You are on page 1of 36

Forensic Statement Analysis as an investigation technique.

The truth evades those who do not listen.

1.

INTRODUCTION

Forensic Statement Analysis (FSA) may not be a new investigative technique (Colwell et al) but it certainly been one of the most underutilised and definitively one of the most misunderstood techniques. FSA is also based on human behaviour thus requiring some understanding of psychology is required on the part of the forensic interviewer as it sets out to analyse the psycholinguistic behaviour of individual people to establish their truthfulness or deception (Adams 1996:2). This process thus, requires intense training for prospective forensic interviewers based on current research and available material in a field that requires additional extensive research and modification. Essentially FSA is based on psycholinguistic behaviour, which comprises of four components; speech parts, extraneous information, lack of conviction and the balance of the statement or structure and is utilised to detect whether a person is being either truthful or deceptive. Traditionally, any form of analysis of deception was purely focused on deceptive behaviour only and not truthful. This researcher has for approximately 10 years has had a self-interest in this topic and has had the honour of meeting Nathan Gordon one of the worlds recognised leaders on the study of detection of deceptive behaviour.

2.

DEFINITION Forensic Investigation Hans Gross who a century ago was regarded as the leader of criminalistics (the science of criminal detection), viewed each crime as a scientific quandary requiring the application of objective scientific analysis, the crime could be fully studied in order to individualise and identify the perpetrator involved supported by scientific proof (Marais 1992:1, Van der Westhuizen 1996:7, Swanson et al 1988:12). To place the forensic element in the correct order, Russel & Coetzee (2000:xiii), refer to a forensic interview as an interview conducted within the criminal context and a non-forensic interview has no relevance to the criminal context.

According to Readers Digest Illustrated Oxford Dictionary forensic is: forensic adj. 1 of or used in connection with courts of law; esp. in relation to crime detection. 2 of or employing forensic science [from Latin forensis] It further separates the term forensic and forensic science defining it as: forensic science n. the application of biochemical and other scientific techniques to the investigation of crime. Taking the criminalistic approach which distinctly separates the criminal investigation process into two concepts (Marais 1992:1 and Van der Westhuizen 1996:9) namely: Criminalistic Tactics; focuses on the aptitude and dedication of the investigator through surveillance, interrogation, observation, searching methods. Criminalistic Techniques; technical analysis of evidence through scientific

applications as listed above. It is important to note the investigation process seeks to discover both objective and subjective clues; Objective clues; is physical evidence that is either left behind at the scene of the crime or found in the possession of the perpetrator that is silent, indirect or circumstantial but has an objective explanation which associates the evidence with the perpetrator and the criminal incident. Subjective clues: is the evidence of people such as complainants, eye-witnesses, victims, perpetrators who are directly or indirectly involved in the crime. (Van der Westhuizen 1996:1 & 11, Marais & Van Rooyen 1994:17, Cloete & Stevens 1990:186) Therefore, forensic science is the criminalistic technique based on the applied sciences and generally lead laboratory analysis-type investigations, by example listed below: (Fish 2004:171, Dienstein 1974:234-240) Comparative Science: comparative methods used for the analysis of fingerprints, questioned documents and firearm evidence. Entomology: taxonomic identification of insects, methods of collection, and the importance to determination of time of death in murder investigations.

Medicine: anatomy and physiology of the human body with an emphasis on traumatic and unexpected deaths. Pathology: terminology and scientific techniques used in medico-legal investigations of sudden or unexpected deaths. Psychology: overview of the major psychological profiles, levels of motivation, and prognoses of the homicidal offender; criminal profiling. Serology: analysis of suspected body fluid evidence. Forensic investigation (criminalistic tactics) could include the following disciplines: Auditing: financial auditing methods and techniques in the investigation of financial irregularities in fraud investigations. Crime Analysts/Intelligence: collection, evaluation, interpretation and

distribution of criminal information and intelligence, including surveillance and observation. Crime Evidence/Scene: the search for and collection of physical evidence at a crime scene. Identification: the identification of persons either through visual observations, documents, photographs. Interviewer: the interviewing of victims, witnesses and suspects to obtain and determine the reliability of statements of witnesses and truthfulness of suspects. The term "forensic" is effectively a synonym for "legal" or "related to courts" as based on the Latin term of forum. In the modern realm of investigations, there is a requirement for investigators not only to complete continued training but to become specialists in various fields of investigation as Marais (1992:1) with reference to Gross, criminal investigators would achieve greater success in solving crimes if they are equipped with selected scientific knowledge that he is aware of what is to be done with a clue to extract the full evidential value of the clue. Califana and Levkov (1978:5) support this notion as they claim criminalistics and forensics are the same issue, the tactics and techniques thereof make the forensic investigation process a field of multi-applications, which entail a number of individual

(own emphasis) forensic specialists utilised dependent on the nature of the offence or crime under investigation to systematically collect, analyse and present evidence.

3.

FORENSIC INTERVIEWING 3.1. The fundamental difference between normal interviewing and forensic interviewing. Forensic interviews relate to the investigation of crime Gordon (2004). Russel and Coetzee (2000:xiii) add the crime can be actual or suspected and non-forensic interviews still search out the truth with no links to crime e.g. a job interview. In terms of criminal interviews, ascribed as forensic interviews based on the definitions of forensic, are defined as; entail the systematic questioning of any person who has knowledge of a crime and may assist in a criminal investigation Dowling (1979:17). Swanson et al. (1998:123) supports this definition and includes the term personal knowledge which is important as forensic relates to the courts and litigation, in interviewing the lack of personal knowledge could render the information as hearsay resulting in exclusions of evidence.

Forensic interviewing is applicable to anyone with knowledge that may assist a criminal investigation and forensic relates to the courts and litigation process therefore subjecting the interviewing of any person to the rules of evidence in the litigation process in terms of admissibility, reliability and relevance, these rules also being applicable to confessions made by suspects. Marais and Van Rooyen (1991:164) submit interviewing is the questioning of people as a face-to-face conversation who have knowledge, which may assist in the investigation of a crime, and only those who are not suspects themselves rather victims, complainants or witnesses. Their argument primarily differentiates that

suspects are subjected to interrogations only, Marais and Van Rooyen (1991:173) in support of Van der Westhuizen (1996:80) cites a number of authors who agree with this synopsis. Dowling (1979:17,29) also differentiates between interview and interrogation, which he describes as primary purpose of obtaining an admission of guilt. Gordon

and Fleisher (2002:27) state the interview is an information gathering process and interrogation searches for the guilty to confess to their complicity in a crime. Buckwalter (cited in Van Westhuizen 1996:80) supports the view that the interview process takes place irrespective of a witness or suspect but rather dependent on the level of voluntary behaviour and cooperation which if not forthcoming the forensic interview can always be switched to an interrogation phase which is focused at uncooperative or hostile persons. In instances of a suspect offering cooperation with the intention of submitting a confession this can be performed within the forensic interview environment and suspects must be afforded the opportunity with the establishment of an environment to confess (Russel & Coetzee 2000:10-14,44-45). In addition, interrogations are confrontational with any amount of time available, whereas a forensic interview is non-confrontational, free flowing and does not last longer than 60 minutes Gordon and Fleisher (2002:29). Notwithstanding, in Gordons submission for the requirements of a Master of Arts in Criminology; Validation of the Forensic Assessment Interview Technique, University of South Africa (UNISA) November 2004, he proposes the Forensic Assessment Interview Technique (FAINT) that includes an assessment of non-verbal behaviour, analysis of unwitting verbal clues and FSA, Gordon and Fleisher (2002:89) which in sum establishes a parameter against which a witness or suspect is measured as to their truthfulness or deception. The FAINT

