s.1.
In six cases (2 capuchins, 1 howler, and 3 spiders), an infant did not survive
until the mother’s subsequent parturition. Samples are small, but IBIS following
infant death were significantly shorter than other (uncensored) intervals in all
three species (capuchins: IBI after infant death = 16.0, IBI after surviving infant
= 26.9, Mann-Whitney U = 2.5, P = 0.034; howlers: IBI after infant death = 6.0,
IBI after surviving infant = 22.1, U = 0 , P = 0.05; spiders: IBI after infant death
= 11.0, IBI after surviving infant = 32.8, U = 0.5, P = 0.004).

Intergroup and Seasonal Variation in Birth Rates


For capuchins and howlers, an extensive set of annual birthrates for one large
group (n > 15) and one small group (n < 15)suggest that large groups have higher
birthrates than small groups (Table I). However, the differences did not reach
statistical significance (capuchins: U = 25, P = 0.407; howlers U = 19, P =
0.140). There were considerable fluctuations in birth rates from year to year for
each species, but no consistent pattern of annual variation across species was
apparent (Table I). There were no significant correlations between annual rainfall
and birthrates in the subsequent year (Fig. 1; capuchins: Rho = 0.458; howlers:
Rho = 0.238; spiders: Rho = 0.122). There were also no significant correlations
between rainfall in the first month of the wet season and birthrates in the follow-
ing year (capuchins: Rho = 0.276; howlers: Rho = -0.262; spiders: Rho = 0.366).
There was a tendency for births to occur more frequently in the dry season,
from December through May (Fig. 2). A binomial test applied to births of known
dates to known mothers during the study period indicates that significantly more
births occurred in the dry season than in the wet season (Z = 3.40, P = 0.0003).
Interspecific Variation in Interbirth Intervals
SURVIVAL analysis of interbirth intervals demonstrates a significant differ-
ence between the three species (W = 7.656, P = 0.0218). Santa Rosa howlers have
the shortest median interbirth interval, and thus the “fastest” rate of parturition
(19.9 months, range 9-40 months); capuchins have a slower rate of parturition
with a median interval of 26.36 months (range 13-55), and spiders have the slow-
est pace of the three species, with a median interval of 34.72 months (range 13-
48).
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys / 13
TABLE I. Birth Rates for Cebus, Howler, and Spider Monkey Groups*
Group
Cebus Howler
1“ 2” 3 1” 2” 3 4
Year Small Large Large Small Large Small Large Spider
1984 .50 - - - - - - -
1985 .oo .60 - - .50 - .36 .22
1986 .50 .60 - 1.00 .50 - .43 .56
1967 .33 25 - .oo .16 - .57 .67
1988 .50 .25 - .50 .66 - - .22
1989 .67 .75 .67 .25 .50 .50 - .25
1990 .67 .25 .33 .oo .50 .oo - .60
1991 .oo .75 .I5 .50 .40 .oo - 25
1992 .33 .60 .40 .oo .27 .50 - .25
Mean .39 .51 .54 .32 .44 25 .45 .37
*Capuchins: Group 1, mean size = 9 (range 4-13); group 2, mean size = 18 (range 12-26); group 3, mean size
= 19 (range 17-22). Howlers: Group 1, mean size = 10 (range 6-14); group 2, mean size = 20 (range 14-28);
group 3, mean size = 6 (range 4-8); group 4, mean size = 38 (range 18-45).
“Groups included in Mann-Whitney analysis.

- --.Q..-
Rainfall (m)
Capuchins
-
----A-- Spiders

Howlers
3-01
2.5 -
2.0 -
1.5-

1.0-

0.0 : I I I I 1 I I I
1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991

Year
Fig. 1. Annual rainfall (in meters) and birthrate in subsequent year for capuchins, howlers, and spider mon-
keys.
14 I Fedigan and Rose
100
DlySeason

Wetseason
80

60

40

20

0
Capuchins Howlers Spiders
Fig. 2. Births during dry season (December through May) and wet season (June through November) at Santa
Rosa National Park, Costa Rica, 1985-1993. Only births to known mothers on known dates are included.
Capuchins: n = 22; Howlers: n = 31, Spiders: n = 18.

