You are on page 1of 11

Developmental Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association

2007, Vol. 43, No. 1, 197–207 0012-1649/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.197

Staying in or Moving Away From Structured Activities: Explanations


Involving Parents and Peers
Andreas Persson, Margaret Kerr, and Håkan Stattin
Örebro University

Adolescent participation in structured activities, meaning those with adult leaders, regular meetings, and
skill-building activities, is related to good adjustment. Participation in unstructured, unsupervised,
peer-oriented activities is related to poor adjustment. Structured activity participation is high in early
adolescence and then declines, raising the question of why youths leave structured activities. The authors
examined explanations involving parents and peers. They used longitudinal data from 861 youths (ages
13–17 years). Results showed that, compared with youths who stayed in structured activities, those who
switched to hanging out on the streets were less likely to have peers in structured activities and had less
positive feelings about the home context and more negative interactions with parents. In addition,
delinquency predicted switching to hanging out in the streets and never joining structured activities in the
first place. The results concerning parents support a theoretical explanation of how parents might
unintentionally affect youths’ leisure choices. Furthermore, the authors found some indications that
positive feelings at home might protect youths who switch from structured activities to hanging out on
the streets from increases in delinquency.

Keywords: structured activities, unstructured activities, delinquency, parent relations, peer relations

Youths spend many of their waking hours in leisure activities. grade, personality characteristics, socioeconomic status, and fam-
These activities can vary considerably in content and form. Among ily factors (Cooper et al., 1999; Eccles & Barber, 1999; Marsh,
other things, some are led by adults, whereas others are not. 1992; McNeal, 1995; Stattin et al., 2005). Thus, even though the
Participation in structured activities, or activities with adult lead- link between leisure activities and adjustment is partly explained
ers, regular meetings, and skill-building activities, is related to by self-selection, youths’ choices of leisure activities seem to have
academic achievement (Cooper, Valentine, Nye, & Lindsay, 1999; implications for their overall adjustment and well-being.
Eccles & Barber, 1999; Marsh, 1992), reduced risk of school Given that participation in structured activities is related to
dropout (Mahoney & Cairns, 1997; McNeal, 1995), increased life positive outcomes and participation in peer-oriented, unstructured
satisfaction (Gilman, 2001), and lower rates of depression (Ma- activities is related to problems, one would like to understand why
honey, Schweder, & Stattin, 2002) and delinquency (Landers & youths get involved in structured rather than unstructured activities
Landers, 1978; Mahoney, 2000). Participation in unstructured, in the first place. Research suggests, however, that this may not be
unsupervised, peer-oriented activities, conversely, is related to the only critical issue. Studies of youths’ involvement in structured
antisocial behavior (Mahoney, Stattin, & Lord, 2004; Osgood, activities show that participation is as high as 70%– 80% in early
Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, 1996; Persson, Kerr, & adolescence (Eccles & Barber, 1999; U.S. Department of Educa-
Stattin, 2004; Stattin, Kerr, Mahoney, Persson, & Magnusson, tion, 1995) but that it declines over time (Mahoney, Larson,
2005). To some extent, this is probably a result of self-selection in Eccles, & Lord, 2005). Some of the youths who quit will undoubt-
the sense that well-adjusted youths choose structured activities, edly start spending their time in unstructured activities, such as
whereas poorly adjusted youths choose unstructured activities. hanging out on the streets, instead, which is associated with
However, many researchers have found a link between leisure problems. Another critical issue, then, is why some youths stay
activities and adjustment even when attempting to control for involved in structured activities, whereas others quit or switch to
selection effects by including background factors such as gender, unstructured activities.
There could be many reasons why youths stay in or drop out of
structured activities. For instance, previous research in the sports
Andreas Persson, Margaret Kerr, and Håkan Stattin, Center for Devel- domain has found that youths who feel competent or find the
opmental Research, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden. activity enjoyable and challenging are more motivated than those
This study was funded by the Swedish Research Council. Margaret Kerr who do not and more likely to stay involved over time (Carpenter
received partial support from Riksbankens Jubileumsfond. We thank Ther- & Scanlan, 1998; Gould, Feltz, & Weiss, 1985; Klint & Weiss,
ése Johansson, Gowert Masche, Vilmante Pakalniskiene, and Stefan Per-
1986, 1987; Weiss, Kimmel, & Smith, 2001). Factors such as too
sson for insightful comments on a draft of this article and William Burk for
statistical advice.
much pressure, restrictions on time use, lack of competence and
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andreas motivation, and the attraction of conflicting activities are related to
Persson, Center for Developmental Research, Department of Behavioral, youths’ dropping out (Gould, Feltz, Horn, & Weiss, 1982; Guillet,
Social and Legal Science, Örebro University, Örebro SE-701 82, Sweden. Sarrazin, Carpenter, Trouilloud, & Cury, 2002; Klint & Weiss,
E-mail: andreas.persson@bsr.oru.se 1986; Sarrazin, Vallerand, Guillet, Pelletier, & Cury, 2002; for a

