Neuroendocrinology Letters Special Issue, Suppl.4, Vol.23, December 2002 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X www.

nel.edu

(Evolutionary) Theories of Warfare in Preindustrial (Foraging) Societies
Johan M.G. van der Dennen Department of Legal Theory, Section Political Science, University of Groningen,
THE NETHERLANDS

REV I E W

Correspondence to: Johan M.G. van der Dennen, Ph.D. Department of Legal Theory, Section Political Science University of Groningen Oude Kijk in ’t Jatstraat 5, 9712 EA, Groningen, THE NETHERLANDS PHONE : +31 50 3635649 FA X : +31 50 3635635 EMAIL : j.m.g.van.der.dennen@rechten.rug.nl

Submitted: Accepted:

September 19, 2002 October 15, 2002
war causation; evolution; ‘primitive’ warfare; (pitched) battle; lethal male raiding; kin selection; sexual selection
pii: NEL231002R06 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters www.nel.edu

Key words:

ART ICL E

Neuroendocrinology Letters 2002; 23(Suppl.4):55–65

Abstract

I present an inventory of theories of war causation (and on the origin of war) in preindustrial (traditional, foraging, ‘primitive’, hunter-gatherer, band- and tribe-level) societies, with emphasis on the roles of natural selection, sexual selection and kin selection. Also the school of sociocultural evolution is briefly discussed.

Many anthropologists do not acknowledge a (bio)evolutionary background to war, adhering to Mead’s [1] famous dictum ‘War is only a cultural invention’ (e.g., Harris [2 to 10], Ferguson [11 to 20], Keeley [21]). Many students of war do not consider an evolutionary approach useful or relevant, even though the prevailing cultural-materialist theory is highly compatible with the evolutionary paradigm (Sanderson [22]). A variety of scholars associate the origin of war with the Mesolithic way of life. Ferguson [17] is the most outspoken advocate of this position: “In sum, the evidence is fully consistent with the conclusion that war first became a social institution in Mesopotamia some 8,000 years ago, and has been reinvented in many times and places since, and flatly inconsistent with the idea that war has been a regular occurrence throughout human history”. In its most general terms, the rationale of warfare, genocide, and other forms of collective violence may be epitomized as ‘getting rid of the competition’ or eliminating the sources of fear and terror. “War is obviously one way of gaining access to needed resources - and of eliminating potential threats to your own population or resources” (Corning [23]).

(An abbreviated version of this article appeared in Pagel M. (Editor-in-Chief) The Oxford encyclopedia of evolution. Vol. 2. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press; 2002. p.1146-1149).

55

or propensities for warfare. favor destructiveness as such. Turney-High [54] has illuminated the ‘biomechanics’ of the line which develops more or less automatically when two groups meet in an agonistic encounter and every individual organism strives to have its vulnerable flanks protected by its neighbors. sapiens) and the common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). When a battle is prearranged it is called a ‘pitched battle’. Van der Dennen [30]). van der Dennen. but always with the implicit assumption that war was (a) uniquely human. including Man (Homo sapiens sapiens). Manson & Wrangham [33]. The discovery of male coalitional aggression and ‘lethal male raiding’ in free-ranging chimpanzees (Goodall [32]. van der Dennen ([30]. and among standing armies in historical and contemporary warfare when the armies are too big to operate undetectedly.. Otterbein. 40. Phylogeny refers to the ‘ultimate’ dimension of causality: “Why has warfare. Tooby & Cosmides’ [57] ‘risk-contract’ theory (see also MalagonFajar [58]). of course. which should be distinguished from the proximate causative factors. Battle-type warfare occurs in many primate species and some other group-territorial mammals. Wrangham [34] has also suggested the distinct possibility that the chimpanzee-hominid common ancestor already had this lethal male raiding pattern in its behavioral repertoire (panid-hominid synapomorphy) some 6 million years ago. [39]).G. sexual selection and kin selection In general. a view of phylogenetic continuity. male bottle-nosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Cioffi-Revilla [31]). that all living organisms descend from other organisms that successfully mated and reproduced in past environments. Low. Cheney [36]. “Cooperation-for-conflict has probably always been the key to human survival” as Bigelow put it. Ultimately. as first proposed by Darwin [37].g. [39]). Gat [50]. 29]. it has solved the problem of warfare because this kind of ‘selfish cooperation’ (Corning) is an excellent instrument for escalated and violent intercoalition and intergroup contest competition. [29]). Otterbein. van Hooff [43]. all organisms are the products of the former strategies of their genes. Gat ([50] to [52]) is worth exploring. TurneyHigh [54]. and elaborated by many others (Meyer [24] to [27]). [44] to [49]). Wrangham [34]. Manson & Wrangham [33]..g. van der Dennen [30]) makes the conventional view of warfare as a singularly human ‘cultural invention’ of some few thousand years old increasingly untenable. Bigelow ([38]. as opposed to the mostly implicit assumption of Man’s uniqueness and the conviction that war is a one-time cultural invention. van der Dennen). including warfare. Low [28. Wrangham): continuity between humans and nonhuman primates. van der Dennen Natural selection does not. Slurink [42]. van der Dennen [30].Johan M. Wrangham & Peterson [35].g. and thus are phylogenetically related to one another. such as social carnivores. Davie [53].. Wrangham [34]. two types of warfare (broadly defined as organized intergroup or intercommunity contest competition) in animals and man have been distinguished: Raiding (‘lethal male raiding’ or ambush or dawn surprise attack). who are capable of ‘super- 56 . have evolved. Wrangham & Peterson [35]) and battle-type intergroup violence in social carnivores and a great number of primates (Cheney [36]. A great number of disciplines have traditionally studied war in contemporary as well as ‘primitive’ or preliterate.g. failed raids or failed surprise attacks and chance encounters in ‘primitive’ peoples. Bigelow ([38]. The evolutionary (or selectionist or Darwinian) paradigm proceeds from the assumption that all organisms. violent interactions. Low [28. preindustrial. evolved in the first place?” and “Why has warfare evolved in only so few species?”. at least two (brainy) species have accomplished this: Man (Homo s. and occa- sional near-genocidal routing (e. “What are the motives or conditions that led to this particular war?”. Ultimate and proximate causes are complementary rather than mutually exclusive. e. Divale [55]. but reproductive success. In social carnivores and ‘female bonded’ (or female philopatric) primate species. New Guinea). and is supposed to test the (numerical) strength of the opponent while leaving room for a more peaceful solution of the conflict by mediators – but the ritual battle can easily develop into a rout and a massacre if a substantial imbalance of power is detected by one of the parties involved (Divale. Corning [23. Tournament-like ‘ritualized’ combat is generally found among ‘advanced’ tribal societies with fairly dense populations (e. Durham [59. nonstate. Darwinian (bio)evolution: natural selection. In ‘primitive’ societies raiding is the most bloody and lethal form of warfare due to small but rapidly accumulating casualties. make evolutionary sense only if they serve reproduction (Meyer [24] to [27]). Lethal male raiding has been explained by Wrangham’s ‘imbalance-of-power’ hypothesis. or a number or series of independent inventions (e. Some Australian tribes practiced so-called ‘expiatory combats’ (comparable to medieval duels) for settling disputes (van der Dennen [61]). female participation in these – more noisy than bloody – battles commonly exceeds male participation. 60].. and Low’s [28. Until now. Instead. (b) the Multimale (or Polyadic) Coalition argument: as soon as a species has solved the problem of coordinated cooperative action by more than two individuals. and (b) a one-time cultural invention spread by diffusion. Low ([28]. The contention that war must have existed before mankind is based on (a) the Phylogenetic Continuity argument (Bigelow. Keeley [21]. 29] and van der Dennen’s [30] sexual selection approach (vide infra). Battles result mainly from chance encounters by primate groups. Otterbein [56]). band-level and tribal (hunter-gatherer and horticulturalist) societies. 29]. and that – ultimately – all organisms have come in one uninterrupted chain from the first simple multiplying cells. and battle (confrontation of two opposing lines or phalanxes). 41] among others.