This comparison between interbirth intervals is put into a larger context in


Table 11, in which some of the standard life history traits used in allometric anal-
yses are presented for the three species. It is evident that the order of increasing
body weights among the three species is in the wrong direction to explain the
observed differences in interbirth interval length, or to explain many of the other
comparisons of traits reflecting the “pace” of development and reproduction (e.g.,
weaning age, estrous cycle length, menarche, age a t first birth, maximum life
span, maximum intrinsic rate of natural increase [rl). From relative body weights,
we would have expected the order from fastest to slowest to be capuchins-howlers-
spiders, rather than howlers-capuchins-spiders. On the whole, spider monkeys
have the “slowest” life history pattern as would be predicted from their largest
relative size of the three species, but either howlers are faster, or capuchins are
slower, than would be predicted from their relative body sizes. The exception to
this rule is gestation length, which is in the order predicted by relative body
weights.
Table I1 also presents absolute and relative brain weight values, which dem-
onstrate that the relative “speed” of the life histories correspond to comparative
adult brain weights. Thus, howlers have the smallest adult brain size (relative and
absolute) and the fastest life history pattern, capuchin brain size values are in the
middle, and spider monkeys have the largest brains (absolute value) and, on the
whole, the slowest life history pattern (i.e., longest IBIS, slowest pace of develop-
ment, lowest intrinsic rate of increase [I-]).
Interbirth intervals are made up of three components: postpartum amenorrhea
(presumably corresponding to the early phase of intensive lactation), cycling, and
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys / 15
TABLE 11. Life History Traits*
Cebus capucinus Alouatta palliata Ateles geoffroyi
Female weight (kg) 2.28 (1) 5.7 (1) 6.75 (1)
Male weight (kg) 3.23 (1) 6.6 (1) 8.38 (1)
Mean adult weight 2.75 (1) 6.15 (1) 7.57 (1)
Femlmale wt (sex dim) .71 (1) ,236 (1) .81 (1)
IBI (months) 26.36 (1) 19.90 (1) 34.72 (1)
Gestation (days) 162 (9) 186 (2) 229 (8)
Gestation (months) 5.4 (6) 6.2 (2) 7.6 (8)
Weaning age (months) 12-24 (1) 12 (2) 27 (4)
36 (8)
Estrous cycle (days) 22 (5) 16 (2) 27 (8)
Menarche (months) 54 (5) 36 (2) 72 (4)
Age first birth (months) 60 (1) 42 (2) 84 (4)
Maximum life span (yr) 47 (7) 20 (7) 27 (7)
Max intr rate inc (r) .17 (7) .18 (7) .11 (7)
Neonatal wt (g) 230 (3) 480 (3) 426 (3)
Neonat brain wt (g) 29.0 (3) 30.8 (3) 64.0 (3)
Adult brain wt (g) 79.2 (3) 55.1 (3) 110.9 (3)
Neon braidneon wt ,126 .065 .150
Neon brainlad brain ,367 ,560 .576
Adult brainlad wt .029 ,009 .015
‘Numbers i n parentheses represent data sources: (1) Fedigan (unpublished, see also Glander et al., 1991); (2)
Glander, 1980;(3) Harvey e t al., 1987; (4) Milton, 1981; (5) Nagle & Denari, 1982; (6) Robinson & Janson, 1987;
(7) Ross, 1988; (8) van Roosmalen, 1985; (9) Watts, 1990.

gestation. Unfortunately, neither the field conditions of the present study, nor our
knowledge of the reproductive biology of captive neotropical cebids [e.g. Nagle and
Denari, 19821 allowed us to reliably separate the early lactation (and presumably
anovulatory) phase from the cycling phase of the IBI as has been done for baboons
[Altmann et al., 1978; Smuts & Nicholson, 19891; but see some information on
howler gestation and lactation in Crockett & Sekulic [1982], Crockett and Rudran
[1987al, and Glander [19801. However, it was possible to roughly divide the mean
IBI lengths found in this study into gestation versus lactation-cycling phases.
Figure 3 shows that although howlers have the shortest absolute IBI of the three
species, they devote the largest proportion of their IBIS to gestation: 31% as com-
pared to 20% and 22% in capuchins and spiders. If body size should predict relative
IBI length, these findings suggest that the apparent “reversal” of capuchin and
howler IBI values is not due to gestation length, but due to the lactation/cycling
phases of the cycle.
To further test this idea, we compared our capuchin and howler values for
gestation length, and lactationlcycling lengths to values published for other pri-
mates [see Table 16-1 in Harvey et al., 19871. Using T-tests [single observation
against the mean of a sample, Sokal & Rohlf, 19811, we determined that howlers
are not significantly different in gestation or lactationlcycling lengths from other
primate species of similar adult female body size, adult brain size, neonatal size, or
adult braidbody ratio (Table 111). Similarly, capuchin gestation length is not sig-
nificantly different from gestation lengths given for other primate species of sim-
ilar body size, brain size, neonatal size, or braidbody ratio. However, the capuchin
lactation/cycling phase is significantly longer than that of other primate species of
similar body size (T = 3.081, P = .05), brain size (T = 3.414, P = 0.05), neonatal
size (T = 3.858, P = 0.021, and braidbody ratio (T = 6.242, P = 0.01). This
16 / Fedigan and Rose

Cebus capucinus (2.28 kilos)


Lactation - Cycling Gestation

Months 0 21.0 26.4

Lactation - Cycling
Alouaftu puUiata (5.7 kilos)

Gestation
!
Anovul.