197
198 PERSSON, KERR, AND STATTIN

review, see Brustad, Babkes, & Smith, 2001). Thus, concerning risive comments, or inconsistent discipline could make children
sports, abilities, experiences, and scheduling issues seem to play a feel unvalued, disrespected, and not in control of their circum-
part in whether youths stay involved or drop out. stances, and the negative emotions connected with these kinds of
Another potential reason why youths stay in or drop out of experiences could become conditioned to fundamental aspects of
structured activities is what their peers are doing. Research on the home context, such as having adults present and more or less
social relationships in the sports domain has found that youths who in control. (Regardless of how controlling parents are, they define
have positive peer relationships show greater enjoyment of and the family’s living situation in ways that children do not and, as
commitment to the activity (Duncan, 1993; Smith, 1999; Weiss & such, are in control.) These same negative emotions might gener-
Smith, 2002). Similarly, qualitative research on highly talented alize to adult-controlled contexts outside the home. If so, then
youths’ extracurricular involvement has found that peer relation- when children grow old enough to choose their own leisure con-
ships contribute to the decision to stay in or quit structured activ- texts, they should gravitate away from adult-led, structured set-
ities (Fredricks et al., 2002; Patrick et al., 1999). In addition, tings and toward unstructured settings. In other words, they might
quantitative research has linked friends’ endorsement of structured begin hanging out on the streets or in public parks or going to
activities and peer pressure to structured activity participation parties where parents are not present. If the emotions generated at
(Huebner & Mancini, 2003). Hence, previous research suggests home are positive, however, then youths should gravitate toward
that peers might play a part in whether youths stay in or quit leisure settings that can elicit the same good feelings—adult-led,
structured activities. However, these studies have not discrimi- structured activities. Thus, according to this context-choice expla-
nated between quitting and switching to unstructured activities. nation (Kerr et al., 2003), emotions generated in the home setting
Thus, when investigating the role of peers in youths’ leisure help to steer youths’ choices of leisure contexts. In this way,
choices, previous research has not considered that youths might parents might have an unintended influence on youths’ choices of
switch from structured to peer-oriented, unstructured activities to leisure activities.
spend more time with their important peers. A couple of recent studies have offered preliminary support for
Several studies have also examined the role of parents in youths’ this idea. In one study, girls who were engaged in unstructured
leisure choices. Cross-sectionally, parental support (Anderson, leisure activities had poorer relationships with their parents and
Funk, Elliot, & Hull Smith, 2003) and endorsement (Huebner & more negative emotions connected with the home setting than
Mancini, 2003) of structured activities have been linked to struc- those who were not involved in unstructured activities (Persson et
tured activity involvement, and lack of support has been linked to al., 2004). In another study, youths whose parents knew little about
unstructured activity involvement (Mahoney & Stattin, 2000). their children’s daily activities and who disclosed little to their
Although these cross-sectional studies cannot explain youths’ lei- parents were more likely to enter into unstructured activities over
sure choices over time, one longitudinal study has linked parental time (Mahoney et al., 2004). Although feelings about parents were
reinforcement to adolescent participation in structured activities, not measured directly in this study, to the extent that low parental
both concurrently and longitudinally (Fletcher, Elder, & Mekos, knowledge reflects youths’ negative feelings about the family
2000). Taken together, these cross-sectional and longitudinal stud- context, this might be taken as support for the idea. Both these
ies suggest that parents’ support for structured activities might be studies, however, concentrated on unstructured activities. As such,
important for engagement in structured or unstructured activities. the context-choice idea has not been tested against an alternative—
These studies, however, have focused on involvement in either that youths who have negative feelings about their parents go into
structured or unstructured activities but have not looked at chang- structured activities to form relationships with other adults. Thus,
ing from one to another. Therefore, they cannot answer why the questions remain regarding whether and how feelings about the
youths quit structured activities or switch from structured to un- family context are linked to both (a) structured and unstructured
structured activities. Furthermore, although these studies suggest activities and (b) decisions to switch from structured to unstruc-
that parents’ support and encouragement influence youths’ leisure tured activities.
participation, it is unlikely that parents’ influence only occurs Another possible role that good relationships with parents might
through this mechanism. If parents affect youths’ leisure choices, play is to moderate the potential negative consequences of switch-
their influence is likely to work through several mechanisms, and ing from structured to peer-oriented, unstructured activities, such
some might be much less direct. The question, then, is whether as hanging out on the streets. Having negative feelings at home is
parents might influence youths’ leisure choices or movements one possible reason why youths might not stay in organized
from structured to unstructured activities, such as loitering, in activities, but there are others, such as lack of talent or interest or
ways other than through reinforcement of structured activities. the desire to spend time with peers who are not involved in those
One theoretical idea that has been offered about this is that activities. Given that a youth has dropped out of structured activ-
parents can influence youths’ leisure choices unintentionally by ities and started engaging in unstructured activities, such as loiter-
influencing the way youths feel about adult-controlled, structured ing on the streets, relationships with parents might determine
settings in general (for a detailed account of proposed mechanisms, whether this becomes problematic. When youths are loitering,
see Kerr, Stattin, Biesecker, & Ferrer-Wreder, 2003). In brief, the opportunities arise to engage in criminal acts, such as shoplifting
idea is that, from family interactions, children derive a clear sense and vandalism. Strong parent–youth relationships might prevent
of how much they are valued and respected and how much they youths from acting in these situations, perhaps through a mecha-
can control or predict what happens to them. In the psychological nism such as that proposed by Hirschi (1969), whereby youths
literature, broadly speaking, these are critical for health and well- with strong bonds to their parents avoid performing delinquent acts
being and linked to positive emotions (e.g., Rodin & Langer, 1977; because they might jeopardize their relationships with their par-
Seligman, 1975). Parental behaviors such as angry outbursts, de- ents. Hirschi (1969) suggested that strong bonds make parents
STAYING IN STRUCTURED ACTIVITIES 199