e. among others. Melotti ([78] to [81]). Falger & Vine [63].edu 57 . Warfare would be the best alternative for the group that practiced it successfully. live in closed social groups containing multiple females. The rationale for groups to compete as groups has been illuminated by Pitt [91]. among chimpanzees (a male philopatric species). designed for coalitional warfare. Wilson). Vol. and (2) sexual access to women will be the primary benefit that men gain from joining male coalitions. and Neuroendocrinol Lett Special Issue. and kill their rivals as do men” (Ghiglieri [74]). mate polygynously.nel. it is suggested. Under these conditions. restrict their ranging to a communal territory. it makes better evolutionary sense for the troops to determine ownership of the resources as groups. warfare. are a serious third candidate. and (e) violent group conflict. when a neighbouring community weakens. Trivers [72].. This is the imbalanceof-power hypothesis.. males of the chimpanzee-bonobo-human clade retain their male offspring predominantly.g. Van der Dennen. assuming it to have been within their biological reach. with most other primate (and social carnivore) species in which the females have more ‘vested interests’ in the defense of their lineage and the integrity of their group territory. Especially. men have been able to gain reproductively by warring behavior. Bigelow.g. genocide or genosorption strongly favoring the aggressor need take place only once every few generations to direct evolution. rather than having both conflict and decreased inclusive fitness (which would accompany a merger). Bigelow. will have evolved psychological mechanisms (‘Darwinian algorithms’). which suggests that coalitional killing is the expression of a drive for dominance over neighbors.4. Dow [73]. there is usually a sharp drop in relatedness. some ethnocentric xenophobia between groups would be selectively favored in a potentially hostile and violently competitive environment (e. Suppl. and.23. If conflict is inevitable.War in Preindustrial Societies coalitions’ for the purpose of forcefully monopolizing females. Two conditions are proposed to be both necessary and sufficient to account for coalitional killing of neighbors: (1) a state of intergroup hostility. this drop may be such as to promote active hostility between neighbouring groups” (Hamilton [67]). although in the bonobo (Pan paniscus) some intergroup agonistic behavior but no lethal raiding (nor communal hunting) has been observed. it is easy to see that the warprone group would be the most successful. This strategy may be a pattern common to the chimpanzeebonobo-human (or HUCHIBO) clade: “Unlike gorillas and orangutans. Tooby & Cosmides. (2) sufficient imbalances of power between parties that one party can attack the other with impunity. women have almost never been able to do so” (Low [28]). apparently. Low. pre-state warrior societies. 70]). i. as we have seen. human urban gangs. Wrangham [34] presents the principal adaptive hypothesis for explaining the species distribution of intergroup coalitional killing. Alexander [82 to 90]. Wrangham & Peterson note that the underlying psychology of ‘male bonding’ is no different for chimpanzee raiding parties. selection favors the tendency to hunt and kill rivals when the costs are suf- ficiently low. the males of some communities make a concerted strategic effort to stalk. (c) violent conflict between individuals. Assuming that different groups tended towards one or another of these strategies. arguably. and contemporary armies. Hamilton [65 to 67]. groupterritoriality and female transfer has been singled out as the starting condition for lethal intergroup violence (Goodall. (b) peaceful competition between groups with the losers starving.. via either incorporation of neighboring females or encroachment on the territory of neighboring groups and elimination of numerically weaker males. Accordingly. Shaw & Wong [64]. Manson & Wrangham. This contrasts. exacerbated by the sharply decreasing (genetic) kinship at the groups’ boundaries: “At the boundary of the local group. ‘proto-ethnocentrism’. Given the chimpanzee evidence. even as a parental investment strategy). in varying degrees. in Low’s words: “Through evolutionary history. Or. Wilson [69. For male organisms females are generally the limiting resource: for human males women are the highly strategic ‘good’ (always in short supply) that can convert the other resources controlled by the males into offspring (Borgia [77]. Ghiglieri. Tooby & Cosmides enumerate some significant implications of their Risk Contract of War: (1) men. Additionally. Reproductive success is the only criterion in the currency of evolution. (d) scramble competition. Wrangham. van der Dennen. Male and female organisms have evolved different strategies for the optimalization of their reproductive success because what for the one sex is a highway to genetic immortality is for the other sex a one-way ticket to genetic oblivion (paraphrasing Symons). van der Dennen. van den Berghe [62]. The question why males are the warriors in raidingtype warfare has been addressed by Symons [71]. Symons). Slurink. To understand this – for some undoubtedly extravagant – claim. are cooperatively active in territorial defence. McEachron & Baer [91] and Baer & McEachron [92]. Wrangham & Peterson. It has been noted by many scholars that intergroup competition can be a potent and relentless selective force without high levels of violence and killing (e. Irwin [68]. the combination of male-male cooperation. and Ghiglieri [74 to 76]. Manson & Wrangham proposed that such imbalance-of-power mechanism must have been an important factor favoring the evolution of lethal male violence in humans also – and even before the evolution of weapons. but not women. Reynolds. Their answers are remarkably compatible: raiding-type warfare evolved as a high-risk/high-gain malecoalitional reproductive strategy (or. “By current theory. Several scenarios can be pictured in intergroup contest competition: (a) peaceful coexistence or merging of the groups. attack. Wrangham). the ultimate benefit of lethal male raiding has been hypothesized to be increased access by raiding males to reproductively valuable females. December 2002 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X www. the following observations are relevant. This alone could push truly altruistic genes to a high frequency within the bands” (Wilson [69]).

not to Darwinian (bio)evolution. they were able to kill adult conspecifics by surprise. Gat). Wilson. van der Dennen). (c) Hereditary chiefs and primordial warrior society. which in turn required (b) sociality (intergroup conflict will occur only in long-lived social species: Low [28]). and political warfare as distinct progressive social-evolutionary stages. To these levels of sociopolitical organization correspond the levels of military organization as distinguished by Feest [125]: (a) War-chiefs on the basis of reputation. Morgan [109]. the ability to recognize ingroup versus outgroup members. (c) Machiavellian (opportunistic) intelligence. Indeed. which Darwin himself liked to call “descent with modification”. the predominant school to the end of the 19th century. but these refer to altogether different phenomena and may cause considerable confusion).. Unlike other animal species.G. and glory”). Similarly. and the state. and ‘adaptation’ are used by both bio-evolutionists and sociocultural evolutionists. Sociocultural evolution: stages in sociopolitical complexity The basic motives and practices of ‘primitive’ war were already known to the classical historians such as Herodotus and Tacitus (Turney-High. van der Dennen). Van der Dennen’s investigation of the evolutionary origins of intergroup conflict in social carnivores and primates identified (a) the capability to form polyadic coalitions (selfish and opportunistic cooperation with more than one conspecific) as the necessary precondition. Low. tribe. it will be the warmongers whose genes are represented in the next generation. Also Fried [120 to 122] and Hunter & Whitten [123] identified four stages or levels of ‘sociopolitical’ evolution: egalitarian society. economic. Richerson [94] advances what he calls the ‘evolutionary tragedy’ hypothesis: Warfare is liable to evolve even if it makes everybody worse off. in which he concluded: “We had occasion to observe that in every rude state the great business is war. Symons. and from a substantial distance by throwing projectiles (Pitt. Diametrically opposed views were espoused by Rousseau [106] in his Le Contrat Social. Steward [111] and some others saw in cultural evolution a multilineal phenomenon). The concept of evolution as an ordering principle in cultural anthropology was proposed about 1840. ingroup altruism. A century later Sahlins [112 to 115] and Service [116 to 118] proposed a scheme of social evolution in four stages: the band. Plainly. War. Tylor [110]. is warfare itself. But this referred to (Lamarckian. Bingham [95]. Baer & McEachron. It is the perversion of the situation (the logic of the war ‘game’) rather than that of the actors involved. and state-level society (more or less equivalent with the band.g. the tribe. One of the first accounts in Europe of ‘primitive’ cannibalism and warfare was Hans Staden’s [103] story of his life among the Brazilian Tupinamba (though some doubt has risen about its authenticity). stratified society. Spencerian) sociocultural evolution. even before Darwin’s Origin of Species [107]. to discriminate between these categories. Low. (d) Full-blown military societies (or fraterni- 58 . Evolutionism. and to preferentially treat ingroup members to positive reciprocal (altruistic) interactions such as protection. chiefdom and state stages). Proto-ethnocentrism is supposed to imply some kind of group identity. either defensive or offensive. when their adversaries were unarmed and vulnerable. This sequence is assumed to represent a cultural-evolutionary development. in which he introduced the concept of the ‘Noble Savage’ who did not wage war simply because there were no (material) benefits to be gained by waging war (This so-called Hobbes-Rousseau controversy. intergroup antagonism. has dominated the anthropological literature until today). rank society.e. and/or his access to nubile women (e. and intergroup agonistic behavior (i.Johan M. Van der Dennen’s evolutionary scenario or ‘evolutionario’ also outlines the phylogenetic and socio-ecological principles governing group formation. being generally divided into smaller parties. are engaged in almost perpetual hostilities”. (b) Dual leadership: formal peace. through a barbarian stage. (The terms ‘evolution’. subjugation and genocide). Quincy Wright [124] distinguished social. to civilization (Spencer [108]. the only possible competitive strategy for survival in competition with a group practicing warfare. Most of the many factors that favor the reproductive potential of cooperative people and good warriors can be grouped under two categories: (1) the genetic effects of increased Lebensraum (territory and resources) and (2) the genetic effects of polygyny (Bigelow. “has been the great business of mankind since time immemorial”. the chiefdom. that is. territorial expansion.and warchiefs. nepotism. Humans thus became quintessentially first-strike creatures. Hamilton. conquest. with concomitant changes in warfare patterns and motives (from what Muehlmann [119] and Meyer called endemic feuds and wars of retaliation to war as an instrument of predation and plunder. a persistent and irreconcilable one. and (d) proto-ethnocentrism.. Hawkes [102]. and that in barbarous times. assumed a linear and progressive conception of evolution and history: human societies advance from the simple to the complex. diffidence. and sharing of resources. The nature of warfare as it is conducted at each of these levels appears to differ in a systematic way. mankind. van der Dennen). ‘function’. Chagnon. war and its non-human equivalent). from the ‘savage’ primitive horde. Among many hunting-gathering peoples a man’s quality as a warrior and his hunting prowess is directly related to the number of wives he can obtain. In 1767 Adam Ferguson [104] published An Essay on the History of Civil Society (the first attempt at an empirical cross-cultural study). van der Dennen could indeed overrun any group attempting to practice one of the other strategies. This conclusion neatly confirmed Hobbes’ [105] gloomy view of the war-ridden condition of primitive society (the causes of which he identified as “competition. outgroup antagonism.