13.7 19.9

I Lactation - Cycling
Ateles geo.ffroyyi(6.5 kilos)

Gestation
22 %
/Months 0 27.1 34.7

Fig. 3. The characteristic interhirth interval and its component phases in capuchins, howlers, and spidc
monkeys.

suggests t h a t capuchins are slow for their body size with respect to the lactation/
cycling phase of the IBI.
DISCUSSION
In this study we found no significant differences in IBI lengths that were at-
tributable to individual differences in the age, parity, weight, or rank of the moth-
ers. It is possible that these variables, often identified in the literature as proxi-
mate causal factors in IBI length, are not operating in the life histories of this
study sample of capuchins, howlers, and spider monkeys; however, we were only
able to test for weight relationships in two of the three species and a relationship
between IBIS and dominance in one of the three. It is also possible that, despite the
near-decade length of this study, we do not yet have a sufficient sample size to
discriminate individual differences brought about by age, parity, weight, or rank
patterns.
We did find t h a t “older” capuchin and spider females tended to have shorter
intervals than younger females, and that multiparous capuchin females tended to
have shorter intervals than primiparous females. (Parity could not be tested for
spider monkeys a s there were no primiparous spider monkeys females in our sam-
ple.) This non-significant trend corresponds to previous reports on other primate
species [e.g., Silk, 1990; Smuts & Nicholson, 19891. However, we found that the
trend was reversed in howling monkeys; in this species younger and primiparous
females tended to have shorter intervals. This reversal supports earlier conclu-
sions by Glander [1980] and Clarke & Glander [19841that reproduction in mantled
howling monkeys is biased toward early fecundity in young adult females. Crock-
ett and Rudran [1987a] found that age had no significant effect on IBI length in red
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys / 17
TABLE 111. Gestation and LactatiodCycling Length Comparisons Between Capuchins,
Howlers, and other Primates of Similar Body Weight, Brain Weight, Neonatal Body
Weight, and Brain. Body Ratio (Harvey et al., 1987)
Mean
Species Variable Comparison values for
being being groups comparison
compared compared similar in- groups n t= P Comment
Capuchins Gestation Body weights 145 6 0.96 n.s. Slightly long
(days) 162 Brain weights 168 6 0.29 n.s. Typical
Neonate weights 167 6 0.50 ns. Typical
Brain:body ratios 161 7 0.08 n.s. Typical
Capuchins Lactation/ Body weights 28 1 5 3.29 c.05 Long
cycling Brain weights 282 4 2.97 <.05 Long
(days) 629 Neonate weights 259 5 2.72 <.05 Long
Brain:body ratios 183 4 6.06 C.01 Very long
Howlers Gestation Body weights 187 8 0.02 ns. Typical
(days) 186 Brain weights 167 10 1.42 n.s. Slightly long
Neonate weights 179 18 0.27 ns. Typical
Brain:body ratios 178 24 0.20 n.s. Typical
Howlers Lactation/ Body weights 439 7 0.12 n.s. Typical
cycling Brain weights 252 6 1.76 n.s. Slightly long
(days) 412 Neonate weights 385 11 0.15 ns. Typical
Brain:body ratios 442 18 0.04 n.s. Typical