psychologically present with the youth in tempting situations, so peers are doing. We test another explanation that has to do with
that the youth actually thinks about his or her parents when parents and how they might unintentionally influence whether
deciding whether to join in a delinquent activity. Indeed, there is a youths stay in structured activities. The mechanism involves
fair amount of evidence linking close parent–youth relationships youths’ feelings in the family context and how these feelings, by
with lower levels of delinquency (Dishion, French, & Patterson, being generalized to other contexts, might guide youths’ choices in
1995; Fincham, Grych, & Osborne, 1994; Maccoby & Martin, the leisure context. First, we test these peer and parent explana-
1983). Thus, even though youths who switch from structured tions separately; then we examine whether they are two indepen-
activities to unstructured activities, such as hanging out on the dent explanations by including them in the same model. Finally,
streets, should be expected to increase in problem behavior be- assuming that leaving structured activities to hang out on the
cause of increased opportunities, strong relationships with parents streets will be followed by increased problem behavior, we look at
might make this less likely, thus moderating the expected link whether positive feelings about the family context might moderate
between hanging out on the streets and increases in delinquency this effect. Thus, we test a dual protective role for parents, in the
over time. sense that feelings about parents might prevent youths from
In sum, then, studies that have examined youths’ leisure choices switching from structured activities to hanging out on the streets
and the consequences of those choices leave a number of questions but also protect youths from the negative consequences of switch-
unanswered. First, they have mainly focused on engagement in ing, if they make that choice.
structured activities, without considering that some of those youths
who are not engaged in structured activities might have quit. Method
Hence, there is a lack of research on why youths quit structured
activities. Second, prior studies have focused on either structured Participants and Procedure
or unstructured activities but not both together. Therefore, they do
not reveal why some youths switch from structured to unstructured The data are from the first two waves of a longitudinal study that is
activities. Third, few studies have considered peers as a factor taking place in a community in central Sweden. The primary purpose of
this investigation is to study the development of problem behavior. The
influencing youths’ choices of staying, quitting, or switching.
community has 36,000 inhabitants. The unemployment rate is similar to
Fourth, prior studies have not considered how parents might in- that in Sweden as a whole (6%). The mean income is somewhat lower than
fluence youths’ leisure choices other than through reinforcement. in the rest of the country (214,000 Swedish Crowns per year, compared
Moreover, they provide little information about the factors that with 223,000 for the rest of the country). Twelve percent of the inhabitants
determine how negative the consequences of switching from struc- have a foreign background. The community offers a wide range of adult-
tured to peer-oriented, unstructured activities might be. A fifth led, organized leisure activities for youths. Some of the most popular are
unanswered question is, therefore, whether feelings about parents sports associations, such as soccer, basketball, handball, badminton, and
might play an important role in moderating the consequences of floor hockey. The community also organizes theater groups and art asso-
switching from structured to unstructured activities as well as in ciations for youths as well as several different hobby associations, involv-
the choice itself. ing, for example, stamp collecting, photography, and model car or airplane
building. Other activities include scouts and church youth associations. All
In this study, we address each of these unanswered questions.
these are group oriented, have adult leaders, and have regularly scheduled
We (a) focus on why youths quit structured activities; (b) consider meetings.
switching from adult-led, structured activities to the unstructured The data were collected in the fall, during which most of the commu-
activity of hanging out on the streets, which clearly fits the criteria nity’s leisure activities were going on. All students in Grades 4 through 12
of being peer oriented and having no adult supervision, rules, or are invited to participate in the study each year. We are targeting all youths
structure and which has been considered an unstructured activity in in the community so that when youths name peers who are important to
previous research (Mahoney et al., 2005; Mahoney & Stattin, them, if they live in the community, those peers are likely to have
2000); (c) look at an explanation involving peers; (d) consider participated in the study as well and self-reported on their own behavior. In
youths’ feelings associated with the home setting rather than this way, data on peers’ behaviors are independent of the youths who
parents’ direct influence; and (e) consider the consequences of named those peers and, as such, are not affected by the youths’ own
perceptions and biases, which inflate similarity (e.g., Iannotti, Bush, &
dropping out of structured activities and whether feelings about
Weinfurt, 1996).
parents and the home setting might play a protective or moderating Youths were recruited in their classrooms during school hours. They
role. were told what kinds of questions would be included in the questionnaires
Using longitudinal data over 1 year, we begin at an age when and how long it would take to fill them out. They were informed that
structured activity participation is high. We identify youths who participation was voluntary and that, if they chose not to participate, they
stayed in structured activities (stayers), dropped out (quitters), or were free to do something else instead of filling out the questionnaire. They
switched from structured activities to hanging out on the streets were assured that if they did participate, their answers would not be
(switchers). As comparison groups, we also include those who revealed to parents, teachers, the police, or anyone else. Parents were
were never, as far as we know, in structured activities (nonjoiners) informed about the study ahead of time in meetings held in the community
and those who were involved in both structured activities and and by mail. They received a postage-paid card to return if they did not
want their children to participate (1% did so). They were told that they
hanging out on the streets (both). Thus, we focus on both struc-
could withdraw their child from the study at any time they chose. Thus,
tured activities and the unstructured activity of hanging out as well youths participated if they voluntarily chose to do so and if their parents did
as movements between them. We try to explain youths’ decisions not object to their participation. They filled out the questionnaires during
to stay in structured activities, quit, or switch to hanging out on the regular school hours in sessions administered by trained research assistants.
streets. We test one explanation that has to do with peers—that Teachers were not present. Youths were not paid for their participation, but
youths stay in or drop out of structured activities to do what their for each of the classes in Grades 4 through 6 we made a contribution to the
200 PERSSON, KERR, AND STATTIN