process is independent of the crime under investigation and the forensic investigator is not entirely privy to the physical facts whereas in the interrogation, an interrogator is dependent on the physical facts of the investigation that focuses on analytically extracting truth from any person. With developments in interview techniques there seems to be an apparent shift away from interrogations per se, consider Gordon (2004), there is no discussion relating to interrogation utilisation. Interrogations are loosing momentum as mentioned

confrontational, hostile and highly pressurised with forceful techniques applied in an attempt to for a person to tell the truth with interrogators spending too much time on endeavours to trap a suspect when he/she is not telling the truth or forcing them to tell the truth. The time is better spent on a forensic interview which in its own environment

persists with some form of pressure on the subject of the interview, or a study of, what (own emphasis) the subject is telling the interviewer, Russell and Coetzee (2000:10-19, 80-82) also support this notion. Colwell et al (2002:289) quote Yuille the goals of an effective interview are (a) minimize the trauma of investigation, (b) maximize the information obtained about the event(s), (c) minimize the contamination of the memory trace by the interview, and (d) maintain the integrity of the investigative process. This process is critically applicable to the FSA objectives as will be later discussed. On the other hand the argument is presented in a case when a suspect is uncooperative and remains silent or bluntly refuses to discuss anything nothing will be discovered. In such circumstances albeit under the traditional interrogation or a

forensic interview process, the result remains the same in that the suspect is not offering information and consider; no answer is also an answer (own emphasis). Forensic interviews coupled to the description of forensic investigations in which Gross said that investigators should not solely depend on interrogations, the sum of the forensic investigation is required to discover the suspects individual complicity not making the forensic interview less vital, but it focuses to extract information, which in turn will be beneficial when linked directly to physically evidence, modus operandi and obviously other statements such as witness accounts for example. 3.2. Forensic interviewing is a specialists domain

An aspect of interviewing is some understanding of human psychology and behaviour as described above and based on the forensic investigation description, and again considering Van der Westhuizen (1996:8) pointing to Gross that all investigators must be equipped with some scientific skills in order to achieve higher success rates in investigations rather than merely resort to interrogations techniques. This is interesting, if they are lacking in interrogation or even basic interview techniques forensic or otherwise what are they left with? For this reason the forensic investigator must

acquire higher levels of skill especially in his/her field of speciality to this end those that are trained to conduct forensic interviews should be conducting them - as a bullet from a crime scene that requires ballistic testing handed to a Crime Analyst who may well be

able to identify a type of weapon with an area of frequent robberies but would be hopelessly lost in individualising a firearm from which the bullet may have been fired.

4.

FORENSIC STATEMENT ANALYSIS 4.1. FSA and its objectives

FSA is based on the Scientific Content Analysis (SCAN) (Adams 1996; Gordon 2004:59; Smith 2001:iii) and Statement Verification Analysis (SVA) (Vrij 2002:3) but for the purpose of this discussion we will remain with FSA which is an analytical technique able to detect deception within the psycholinguistic performance of an individual. Deception in a statement is not proof of someone lying rather it is a method to direct away from the truth. The results of a FSA analysis highlight areas within a statement that requires clarification and so can be used to form part of an interviewing strategy to elicit more information from the individual (Sapir, 1987). If valid, such a method can be usefully applied within an investigative context. The results provide investigators with a means of analysing the statements of witnesses, victims and/or suspects to help ascertain the presence of deception (Adams 1996). Applying formal techniques to establish truthfulness rests upon the assumption that there are observable differences between the language behaviour of truthful and deceptive individuals. 4.2. The origins of FSA

SCAN was developed by Avinoam Sapir who started teaching it when he arrived United Sates of America in the late 1980s. Sapir was trained in code breaking while in the service of Israeli Intelligence and later conducted extensive research into the psycholinguistic behaviour to distinguish between truthful and deceptive individuals (Gordon and Fleisher 2002:42; Gordon 2004:58). FSA using the SCAN concepts finds a basis in Criteria-Based Content Analysis (CBCA) developed by a forensic psychologist Udo Undeutsch (Russell & Coetzee 2000:71; Schollum 2005:73) who Gordon (2004:55) submits, developed an operational hypothesis to determine the credibility of a statement. Colwell et al. (2002:290) is in agreement with Gordon (2004:55), deception is a cognitive demanding task to design and implement, and therefore a statement not based on a real

situation or experience will not be rich in detail compared to a statement based on real events and to analyse the statement CBCA makes use of 19 criteria Vrij (2005:29) namely: a) Logical Structure b) Unstructured Production c) Quantity of details d) Specific contents e) Contextual embedding f) Descriptions of interactions

g) Reproduction of discussions h) Unexpected complications during the incident i) j) Unusual details Superfluous details

k) Accurately reported details misunderstood l) Related external associations

m) Accounts of subjective mental state n) Attribution of perpetrator's mental state o) Motivation-Related contents p) Spontaneous corrections q) Admitting lack of memory r) Raising doubt about one's own testimony

s) Self-deprecation t) Pardoning the perpetrator

u) Offense-Specific Elements v) Details characteristics of the offense The criteria above will become relative as FSA is increasingly described in this research. 4.3. FSA and other forms of deceptive behaviour detection

The essential difference between FSA and other deception detectors is that FSA is based on the analysis of psycholinguistic behaviour either written or verbal, where as other deception detectors are mechanical as they analyse physiological stress indicators, and

the most common one utilised in the present days are polygraphs and voice stress analysis. What has not been included here is the FAINT method which is a non-

mechanical interview technique utilising a combination of analytical applications a) body language b) Morgan Interview Theme Technique (MITT) c)

Projective/Relevant/Comparison questions d) the written statement - FSA and e) afterinterview interview (Gordon and Fleisher 2002:89). The Polygraph In the early 1920s the polygraph was developed and introduced in American police detective units. The polygraph is able to measure and calculate the level of stress an individual by analysing pulse, blood pressure, respiration and skin sensitivity known as Galvanic skin response. These physiological responses or stress

indicators are beyond the control of the individual being tested i.e. anyone can hold their breath for a given period but they are not able to increase or decrease their heart rate. The responses based on stimuli, which are in the form of questions agreed to, by the polygraph examiner and the subject prior to commencement of the test. Later in this research question formulation will be discussed in detail. All questions formatted to provide that the answer is a Yes or No answer are divided into the three types (Califana & Levkon 1978:238-240; Dowling 1979:33; Gordon & Fleisher 2002:7-9; Russell & Coetzee 2000:73; Swanson et al 1988:177179): Irrelevant questions establish neutral ground for the subject as the questions are non-threatening and allow for the polygraph to record the emotional stress level of a person in lowered anxiety such as: Is you name John? Is there a door in the room? Relevant questions are formulated with issues under investigation such as: Did you commit that crime? Did you take the money? Did you assist anyone to take the money? Comparison questions are broader in nature and identify the truthful person:

10

In your entire life did you take anything that did not belong to you? In your entire life did you damage anything? Between the age of 19 and 29 did you ever hit anyone? What is critical about the questions, is the question formulation which is applicable to all forms of forensic interviewing, but in particular the polygraph as they can be the difference between a professional polygraph examiner and an inexperienced polygraph examiner more importantly accurate or inaccurate polygraph examination. It must also be kept in consideration when framing a question the subject must be given the opportunity to rationalise an answer i.e. is able to then justify a truthful answer which would defeat the polygraph or use what Gordon (2004:40) describes as personal coding i.e. whatever the matter is under investigation the question for instance relates to the actual criminal act but the question is directed at the motive, will provide a false answer. This simple example is one of many that obviously has a bearing on the reliability of polygraph examinations which debate rages on Russell & Coetzee (2000:73) the results are not legally admissible and remain as an investigation resource. Voice Stress Analysers The human voice has two modulations when a person speaks the audible module and the inaudible module, which causes tremors the human ear, is incapable to hear. The Voice Stress Analyser (VSA) is able to detect the tremors, which are involuntary especially during times of stress a response to deceptive behaviour. Furthermore, they are easier to operate with minimal training and do not require any attachments to be placed on the subject as with the polygraph. Levkov 1978:241-243; Russell & Coetzee 2000:74,75; (Califana & et al

Swanson

1988:176,177). The VSA then analysers speech either directly or recorded and further are not dependent on question formulation as stringent as the polygraph. Due is relative uncomplicated operation, the VSA can be used in a covert role Russell and Coetzee (2000:76,77) such as recording an individual over a telephone and later analysing their speech. As with the polygraph the accuracy of the VSA remains under question and is not admissible in legal proceedings.

11

4.4.

The structure of spoken or written deception

As with any form of investigation it is important the forensic investigator knows what to look for, in FSA it is no different, therefore it is important to know what the structure is of deception. As Gordon (2004:44) submits and this must be clearly understood, that just about every person has at some stage of their lives, taken something when they did not have permission to do so, told a lie about something important or to someone important, did something that they are now ashamed of or cheated someone etc. The reason that this must be understood within the FSA concept, the forensic investigator performing interviews will have to formulate questions or scenarios and these will test or even challenge an honest person, this results in past issues suddenly becoming relevant and the honest person may begin to show signs of deception under the incorrect set of conditions which may be misinterpreted. In addition, both children and adults can

obscure what they imagined or what they actually experienced (Vrij 2005:29) again releasing signs of deception. Most forensic interviewers to avoid this problem attempt to establish a base line model of the subject as with the polygraph examination, nonthreatening questions (control questions) or scenarios are established to discovery a neutral response from the subject, this is later compared to other areas of the statement to discover deception (Vrij 2005:31). The focus of FSA as will be shown, is it analyses the words in the statement and their inter-relationships, at least 90% of statements made are truthful, and most people do not attempt to lie directly. Instead, they hedge, omit crucial facts, feign forgetfulness, and pretend ignorance. The reason for this is that liars are reluctant to commit themselves to their deceptions, instead preferring to use conversational tricks to avoid damaging admissions. Of course this presents one of the most obvious pieces of evidence of an untruthful statement, omissions on a Curriculum Vitae where job applicant does not want to reveal a negative experience. Whether in a written statement or verbal statement which can be recorded and later analysed, the word and deception structure remains the same. Blatant deception would be when someone says I didnt do it and they did do something that would be a 100% deception variable, however, the deceptive person is incapable of making a cold denial,

12

for this reason FSA makes use of set parameters to investigate the different aspects of speech (Smith 2001:25,26). 4.4.1. Pronouns

Pronouns I, we, my our, your, his, her, their, she, they; have the same meaning to all of use (Russell & Coetzee 2000:25) and their improper use (Smith 2001:11) results in lack of consistent word usage and commitment to the statement (Gordon 2004:60). On the other hand as Gordon states, the over usage the subject is being careful in the construction of his/her statement. The truthful person has no difficulty in forming a connection to their statement with the use of I and furthermore there would be a consistent pattern in terms of the usage of I, the correct usage of I shows the commitment being made to the statement as Gordon (2004:60) explains that less usage indicates an unreliable statement. However, commitment is also linked directly to blame and therefore, the incorrect usage such as I before another person is introduced in a statement e.g. I went to fetch him, my partner Stephen, gives rise to bad relationship between the two, thus the departure from normal use presents a rise in suspicion and indicate deception in the statement (Gordon 2004:60; Russell & Coetzee 2000:25,26). Gordon illustrates that we indicates a good relationship

between people or even togetherness and Russell & Coetzee (2000:26) describe it as shared commitment to a statement but also demonstrate that people do not live or operate in isolation and therefore, any statement containing a lack of we could indicate deception on the part of the author or orator. Furthermore, every person both in written and verbal statements will construct their speech and this includes talkative people, to get the point of discussion in shortest possible way. Therefore, once the normal introductions have been done of any person in the statement the use of we will be used correctly and flow with the context of the statement. The lack of the use of we (Russell & Coetzee 2000:27) requires further questioning as to what effected the relationship and why. Gordon also argues we can be used in place of I by suspects, this to avoid commitment and develops the theory of shared commitment. In addition, Russell & Coetzee (2000:28) refer to false accusations particularly in rape offences, the accuser will say we went into the room there is

13

togetherness in this statement, the norm would be he forced me into the room. Russell & Coetzee (2000:28) also warn us of changes in the use of we when it refers to a group of people, such as when they is used as reference to the group and later in the statement this changed to we this is evident in offence committed in a group environment such as robberies, the change to we by the author may indicate togetherness with the perpetrators thus shared commitment or involvement. When reading a statement and there is an incorrect usage of pronouns that they are missing, a mark; X (Gordon 2004:60) or similar must be inserted into the place where the pronoun would have been found e.g. I left the building at about 12:30 and walked towards my car. To the left (X) saw two men walking towards the building. (X)Watched as they stood at the door. (X)Walked back towards the building and they started to walk off. I turned around and went back to my car. All other pronouns must be circled in the statement. 4.4.2. Nouns Nouns are the names for things, objects, places, people and groups of things. The critical factor about nouns usage they remain consistent in a statement and any change in the usage must be directly linked to an actual change in the context of the statement e.g. the guy becomes a pilot after he introduces himself. In addition, noun usage can be sensitive or influenced by contamination (Russell & Coetzee 2000:29) and therefore, it is paramount that the statement obtained is the first uncontaminated statement. As the example above nouns can add foundation to any aspect to relationship stability or instability as with the example above which indicated nothing more than an introduction, when a husband and wifes relationship changes and in a statement the wife my describe the man as husband on several occasions and then suddenly without reason refers to him as Mike, this means the relationship has changed both in the statement and in reality the question now is why? Nouns also highlight distance between an object and the subject e.g. the car which belongs to the subject was never described in statement in the statement as my car, this lack of ownership points to the subject attempting to place distance between him/herself and the car which may have killed someone in a hit and run accident.