howlers. In capuchins, the only species for which dominance ranks were available,
high-ranked females had a non-significant tendency toward shorter intervals. The
same trend has been reported in wedge-capped capuchins [Robinson, 1988b1,black
spider monkeys [Symington, 19871, and baboons [Smuts & Nicholson, 1989; but cf.
Altmann et al., 19881.
The one significant finding in our intraspecific analysis, that loss of an infant
shortens the interbirth interval in all three species, supports the observation made
in several previous studies that infant survivorship affects interbirth intervals
[e.g., Altmann et al., 1978; Crockett & Sekulic, 1984; Cords & Rowell, 1987;
Harley, 1985; Scucchi, 1984; Tanaka et al., 19701. This is likely due to the physi-
ological relationship between suckling, lactation, and amenorrhea [Williams,
19841. Simpson et al. [1981] suggested that even mothers who simply reject their
infants earlier and more forcibly experience shorter IBIS than mothers who con-
tinue to allow their infants to nurse.
Another variable that has often been identified as affecting the fecundity of
female primates is the size of the group in which they reside. A major review of 13
primate species by van Schaik [19831 found a significant negative correlation
between group size and fecundity for the vast majority of cases. This effect is
believed to be brought about by increasing competition for food and social stress in
larger groups [e.g., Dunbar, 19871. On the other hand, Robinson [1988al has ar-
gued that larger groups are able to displace smaller groups from monopolizable
resources, and thus females in larger groups should experience greater reproduc-
tive success. In Robinson’s study population of wedge-capped capuchins, fecundity
was much higher in large groups than small ones. In our study sample of white-
faced capuchins, the trend is for larger groups to have higher birthrates than
smaller groups, but the difference is not significant.
18 I Fedigan and Rose
Although there were considerable annual fluctuations over the 8 years of birth
rate data presented in this paper, there was no consistent pattern across the 3
species, and we were not able to isolate specific factors responsible for “good”
versus “bad” reproductive years. The total amount of annual rainfall is only a
broad inferential measure of the relative amount of resources that should be avail-
able to cycling females, and in this study sample, i t does not correlate significantly
with the proportion of infants born in the subsequent year [cf., Crockett & Rudran
1987bl.
In Guanacaste, almost all of the rainfall occurs during the rainy season, from
June through November, and 70% of the infants in this study were born in the dry
season from December through May. Gestation lengths for the 3 primate species
are approximately 5 to 7 months, which indicates a peak in conceptions during the
rainy season. Most researchers have concluded that birth seasons are not evident
in these cebid species [see Clarke & Glander, 1984; Eisenberg, 1973; Glander,
1980; references in Lindburg, 19871. However, seasonal concentrations of births
have been reported in some populations of spider monkeys [Chapman & Chapman,
1990; Milton, 1981; Symington, 1987; van Roosmalen, 19851, howlers [Jones, 1980;
Crockett and Rudran, 1987133, and capuchins [Defler, 1979; Janson, 1984; Robin-
son, 1988133. It is clear that strict seasonality is not the case in the three species
found in Santa Rosa National Park, since some births did occur in the rainy season.
It would be more appropriate to suggest that a birth peak occurs from December-
May.
When we compared the three primate species in the park we found that howl-
ers give birth approximately every 1.5 years, capuchins give birth approximately
every two years, and spider monkeys only every three years. What might be de-
termining these interspecific differences? An interbirth interval is a composite
variable made up of 3 very different reproductive phenomena-the length of ges-
tation, the period of post-partum amenorrhea, and the number and length of cycles
that occur before the female conceives again. We normally think of gestation
length a s being tightly biologically circumscribed, and relatively invariable across
the females of a given species. As well, the length of post-partum amenorrhea may
have a n evolutionarily-fixed aspect, but is subject to environmental variables,
especially the effects of suckling and lactation, and nutritional factors. Finally, the
length of estrous cycles is a biologically-determined phenomenon, but the number
of cycles that it takes for a female to conceive is likely to be highly influenced by
social and other environmental circumstances.
Since length of gestation is the component of IBI least subject to environmen-
tal influences, we might expect that gestation would best fulfill the prediction of
a n allometric relationship with body size. Indeed in this study of cebids, relative
gestation length is the ONLY life history trait that corresponds to relative body
size across the three species. All of the other life history characteristics reflecting
the pace of development and reproduction, indicate that capuchins are “slower”
than would be predicted from their body size. Comparisons with a wide range of
primate life history data (see Table 111) indicate that the long IBIS of capuchins can
be traced to the lactatiodcycling phase of their interbirth intervals. This means
that the white-faced capuchins in our study population either have a long period of
post-partum amenorrhea, andlor they take a long time to conceive again after
resuming cycling. Galdikas and Wood [19901 identified suckling-induced anovula-
tion as the principle determinant of IBI in their study samples of humans and great
apes. In our study of free-ranging, natural cebid populations, it was not possible to
separate the anovulatory from the cycling phase of the IBI, so they were considered
together. From a social point of view, a cycling female primate is usually still
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys I 19
caring for her last offspring, even if it is partially or fully weaned, and thus
maternal care may be implicated in the lengths of both the anovulatory and the
cycling phases of the IBI. Why would spider infants need to be cared for longer than
capuchin infants and capuchin infants longer than howler infants?
As a general rule, bigger species take longer to grow up, and experience
“slower” life histories (i.e., lower birth rates, slower rates of development, and
longer lives). Most of the variation in life history traits can be accounted for by
scaling to body mass. Spider monkeys are the largest of the three species under
consideration and are thus predicted to have the slowest rate of development. Once