class fund, and in each of the classes in Grades 7 through 12 we held a drawing twice. Then, to determine whether the respondents’ peers were involved in
for movie tickets. All those who stayed in the room, whether they filled out structured activities, we used peers’ own reports of their leisure activities
questionnaires or not, were eligible for the drawing. For the present study, we and the same definition of structured activity participation as for the
used Grades 7 through 11, which is roughly ages 13 to 17 (ntarget sample ⫽ respondents (see above). We created a dichotomous measure based on the
1,751; 51% boys and 49% girls), because it is during these ages that adoles- peers’ information (1 ⫽ peers in structured activities, 2 ⫽ peers not in
cents tend to drop out of structured activities. Of the 1,751 students in the target structured activities). Youths were defined as having peers in structured
sample, 1,652 (94%) participated in the study at Time 1. activities if a majority of their peers were involved in structured activities
The second wave took place 1 year later, with the same procedure. In the (71% had all their peers involved in structured activities); they were
second wave, 1,341 (81%) of the participants from the first wave partici- defined as having peers not involved in structured activities if a majority of
pated. We used logistic regression to examine whether any of the measures their peers were not involved in structured activities (53% had all their
included in this study predicted dropout among youths who participated at peers outside of structured activities). According to this definition, 700
Time 1 (remaining in the study ⫽ 1). Age (odds ratio [OR] ⫽ 0.73, p ⬍ youths had peers in structured activities, and 161 had peers who were not
.001), feelings in the family context (OR ⫽ 1.51, p ⫽ .011), and family in structured activities.
structure (1 ⫽ divorced; OR ⫽ 0.56, p ⫽ .012) were significant predictors,
but the other measures were not. Thus, those participants with data at both
time points were somewhat younger, had somewhat more positive feelings Leisure Activities
in the family context, and were more likely to be from intact families than Structured activities. At Time 1, youths were asked about their en-
those who dropped out. gagement in structured leisure activities. The question specified that the
Approximately 15% of the individuals in the longitudinal sample of activity had to have an adult leader and meetings at least once a week at a
1,341 had missing data for at least one of the following measures (see the scheduled time. The activities (and percentages of youths involved) were as
Measures section): structured activities, hanging out on the streets, feelings follows: sports (71%), theater and art (4%), hobby associations (24%),
in the family context, or negative experiences of parent– child interactions. church youth associations (9%), and scouts (4%). The response options
Missing value analyses indicated that these data were missing at random, were “yes” and “no,” and youths were considered involved in structured
so we used the expectation-maximization algorithm to impute the missing activities if they chose “yes” for any of the activities. Twenty-one percent
values. This strategy has been shown to be superior to listwise deletion of the youths were involved in more than one activity. At Time 2, the
(Schafer & Graham, 2002). Analyses without imputed values resulted in a question and the list of activities were identical to those at Time 1, but there
similar pattern of significant findings. was just one yes-or-no option to indicate participation in one or more of the
Because we were interested in youths with particular patterns of activity activities. Seven hundred fifteen participants (83%) were involved in a
involvement over time, we restricted the longitudinal sample of 1,341 to structured activity at Time 1, and 515 (60%) were involved at Time 2.
the 1,186 youths who could be classified as stayers, quitters, switchers, Hanging out on the streets. We wanted to be sure that the activity that
both, or nonjoiners. This leisure-groups sample did not differ significantly we defined as unstructured fit the criteria of being peer oriented and having
from the longitudinal sample on any of the predictors in this study. Eight no adult supervision, rules, or structure. Hanging out in town in the
hundred sixty-one (73%) youths in this leisure-groups sample had com- evenings without doing anything in particular fits those criteria and has
plete self-reported data on all measures and had peer-reported data on been defined as an unstructured activity in previous research (Mahoney et
peers’ involvement in structured activities. They composed the analytic al., 2005; Mahoney & Stattin, 2000). Youths were asked about how
sample in this study (n ⫽ 861). Compared with the leisure-groups sample, frequently they did this. The five response options ranged from seldom or
the analytic sample was somewhat younger, t(1088) ⫽ 4.39, p ⬍ .001; never to almost every night. We dichotomized this measure (1 ⫽ hanging
more likely to be female, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 1,186) ⫽ 11.75, p ⬍ .001; and more out, 2 ⫽ not hanging out), such that those who answered seldom or never
likely to be involved in structured activities at Time 2, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 1,186) ⫽ were defined as not hanging out. By this definition, 384 participants (45%)
5.01, p ⫽ .025; however, the analytic sample did not differ on any other were hanging out on the streets at Time 1, and 258 (30%) were hanging out
measure in this study. at Time 2.
Group definitions. The participants were divided into five groups
Measures (stayers, quitters, switchers, both, and nonjoiners) on the basis of their
involvement in structured activities and hanging out on the streets at the
Peer Measures two time points. Stayers comprised 385 youths (54% female) who were
involved in structured activities at both times and were not hanging out on
Important peers. At Time 1, adolescents nominated up to three impor-
the streets at Time 2. Quitters comprised 129 youths (48% female) who
tant peers, defined as “someone you talk to, hang out with, and do things
were involved in structured activities at Time 1 but not involved in either
with.” They were told that an important peer could be a friend, a sibling,
structured activities or hanging out at Time 2. Switchers comprised 71
or a boyfriend or girlfriend but could not be a parent or another adult. They
youths (52% female) who were involved in structured activities at Time 1
also reported where they spent time with these peers (at school, in free
but were only hanging out on the streets at Time 2. Nonjoiners comprised
time, or both in school and in free time). In this study, we only included
146 youths (63% female) who were not involved in structured activities at
important peers the respondents spent time with in free time or both in
either time. The both group comprised 130 youths (49% female) who were
school and in free time.
involved in both structured activities and hanging out on the streets at both
Peer group. This instrument was adapted from Cairns and Cairns’s
times.
(1994) assessment of youth groups. Youths were asked to identify their
leisure-time peer groups. The definition was “at least three people who
hang out together outside of school (several people who are often to- Measures of Feelings About Parents and the Family
gether).” They wrote down the names of all their leisure-time peer group Context
members.
Peers in structured activities. First, we defined peers as the combina- Feelings in the family context. At Time 1, participants answered three
tion of both (a) the three most important peers and (b) the individuals questions regarding how often, when they were together with their family
named as part of the leisure peer group. We made sure that each peer was at home, they felt (a) respected, (b) that they were an important person, and
only included once. That is, if the most important peers were also men- (c) proud of themselves. The four response options ranged from never to
tioned in the leisure peer group, we made sure that they were not counted very often (M ⫽ 3.06, SD ⫽ 0.67). The Cronbach’s alpha was .82.
STAYING IN STRUCTURED ACTIVITIES 201