14

Distance (own emphasis) must always be searched for in a statement as it is evident in pronouns it is just as nouns (Russell & Coetzee 2000:29), a subject is investigated in his involvement in the murder of his wife, repeatedly made use of the term my wife and I instead of we (togetherness) indicates a difficult relationship as the literal measurement of my wife and I reflects the difficult relationship in reality. He

continues in his statement to say I found Lauren shot in the car indicating change in the noun of wife to Lauren and the additional distance notification of the car instead a truthful statement would have read I found her shot in my car; my being a possessive determiner. Smith (2001:13) has not correctly interpreted Sapirs theory of distance measurement correctly by describing it is vagueness or the ambiguity a subject socially introduces or talks of a person in a statement, there is no such ambiguity; the related distance rule simply determines the usage of pronouns or nouns correctly to reflect upon actuality (Russell & Coetzee 2000:29). Furthermore, as shown above distance is also applicable to other nouns which refer to objects such a cars, it has been seen in theft cases where the subject claims to not know anything about an object such as money once again placing distance between themselves the disappearance of money. 4.4.3. Possessive Pronoun A possessive pronoun is a word such as hers, his, mine, our, theirs and yours the usage of them should flow with the sequence or actual events within a statement and any change in them should be directly associated with the actual change of the statements context, if not they need to be investigated. Included in this category is my a possessive determiner, Gordon (2004:74) says my is possessive pronoun, notwithstanding, it is utilised and exposes a high level commitment and reduces the distance to zero such as my wife; my car; my money; my room etc. enforcing acknowledgment to the person or ownership of an object being referred to. Gordon (2004:74) offers the example of the subject talking of his dog as my dog and his son as the baby in the same context a distinctive change in the relationship with his son. Similarly during the investigation of a vehicle theft and the owner attempted to claim for a replacement from insurance his statement read: when I arrived at the shopping

15

centre I parked my car on the forth level of the parking area. I went into the shopping centre did some shopping and had a sandwich. Afterwards I left the shopping centre drove back to my office to collect some files so that I could do some work at home. I arrived at home at around 16:30. At about 19:00 my cousin fetched me to go out to dinner and the car was parked in the garage. At about 23:30 we arrived at my house my car was gone. There is the obvious change from my car to the car and with this switch interestingly the ownership of the vehicle is given up fairly early in the statement meaning the subject no longer possessed the car at 19:00 when her cousin came to fetch her for dinner. It would have made more sense if the subject said when she arrived home the car was gone as in a normal theft this would be logical sequence as she no longer owned the car as it had been stolen at this point. It furthermore, exposes the relationship (Russell & Coetzee 2000:32) the subject had with the car as it supports the fact the subject was no longer in possession of the car at the time her cousin came to fetch her therefore the relationship had ended. 4.4.4. Verbs and Tense

Smith (2001:13) cites the Sapir assumption of a statement that changes from the singular past tense to a passive tense would indicate deception. A narrative that occurs based on memory recall will consist of verbs (or doing words) in the past tense, such as walked, woke, stood, looked, watched, drove etc. A statement based on imagination is a story created in the current or present tense Russell & Coetzee (2000:30) and changes in tenses in the sequence of the statement will be a clear indicator of linguistic deception. The exception is in missing person/murder cases it is normal to talk of the missing person in the present tense Russell & Coetzee (2000:30) who also refer to the Susan Smith case in America during which she described how her children had gone missing and she was later convicted for their murder. Adams (1996:1) quotes from Smiths statement to the press: "My children wanted me. They needed me. And now I can't help them." The father of the children not having anything to do with their disappearance, kept on referring to the children in the present tense: They are okay. They are going to be home soon.

16

Adams describes when analyzing the parents statements independently the conclusion is the father had no knowledge of where the children were and believed them to be alive, therefore speaking in the present tense. While the mother had full knowledge and continually used the past tense and verbs simply put, the children no longer wanted or needed her, as they were dead. In other cases the narrated statement will always be in the past tense such as I walked to the bank and the money was in my jacket pocket. As I turned the corner I saw two guys standing there waiting for me. They both pushed me against the wall, searched my jacket took the money and ran away. The deceptive version would be I walked to the bank and the money is in my jacket pocket. As I turned the corner I see two guys standing there waiting for me. They both push me against the wall, search my jacket take the money and run away. This type of deception is

convenient as the subject has described exactly where and how the money was taken and with some more encouragement who took the money as it can be safely assumed that the two guys were acquaintances of the subject. 4.4.5. Conviction Gordon (2004:65) submits the phrase I dont remember in an open statement (opened ended and uncontaminated statement); means the subject does remember. When a subject is asked to describe how a robbery incident occurred I saw the car with perpetrators pull to the front of the shop but I dont remember the colour or the type of car, this shows the subject does not want to commit to the statement being a lack of conviction. Smith (2001:12) cites Sapir as claiming a subject who is deceptive will not display conviction when relating to their version of events. Smith continues, suspects may do this to avoid their own involvement in an incident and this would include statements that are incapable of recalling specific details in fear implicating themselves. Adams (1996) also cites Sapir and includes suspects show a lack of conviction with phrases of I think, I believe, to the best of my knowledge which Adams describes these phrases as qualifiers as they serve to mitigate the phrase before it is communicated in so doing reduce the effect of what is actually said. Gordon (2004:65) claims that the phrase I dont remember would not be significant

17

if the subject was asked a specific question and applicable.

Russell & Coetzee

(2000:32) refer to the reopening of the investigation into the Helderberg crash a South African Airways (SAA) aircraft into the sea off the Mauritius coast, a number of tapes with recorded conversations went missing but the official involved is unable to recall whether he ever was in possession of the tapes in the context of an aircraft crash resulted in the death of over a hundred people and had international attention, it is unlikely that he does not have such a recall. Adams (1996) refers to a student who was allegedly attacked and raped by a masked man in her room at 03:30 in the morning and in her statement she makes use of the phrase kind of startled on waking up and realising a knife was being held to her throat, if taken into context would hardly be the response of single young woman who had been raped, terrified would be more appropriate. 4.4.6. Distance Through out the FSA structure analysis we have been discussing distance and this very aspect of deception is fundamental when utilising FSA as people want to place distance between themselves and deception (Russell & Coetzee 2000:15). Smith (2001:13) cites Sapir in the context and in particular, when a suspect places distance between themselves and another person together with the improper use of pronouns indicates deception. Smith (2001:27) who attempts to test the reliability of FSA,

continues by citing a result of a group study of a victim who had submitted a statement in a rape case the group found she was truthful on the basis that she utilised the correct pronoun usage to place distance between herself and the suspect. Remaining with this submission Gordon (2004:60) also agrees with the lack of pronouns when introducing a person within the context of the statement indicates a negative relationship between the persons supports the distance theory. And as (Adams 1996:5) points out the use of we contextually is the shortest way to refer to another person, that being when the relationship is stable. This has been noticed in practise particularly in murder cases where the suspect makes a concerted effort to place distance between themselves and the victim with most obvious statement in the context of the death of a close relative as the body.