nor seizure of slaves nor plunder of economic goods is characteristic of primitive warfare” and “the more primitive the people the less warlike it tends to be”. Fiji was seldom without war. middle. cannibalism. they have frequently killed off or enslaved the males of their vanquished opponents and preempted the women” (Corning [40]). i. and reproduction is the root cause of conflict and fighting in humans as in all other animal species. Carneiro). and unending” [131]. spiritual) evolution: the Agent of Progress. it has been observed time and again that states make war and war makes states. Also. but one of social organization. Chagnon. as secondary and only reinforcing other trends (e. and Gat could confirm recently that ‘primitive’ warfare (among hunter-gatherers.100 years ago (Sanderson [22]). Hobhouse. who thought that primitive warfare was so desultory because it was so thoroughly uneconomic. and finally (e) Standing armies (see also Andreski [126]. the role played by war in the creation of more complex societies is divided between those who see it as a prime mover (e. scalping. a perpetual struggle for territory and power.g. In a 1978 paper Carol Ember [145] (definitively?) shattered “the myth about peaceful hunter-gatherers”. lower. van der Bij [133]. or as one of a set of interacting variables (e.edu 59 . territory. and sometimes even genocidal business: guerre à l’outrance (due to rapidly accumulating casualties in raiding and routing). beginning in Mesopotamia around 5. Other causes and expressions of fighting in nature. Revenge.g.4. lower and higher hunters. Otterbein. Group extinctions due to chronic warfare between (horticultural) village communities are quite common in New Guinea. the obligations of the blood feud and other ‘magico-religious’ motives (cf. trophy taking. December 2002 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X www. See Table I for the main theories of ‘primitive’ war.. closely followed by status. they strongly suggest that there is a process involved. Wheeler & Ginsburg [132]. chiefdoms can put together fighting forces in the hundreds or thousands. bloody. This is a fortiori the case of states and empires.. war and intergroup conflict have been the principal factors of human progress. human sacrifice. A concomitant (mainly social-Darwinist) theory in this connection is the view that struggle. and Quincy Wright were the first to apply crude statistics to their cross-cultural sample of some 650 distinctive ‘primitive’ societies. abduction of women. then.. Amazonia.War in Preindustrial Societies ties). Ferguson). Turney-High. Among contemporary scholars.nel. Few primitive societies had reached what he called the ‘military horizon’. or that war is the prime mover of human (cultural.23. placating ancestral spirits. and in the Cauca Valley “warfare was universal. and found quite unambiguously that “belligerence is a concomitant of civilization”. as conscious. van der Dennen. Leeds [144].e. Ferguson) also postulate realistic group conflict about material interests. and the motivational and emotional mechanisms associated with them. Broch & Galtung [146] reanalyzed Wright’s data by multivariate analysis techniques. The roots of the materialist school sprouted in the 1940s when a number of anthropologists reinterpreted Plains Indian. As Gat reasoned: “The interconnected competition over resources. these primary causes. Quite a different tradition was started by Steinmetz [134. Fried). moral. prestige and glory. He notes that warfare among chiefdoms in these regions was nearly constant. Keeley. vital or strategic resources such as land and game (high-quality protein). and higher agriculturists. status and prestige. and scarce resources (including the never-ending search for security) are the main proximate causes or motives of war and feuding in ‘primitive’ societies reported in the literature. Divale. Van der Dennen... women. and Zulu warfare in thoroughly economic terms. Carneiro studied chiefdomlevel warfare in the Cauca Valley of Colombia and in Fiji. horticulturalists. Ferguson. warfare is depicted as a strategy to secure scarce. The absence of ‘economic’ motives in the warfare patterns of primitive societies (at least the hunter-gatherers) was also emphasized by Turney-High. “The military horizon depends. as Low suggested. are derivative of. Keeley. Wright concluded that 95% of his sample were warlike peoples (which seems the confirm the notion of universal belligerence). not upon the adequacy Neuroendocrinol Lett Special Issue. Carneiro [128 to 131] noted that whereas smallscale band-level and tribal societies are usually capable of putting together warrior bands of a few dozen at most. Harris. etc. and subordinate to. Turney-High repeatedly emphasized that the evolution of warfare is not simply a matter of (weapons) technology. The evolution of historical war may be succinctly described as the transformation of armed men into manned arms. and lower and higher pastorals. Suppl. Iroquoian. 13]). i. Gat). while the reproductive rewards have become increasingly ‘unhooked’ from the warring behavior. 135] (who retorted to van der Bij’s finding that primitive peoples are unwarlike by stating that primitive peoples are primitive precisely because they are unwarlike) and especially Davie [53] who emphasized sanguinary war for plunder. in the sense that increasing civilization would lead to increasing warlikeness or bellicosity.) (Davie. Ferguson [11. Vol. “Conquerors have usually been very generous with their genes. although also acknowledging ‘non-economic’ motives such as revenge. and originally evolved this way in humans as well”. The contemporary schools of (multi)functionalism and (eco)materialism (Vayda [136 to 143]. as well as distinguishing levels of ‘economic culture’. deliberate and violent struggles over material resources. The formation of the state in codified history represents a remarkable process of parallel evolution. and other regions where feuds and wars are endemic.e. etc. acute. simple agriculturalists) and prehistoric warfare is generally a lethal. Though their data are synchronic rather than diachronic..g. and supernatural or magico-religious motives (such as headhunting. Other important conclusions were: “Neither territorial conquest. [127]). territorial conquest.