Negative experiences of parent– child interactions. This was a compos- dropped substantially over this 1-year period. Boys seemed to have
ite of two scales that tap specific parenting behaviors that youths should been involved in structured activities to a somewhat larger extent
experience negatively (parents’ bad reactions to the youth’s disclosure) and than girls at Time 1, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 861) ⫽ 6.65, p ⫽ .010, but not at
specific negative feelings that youths attribute to parenting behaviors (feelings Time 2, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 861) ⫽ 0.60, p ⫽ .440. Because of the
of being controlled by parents). The Cronbach’s alpha for this composite scale
difference at Time 1, however, we included gender in the follow-
was .81. Concerning parents’ bad reactions to disclosure, at Time 1 partici-
ing analyses.
pants answered six questions about their parents’ tendencies to react disre-
spectfully to their disclosure attempts. The questions were “Has it happened
that you told your parents things and later regretted that you did?” “How often Youths’ Choices in the Leisure Context—the Role of the
have you regretted that you told your parents too much about yourself, your Peers
friends, and your free time?” “Have you been punished for something that you
spontaneously told your parents?” “Have your parents ever used what you told Do youths drop out of structured activities to spend more time
them against you?” “Do your parents bring up things that you have told them with their peers? Similarly, do they stay in structured activities if
in confidence again and again?” and “Have your parents ever made fun of their peers are involved? To test this, we performed multinomial
things you happened to tell them about yourself and your life?” They answered logistic regression with group membership (involved in both struc-
on a 5-point response scale that ranged from has never happened to very often
tured activities and hanging out on the streets, quit structured
(M ⫽ 1.93, SD ⫽ 0.84) The Cronbach’s alpha was .82. Concerning the youths’
activities, switched to hanging out on the streets, never joined
feelings of being overly controlled by parents, at Time 1 participants answered
these questions: “Do you feel that your parents demand to know everything?” structured activities, and stayed in structured activities) as the
“Do you think you get enough freedom from your parents to do what you want dependent variable. We chose staying in structured activities as the
in your free time?” “Do you think that your parents control everything in your reference category. The independent variable was whether the
life?” and “Do you think that your parents interfere with what you do in your peers were involved in structured activities. Furthermore, because
free time?” The five response options ranged from yes, always to no, never previous research has found that age (Mahoney et al., 2005),
(M ⫽ 3.65, SD ⫽ 0.69). The Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .76. family factors such as divorce (McNeal, 1998), and delinquency
(Larson, 1994) are related to participating in or dropping out of
Other Measures structured activities, we entered them as control variables in this
analysis and all further analyses reported in this study.
Family structure. At Time 1, participants were asked whether their
parents were divorced. Two hundred fifty-six (30%) had divorced parents.
The model outperformed the null model, ␹2(20, N ⫽ 861) ⫽
Delinquency. Youths answered 21 questions about whether they had 197.42, p ⬍ .001. As shown in Table 2, delinquency, family
engaged in various delinquent behaviors during the past year. The response structure, gender, and age were all significant predictors of some
scale was a 5-point scale ranging from never (1) to more than 10 times (5). leisure choices. For instance, delinquency increased the likelihood
The questions were about breaking into houses; taking bicycles, motorcy- of being involved in both structured activities and hanging out in
cles, and cars without permission; shoplifting; vandalizing; smoking hash- the streets, switching to hanging out in the streets, and never
ish and using other drugs; hitting someone so hard that he or she needed to joining structured activities. Even after we controlled for these
be treated at the hospital; hurting someone with a knife or some other other factors, however, peers’ leisure activities uniquely predicted
weapon; and threatening or forcing someone to do something he or she did switching and never having joined structured activities. Having
not want to do (Time 1, M ⫽ 1.17, SD ⫽ 0.30; Time 2, M ⫽ 1.14, SD ⫽
peers in structured activities significantly decreased the likelihood
0.34). The Cronbach’s alphas for Times 1 and 2 were .89 and .92,
respectively. This scale has been validated as a part of a longitudinal
of switching from structured activities to hanging out on the streets
project titled Individual Development and Adjustment (Magnusson, Dunér, (OR ⫽ 0.47, confidence interval [CI] ⫽ 0.25, 0.91, p ⫽ .024) and
& Zetterblom, 1975). of never having joined structured activities in the first place (OR ⫽
0.24, CI ⫽ 0.15, 0.38, p ⬍ .001). Thus, youths who switched to
Results hanging out on the streets or who never joined structured activities
were less likely to have peers in structured activities than youths
Descriptive Results who stayed in structured activities. Peers’ leisure activities were
not related to quitting structured activities or being involved in
Table 1 shows participation in structured activities for the dif- both structured activities and loitering. These results are consistent
ferent grades involved in the study and for boys and girls sepa- with the idea that youths’ decisions to stay involved in or leave
rately. As the table shows, participation in structured activities structured activities are partly based on whether their friends are
involved.
Table 1
Percentages of Participants Involved in Structured Activities by Youths’ Choices in the Leisure Context—The Role of
Grade and Gender Parents
Time 1 Time 2 Are youths’ choices of leisure activities also partly based on
their experiences in the family context? In particular, do youths
Grade Boys Girls Total Boys Girls Total who have generally negative feelings associated with the home
7–8 88 87 88 66 65 65 context avoid adult-led, structured activities and spend more time
8–9 88 78 83 65 61 63 loitering, and do youths who have generally positive feelings at
9–10 89 78 84 70 57 64 home stay involved in structured activities over time? To examine
10–11 79 76 78 57 56 57 this, we conducted two multinomial logistic regression analyses
11–12 88 78 83 41 52 47
with two different indicators of youths’ feelings at home: feelings
202 PERSSON, KERR, AND STATTIN

Table 2
Multinomial Logistic Regression Predicting Group Membership From Peers’ Activity Participation and Controlling for Age,
Delinquency, Gender, and Family Structure

Both Quitters Switchers Nonjoiners

Variable OR CI OR CI OR CI OR CI sr2

Peers in structured activities (1 ⫽ yes) 1.06 0.56, 2.00 0.76 0.44, 1.31 0.47* 0.24, 0.90 0.24*** 0.15, 0.38 .043
Age 0.82** 0.71, 0.95 1.26*** 1.10, 1.44 0.82* 0.68, 0.99 1.09 0.95, 1.24 .032
Delinquency Time 1 14.23*** 6.39, 31.66 0.96 0.30, 3.10 13.76*** 5.71, 33.15 9.31*** 4.08, 21.26 .082
Gender (1 ⫽ boys) 1.06 0.70, 1.60 1.35 0.90, 2.04 0.96 0.57, 1.62 0.65* 0.43, 0.98 .009
Family structure (1 ⫽ divorced) 1.29 0.80, 2.06 2.41*** 1.56, 3.75 2.44*** 1.41, 4.22 1.95** 1.26, 3.01 .023