18

Consider the following example: I awoke at 7:30. I got up and made coffee. I drank my coffee and then showered and dressed. I read the newspaper. I left the house about 8:15. I arrived at work at 8:45. I worked until lunchtime, which was about 12:30. I left work for lunch and drove to House of Coffees. XMet David Morgan for lunch and then returned to work about 1:30. I worked until about 5:00. The subject did not use the first person pronoun "I" in his statement when he described meeting Morgan for lunch i.e. I met. This is a red flag as deviation from the first person pronoun in a statement, indicates the subconscious attempting to distance one's self from any individual involvement. When the pronoun "we" is used when it appears that the first person pronoun "I" should be used, the same situation applies. The subject's subconscious is attempting to distance himself or herself by having others involved when no one else is. This example illustrates the relationship between the subject and Morgan was not on a solid basis as Gordon describes a theory which Russell & Coetzee (2001:15-16) also subscribe to by substantiating that a subject depersonalizes the other person and as in the reference case they cite (Russell & Coetzee 2001:15-16) the depersonalization process is similar to reference to objects when ownership is relented (own emphasis). The use of the word relent is a descriptive word of the destruction of conviction in a statement where deception leakage is evident, ownership or personalization are components of conviction and as will be explained with the use of surplus information introduces distance. example often cited: I was cleaning my gun and it suddenly went off and the shot hit her between the eyes. I dont understand, it is an old gun, which my father gave me, after the shot I dropped the gun. The switch from my gun to it and the gun relenting ownership. 4.4.7. Surplus information and time sequence As with CBCA surplus information (criteria number 12) and out of time sequences are cited as possible deviations from the story (Vrij 2005:5). Adams (1996) claims that the truthful person will relate a statement in chronological order and concisely with The

19

any information not relating to the question is surplus. Gordon (2004:64) does issue caution on this aspect in stating the subjects story is not a chorological report but rather what is important to the subject will be related and where a statement commences is important. Smith (2001:14) in citing Sapir submits that subjects may

include information that is unimportant to the forensic interviewer but may be important to the subject, in that, its inclusion is an attempt to minimise the importance to the interviewer or reader. It can be assumed that Smith in her analysis is

describing a so-called filter; greater the surplus information (greater the filter) will steer the interviewer/reader away from the truth. Colwell et al. (2002:288) further support this assumption within the cognitive interview parameter, in stating the deceiver is in control of the release of information to avoid their detection and thus deceptive information will differ in the sense of quality, quantity, manner and relevance this submission appears in another Colwell et al. undated document page 4 and suspects will also become entangled with memory recall while experiencing both guilt and anxiety. Colwell et al. (2002:289) adds linguistic circumventions, which include unnecessary connections, repeating of questions, feigned lack of memory and repeated information that lacks clarity is a further distortion of the truth. The crux here is relevance (Gordon & Fleisher 2002:43), therefore, the greater irrelevant information (lack of details) submitted the greater the filter and the diversion from the truth. Consistent throughout this theory of surplus information is the relation to the information, Russell & Coetzee (2000:33 & 34) even though the surplus may not be logical to the interviewer/reader it must be accepted as it is important to the subject/writer/suspect. In addition, as deduced earlier the filter is created for the purpose of concealing actions, activities and importantly time, which if missing would obviously indicate deception. Rassin (1999:273) argues that truth seekers should focus on searching for signals of deception and within the context of surplus information as discussed here, Vrij (2005:6) takes this further in identifying, based on CBCA, a statement must have a logical structure i.e. it is coherent and has details, Russell and Coetzee (2000:80) and Gordon and Fleisher (2002:43) makes reference to the truthful subject will offer

20

specific details to focus a statement to pertinent points whereas the untruthful person will provide a lack of details and thus a lack of detail either through brief statements or containing a substantial amount of surplus information. Surplus information

(Gordon 2004:63) as discussed is a filter to disguise information that is sensitive to the subject/suspect which is confirmed by Smith (2001:14) in her citations of Sapir. This gives rise to missing time connections and Russell & Coetzee (2000:34) submit that words such as later on; sometime later; shortly after; the next thing I remember; after that and a few minutes later especially at the beginning or end of sentences increases the probability of deception leakage. Smith claims that

reference based on Sapir, will become vague in the statement (Adams 1996), in practise this has been seen repeatedly and is another component of distance and conviction to the statement. Gordon (2004:62) puts it down to missing information (lack of detail) when words such as shortly after or later on is an aversion away from the truth in terms a lack of time connections and presents the theory of people being deceptive by omission (Vrij 2005:6) and this does not mean the statement will lack content, it is the content that will lack detail. 4.4.8. Memory To obtain a complete understanding of FSA it is important the memory or the recall thereof is discussed. As Vrij (2005:3) and Colwell et al. (2002:288) point to the

Undeutsch hypothesis, a statement obtained based on factual experience will differ considerably from a statement based on inventions and fantasy. Adams (1996) also claims the pretension of lack of memory recall with phrases such as I dont remember or I cant recall adds to the lack of conviction to the statement by the subject (Smith 2001:11,24). Gordon (2004:64) argues the phrase I dont remember in an open statement which FSA is in the majority of instances, means the subject does remember as it is an impossibility for one not to remember something if they do not remember unless they specifically asked a question about it. However, as

Smith (2001:7) correctly points out that it is important differentiate between the detection of truthfulness in a statement versus the accuracies in a statement, a statement may be truthful but contains errors and these errors could be a perception

21

of an actual event, which in turn could interpreted as a deceptive. In practise this a common error amongst interviewers and interrogators, therefore it is important that the analyst/interviewer/investigator uses techniques to discriminate between truth and deception or discovering accurate and inaccurate versions. To achieve this Vrij (2005:6) presents in truthful statements, the analyst/interviewer/investigator will find: spontaneous corrections (corrections made without prompting from the interviewer He wore black trousers, no, sorry, they were green), admitting lack of memory (expressing concern that some parts of the statement might be incorrect: I think, maybe, I am not sure, etc.), raising doubts about ones own testimony (anticipated objections against the veracity of ones own testimony: I know this all sounds really odd), self-deprecation (mentioning personally unfavorable, self-incriminating details: Obviously, it was stupid of me to leave my door wide open because my wallet was clearly visible on my desk), and pardoning the perpetrator (making excuses for the perpetrator or failing to blame him or her, such as a girl who says she now feels sympathy for the defendant. In the same vain Russell & Coetzee (2000:66) speak of three step phase as to how memory functions encoding, storing and retrieval. Encoding is as information enters memory is encoded and stored, in the stored state it can later be retrieved and remembered in the conscious mind. However as we all know failures do occur and the human mind is not able to raise information into the conscious mind and this may occur when the person was not interested sufficiently in a particular topic of the moment, sensed it but did not encode it for storage and obviously for later retrieval. While suspects being interviewed are selective in what they recall (Colwell 2002:290). It has been seen in practice that victims of rape, robberies and other traumatic incidents have no recall of specific facts and this does necessarily mean the subject/suspect is being deceptive. Trauma is not restricted to victims and witnesses, but also suspects in situations as vehicle accidents where they were the driver and killed the occupants of another vehicle or in domestic violence cases when the wife kills the husband out of desperation may not have any recall of the incident. In cases