body weight is accounted for, the remaining variation has been attributed to a
number of different factors, including brain size. Capuchins have the greatest
braidbody ratio of the three species, and they have the lowest neonatal braidadult
brain ratio. When a capuchin infant is born, its brain is only Y3 of its adult size,
whereas howler and spider neonates have brains 1/z of adult size. According to
Watts [19901, neontatal capuchins are less mature than newborn cercopithecoids,
and are more similar to the apes than to monkeys in skeletal maturity at birth.
There has been some debate in the literature as to whether body size or brain
size or basal metabolic rate is the factor that best explains variation in life history
patterns [e.g., Economos, 1980; Harvey et al., 1991; Hofman, 1983; McNab, 1988;
McNab & Eisenberg, 1989; Ross, 19921. Without entering into that debate, it is
clear that in this comparison of three cebid species representing 3 different sub-
families (Cebinae, Alouattinae, Atelinae), body size does not provide an adequate
explanation of the relative pace of life history patterns. Perhaps as noted by Galdi-
kas and Wood [19901, Harvey et al. [19871, and Rasmussen & Izard [19881, the
allometric relationships that characterize primates as an order break down when
attention is focused on closely-related species. However, if the energetic demands
of brain growth and the maintenance of a relatively large brain size do act as a
“pacemaker” for life history patterns, then we might predict that capuchins with
their unusually high brain to body ratio, would have slower than expected life
histories patterns for their size [see Parker, 1990; Watts, 1990; and Sawaguchi,
19921. We might also predict that since a cebus neonatal brain is only % of its adult
size, capuchin mothers would devote relatively long periods of time to nurturing
each infant before giving birth to the next (i.e., exhibit relatively long interbirth
intervals). One independent test of this argument would be to determine the rel-
ative length of the IBI (especially the lactationlcycling phase) in free-ranging
squirrel monkeys, another cebid species with an unusually high brainlbody ratio.
A second major variable that has been implicated in the pace of reproduction,
once the effects of different body sizes have been removed, is the pattern of habitat
andor resource use [Cords and Rowell, 1987; Ross, 19881. Monkeys found in pri-
mary forests tend to exhibit slower life histories and those found in open, variable
habitats have faster life histories. Capuchins have been identified as an exception
[ROSS, 19911. They have very slow life histories for their body size but they are not
limited to primary forests. Indeed their habitats today are more likely to be char-
acterized as secondary growth areas. Ross suggests that perhaps capuchins evolved
in stable primary forests, and later colonized secondary forests.
Diet is another aspect of habitat use that may be pertinent to this consider-
ation of variation in IBIs. It has been correctly pointed out to us (Crockett, pers.
comm.) that the relative length of average IBIs in howlers, capuchins, and spider
monkeys corresponds to the proportion of fruit in their diets. Although based on a
sample of only three species, we would suggest that in this apparent relationship
between fruit-eating and birth intervals, the intervening variables are intelligence
and relative brain size. The “ecological origin” theory of intelligence holds that
20 / Fedigan and Rose
ecological pressures, such as foraging strategies, selected for intelligence and large
relative brain sizes in primates [e.g., Gibson, 1986, Milton, 19881. Foraging on
fruit, in particular, has been identified as selecting for intelligence, since fruit is
patchily distributed in time and space. Milton [19881 noted that spider monkeys,
who are strictly frugivorous, have larger relative brain sizes than howlers, who
will eat some fruit, but are fundamentally folivorous. Capuchins are omnivorous
and eat a higher proportion of fruit than howlers, but a lower proportion than
spider monkeys [Chapman, 19881.Furthermore, capuchins are noted for their abil-
ity to find, extract, and process embedded foods and these extractive foraging
techniques have been argued to be indicative of higher order cognitive skills, and
related to their large brain sizes [Gibson, 1986; Parker, 19903. Thus, it is possible
to argue that differential foraging patterns was one of the selective pressures for
relative brain sizes in these three species, and that brain size in turn acts as a
“pacemaker” for life history traits such as interbirth intervals.
The IBIS reported in this study are comparable to those suggested by some
earlier reports for these three species and their congeners. The median IBI for
Alouatta palliata (19.9 months) falls between the averages of 17 and 22.5 months
previously reported for this species by Milton [1982], Glander [1980], and Clarke
and Glander [1984], and is close to the 17 months reported for A. seniculus [Crock-
ett and Sekulic, 1984; Crockett and Rudran, 1987al. However, the range of IBI’s in
the Santa Rosa population (9-40 months) exceeds those previously reported for
either species [see also Crockett & Eisenberg, 19871. The median IBI for Ateles
geoffroyi is similar to the 31.9 months reported by Milton [19811 and the 34.5
months reported for A. paniscus chamek [Symington, 19873.However, the range of
IBI’s a t Santa Rosa (13-48 months) again exceeds that previously reported [see
also Eisenberg, 1973; van Roosmalen, 19851. There are no comparable data from
wild populations for Cebus capucinus, but the median IBI of 26.4 months exceeds
the 22 months reported for C . apella, and is similar to the 26 months reported for
C . oliuaceus [Robinson & Janson, 19871.
In summary, the comparative pattern of interbirth intervals that we found in
our three study species is most consistent with allometric variation in brain size.
However, interbirth intervals may also be affected by the constraints of other life
history variables such as infant survivorship. Further, there may be some type of
selection for the overall pace of reproduction (e.g., K versus r selection, or pleiot-
ropy), such that IBI is only one aspect of a lifetime reproductive pattern. Finally,
environmental factors, such as habitat and diet, may affect the pace of reproduc-
tion. Although interbirth interval is a widely used life history trait, with a re-
spected tradition behind it, we found it to be a heterogeneous category. If we wish
to address the question of why females of a species space their births in a particular
manner, we need to focus in on the components of interbirth intervals-gestation,
amenorrhea, and cycling to conception-and the factors which affect these compo-
nents in multivariate ways.