Note. Staying in structured activities was the reference category for the equation. All predictors were assessed at Time 1. n ⫽ 861. sr2 represents the
reduction in Nagelkerke R2 when the variable is removed from the equation. Nagelkerke R2 ⫽ .22. OR ⫽ odds ratio; CI ⫽ confidence interval.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

in the family context, and negative experiences of parent– child structured activities to have positive feelings in the family context.
interactions. In these models, leisure group membership was en- Feelings in the family context were not related to quitting struc-
tered as the dependent variable, and feelings in the family context tured activities or being involved in both structured activities and
(or negative experiences of parent– child interactions), gender, age, hanging out on the streets.
family structure, and self-reported delinquency at Time 1 were The model that included negative experiences of parent– child
entered as control variables. Staying in structured activities was interactions as the indicator of youths’ feelings at home also
chosen as the reference category. outperformed the null model, ␹2(20, N ⫽ 861) ⫽ 168.27, p ⬍ .001.
The model that included feelings in the family context outper- As shown in the lower part of Table 3, negative experiences of
formed the null model, ␹2(20, N ⫽ 861) ⫽ 174.93, p ⬍ .001. As parent– child interactions predicted being involved in both struc-
reported in the upper part of Table 3, positive feelings in the family tured activities and hanging out on the streets (OR ⫽ 1.47, CI ⫽
context significantly reduced the likelihood of switching from 1.11, 1.93, p ⫽ .007) and switching to hanging out on the streets
structured activities to hanging out (OR ⫽ 0.59, CI ⫽ 0.40, 0.86, (OR ⫽ 1.52, CI ⫽ 1.08, 2.16, p ⫽ .017) but not any other leisure
p ⫽ .007) or of never joining structured activities (OR ⫽ 0.53, choice. Hence, both feelings in the family context and negative
CI ⫽ 0.39, 0.72, p ⬍ .001), independently of age, gender, delin- experiences of parent– child interactions, beyond the other predic-
quency, and family structure. Hence, youths who switched from tors, predicted switching from structured activities to hanging out
structured activities to hanging out on the streets or never joined on the streets. Therefore, these results are consistent with the idea
structured activities were less likely than youths who stayed in that the feelings youths have connected with the home context

Table 3
Multinomial Logistic Regression Predicting Group Membership From Feelings About the Home Context and Parent–Youth
Interactions and Controlling for Age, Delinquency, Gender, and Family Structure

Both Quitters Switchers Nonjoiners

Variable OR CI OR CI OR CI OR CI sr2

Feelings in the family contexta

Feelings in the family context 0.84 0.61, 1.15 0.91 0.66, 1.25 0.59** 0.40, 0.86 0.53*** 0.39, 0.72 .021
Age 0.82* 0.71, 0.95 1.27*** 1.11, 1.45 0.84 0.70, 1.02 1.15* 1.00, 1.31 .036
Delinquency Time 1 13.66*** 6.14, 30.40 0.95 0.30, 3.05 12.60*** 5.22, 30.42 8.33*** 3.67, 18.91 .079
Gender (1 ⫽ boys) 1.07 0.71, 1.62 1.37 0.91, 2.06 1.00 0.59, 1.71 0.67 0.44, 1.00 .009
Family structure (1 ⫽ divorced) 1.31 0.82, 2.09 2.43*** 1.57, 3.77 2.48* 1.43, 4.28 1.99** 1.29, 3.06 .025

Negative experiences of parent–child interactionsb

Negative experiences 1.47** 1.11, 1.92 0.94 0.71, 1.24 1.52* 1.08, 2.15 0.94 0.72, 1.24 .014
Age 0.84 0.72, 0.97 1.26* 1.10, 1.45 0.85 0.70, 1.03 1.14 0.99, 1.30 .031
Delinquency Time 1 12.51*** 5.61, 27.90 1.01 0.31, 3.25 12.16*** 5.03, 28.41 9.27*** 4.10, 20.99 .077
Gender (1 ⫽ boys) 1.07 0.71, 1.62 1.36 0.90, 2.05 0.95 0.56, 1.60 0.62* 0.41, 0.93 .011
Family structure (1 ⫽ divorced) 1.26 0.79, 2.03 2.45*** 1.58, 3.80 2.35** 1.36, 4.07 2.02** 1.32, 3.10 .025

Note. Staying in structured activities was the reference category for the equation. All predictors were assessed at Time 1. n ⫽ 861. sr2 represents the
reduction in Nagelkerke R2 when the variable is removed from the equation. OR ⫽ odds ratio; CI ⫽ confidence interval.
a
High scores mean positive feelings, Nagelkerke R2 ⫽ .20. b High scores mean negative experiences, Nagelkerke R2 ⫽ .19.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.
STAYING IN STRUCTURED ACTIVITIES 203

influence their choices of leisure contexts—in particular, whether Feelings in the Family Context as a Moderator of the
they choose adult-led, structured contexts or avoid them. Consequences of Switching From Structured Activities to
Hanging Out on the Streets
Peers and Parents Previous research suggests that youths who spend time in peer-
oriented, unstructured activities increase in delinquency over time
The peer and parent explanations are both supported by the data, (Mahoney, Stattin, & Magnusson, 2001). Thus, we expected to
but are they two independent explanations? To test this, we used find that those who switched from structured activities to hanging
multinomial logistic regression to predict group membership and out on the streets increased their delinquent activity over time.
entered both feelings in the family context and peers in structured That expectation was confirmed. In a multiple regression analysis,
activities into the equation. In addition, we computed an interac- switching from structured activities to hanging out on the streets
tion term between feelings in the family context and peers in (dummy coded) predicted delinquency at Time 2 (␤ ⫽ .113, SE ⫽
structured activities to examine whether, for example, feelings in .029, p ⬍ .001) when we controlled for delinquency at Time 1,
the family context influence how likely youths are to gravitate to age, gender, and family structure. Our question, however, was
activities in which their peers are involved. As before, we entered whether this tendency would be less for youths who had close ties
adolescents’ gender, age, family structure, and self-reported delin- to their parents, as evidenced by positive feelings in the family
quency at Time 1 as control variables. In this model, both feelings context. To test this, we used multiple regression, predicting de-
in the family context and peers in structured activities remained linquency at Time 2 while controlling for delinquency at Time 1.
unique, significant predictors of switching and never joining struc- We ran two different models, one with feelings in the family
tured activities when we controlled for the opposite variable and context, and one with negative experiences of parent– child inter-
the other factors (see Table 4), and the interaction effect was not actions. In the first model, switching from structured activities to
significant. hanging out on the streets, feelings in the family context, and the
Then we repeated the procedure with negative experiences of interaction of the two were entered as predictors along with age,
parent– child interactions as the indicator of youths’ feelings at family structure, and gender as controls. A significant interaction
home. Again, both the peer and the parent measures remained term would indicate that feelings in the family context moderated
significant, unique predictors when included in the same model the negative effect of switching from structured activities to hang-
(see Table 4). Therefore, it seems that feelings at home and peers’ ing out on the streets. As reported in Table 5, the interaction was
leisure activities are two independent explanations that are both not significant.
useful for understanding why some youths switch from structured In the second model, switching from structured activities to
activities to hanging out on the streets. hanging out on the streets, negative experiences of parent– child