22

as these they must be treated with utmost caution and best dealt with by a forensic psychologist, cognitive interviewing or forensic hypnotist (Swanson et al. 1988:127130). In other events a persons memory may be impaired by liquor or narcotic intoxication, can lead to false confessions such as when an intoxicated person woke up with a gun in their hand and a body next to them that it can be assumed that they killed the victim, the opposite can be true and person has no recollection of the events (Russell & Coetzee 2000:69). 4.4.9. Denial

As Colwell et al. (2004:299) submits that deception is a demanding act, Gordon and Fleisher (2002:12,13) also present that lying by commission involves a number of conditions such as commitment, invention and the increased risk as to been found in contradiction to previous statements that have been made. Therefore, denial would be when a subject denies outright that they have not done something e.g.; I didnt do it is simple enough denial. Already discussed it is difficult for a suspect to make a cold denial that being and as will be illustrated in question formulation, an outright opportunity for denial by a suspect being deceptive must not be offered e.g. two suspects were involved in a rape and murder of a victim, the one suspect raped the victim and later the other one murdered the victim. If the question is phrased were you involved in the rape and

murder of the victim the suspect would answer no (the cold denial) as they were individually responsible for the one act but not the other and the question is seeking blame for both acts in one attempt, the suspect will be able rationalise his denial (Gordon & Fleisher 2002:12). In an open statement if denial is presented without hesitation and directly, it is often an indication of a truthful statement. 4.4.10. Statement Structure In applying FSA is to obtain an uncontaminated or as Gordon (2004:59) puts it, a pure statement where the writer is afforded the opportunity to provide a version in their own words without the contamination of questions or other scenarios being a factor, in these instances the question would be similar to Explain everything you know about this incident and how it could have happened. In analysing the statement the above

structural principals will be applied moreover it must be borne in mind, opposite to the

23

discussion of surplus information, the honest person will apply editing to their statement which will have a logical flow. This leads to the statement structure per se and not only the psycho-linguistic structure (the content) which is based on the logical information flow, Schollum (2005:73) cites Zulawski and Wicklander, that each statement will have 3

segments based on flow namely the pre-event - all aspects leading up to the actual event; the actual event - who, what, where, how etc of the incident and the post event the closing down of the event and statement which will normally include emotional information as to how a subject felt e.g. fear etc. Remaining with Schollums citation above, truthful statement segments will be in almost equal parts i.e. 33.3% each and in deceptive statements they will have vastly different percentage proportions. Schollum (2005:75) compares this statement structure with one of Adams and Jarvis, which is roughly divided into pre-event of 20% main event 60%, and post event of 20%. Schollum places the two statement structures into dispute and issues caution in the utilisation of information flow as an indicator of truthfulness or deception. Gordon and Fleisher

(2002:43) and Gordon (2004:59) statement structure is 20% pre-event, 50% event and 30% post event, who furthermore claim that deceptive subjects will have long pre-events which Schollum in utilising Adams and Jarvis example is in agreement, short actual events and small or no percentage of post event. Russell and Coetzee (2000:37) make use of the same 20%, 50%, 30% model which they describe as the statements time-line which remains the same in both truthful and deceptive suspects. However, they

substantiate that deceptive subjects will make a fleeting reference to the actual event as this is where the sensitivity of the story is and the deception which is by omission, Gordon (2004:59) long pre-event and short or no post event as there is nothing to relate to in reality as they created by imagination and not founded on memory (Colwell et al. 2002:2). Therefore, should the statement structure actual event exceed 65% the content is becoming deceptive and naturally this variation will effect both pre and after event proportions. Gordon also submits that in false allegations e.g. rape and molestation

cases the truthful subject will relate to the investigative process and the embarrassment of the situation. Smith (2001:13) in reference to Sapir makes use of the same 20, 50 and 30 percentages and this is described as the statements balance and any diversion from

24

this is the unbalanced statement, an indication of deception. Superfluous information, during the analysis of

As described under the statement the

analyst/interviewer/investigator must observe changes in time or lack of time connections. This remains consistent with the statement structure (Russell & Coetzee 2000:38) and in some deceptive statements there is a minimal or none existing pre-event or after event, Gordon (2004:63) also cautions of this within the content i.e. where there is a beginning there must be and end. This further relates to what is known subjective time and

objective time Smith (2001:14) based on Sapir should be equal. The subjective time will be the amount of text taken by a subject to describe an event over a period time will equal the objective time in the statement if it is truthful. If a subject describes a 1 hour event over 12 lines and subsequently utilises 6 lines to describe a 3 hour period means the subjective time and the objective time are not equal and therefore deception is at play. Gordon (2004:63) on the other hand also adds a pace to statement; 1 line or less per hour means a crime took place, 3 lines per hour is the norm, 4 7 lines per hour would be inconclusive and 8 and more lines per hour is an intentional attempt to slow down the statement and further claims in 85% of cases subjects will be deceptive later in their statements. Furthermore, most statements are 1 to 1 pages and between 25 to 37 lines. 4.4.11. The Method Truth and deception has a descriptive structure the next step is to pull it together with the application of FSA analysis and in using the Russell and Coetzee (2000:35,93) criteria: Underline and link all changes in verb tenses i.e. change from past to present tense Circle all pronouns and note missing pronouns in the margin Make a note of all times mentioned in the margin i.e. subjective times and real times Highlight superfluous (surplus) information or unnecessary information Underline all nouns

25

Bracket any words that show lack of conviction e.g. someone took the money Circle all the persons as they come into the statement, this usually will indicate their hierarchy or importance in the subjects life Use a bold vertical line to divide the statement into 3 segments pre-event (1 issue), the actual event (main issue) and post event (2 note the balance. Gordon (2004:59-63) cites Sapir as teaching a colour coded analysis technique: Circle all pronouns and place an X where a pronoun is missing and circle it Blue highlight of aspects where the subject explains why they did something or why something happened, also applies to when a subject relates to leaving or departing from a place. These areas of a statement reflect sensitivity and an attempt to slow the statement down. Green highlight of any person mentioned in the statement, this will allow for changes in language to be identified Pink highlight areas where there is missing information or missing time which may consist of phrases such as after that, later on and I dont remember. Purple highlight for objective time Yellow highlight for all unimportant information. A cluster of yellow and blue together means a sensitive area to the subject Orange highlight for any unnecessary links such as said, told, mentioned etc. but does not include ask. The truthful story will relate to incidents beyond those under investigation as they reliant on memory, which is rich in unnecessary information, and deceptive statements are based on imagination.
st nd

issue) and

26

Gordon (2002:43) offers a psycho-linguistic behaviour evaluation table:


Truthful 1 2 Rich in details First person singular, past tense Deceptive Lack of details Deviates from the first person

singular, past tense 3 Proper introduction of the victim: My daughter. 4 Uses possessive pronoun: My Improper introduction of the victim: She.. Lack of possessive pronoun: The child.. Missing time: Two hours later.. No emotions

daughter.. 5 6 No gaps in time. Appropriate emotions in the right place (post-incident) 7 Will deny doing the crime before being asked. 8 Flow of story proper.