and related early ‘Instinct of Pugnacity’ formulations by James and McDougall [205]. ‘Killer Ape’ popularizations: Dart [202]. incorporated. Ferguson. [203]. Alexander. ♦ Warfare and sexual selection (sexual – or reproductive – competition and the evolution of a ‘male coalitional psychology’. chiefdom. Andreski. Bernard. Multifunctionalists envisage not only the adjustment of the man/land ratio due to warfare. Benedict [151] and Mead. Muehlmann and others (e. Goodall. Lumsden & Wilson [217]. Meyer and Wilson regard warfare as well as ethnocentrism as cultural hypertrophications of biological predispositions. Service. Goodall. Alexander & Tinkle. Corning. [3]). (4) (Bio)evolutionary theories. Feest. Boulding [153].g. Vayda & Leeds. ranging from strictly genic selectionists (Durham) to group selectionists. Knauft ([183] to [185]).g. War as (evolved from the) man hunt: Frobenius. without taking the ultimate dimension into consideration). van der Dennen. Wilson. A typical proposition of this school is: “Primitive warfare arose as part of a complex system that prevented human populations from exceeding the carrying capacity of their habitats” (Harris. Ferguson. Boyd & Richerson [215]. economic. “Fear of hostile foreigners has probably always been the most effective promotor of social unity among related bands of people” (Bigelow. Eibl-Eibesfeldt particularly emphasizes the role of indoctrination in creating the ‘warrior-type’ personality. Fried. Leakey & Lewin [154].G. Wrangham.. These last approaches form a transition to the (bio)evolutionary theories. This body of theories is predicated upon the assumption of phylogenetic continuity. Malinowski [176]. Kelly [190]) proposed that war probably originated in and evolved from the blood feud. Meyer. Starr [160]. [129]): White [158]. Davie. Bigelow. subjugated. “Group aggression confers such a huge winning edge against single competitors that. among others. White [158]. Chagnon. Chagnon. This programmatic and dogmatic rigor has done more harm than good (e. not individuals. and the so-called ‘male supremacist complex’ (Harris. Intimate relationships have been proposed between ♦ Warfare and group territoriality: Tinbergen [192]. ♦ Warfare and kin selection (ethnocentrism-cum-xenophobia): van den Berghe.000 years ago are the common themes in all general history textbooks: e. Most or all theorists of these school acknowledge sociocultural evolution but do not see any special role for ‘biology’ (which is usually conceived of as ‘genes and hormones’. kin selection instantly forged it into the most serious weapon in any male’s behavioral arsenal” (Ghiglieri [76]). Tiger & Fox [223]. ♦ Warfare and hominid/human brain evolution (trebling of human brain due to relentless groups competition): Darwin. (Multi)functionalists are Vayda. ‘Warfare is only a cultural invention’ (Mead [1]) and ‘War is not in our genes’ are the best known adages of this inventionist (Boasian) school which begins with Boas [149] and his students Dewey [150]. The warrior-type personality may be viewed as a product of sexual selection. 60 . Borgia. Leeds [168]. Hunter & Whitten. Frobenius [198]. McEachron & Baer. and warfare as plunder writtenlarge concomitant with the rise of the first states and empires some 5. Thienpont & Cliquet [220]. and sociopolitical variables. Washburn & Lancaster [199]. and spread mainly by cultural diffusion). The masculine ‘hunting mystique’ has lost much of its appeal lately. van der Dennen. Richerson & Boyd [216].g. [189]. Pitt. Hamilton and Wilson envisaged a ‘stepping stone’ model of territorial aggrandizement and genosorption. “the less well adapted tend to fall by the wayside. van der Dennen. and thus (bio)evolution is commonly rejected as irrelevant (e.g. warfare. critic of this school is Hallpike [170]. Boehm [186]. and the Marxist-Leninists. Adams [225]. sociologists.. Corning. McNeill [152]. Reynolds et al. meaning that both our biological and sociocultural evolution are acknowledged. cf. [222]. or eliminated other human groups): Darwin. Keeley). Masters [209]. preferential female infanticide. A typical proposition of this school is: As societies compete. Meyer envisaged a sociocultural development from endemic war (for metaphysical power) to its instrumentalization (for material power). Wrangham & Peterson have taken chimpanzee ‘lethal male raiding’ into account in explaining human raiding. war for women). Muehlmann. Tooby & Cosmides. [174]. Lee & DeVore [200]. Davie.Johan M. Melotti.. [101]). Steward. Symons. [175]). ♦ Warfare and hunting (or ‘Carnivorous Psychology’) theories: James [196]. Ardrey [204]. and sarcastic. Ehrenreich [226]. population pressure in relation to ecological and/or climate changes) in the causation of war: Sumner [164]. This is the prevailing paradigm among cultural anthropologists. Steward. Dyer [161]. (2) (Eco)materialist and related (multi)functionalist theories. (3) Sociocultural selection and evolution of warfare theories (Evolutionism for short). Slurink. with sexual selectionists and kin selectionists somewhere in between. Wilson. Keeley. Andreski. Divale [171]. or in Vayda’s terminology: War functions to adjust the man/land ratio. Alexander. ♦ Warfare and human (ultra)sociality and balance-of-power theorists (human groups as predators upon one another. [165] Sumner & Keller [166]. [197]. tribe. Keith [208]. [193]. Tiger [221]. Andreski. ♦ Warfare and group selection (during specifically human evolution. These theorists are a heterogeneous lot. Bronowski [159]. Meyer. Sahlins. Its explanation is therefore social or cultural. Pfeiffer [201]. Eibl-Eibesfeldt ([210] to [214]). Turney-High. General criticisms by Hallpike and Robarchek [188]. Wilson. Holsti [194]. Harris. Warfare as one-time invention (or series of inventions. Schneider ([155] to [157]). Trivers. van Hooff. the diffusionists. Bigelow. Shaw & Wong. Baer & McEachron. Bouthoul. Manson & Wrangham.. leaving outstanding those best able to withstand the competition” (Carneiro. [38]). White (cf. van der Dennen. Lathrap [167]. Wittfogel [163]. Turney-High. [218]. not psychological”. but also the regulation of psychological. Alexander. once it entered the arms race of sexual selection. human groups have continuously replaced. Ghiglieri. Lopreato’s [191] ‘biocultural’ approach roots war and general human nastiness in evolved behavioral predispositions of self-enhancement. Most students of this school adhere to the band. Alcock [195]. Pitt. Leeds and Vayda [169]. and gene-culture co-evolutionary or ‘dual inheritance’ models. Melotti. Low. Kroeber & Fontana [227]). Divale & Harris [173].. Lorenz [207]. Scott [206] proposed that early hominids did not evolve as formidable hunters and warriors but as timid scavengers and ‘fear biters’. [172]. and macrohistorians. Otterbein ([177] to [181]). van der Dennen Table I: Theories of ‘primitive’ warfare The main theories of ‘primitive’ warfare can be categorized (more or less arbitrarily and overlapping) as follows: (1) Warfare as cultural invention and macroparasitism. Newcomb [187]) claimed that “Warfare is a struggle between social organisms. Dawson [162] Cioffi-Revilla. A great number of these theorists are true heirs of Malthus in emphasizing the demographic factor (overpopulation. Falger [219]. Gat (Goldstein [224] discusses ‘War and Gender’ without invoking the evolutionary dimension. The most caustic. Divale et al. One offshoot of this school tries to relate population regulation. Naroll & Divale [182]. after the rise of human groups to ecological dominance): Alexander. Bigelow. state sequence of sociocultural stages (see text).