CONCLUSIONS
1. We found significant differences in the length of interbirth intervals of
capuchins (26.36 months), howlers (19.90 months), and spider monkeys (34.72
months).
2. Capuchins have a “long” interbirth interval for their relative body size,
attributable to a prolonged lactatiodcycling phase.
3. Differences in interbirth interval length correspond to relative brain size,
rather than relative body size.
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys / 21
4.Within species, there were no significant differences in interbirth interval
length due to age, parity, weight, or dominance rank.
5. Loss of an infant resulted in shorter subsequent birth intervals in all three
species.
6 . We found no clear correlation between birth rates and annual rainfall, but
births in all three species clustered in the dry season.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to the National Park Service of Costa Rica for allowing us to
work in Santa Rosa National Park from 1983 to 1988, and to the administrators of
the Area de Conservaci6n Guanacaste for permission to continue research in the
park to the present day. Many people contributed to the collection of data for this
ten year project, but the efforts of Colin Chapman, the late Larry Fedigan, Ken
Glander, Francois LaRose, Katie MacKinnon, and Rodrigo Morera are particularly
acknowledged. Terry Taerum assisted with the SURVIVAL analyses, and John
Addicott with the tables. We thank Carolyn Crockett, Samuel Wasser, Michael
Raleigh, and one anonymous reviewer for many helpful suggestions that improved
the manuscript. The research of L.M.F. is funded by a n on-going operating grant
(A7723 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada
[NSERCCI), and that of L.M.R. was funded by a n NSERCC post-graduate schol-
arship.
REFERENCES
Altmann, J.; Altmann, S.A.; Hausfater, G. Clutton-Brock, ed. Chicago, University of
Primate infant's effects on mother's future Chicago Press, 1988.
reproduction. SCIENCE 201:1028-1030, Clarke, M.R; Glander, K.E. Female repro-
1978. ductive success in a group of free-ranging
Altmann, J.; Hausfater, G; Altmann, S.A. howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in
Determinants of reproductive success in Costa Rica. Pp. 111-126 in FEMALE PRI-
savannah baboons, - Papio cynocephalus. MATES: STUDIES BY WOMEN PRIMA-
PD. 403-418 in REPRODUCTIVE SUC- TOLOGISTS. M. Small, ed. New York,
CESS: STUDIES OF INDIVIDUAL VARI- Alan R. Liss, 1984.
ATION IN CONTRASTING BREEDING Clutton-Brock, T.H. REPRODUCTIVE
SYSTEMS. T.H Clutton-Brock, ed. Chi- SUCCESS: STUDIES OF INDIVIDUAL
cago, University of Chicago Press, 1988. VARIATION IN CONTRASTING BREED-
Chapman, C.A. Patterns of foraging and ING SYSTEMS. Chicago, University of
range use by three species of neotropical Chicago Press, 1988.
primates. PRIMATES 29:177-194, 1988. Cords, M.; Rowell, T.E. Birth intervals of
Chapman, C.A. Association patterns of spi- Cercopithecus monkeys of the Kakamega
der monkeys: the influence of ecology and Forest, Kenya. PRIMATES 28:277-81,
sex on social organization. BEHAVIORAL 1987.
ECOLOGY AND SOCIOBIOLOGY 26: Crockett, C.M.; Eisenberg, J.F. Howlers:
409-414, 1990. variations in group size and demography.
Chapman, C.A.; Chapman, L.J. Develop- Pp. 54-68 in PRIMATE SOCIETIES. B.B.
ment of howling monkey twins (Alouatta Smuts; D.L. Cheney; R.M. Seyfarth; R.W.
palliata) twins in Santa Rosa National Wrangham; T.T. Struhsaker, eds. Chicago,
Park, Costa Rica. PRIMATES 27:377-381, University of Chicago Press, 1987.
1986. Crockett, C.M.; Rudran, R. Red howler
Chapman, C.A.; Chapman, L.J. Reproduc- monkey birth data 11: interannual, habitat
tive biology of captive and free-ranging and sex comparisons. AMERICAN JOUR-
spider monkeys. ZOO BIOLOGY 9:1-9, NAL OF PRIMATOLOGY 13:369-384,
1990. 1987a.
Cheney, D.L.; Seyfarth, R.M.; Andelman, Grockett, C.M.; Rudran, R. Red howler mon-
S.J.; Lee, P.C. Reproductive success in key birth data I: seasonal variation.
vervet monkeys. Pp. 384-402 in REPRO- AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PRIMATOL-
DUCTIVE SUCCESS STUDIES OF OGY 13:347-368,1987b.
INDIVIDUAL VARIATION IN CON- Crockett, C.M.; Sekulic, R. Gestation length
TRASTING BREEDING SYSTEMS. T.H. in red howler monkeys. AMERICAN
22 I Fedigan and Rose
JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY 3:291- Glander, K.; Fedigan, L.M; Fedigan, L;
294, 1982. Chapman, C. Field methods for capture
Crockett. C.M.: Sekulic. R. Infanticide in red and measurement of three monkey species
howler 'monkeys (Alohatta seniculus). Pp. in Costa Rica. FOLIA PRIMATOLOGICA.
173-191 in INFANTICIDE: COMPARA- 57:70-82, 1991.