Only

makes

denials

to

direct

question. Incorrect flow of story.

Of the two techniques presented; it must be said the Russell and Coetzee is less restrictive in practice, however, Gordons is thorough minimising opportunities of misinterpretations.

5. VERBAL INQUIRY EFFECTIVE WITNESS (VIEW) Developed by Avinoam Sapir and according to Laboratory for Scientific Interrogation, Inc. (LSI) web site and a company owned by Sapir, it improves the interview process dramatically as it does not require the interviewer to become involved with the subject/suspect/interviewee, the analyst/interviewer/investigator can assess the facts and reliability of information prior to any interaction the subject/suspect/interviewee. VIEW is based on FSA directly, makes use of the exact same analytical process, the different being, as LSI describe it, it is in a ready-to-use format, as it consists of predetermined questions and takes the form of a questionnaire. It may be applied to witnesses, victims and suspects. VIEW is particularly effective when a large number of people are required to be interviewed or to establish their involvement in a crime or incident. Smith (2001:10) makes reference to VIEW questionnaire as an alternative to

27

the FSA technique but does not offer any particular detail other than referring to Sapirs work. Gordon in Gordon and Fleisher (2002) and Gordon (2004) does not make any specific reference to VIEW, in Gordon and Fleisher (2002:133-141) the Forensic Assessment Interview (FAINT) format is available and this is largely based on the VIEW concept other than FAINT is utilised as an interview strategy. VIEW on the other hand as described earlier, does not require the analyst/interviewer/investigator to become involved and therefore, VIEW may be utilised as a remote method i.e. it may be faxed or e-mailed to the subjects/suspects/interviewees. completed it established as basis for additional Once it has been with the

interviews

subjects/suspects/interviewees. VIEW also removes the confrontational or intrusion of a normal interview. The questions required in a VIEW questionnaire are varied from incident to incident, formulation is discussed in greater detail in the next section, but similar to the polygraph question development system. Each answer is then scored +1 for truthful, 0 for neutral, undetermined or inconclusive and 1 for deception. For each point awarded or deducted, the analyst also submits a short synopsis for the point scoring. The point scoring similar to that used by Gordon and Fleisher (2002:89-101). If the total score of the questionnaire is a negative score, the subject/suspect is deceptive. Questions may include: Have you discussed this incident with anyone? How did you hear about this incident? How do you think this happened? Why do you think you have been included in the investigation? Did you take that money? Were you present when the money was taken? If you were asked, how much of the missing money would you pay back to have this investigation stopped? In your entire life have you ever stolen something? How do you feel completing this questionnaire? What do you think should happen to the person who has done this?

28

Is there any reason that someone could name use as a suspect? Each questionnaire contains approximately 25 questions and the

analyst/interviewer/investigator formulates them depending on the incident under investigation. 5.1. The Question formulation. reason that question formulation is discussed is if every

analyst/interviewer/investigator carefully formulated a question prior to asking it, they would soon find that the answers they receive are of an improved quality and will offer vital clues to the investigation. Even though a FSA statement only requires 1 general open question e.g. tell me everything that happened here today, the

analyst/interviewer/investigator must still consider what content response he/she will receive as the example question differs considerably should it be amended to tell me everything that happened? The today has been left out, based on Gordon and

Fleisher (2002:45), each question has a place but each is important (own emphasis). In terms of VIEW and generally interviewing, the analyst/interviewer/investigator must formulate their questions to achieve the maximum response. There are four types of questions irrelevant, relevant, comparison and projective questions. In question

formulation it is important that questions are utilised to recall and exhaust actual memory (truth) and create areas for unrehearsed deception (untruthful), which as Colwell et al. (2002:290,291) submits, will have minimal or no bearing on linguistic patterns of the truthful person and questions must not introduce new substance to the issue under question. 5.1.1. Irrelevant Questions Is your name John? Is there a door in the room? Are you 27 years old? Is you residence situated in Mowbray? Irrelevant questions, they are also known as control questions, are non-threatening and offer neutral territory for the subject who is under less stress and this provides the polygraph with a comparative basis of lowered emotional stress levels to when

29

they are raised due to responses caused by stimuli of the questions asked (Gordon & Fleisher 2002:22 & 45; Califana & Levkon 1978:238-240). 5.1.2. Relevant Questions Did take the money from the draw? Did you stab that man? Did you force that woman to have sex? Did you commit that crime? Conspiracy crimes require adapted questions that include the potential involvement of the subject (Gordon & Fleisher 2002:46): Did assist anyone to take the money? Did you plan with anyone to commit the crime? Do you know who took the money? Were you in the room when the money was taken? In Gordon and Fleisher (2002:47) propose threatening words such as steal, kill, rape should be avoided and legal jargon e.g. murder, robbery, extort, theft etc. The subject/suspect/interviewee must be left in a mental comfort zone, they understand the question while keeping the relevant question short and focused. In practise it has been seen time again, a relevant question is phrased as are you responsible for the loss of that money, the person answers yes and the polygraph confirms they are honest. However, in context the person is responsible for the loss of the money because they left the safe open, but did not steal it, which is the matter under investigation. Investigators then spend an inordinate amount of time attempting to convict an innocent person for theft based on incorrect relevant questions. 5.1.3. Comparison Questions

Gordon (2004:42) describes the comparison question as they are broad in scope and deal with issues similar, but less threatening than, the relevant issue. General comparisons In you entire life did you do anything that could have resulted in you arrest?

30

Theft comparisons In your entire lift have you ever stolen anything? Arson comparison In your life did you ever intentionally burn anything? Homicide comparison Between the age 25 and 34 did you ever hit anyone? Sex comparison In you entire life did you ever lie about anything sexual? Gordon (2004:44) argues that the comparison question will threaten the truthful person as it forces them to admit something they have actually done, whereas the guilty person in an incident will not be effected as the comparison question does not relate to the incident under question and therefore their answer to a relevant question will be the threat which is the opposite for the truthful person. In addition, Gordon and Fleisher (2002:50,51) cite Cleve Backster in a recommendation instead of utilising In your entire life.. reverse the subject/suspect/interviewees life by two years and commence the question with In the first 21 years of your life, as this will continue to place the truthful person under stress but will not contaminate the answer of a guilty suspect on the relevant question. This is known as the Backster Comparison Question. 5.1.4. Projective Questions

In the course of any investigation a truthful person and a dishonest person will have fundamental differences regarding the outcome of an investigation, in that the honest person will want the investigation to be successful in identifying the culprit of the incident under investigation and the dishonest will want the investigation to fail (Gordon 2004:45). Projective questions are formulated to highlight these

differences, what is important in the utilisation of projective questions, all subjects/suspects must be asked exactly the same questions in exactly the same order. Gordon and Fleisher (2002:54) have identified that honest people will make use of strong terms and morals to condemn a crime whereas as the dishonest will

31

be comparatively less determined in their condemnation and will attempt to place distance between themselves and the incident. Examples of projective questions: What will you say if the investigation identifies you as a suspect? The person who has committed this crime, do you think they deserve a second chance? Why do you think you are being interviewed? What do you think should happen to the person who did this? How do you think this happened? Why would anyone names you as a suspect? 5.2. FSA the investigation of written statements, is graphology utilised in the process? Graphology is the study of handwriting to determine a persons character, and Pistorius (2005:411) recommends it when performing criminal profiles of suspects, it is equally useful when performing FSA but only by a trained graphologist.