5 Harris M. 1980. Ansätze zu einer bio-soziologischen Synthese. New York: Gordon & Breach. 8 Harris M. essays in honor of Charles Wagley. editors. p. (1975) Culture. 24 Meyer P. Materialist. Warfare. territorial aggrandizement. people. New York: Harper & Row. But if contemporary terrorism. then we may be going ‘back to the future’.26–55. Anthropological theory. Warfare.nel. irredentist and ethnonational or ethnopolitical wars are. J Anthropol Res 1984. 12 Ferguson RB. and campaigns for plunder. 27 Meyer P. War in the tribal zone: expanding states and indigenous warfare. Discipline and coordination in battle provide the watershed between warrior and soldier. Origins and evolution of war and politics. The Yanomamö and the cause of war in band and village societies. 1998. Triangle Inst for Security Studies. p.4. man and nature. 1995. p. the means of production. 2001. New York: Columbia Univ Press. Ann Arbor: Univ. editor. and probably will be. 1994. The evolution of human sociality: a darwinian conflict perspective. The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. 3 Harris M. The Hague: Mouton. Yearbook Phys Anthropol 1999. War before civilization: the myth of the peaceful savage. 1996. Chapter in: Harris M. Asia 1940. In: Van der Dennen JMG Falger VSE. Human nature and the function of war in social evolution: a critical review of the naturalistic fallacy. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press. 1979. In: Ferguson RB. Neuroendocrinol Lett Special Issue. Santa Fe: School of American Research. Vol. Anthropol Quart 2000. Evolution of coalitionary killing. p. Thus armies have been characterized by increasing hierarchization of command structure. 11 Ferguson RB. Hist. 9 Harris M. 1977. 2001. culture and environment. 17 Ferguson RB. Suppl. Culture. 40:402–5. 1992. culture and environment. Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. 25 Meyer P. New York: Oxford Univ Press. and especially since September 11. 1. Yanomami warfare: a political history. and will continue to shape human sociocultural evolution (Schmookler [148]. 14 Ferguson RB. 1975. p. War has become an increasingly maladaptive and cost/benefit suboptimal solution to political problems. pigs. Culture. Anthropological perspectives on war. editors. Evolution und Gewalt. p. (1971) Ecology. 10 Harris M. Animal capture and Yanomamö warfare: retrospect and new evidence. ‘security’ and ‘ethnic nationalism’ (secessionist. In: Cauchy V. p. Nat. Princeton: Princeton Univ Press. Explaining war. 3rd ed. editors. its causes and correlates. of Michigan Press. 34 Wrangham RW. Do Yanomamo killers have more kids? Amer Ethnol 1989. and empire. Cannibals and kings: the origins of cultures. Introduction: studying war. Cows. 1990. 73:159–64.23. New York: Academic Press. 22 Sanderson SK. 28 Low BS. and command working along certain simple [tactical] principles”. 1981. 30 Van der Dennen JMG. 1984.121–32. Hamburg: Parey Verlag. 1986/1987. The origin of war: the evolution of a male-coalitional reproductive strategy. 40:183–201. In: Margolis M Carter W. Whitehead NL.359–87. Kriegs. editor. Warfare old and new. self-discipline takes the place of menos. 16 Ferguson RB. In: Ferguson RB. Santa Fe: School of American Research. editors. 18 Ferguson RB. editors. 2 Harris M.und Militärsoziologie. Groningen: Origin Press. London: Chapman and Hall.227–40. Why sex matters: a darwinian look at human behavior. In: Nettleship MA Givens RD Nettleship A. 29 Low BS. Cambridge (MA): Harvard Univ Press. and conflict in world politics. 32:369–77. 2000. and the means of destruction have largely shaped. 1975. 7 Harris M. culture. 31 Cioffi-Revilla C.War in Preindustrial Societies of weapons but the adequacy of team work. New York: Thomas Crowell. supremacy. December 2002 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X www. 32 Goodall J. Sage. 40:1–22. A savage encounter: Western contact and the Yanomami war complex. cooperation. An evolutionary paradigm for the study of human aggression.111–40. 15 Ferguson RB. 1974. 1972. 1995. p. editor. The anthropology of war. wars and witches: the riddles of culture. In: Haas J. 1971. 1993. New York: Thomas Crowell. A cultural materialist theory of band and village warfare: the Yanomamo test. Current Anthropol 1991. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. particularly sweet water. editor. 2001. Sanderson). mainly intranational). 13 Ferguson RB. 6 Harris M. the means of reproduction. the state of warrior frenzy” (Vernant [147]). In: Reyna SP Downs RD.99–116. Behavior. 1990. demography and war. p. 2nd ed. 20 Ferguson RB. man and nature. 81:18–20. New York: Random House. not a biological necessity. 1977. cultural and biological theories on why Yanomami make war. 1995. War in the tribal zone: expanding states and indigenous warfare.1–81. In: Reyna SP Wilson RA. editors.edu 61 . p. New York: Random House. nature: an introduction to general anthropology. New York: Academic Press. 19 Ferguson RB. warlordism and ‘low-level conflict’ would prove to be indeed the threats they are said to constitute since World War II. In: Ferguson RB Whitehead NL. Santa Fe: School for American Research Press.13–56. editors. p. New York: Vintage Books.199–228. Paper for ‘Study of War’ volume. organization. oil. 1978. violence and warfare. But the phalanx implies a radical transformation of the warrior ethos: Collective discipline takes the place of individual exploits and sophrosune. 1984. Violence and human coexistence. guerrilla warfare. Warfare is only an invention. An evolutionary perspective on war. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield. Brazil: an anthropological perspective. 23 Corning PA. 33 Manson JH. 1992. The evolution of warfare: origins of cultural variation. but will in this century probably be waged for scarce resources. War. editors. The psychology of the warrior gave way in western history when warfare changed from guerrilla-like raids and ambushes to massive battle formations: “The first phalanx might have comprised a small elite group of fighting men and assured these specialists in warfare of success in the works of Ares. 26 Meyer P. The causes and origins of ‘primitive warfare’: on evolved emotions for war. Studying war: anthropological perspectives. Internat Stud Quart 1996. Montréal: Editions Montmorency. 4 Harris M. fighting phalanx-type battles of ever increasing size. München: Goldmann. BIBLIOGRAPHY 1 Mead M. 42:1–30. The general consequences of war: an Amazonian perspective.16–22.85–112. 16:564–5. Wrangham RW. In: Zimmerman W Jacobson HK. Thus. Ehrenreich. and political subordination: power. 21 Keeley LH. Vol.