Table 4
Multinomial Logistic Regression Predicting Group Membership From Peers’ Activity Participation, Feelings About the Home
Context, and Parent–Youth Interactions and Controlling for Age, Delinquency, Gender, and Family Structure

Both Quitters Switchers Nonjoiners

Variable OR CI OR CI OR CI OR CI sr2

Feelings in the family contexta

Peers in structured activities (1 ⫽ yes) 1.07 0.55, 1.98 0.77 0.44, 1.34 0.47* 0.24, 0.93 0.30*** 0.18, 0.49 .025
Feelings in the family context 0.89 0.64, 1.24 0.93 0.67, 1.28 0.60* 0.40, 0.89 0.63** 0.46, 0.87 .012
Peers ⫻ Feelings 1.23 0.94, 1.62 1.05 0.83, 1.34 1.08 0.83, 1.42 0.86 0.71, 1.05 .008
Age 0.82** 0.70, 0.95 1.26*** 1.10, 1.45 0.83* 0.68, 1.00 1.10 0.96, 1.26 .032
Delinquency Time 1 14.07*** 6.30, 31.341 0.94 0.30, 3.30 12.78*** 5.28, 30.94 8.01** 3.47, 18.46 .075
Gender (1 ⫽ boys) 1.06 0.70, 1.62 1.37 0.91, 2.06 1.02 0.60, 1.73 0.68 0.45, 1.03 .008
Family structure (1 ⫽ divorced) 1.28 0.79, 2.04 2.41*** 1.55, 3.75 2.44*** 1.41, 4.22 1.96** 1.26, 3.05 .023

Negative experiences of parent–child interactionsb

Peers in structured activities (1 ⫽ yes) 1.07 0.55, 2.09 0.75 0.43, 1.31 0.45* 0.23, 0.88 0.23*** 0.14, 0.36 .043
Negative experiences 1.49** 1.12, 1.98 0.95 0.71, 1.26 1.53* 1.08, 2.18 0.80 0.59, 1.10 .017
Peers ⫻ Experiences 1.03 0.80, 1.32 1.04 0.84, 1.30 0.96 0.74, 1.24 1.18 0.98, 1.42 .003
Age 0.84* 0.72, 0.97 1.26** 1.10, 1.44 0.83 0.69, 1.01 1.07 0.94, 1.23 .026
Delinquency Time 1 12.62*** 5.65, 28.16 0.99 0.31, 3.18 12.31*** 5.08, 29.84 9.41*** 4.10, 21.61 .072
Gender (1 ⫽ boys) 1.05 0.69, 1.59 1.36 0.90, 2.05 0.94 0.55, 1.60 0.66* 0.43, 1.00 .008
Family structure (1 ⫽ divorced) 1.23 0.76, 1.97 2.43*** 1.57, 3.77 2.32** 1.34, 4.02 1.95** 1.26, 3.03 .022

Note. Staying in structured activities was the reference category for the equation. All predictors were assessed at Time 1. n ⫽ 861. sr2 represents the
reduction in Nagelkerke R2 when the variable is removed from the equation. OR ⫽ odds ratio; CI ⫽ confidence interval.
a
High scores mean positive feelings, Nagelkerke R2 ⫽ .24. b High scores mean negative experiences, Nagelkerke R2 ⫽ .24.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.
204 PERSSON, KERR, AND STATTIN

Table 5
Multiple Regression Analyses Testing Whether Positive Feelings About the Home Context and
Parent–Youth Interactions Moderate the Link Between Switching From Structured Activities to
Hanging Out on the Streets and Increases in Delinquency Over Time

Variable ␤ CI sr2 p

Feelings in the family contexta

Delinquency Time 1 .543 .50, .62 .278 ⬍.001


Age ⫺.118 ⫺.17, ⫺.06 .014 ⬍.001
Gender ⫺.063 ⫺.12, ⫺.01 .004 .026
Family structure ⫺.065 ⫺.12, ⫺.01 .004 .022
Switching to hanging out on the streets .123 .06, .18 .014 ⬍.001
Feelings in the family context ⫺.020 ⫺.08, .04 .000 .475
Switching ⫻ Feelings in the Family Context .046 ⫺.01, .10 .002 .110

Negative experiences of parent–child interactionsb

Delinquency Time 1 .543 .50, .62 .278 ⬍.001


Age ⫺.118 ⫺.17, ⫺.06 .014 ⬍.001
Gender ⫺.063 ⫺.12, ⫺.01 .004 .025
Family structure ⫺.071 ⫺.13, ⫺.02 .005 .012
Switching to hanging out on the streets .092 .03, .15 .007 .003
Negative experiences .009 ⫺.05, .07 .000 .768
Switching ⫻ Negative Experiences .062 .00, .12 .003 .039

Note. All predictors were assessed at Time 1.


a
High scores mean positive feelings, R2 ⫽ .37. b
High scores mean negative experiences, R2 ⫽ .37.