TIVE AND EVOLUTIONARY PERSPEC- Harley, D. Birth spacing in langur monkeys
TIVES. G. Hausfater; S.B. Hrdy, eds. New (Presbytis entellus). INTERNATIONAL
York, Aldine, 1984. JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY 6:227-
Defler, T.R. On the ecology and behavior of 241, 1985.
Cebus albifrons in northern Colombia. I: Harvey, P.H.; Martin, R.D.; Clutton-Brock,
Ecology. PRIMATES 20:475-490, 1979. T.H. Life histories in comparative perspec-
Deputte, B.L. Reproductive parameters of tive. pp. 181-196 in PRIMATE SOCIET-
captive grey-cheeked mangabeys. FOLIA IES. B.B. Smuts; D.L. Cheney; R.M. Sey-
PRIMATOLOGICA 5757-69, 1991. farth; R.W. Wrangham; T.T. Struhsaker,
Dunbar, R.I.M. Demographic and life history eds. Chicago, University of Chicago Press,
variables of a population of gelada baboons 1987.
(Theropithecusgelada).JOURNAL OF AN- Harvey, P.H.; Pagel, M.D.; Rees, J.A. Mam-
IMAL ECOLOGY 49:485-506,1980. malian metabolism and life histories.
Dunbar, R.I.M. Demography and reproduc- AMERICAN NATURALIST 1 3 7 5 5 6 4 6 6 ,
tion. Pp. 240-249 in PRIMATE SOCIET- 1991.
IES. B.B. Smuts; D.L. Cheney; R.M. Sey- Hofman, M.A. Energy metabolism, brain
farth; R.W. Wrangham; T.T. Struhsaker, size and longevity in mammals. QUAR-
eds. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, TERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY 58:495-
1987. 512,1983.
Economos, A.C. Brain-life span conjecture: a Janson, C.H. Female choice and the mating
reevaluation of the evidence. GERONTOL- system of the brown capuchin, Cebus apella
OGY 26:82-89, 1980. (Primates: Cebidae). ZEITSCHRIFT FUR
Eisenberg, J.F. Reproduction in two species TIERPSYCHOLOGIE 65177-200, 1984.
of spider monkeys, Ateles fusciceps and Janzen, D.H. Daily temperature and rainfall
Ateles geoffroyi. JOURNAL OF MAM- records from the administration area of
MALOGY 54:955-57,1973. Santa Rosa National Park, Area de Con-
Fedigan, L.M. Demographic trends in the servacion Guanacaste, Northwestern
Alouattapalliata and Cebus capucinus pop- Costa Rica, 1979-1991, unpublished
ulations in Santa Rosa National Park, manuscript, 1991.
Costa Rica. Pp. 285-293 in PRIMATE Jones, C.B. Seasonal parturition, mortality,
ECOLOGY AND CONSERVATION. J. and dispersal in the mantled howler mon-
Else; P. Lee, eds. Cambridge, Cambridge key, Alouatta palliata. BRENESIA 17:l-
Univ. Press, 1986. 10, 1980.
Fedigan, L.M.; Fedigan, L; Chapman, C. A Lee, E.T. STATISTICAL METHODS FOR
census of Alouatta palliata and Cebus ca- SURVIVAL ANALYSIS. New York, John
pucinus monkeys in Santa Rosa National Wiley & Sons, 1992.
Park, Costa Rica. BRENESIA 23:309-22, Lindbura, D.G. Seasonalitv of reproduction
1985. in primates. Pp. 167-218 in COMPARA-
Fedigan, L.M.; Fedigan, L; Chapman, C; TIVE PRIMATE BIOLOGY. VOL. 2B: BE-
Glander, K. Spider monkey home ranges: a HAVIOR, COGNITION AND MOTIVA-
comparison of radio telemetry and direct TION. G. Mitchell; J. Erwin, eds. New
observation. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF York, Alan R. Liss, 1987.
PRIMATOLOGY 16:19-29, 1988. McNab, B.K. Complications inherent in
Galdikas, B.M.F.; Wood, J.W. Birth spacing scaling the basal rate of metabolism in
patterns in humans and apes. AMERICAN mammals. QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BI-
JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOL- OLOGY 63:25-54, 1988.
OGY 83:185-91,1990. McNab, B.K.; Eisenberg, J.F. Brain size and
Gibson, K.R. Cognition, brain size and the its relation to the rate of metabolism in
extraction of embedded foods. Pp. 93-104 mammals. AMERICAN NATURALIST
in PRIMATE ONTOGENY: COGNITIVE 133:157-167, 1989.
AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR. J.G. Else; P.C. Martin, R.D. Relative brain size and basal
Lee, eds. Cambridge, MA, Cambridge Uni- metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates.
versity Press, 1986. NATURE 293:57-60, 1981,
Glander, K.E. Reproduction and population Martin, R.D. PRIMATE ORIGINS AND EV-
mowth in free-ranains mantled howling OLUTION: A PHYLOGENETIC RECON-
monkeys. AMERIGAR JOURNAL OF STRUCTION. Princeton, New Jersey,
PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 53:25-36, Princeton University Press, 1990.
1980. Milton, K. Estimates of reproductive param-
Birth Intervals in Neotropical Monkeys f 23
eters for free-ranging Ateles geoffroyz. PRI- (Ateles paniscus) in Surinam, ACTA AM-
MATES 22:574-9,1981. AZONICA (supplement) 15:l-238, 1985.
Milton, K. Dietary quality and demograph- Ross, C. The intrinsic rate of natural in-
ic regulation in a howler monkey popu- crease and reproductive effort in primates.