Columbia: Univ. small-scale. London: Dent. and derivative causes. The dawn warriors: man’s evolution towards peace. Paper Conference Association for Politics and the Life Sciences. 55 Divale WT. 8:265–73. 1998. New York: McGraw-Hill. 60 Durham WH. peace. Cambridge (MA): Harvard Univ Press. Cosmides L. Resource competition and human aggression. 76 Ghiglieri MP. editors. 1949/1971. In: Thienpont K Cliquet R. Human aggression as a biological adaptation. 66 Hamilton WD. Ecological dominance and the final sprint in hominid evolution. 62 Van den Berghe PL. 24:379–92. 1996. 38 Bigelow R. 1. 51 Gat A. The pattern of fighting in simple. Clan and tribal conflict. p. p.97–115. In: Wind J Reynolds V. discrimination. New York: Berghahn Books. Peterson D. 1980. The synergism hypothesis: a theory of progressive evolution. 1987.151–185. Wong Y. 43 Van Hooff JARAM. Biosocial anthropology. its causes and correlates. Brussels: NIDI GBGS Publ.289–96. Hubris. p. 1973. 1986. South Carolina Press. Proc Inst Evol Studies 1988. editors. editor. The dark side of man: tracing the origins of male violence.267–81. 1979. editors. editors. cooperation. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. Origin and evolution of ‘primitive’ warfare. Oxford: Freeman. USA. V. The ethnic phenomenon. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press. The Mankind Quart 1985. 1999. Boston: Little. 1998. 73:74–88. editor. 1999. 53 Davie MR. 1987. In: Falger VSE Meyer P van der Dennen JMG. London: Croom Helm. Narrow roads of gene land. 46 Van der Dennen JMG. 2001. 75 Ghiglieri MP. p. 77 Borgia G. discrimination. J Anthropol Res 1999. and conflict. Primitive war: its practice and concepts. 1990. 1985. 61 Van der Dennen JMG. Vine I. Also in: Eibl-Eibesfeldt I Salter FK. Human Evol 1993. JMG. Santa Barbara: Clio Press. Intergroup competition and conflict in animals and man. Stamford CT: JAI Press. London: Murray. Of badges. San Diego: Academic Press. Competition and cooperation in human evolution. 56 Otterbein KF. Cambridge MA: Harvard Univ. In: Dyson-Hudson R Little M. The evolution of social behaviour: Vol. racism and nationalism. 37 Darwin CR. 1978. Towards a coevolutionary theory of human biology and culture. and selection in relation to sex. p. London: Chapman and Hall. Vol. The Inuit and the evolution of limited group conflict. The descent of man.150–85. 1981. p.c. 88:1–15. 1871. 1975. In: Smuts BB Cheney DL Seyfarth RM Wrangham RW Struhsaker TT. Paper US Army War College. Encyclopedia of violence. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. p. 67 Hamilton WD. Innate social aptitudes of man: an approach from evolutionary genetics. In: Van der Dennen JMG Falger VSE. violence and warfare. 71 Symons D. 59 Durham WH. New York: Elsevier. Reading (MA): Perseus Books. 57 Tooby J. Demonic males: apes and the origins of human violence. The evolution of war and its cognitive foundations. War. f. 36 Cheney DL. Brown. Menlo Park: Benjamin/Cummings. editors. The darwinian heritage and sociobiology. 1999. warriors and evolution. Selection of selfish and altruistic behavior in some extreme models. cooperation. Wilson DR. editors. editors. editors. 1990. 69 Wilson EO. 1975. The evolution of human sexuality.Johan M. In: Reynolds V Falger VSE Vine I. 1989. 78 Melotti U. 68 Irwin CJ. Westport CT: Praeger/Greenwood Press. In: Chagnon NA Irons W. 51:385–415. 70 Wilson EO. 16:319–58. London: Chapman and Hall. Falger VSE.149–88. Smillie D. pre-state societies. editors. The Hague: Mouton.23–54.69–82. North Scituate: Duxbury Press. 41 Corning PA. The origins of war: evolution and intergroup violence in animals and man. p. 25:323–51. subordinate. Indoctrinability. Vol. 47 Van der Dennen. Oct 18–21. Synergy goes to war: an evolutionary theory of collective violence. 1983. 44 Van der Dennen JMG. London: Croom Helm. 1. J Human Evol 1987. 73 Dow J. Essays in human sociobiology.39–58. Press. 73:165–8. editors. The causes and origins of ‘primitive warfare’: reply to Ferguson. War among the chimps. 49 Van der Dennen JMG.235–61. In: Eisenberg JF Dillon WS. 48 Van der Dennen JMG. editors. editors. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. The politics of peace in preindustrial societies: the adaptive rationale behind corroboree and calumet.G. 8:67–76. In: Van der Dennen JMG Falger VSE. Boulder: Westview Press. In: Kurtz LR. p. London: Unwin Hyman. 58 Malagon-Fajar M. editors. racism and nationalism. Sociobiology of the great apes and the hominid ancestor. The sociobiology of ethnocentrism: evolutionary dimensions of xenophobia. The human motivational complex: evolutionary theory and the causes of hunter-gatherer fighting. Carlisle Barracks. New Haven: Yale Univ Press. Social evolution.37–74. Sociobiology: the new synthesis. violence and warfare. In-group/out-group behaviour in modern societies: an evolutionary perspective. Ethnocentrism and in-group/out-group differentiation: a review and interpretation of the literature. Part 1: Primary somatic and reproductive causes. Part 2: Proximate. editors. p. p. editor. 1999. p. nationalism. Westport CT: Greenwood Press. In: Fox R. cooperation. 1990. Chicago: Univ Chicago Press. Rethinking human adaptation: biological and cultural models. Women capture as a motivation for warfare: a comparative analysis of intra-cultural variation and a critique of the male supremacist complex. editors. ideology and warfare: evolutionary perspectives. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. violence and warfare. The origin of human aggression: a new evolutionary view. 45 Van der Dennen JMG.133–55. 73:20–34. p. London: Chapman and Hall. Discover 1987. 1975. 1983. 62 . editors. In: Nettleship MA Givens RD Nettleship A. The sociobiology of ethnocentrism: evolutionary dimensions of xenophobia. Primate societies. 1990.57–92. The role of competition and cooperation in human aggression. 54 Turney-High HH. 63 Reynolds. violence and warfare. Genetic seeds of warfare: evolution. Falger VSE. 65 Hamilton WD. van der Dennen 35 Wrangham RW. and patriotism. Interactions and relationships between groups. 55:563–83. 74 Ghiglieri MP. In: Van der Dennen JMG Falger VSE. The Mankind Quart 1984. Man and beast: comparative social behavior.159–192. In: Lockard JS. 40 Corning PA.1–47.189–226. Washington DC: Smithsonian Inst Press. Sociobiology and politics. bonds and boundaries: ingroup/out-group differentiation and ethnic conflict revisited. p. cooperation. 64 Shaw RP. Warfare in primitive societies: a bibliography. 52 Gat A. Part 1: A review of primitive war. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. Anthropol Quart 2000. 1996. p. On the evolution of human aggressiveness. 50 Gat A. Quart Rev Biol 1976. 6. The evolution of war: a study of its role in early societies. On human nature. 1987. Charleston. 42 Slurink P. 1969. 1929. 1999. 72 Trivers RL. 80 Melotti U. Research in biopolitics. The evolution of human social behavior. Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: an anthropological perspective. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 1971. London: Chapman and Hall. 79 Melotti U. Anthropol Quart 2000. 39 Bigelow R. p. Brussels: VUB Press. 1979.