6.

How is it used in the forensic investigation process? 6.1. FSA in the investigation of crime

FSA may be used in all criminal scenarios with some exception with serial offenders (see Pistorius 2005) where it would be advised that the forensic interviewer has sufficient experience and is supported by a forensic psychologist. 6.2. Forensic investigation indicators

As illustrated above through the FSA process, the deception occurs in specific areas of a persons statement almost similar to inhibitors, once they back to the truth, the information flow increases smoothly. The forensic investigator trained in FSA may utilise the statement and physically visit these areas of deception i.e. a murder suspect is returned to the scene, where a body was found and not the weapon however, the statement may be truthful in the sense of the murder of the victim it however, becomes deceptive about the weapon, the deception at the physical scene

32

can be linked and the weapon located through either actions or omissions in the suspects linguistic behaviour. 6.3. Interrogation and the truth

Once deception has been detected and analysed, and the suspect remains none committal to confess they should be moved towards making a confession, the matter of increasing the possibility of eliciting confessions offers cause to another research topic will be discussed briefly within this context (Russell & Coetzee 2000:42-48): 6.3.1. The subject must be told that he/she is not being truthful. The interviewer must provide a clear summary and reasons of the deception Summary must be to the point and sharp The interviewer is to have a calm demeanour 6.3.2. Notify the subject an apology will be made if the analysis showing deception is incorrect: In making this statement the interviewer must be sincere The attitude held is searching for the truth 6.3.3. Advise the subject of the lesser consequences of telling the truth The interviewer must be prepared to follow through i.e. do not make false promises in e.g. offering bail or release from detention etc. Appreciation for any truth must be shown 6.3.4. Caution the subject of the consequences of continuing with the deception The interviewer must not display any exaggeration with threats of punishment Again the interviewer must be prepared to follow through with threats of action e.g. continued detention or refusal for bail etc. 6.3.5. Ask the subject for the truth.

33

7.

FINDINGS

The use of FSA in forensic interviews can be highly effective, but it is an involved process of systematically seeking for points in the subjects psycholinguistic behaviour indicating either deception or truth. It is not well known in the courts and when used it is often provided in testimony by Forensic Psychologists or Profilers where there is reference to the suspects overall deceptive behaviour rather than focusing on FSA. Therefore,

currently it has no legal standing however, in the future it could be shown as circumstantial evidence when substantiated correctly. In addition, in obtaining the original statement (written) it remains to hold some legal questions and further avoidance has to be taken not to entrap the subject in making a statement not knowing its purpose or being appropriately warned. Conversely the investigator may record the subject with the use of a tape recorder, either covertly or overtly, this may in addition lead to legal admissibility and challenged evidence. Despite this, the advantages of FSA in that it can be used remotely i.e. the statement is obtained in one location and is sent (e-mail or fax) to the FSA analyst in another, will certainly expedite investigations as the analyst could provide a result in a relative short space of time, this being in the ideal context when one considers the amount of statements that are processed on a daily basis by the police. There does remain a need for specialist Forensic Interviewers who have included in their repertoire FSA analysis.

8.

CONCLUSION

FSA in its current format is a complex subject and requires extensive training and understanding of human behaviour, therefore, it is only conducted by a few however, as recommended additional research should attempt to identify a less complex point matrix (though there are some they are not frequently used) for the investigator out in the field to utilise to detect deception but not analyse it. With deception detected may be sent to a FSA analyst.

34

References

Adams S 1996, Statement Analysis: What do Suspects Words Really Reveal?. Califana & Levkov, 1978, Criminalistics for the Law Enforcement Officer, McGraw-Hill Book Company, New York Cloete M & Stevens R, 1990, Criminology, Southern Book Publishers, Halfway House Colwell K, Hiscock C, Memon A, 2002, Interviewing Techniques and the Assessment of Statement Credibility, Applied Cognitive Psychology 287300 (2002), Published online in Wiley InterScience 11 February 2002 (www.interscience.wiley.com) viewed on 16 March 2006 Colwell K, Hiscock C, Memon A, Yaeger H, Michlik P, Sjerven E, Strategies of Impression Management Among Deceivers and Truth-Tellers: How Liars Attempt to Convince, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Tyler Colwell K, Malingering and Deception, Presented to the Central California Psychology Internship Consortium Diestein W 1974, Technics for the Criminal Investigator, Charles Thomas, Springfield, Illinois Dowling, J 1979, Criminal Investigation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York Du Preez G. 1996, Criminal Investigation. In Forensic Criminalistics, Van der Westhuizen J. (ed), Heinemann, Johannesburg Gordon N. 2004, Validation of the Forensic Assessment Interview Technique, Criminology Master of Arts, University of South Africa Gordon N & Fleisher W. 2002, Effective Interviewing and Interrogation Techniques, Elsevier Butterwoth Heineman, Oxford Marais C. 1992, Physical Evidence in Criminal Investigation, Henmar Publications, Wierda Park Marais C. & Van Rooyen H. 1991, Crime Investigation, 2 edn, Promedia Publisher, Silverton Pistorius M, 2005, Profiling Serial Killers and other crimes in South Africa, Penguin Books, Johannesburg Rassin E, 1999, Criteria Based Content Analysis: The Less Scientific Road to Truth, Expert Evidence 7, 265-278, Kluwer Acdemic Publishers, Netherlands Russell. M & Coetzee. C 2000, Truth Extraction, Spearhead Press, Cape Town
nd

35

Smith N, 2001, Reading Between the Lines: An Evaluation of the Scientific Content Analysis Technique (SCAN), Police Research Series Paper 135, Home Office, Police and Reducing Crime Unit http://www.homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/prgpdfs/prs135.pdf viewed on line 6 March 2006. Schollum M, 2005, Investigative Interviewing: The Literature, New Zealand Police, www.police.govt.nz, viewed 16 March 2006 Swanson C, Chamelin N, Territo L, 1988, Criminal Investigation, 4 edn, Random House, New York Van der Westhuizen J. 1996 Forensic Criminalistics, 2
nd th

edn, Heinemann, Johannesburg

Vrij A, 2005, Criteria-Based Content Analysis, American Psychological Association Vol. 11. No.1, 3-41, http://0-www.springerlink.com.oasis.unisa.ac.za/ media/n2v6e3yyuk2yqjplequy/contributions/t/0/4/2/t0426707q2146jw2.pdf, viewed 9 March 2006.

36

You might also like