lation. Pp. 273-289 in THE ECOLOGY OF JOURNAL OF ZOOLOOGY 214:199-219,
A TROPICAL FOREST SEASONAL 1988.
RHYTHMS AND LONG-TERM Ross, C. Life history patterns of New World
CHANGES. E.G. Leigh; D.M. Windsor, Monkeys, INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL
eds., Washington, DC, Smithsonian Insti- OF PRIMATOLOGY. 12:481-502, 1991.
tute Press, 1982. Ross, C. Basal metabolic rate, body weight
Milton, K. Foraging behavior and the evolu- and diet in primates: an evaluation of the
tion of primate intelligence. Pp.285-306 evidence. FOLIA PRIMATOLOGICA 58:
in MACHIAVELLIAN INTELLIGENCE: 7-23,1992.
SOCIAL EXPERTISE AND THE EVOLU- Sawaguchi, T. The size of the neocortex in
TION OF INTELLECT IN MONKEYS, relation to ecology and social structure in
APES AND HUMANS. R. Byrne; A. monkeys and apes. FOLIA PRIMATO-
Whiten, eds. Oxford, Clarendon Press, LOGICA. 58:131-145, 1992.
1988. van Schaik, C.P. Why are diurnal primates
Nagle, C.A.; Denari, J.H. The reproductive living in groups? BEHAVIOUR 87:120-
biology of capuchin monkeys. INTERNA- 144, 1983.
TIONAL ZOO YEARBOOK 22:144-150, Scucchi, S. Interbirth intervals in a captive
1982. group of Japanese macaques. FOLIA PRI-
Oppenheimer, J.R. 1969. Changes in fore- MATOLOGICA 42:203-208, 1984.
head pattern and group composition of the Siegel, S.; Castellan, N.J. NONPARAMET-
white-faced monkey (Cebus capucinus).Pp. RIC STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAV-
36-42 in PROCEEDINGS OF THE SEC- IORAL SCIENCES, 2ND EDITION. New
OND INTERNATIONAL PRIMATOLOG- York, McGraw Hill, 1988.
ICAL SOCIETY, VOLUME 1. C.R. Car- Silk, J.B. Sources of variation in interbirth
penter, ed. Basel, s. Karger, 1969. intervals among captive bonnet macaques
Parker, S.T. Why big brains are so rare: en- (Macaca rudzata). AMERICAN JOURNAL
ergy costs of intelligence and brain size in OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 82:
anthropoid primates. Pp. 129-154 in 213-30, 1990.
“LANGUAGE” AND INTELLIGENCE IN Simpson, M.J.A.; Simpson, A.E.; Hooley, J.;
MONKEYS AND APES. S.T. Parker; K.R. Zunz, M. Infant-related influences on birth
Gibson, eds. Cambridge, Cambridge Uni- intervals in rhesus monkeys. NATURE
versity Press, 1990. 290:49-51, 1981.
Rasmussen, D.T.; Izard, M.K. Scaling of Smuts, B.; Nicholson, N. Reproduction in
Growth and life history traits relative t o wild female olive baboons. AMERICAN
body size, brain size and metabolic rate in JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY 19:229-
lorises and galagos [Lorisidae, Primates]. 246, 1989.
AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL Sokal, R.B.; Rohlf, F.J. BIOMETRY, 2ND
ANTHROPOLOGY 75:357-367, 1988. EDITION. New York, W.H. Freeman,
Richard, A. PRIMATES IN NATURE. New 1981.
York, W.H. Freeman, 1985. Stearns, S.C. The influence of size and phy-
Robinson, J.G. Group size in wedge-capped logeny on patterns of covariation among
capuchins, Cebus oliuaceus, and the repro- life-history traits in the mammals. OIKOS
ductive success of males and females. BE- 41: 173-1 87, 1983.
HAVIORAL ECOLOGY AND SOCIOBI- Strum, S.C.; Western, D. Variations in fe-
OLOGY 23:187-197,1988a. cundity with age and environment in olive
Robinson, J.G. Demography and group baboons (Pupio anubis). AMERICAN
structure i n wedge-capped capuchin mon- JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY 3:61-76,
keys, Cebus oliuaceus, BEHAVIOUR 104: 1982.
202-231,1988b. Symington, M.M. Ecological and social cor-
Robinson, J.G.; Janson, C.H. Capuchins, relates of party size in the black spider
squirrel monkeys, and Atelines: sociologi- monkey, Ateles paniscus chamek. Ph.D.
cal convergence with Old World Primates. Dissertation, Princeton University, 1987.
Pp. 69-82 in PRIMATE SOCIETIES. B.B. Tanaka, T.; Tokuda, K; Kotera, S. Effect of
Smuts; D.L. Cheney; R.M. Seyfarth; R.W. infant loss on the interbirth interval of
Wrangham; T.T. Struhsaker, eds. Chicago, Japanese monkeys. PRIMATES 11:113-
University of Chicago Press, 1987. 117, 1970.
van Roosmalen, M.G.M. Habitat prefer- Watts, E.S. Evolutionary trends in primate
ences, diet, feeding strategy, and social or- growth and development. Pp. 89-104 in
ganization of the black spider monkey PRIMATE LIFE HISTORY AND EVOLU-
24 I Fedigan and Rose
TION. C.J. DeRousseau, ed. New York, primates: effects of pregnancy and nursing.
John Wiley & Sons, 1990. Pp. 375-385 in PRIMATES: THE ROAD
Western, D. Size, life history and ecology in TO SELF-SUSTAINING POPULATIONS.
mammals. AFRICAN JOURNAL OF K. Benirschke, ed. New York, Springer-
ECOLOGY 17:185-204,1979. Verlag, 1984.
Williams, R.F. The interbirth interval in

You might also like