105 Hobbes T. 93 Baer D. 19 of Richerson’s Human Ecology course at the Univ of California at Davis. 130 Carneiro RL. (facsimile of original 1557). Seattle: Univ Washington Press. K. and the evolution of society. McEachron DL. 1915. J Soc & Biol Structures 1982. 97 Chagnon NA. In: Banton M. 109 Morgan LH. Proc Roy Soc Victoria 1971. Genetics of criminal and antisocial behavior. violence and warfare. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. editor. 117 Service ER. Part 1: The origins of culture. Tinkle DW. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Origins of the state and civilization: the process of cultural evolution. 122 Fried MH. Leipzig: JA Barth. 110 Tylor EB. or. A theory of the origin of the state. Military organization and society. p. 92 McEachron DL. Ann Rev Ecol & Syst 1974. New York: Appleton. The search for an evolutionary philosophy of man. The evolution of social behavior. p. The biology of moral systems. War: the anthropology of armed conflict and aggression. 1965. editors. Baer D. Whitten P. Borgia G. Quart Rev Biol 1999. Vol. Response to Marvin Harris ‘Protein theory of warfare’. Ann Arbor: Univ Michigan Press. The natural history of aggression. 239:985–92. Sci 1988. Expansion and warfare among swidden agriculturists. 90 Alexander RD. 1990. 1995. 1967. editor. New York: Natural History Press. London: Thames & Hudson. 124 Wright Q. In: Bock GR Goode JA. 115 Sahlins MD. 107 Darwin CR. 1966.160–67. Natural selection and the analysis of human sociality. A review of selected sociobiological principles: application to hominid evolution I: The development of group social structure.202–36. Political expansion as an expression of the principle of competitive exclusion. p. The art of war. In: Fried MH Harris M Murphy R. editors. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press. editors. The anthropology of war. 129 Carneiro RL. London: Murray. How did humans evolve: reflections on the uniquely unique species. and warfare in a tribal population. Psychol Today 1975. Wheeler G. 1996. Frankfurt a. 111 Steward JH. Berkeley: Univ California Press. Heidelberg: C. 108 Spencer H. 103 Staden H. In: Van der Dennen JMG Falger VSE. J Theoret Biol 1978. 1957). In: Haas J. Amer Anthropol 1961. 9:449–74. 1978. New York: Holt.: Verlag Wuesten. Origins of the state: the anthropology of political evolution. The origin of species by means of natural selection. 1981. Group selection. 1955. editor. 1979. blood revenge. 82 Alexander RD. War and peace in primitive human societies. 112 Sahlins MD. London: Williams & Norgate. An essay on the history of civil society. Ancient society: or researches in the lines of human progress from savagery through barbarism to civilization. 132 Hobhouse LT. 1976. 5:325–83. 84:99–120. 1651. 169:733–8. Life histories. Tinkle DW. 86 Alexander RD.139–237. Warfare. 1990. London: Crooke. A review of selected sociobiological principles: application to hominid evolution II: The effects of intergroup conflict. In: Cohen R Service ER. 63:346–58. Wahrhaftige Historia und Beschreibung eyner Landtschafft der wilden nacketen grimmigen Menschfresser Leuthen in der Newenwelt Amerika gelegen. Spec Publ. 135 Steinmetz SR. 91 Pitt R. and the levels of hierarchical organization of life. A study of war. Theory of culture change: the methodology of multilinear evolution. 1859. 63:22–45. the matter. 12:29–54. Darwinism and human affairs. Ann Rev Ecol & Syst 1978.4. 1877 (reprint: Tucson: Univ Arizona Press). 85 Alexander RD. 1975. Chronic problems in understanding tribal violence and warfare.129–36. 100 Chagnon NA. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. War and our contemporary ancestors. 72:551–75. etc. New York: Harper & Row. 99 Chagnon NA. Amsterdam: MM Rey. altruism. Urbana: Univ Illinois Press. Leipzig: JA Barth. p. 131 Carneiro RL. 118 Service ER. The material culture and social institutions of the simpler peoples. ecclesiastical and civil. Tribesmen. 95 Bingham PM. New York: Holt. 116 Service ER. Leviathan.23. Warfare and hominid brain evolution. Ein Leitfaden der politischen Ethnologie. Warfare. 125 Feest CF. War: the anthropology of armed conflict and aggression. p.nel. Reprint ed (orig 1767). (1991) Showing off: tests of an hypothesis about man’s foraging goals. Evolution and culture.190–211. Service ER. 1968. Ch. London: Tavistock. Rinehart & Winston. 128 Carneiro RL. cooperation. 101 Chagnon. Ann Arbor: Univ Michigan Press. Chiefdom-level warfare as exemplified in Fiji and the Cauca Valley. Study Human Issues. Yanomamö: the fierce people. 114 Sahlins MD. 1762 (The Social Contract. 1940. 1925. December 2002 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X www. 133 Van der Bij TS. 1967.241–46. 94 Richerson PJ.War in Preindustrial Societies 81 Melotti U. 1929. 89 Alexander RD. editors. 1876/1885. Chicago: Univ Chicago Press. Amer Anthropol 1961. Primitive culture. 1980. 3 Vols. 1990. 1960. 1964. Reproductive and somatic conflicts of interest in the genesis of violence and warfare among tribesmen. 1954. editors. London: Murray. 1968. form and power of a commonwealth. editors. 18:245–8. Sci 1970. Philosophie des Krieges. On the sociology of primitive exchange. Chicago: Aldine. 123 Hunter DE. 119 Muehlmann WE. 5:69–90. The evolution of political society: an essay in political anthropology. The relevance of models for social anthropology. 1968. The segmentary lineage: an organization of predatory expansion. In: Carthy JD Ebling FJ. Le contrat social ou principes du droit politique. Chicago: Univ of Chicago Press. The evolution of society. New York: Natural History Press. editors. 1. Ontstaan en eerste ontwikkeling van den oorlog [Origin and initial development of war]. Natural selection and social behavior: recent research and new theory. Philadelphia: Inst. New York: Random House. 1907. Krieg und Frieden. 127 Andreski S.edu 63 . 104 Ferguson A. Harris M.Winters Universitätsbuchhandlung. p. 120 Fried MH. 96 Chagnon NA. or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. 126 Andreski S. A comparative review. 3:134–47. Ginsberg M. 83 Alexander RD. 87 Alexander RD.77–104. Changing Scenes in the Natural Sciences 1977. Human uniqueness: a general theory.205–23. 74:133–69. New York: Aldine. 8:6–7. Murphy R. editor. 1987. New York: Harper. 1990. 102 Hawkes. In: Haas J. 136 Vayda AP. 1966.M. New York: Academic Press. The principles of sociology. 88 Alexander RD. 1942. London: London School of Economics Monogr on Sociol 3. 1871/1874. military organization. 1977/1983. 134 Steinmetz SR. 5:121–139. London: Chapman and Hall. 106 Rousseau JJ. J Soc & Biol Structures 1982. New York: Norton. 1968. New York: Holt. editors. Soziologie des Krieges. 121 Fried MH. The study of anthropology. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. Anthropologica 1961. Origins of war. Suppl. Ethol & Sociobiol 1991. Michigan Mus Zool. The Hunters. 98 Chagnon NA. Groningen: Wolters. 113 Sahlins MD. Biosci 1968. 84 Alexander RD. p. 1968. Yanomamö: the fierce people (2nd and 3rd expanded eds). Neuroendocrinol Lett Special Issue. 1929. 12:283–337. Rinehart & Winston. NA. 1912. New York: Wiley. The anthropology of war. p. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press.

The anthropology of war. 148 Schmookler AB. 3:97–128. New York: Gordon & Breach. The ascent of man. 1972. The upper Amazon. New York: Holt. War and other essays. 169 Leeds A. 167 Lathrap DW. War. 1968. 163 Wittfogel KA. 141 Vayda AP. editor. In: James W. New York: Oxford Univ Press. Migration. Phases of the process of war and peace among the Marings of New Guinea. In: Masserman JH. Peace Res Soc (Internat) Papers 1967. 142 Vayda AP.Johan M. editor. Myths about hunter-gatherers. 11:251–77. In: Haas J. An explanation for primitive warfare: population control and the significance of primitive sex ratios. Divale WT.21–31. Origins: what new discoveries reveal about the emergence of our species and its possible future. 1934. 80:668–71. Critical Rev 1997. 1912. Amer J Sociol 1941. 1949. New York: Praeger. Boston: Little. Current Anthropol 1987. Anthropologica 1961. Violence and war with clinical studies.69–82. 193 Tinbergen N. 15:772–7. Man. 187 Newcomb WW. 1927. 1968. mores and morals. 3:33–45. 164 Sumner WG. p. Straus & Cudahy. manners. Vayda AP. 2000. 52:317–30. 191 Lopreato J. 179 Otterbein KF. New York: Mentor Books. 2nd. ed. 3:353–60. New Haven: Yale Univ Press. 52. On war and peace in animals and man. Harris M. The anthropology of war. p.483–514. and cultural evolution. 192 Tinbergen N. and matrilocal residence: an explanation for matrilineal residence systems. Harris M. 196 James W. War.37–68. 173 Divale. Folkways: a study of the sociological importance of usages. 143 Vayda AP. Cambridge MA: Harvard Univ Press. 145 Ember CR. Warless societies and the origin of war. 181 Otterbein KF. van der Dennen 137 Vayda AP. 144 Leeds A. 165 Sumner WG. 17: 439–48. 32:391–409. 35:1–28. 197 James W. In: Honigmann JJ. Ann Rev Ecol & Syst 1974. 1994. An anthropological analysis of war. Handbook of social and cultural anthropology. Berkeley: Univ California Press. culture and animals. The origins of war: biological and anthropological theories.31–38. 139 Vayda AP. Plagues and peoples. 1985. Motivations and material causes: on the explanation of conflict and war. 172 Divale WT. The war disease. A re-examination of the causes of Plains warfare. 166 Sumner WG. 1977. New York: Zone Books. Violence and sociality in human evolution. 1976/1979. The evolution of war: a cross-cultural study. 182 Naroll R. 7:133–8. Primitive warfare and the ratomorphic image of mankind. Social Forces 1952. 1974. 1911. Belligerence among the primitives: A reanalysis of Quincy Wright’s data. Explaining why Marings fought. 13 (whole issue). 64 . anthropology. Warfare in ecological perspective. International conciliation pamphlet No. and marital residence in pre-industrial societies. 1970. 28:457–500. 1973. editor. p. Ecodynamics: a new theory of societal evolution. New Haven: Yale Univ Press. Lewin R. territorial expansion. In: Sponsel LE Gregor T. New York: Longmans. editors. 183 Knauft BM. J Peace Res 1966. 195 Alcock N.56–76.85–91. Research on the functions of primitive war. Boston: Allen & Unwin. Chamberis F. Sci 1968. On the beginnings of warfare. external warfare. 1963. The anthropology of peace and nonviolence. Current Anthropol 1991. Principles of psychology. peace. The science of culture: a study of man and civilization. 1976. 1973. 178 Otterbein KF. sociology. p. History & Theory 1996. Oakville: Canadian Peace Research Inst Press. 150 Dewey J. Leeds A. Oceania 1971. Ethnol 1978. The science of society. editors. Primitive warfare: a methodological note. WT. p. 1890. 1906. 170 Hallpike CR. 159 Bronowski J. Brown. 3:131–4. 147 Vernant JP. 5:183–93. Reconsidering violence in simple human societies: homicide among the Gebusi on New Guinea. 140 Vayda AP. 149 Boas F. 1957. Human nature and biocultural evolution. 1975. 194 Holsti R. New York: Rand McNally. 161 Dyer G. New York: Grune & Stratton. 2:173–92. Memories and studies. 168 Leeds A. New York: Natural History Press. Natural selection in cultural evolution: warfare versus peaceful diffusion. Amer Anthropol 1976. 1978. New York: Pergamon Press. Man 1973. The moral equivalent of war. J Confl Resol 1976. 42:1–24. 186 Boehm C. Amer Anthropol 1950. 189 Robarchek CA. Amer Sociol Rev 1950. 160:1411–8. 190 Kelly RC. The relation of war to the origin of the state.G. editors. Ann Arbor: Univ Michigan Press. War: the anthropology of armed conflict and aggression. 175 Divale WT. 9:75–133. Anthropology and the study of war. 1994. 157 Schneider J. Boston: Ginn. 153 Boulding KE. editors. The anthropology of feuding and warfare: selected works by Keith F. Behav Sci Res 1974. Saint Vincent: Edizioni Centro Culturale. Human nature and conduct. 188 Robarchek CA. 155 Schneider J. 20:57–78. 138 Vayda AP. 151 Benedict R. War. Population. customs. Patterns of culture. 1985. 146 Broch T. War in ecological perspective: persistence. 154 Leakey RE. The Hague: Mouton. Is war a social problem? J Confl Resol 1959. Otterbein. On the history of war. The New Scholar 1970. 46:521–50. 3:825–7. A history of the ancient world. War: studies from psychology. 185 Knauft BM. warfare. An anthropologist’s view of war. The origins of war. 1990. change. Garden City: Anchor/ Doubleday. Amer Ethnol 1976. 177 Otterbein KF. New Haven: HRAF. Goethals G. 171 Divale WT. Boulder: Lynne Rienner. 158 White LA. Amer Anthropol 1978. Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. 26:68–74. Beverly Hills: Sage. and adaptive processes in three Oceanian societies. Functionalist interpretations of primitive warfare. The parable of the tribes: the problem of power in social evolution. 160 Starr CG. New York: Farrar. 1984. editors. 1981. Oriental despotism: a comparative study of total power. In: Nettleship MA Givens RD Nettleship A. 156 Schneider J. New Haven: Yale Univ Press. Myth and society in ancient Greece. New York: Crown. New York: Dutton. 180 Otterbein KF. Keller AG. The functions of war. Aggression and violence: a psychobiological and clinical approach. 1990. Gangloff D. In Valzelli L Morgese L. p. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press. Washington: Publ Amer Assoc Adv Sci 78. p. 8:451–70. Reprinted in: Bramson L. Warfare. Green & Co.923–58. J Anthropol Res 1989. Amer Anthropol 1989. New York: Holt. 152 McNeill WH. Helsingfors: Annales Academiae Scientiarum Fennicae 1913. 4 Vols. 1984. 45:159–77. 91:903–20. In: Fried MH Harris M Murphy R. 162 Dawson D. Amer Ethnol 1976. 174 Divale WT. The male supremacist complex: discovery of a cultural invention. 184 Knauft BM. Culture and cooperation in human evolution. p. 1999. colonialism and war: An evolutionary perspective. its causes and correlates. and the male supremacist complex. New York: Basic Books. 176 Malinowski B. 1910. Hypotheses about functions of war. 1965. 78:521–38. 1922. Capitalism. Galtung J.

1976. Paper Conference International Society for Research on Aggression (ISRA). New York: Berghahn Books. ideology. 208 Keith A. 226 Ehrenreich B. December 2002 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X www. Chicago Press. 220 Thienpont K. Massacre on the Gila: an account of the last major battle between American Indians. In: Eibl-Eibesfeldt I Salter FK. The emergence of man. 1968. Fox R. 215 Boyd R. 1975. 1970. Neuroendocrinol Lett Special Issue. The hunting hypothesis. The imperial animal. Press. 1990. 1999. editors. Indoctrinability. 1983. Man the hunter. The cerebral bridge from family to foe. 1981. 204 Ardrey R.nel. Promethean fire: reflections on the origin of mind. p. 1966. 211 Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. Zur Naturgeschichte elementarer Verhaltensweisen. Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression. 1997. Wilson EO. editor. 35:161–93. 1998. London: Methuen. DeVore I. The relationship between hunting. Tucson: Univ Arizona Press. Haren. 1971. 1948. London: Watts. War and gender: how gender shapes the war system and vice versa. Culture and the evolutionary process. 201 Pfeiffer JE. 1985. Krieg und Frieden aus der Sicht der Verhaltensforschung. p. Suppl.4. 1998. 223 Tiger L. Adventures with the missing link. 1971. Boyd R. Rinehart & Winston. 212 Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. Evolution and politics: biopolitical essays. predation.293–303. 1915. Why are there so few women warriors? Behavioral Science Research 1983. New York: Atheneum. New York: Bantam Books: 1967 (orig. Beiträge zur Konfliktforschung 1978. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press. Warfare. Lancaster CS. 199 Washburn SL. Men in groups. editors. Cliquet R. 206 Scott JP. Wien: Borotha-Schoeler Verlag. Das sogenannte Böse. p.23. The biological nature of the state. Cambridge (MA): Harvard Univ. London: Panther. editors. London: Chapman and Hall. 222 Tiger L. Chicago: Aldine Atherton. 1968. Richerson PJ. 216 Richerson PJ. The evolution of human ultra-sociality. violence and warfare. 227 Kroeber CB.War in Preindustrial Societies 198 Frobenius L.edu 65 . 8:87–111. mind. 18:196–212. 200 Lee RB. Salter FK. Boston: Luce. Cambridge (MA): Harvard Univ Press. London: Methuen. 1963). 60:177–98. New York: Metropolitan Books. New York: Holt. Liebe und Haß. 209 Masters RD. Vol. the Netherlands 207 Lorenz K. Chicago: Univ. München: Piper Verlag. and agonistic behavior. 210 Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. cooperation. 2001. 1972. In: Lee RB DeVore I. The predatory transition from ape to man. Internat Anthropol Linguist Rev 1953. Blood rites: origins and history of the passions of war. 2nd rev ed. editors. Groningen: Origin Press. with reflections on the origin of war. 221 Tiger L. editors. On aggression. A new theory of human evolution. In: Van der Dennen JMG Falger VSE. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition. and culture: the coevolutionary process. 205 McDougall W.71–98. New York: Berghahn. 218 Lumsden CJ. 213 Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. ideology and warfare: evolutionary perspectives. 1908. Indoctrinability. In-group/out-group behaviour in modern societies: an evolutionary perspective. July. 224 Goldstein JS. 219 Falger VSE. Brussels: NIDI CBGS Publ. The evolution of hunting. Chicago: Aldine. 1989.99–106. 214 Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. Menschenjagden und Zweikämpfe. New York: Harper & Row. man’s indoctrinability and group selection. Man the hunter. Genes. Fontana BL. World Politics 1983. Jena: Thüringer Verlagsanstalt. 1994. 1:201–19. 1914. and warfare: evolutionary perspectives. Wilson EO. 203 Dart RA. 1959. An introduction to social psychology. München: Piper Verlag. 1980. Die Evolution der destruktiven Aggression aus der Sicht der Verhaltensforschung. 225 Adams DB. New York: Harper & Brothers. 202 Dart RA. Z f Tierpsychologie 1982. 217 Lumsden CJ. 1986.

Sign up to vote on this title
UsefulNot useful