interactions, and the interaction of the two were entered as pre- dictors when entered into the same equation. Furthermore, we
dictors along with the same controls as in the previous model. With found some indications that positive feelings at home might pro-
this indicator of youths’ feelings at home, the interaction reached tect youths who switch from structured activities to hanging out on
significance (␤ ⫽ .062, SE ⫽ .030, p ⫽ .039). To understand the the streets from increases in delinquency. Thus, parents seem to
nature of the interaction, we solved the regression equation for play an indirect role in youths’ leisure choices by contributing to
high and low values of switching and negative experiences of youths’ feelings associated with the home, but they might also play
parent– child interactions, and we plotted these points. The plot an indirect role in what happens once choices are made.
indicated that, as expected, youths who switched and had highly This study advances the literature in several ways. First, previ-
negative experiences at home increased in delinquency somewhat ous research has mainly focused on being engaged in or joining
more than switchers with less negative experiences at home. Thus, structured (Fletcher, Nickerson, & Wright, 2003; McNeal, 1998)
there is some evidence, albeit weak and for only one of two or unstructured activities (Mahoney et al., 2001; Persson et al.,
indicators, that positive feelings about home and parents might 2004). To our knowledge, this study is the first to propose and
protect youths once they switch from structured activities to hang- examine explanations for movements between structured activities
ing out on the streets. and unstructured activities, such as hanging out on the streets.
Second, this study examines new explanations regarding the roles
Discussion of parents and peers in youths’ leisure activities. Although some
Youths spend a great deal of time in different leisure activities. studies have considered peer factors (Eccles, Barber, Stone, &
Most are involved in adult-led, structured activities in early ado- Hunt, 2003; Fredricks et al., 2002; Huebner & Mancini, 2003;
lescence, but many eventually drop out. Given that structured Mahoney & Stattin, 2000; Patrick et al., 1999; Smith, 1999; Weiss
activities are related to positive development (Cooper et al., 1999; & Smith, 2002), ours is the first to examine the idea that youths
Landers & Landers, 1978; Mahoney & Cairns, 1997; Mahoney et switch leisure activities to spend time with peers outside of struc-
al., 2002) and peer-oriented, unstructured activities, such as loi- tured activities. Concerning parents, previous research primarily
tering on the streets, are related to increasing problems over time has focused on different forms of reinforcement in explaining
(Agnew & Petersen, 1989; Osgood et al., 1996), one would like to youths’ structured activity participation (Anderson et al., 2003;
understand why youths quit structured activities or switch from Huebner & Mancini, 2003). We have broadened the view of how
structured to unstructured activities. Our findings lend support to parents might have an influence, showing that parents might in-
two explanations. One is that youths leave structured activities to fluence youths’ leisure choices unintentionally by creating positive
spend time with peers who are not in those activities. The other is or negative feelings about adult-controlled, structured settings in
that youths who do not feel valued and respected at home avoid general. Furthermore, our findings regarding delinquency and
other adult-controlled, structured settings when they are old youths’ leisure choices extend previous research. One study found
enough to choose their own leisure activities. These seem to be two that delinquents were inclined to drop out of sports (Larson, 1994).
independent explanations, as they both remained significant pre- In this study, we have found that delinquency was a significant
STAYING IN STRUCTURED ACTIVITIES 205

predictor of not only quitting structured activities but switching to be a good explanation for the switchers in this study, however,
hanging out on the streets and never having joined structured because feelings at home were assessed while youths were still
activities as well. Thus, this study extends previous research on the involved in structured activities. In addition, one previous study
roles of peers, parents, and delinquency in youths’ leisure choices. found that poor parental knowledge preceded youths’ choices of
How do youths end up having negative feelings at home? We unstructured leisure settings (Mahoney et al., 2004). Both of these
have implied that this is something under parents’ control, but findings support the conclusions drawn in this study.
what might parents actually do to create negative feelings? One Some limitations of the study should be mentioned. One is that
possibility is that they do not allow youths a voice in making we did not include any measures of youths’ individual character-
decisions about things that concern them. This could leave youths istics in our analyses. Characteristics such as motivation and
with the feeling that they are not valued and respected and that competence have been related to structured activity participation
they cannot control what happens to them. In line with this, (Fredricks et al., 2002; Klint & Weiss, 1987), whereas factors such
previous research has found that parental efforts to control youths’ as impulsivity and sensation seeking have been related to partici-
freedom— by, for example, requiring that the youth ask for per- pation in unstructured activities (Persson et al., 2004). In this
mission before going out—are linked to youths’ feelings of being study, we focus on youths’ social relationships and the feelings
overly controlled, which, in turn, are related to internal problems, these relationships evoke. The question remains regarding whether
such as depressive symptoms and low self-esteem (Barber, 1996; these different explanations overlap. Another limitation is that our
Kerr & Stattin, 2000). Another way parents might create negative measure of youths’ involvement in structured activities was not
feelings is by consistently reacting to youths’ communication with activity specific at Time 2. Hence, we have no information on
disrespect or insensitivity. They might, for instance, punish or youths’ participation in specific structured activities over time.
ridicule a youth who spontaneously discloses some misdeed or However, the purpose of this study is to test why youths switch
personal information. In previous research, such reactions from from structured activities that have characteristics (i.e., emphasis
parents have been negatively linked to youths’ feelings of being on skill building, regularity, and adult leadership) related to pos-
trusted by their parents (Kerr, Stattin, & Trost, 1999). Hence, there itive adjustment (for a review, see Gilman, Meyers, & Perez, 2004)
are several ways parents might contribute to youths’ negative to a peer-oriented, unstructured activity that has been related to
feelings about the home setting. Other research, however, suggests problems (Persson et al., 2004) and whether there are differences
that parental behaviors such as responding negatively to the child’s in adjustment over time among adolescents with different feelings
disclosure are partly reactions to the youths’ internalizing and at home. Although they might differ in other aspects, all the
externalizing problems and other characteristics (Dishion, Nelson, structured activities involved in this study share the theoretically
& Bullock, 2004; Kerr & Stattin, 2003; Kerr, Stattin, & Pakalni- relevant characteristics.
skiene, in press). Thus, the conditions at home that leave youths Although this study extends previous research on youths’ leisure
not feeling valued or respected might come about through complex choices and the roles of parents and peers, it leaves some questions
family interactions in which parents react to the child’s character- unanswered. One is what role feelings at home play in youths’
istics or behaviors as well as acting on their own characteristics, leisure choices compared with aspects of the parent–adolescent
values, and beliefs about parenting. relationship, such as parental reinforcement of structured activi-
To what extent can our findings based on youths’ leisure activ- ties. In addition, we have focused on specific leisure activities that
ities in Sweden be generalized to other settings? In North America, were clearly within or outside the definition of being adult con-
for instance, leisure activities are often attached to the school trolled and structured, but the actual range of activities that youths
system. In Sweden, they operate independently of the school. can choose from is much broader. Some activities could be con-
Nonetheless, similar percentages of youths in Sweden and North sidered structured, in the sense that there are regularly scheduled
America are involved in structured activities (Eccles & Barber, meetings and that they focus on building skills, but they might still
1999; U.S. Department of Education, 1995). Both in North Amer- lack or have little adult supervision. Others offer close contact with
ica and in Sweden, structured activities are voluntary, have regular adults but little skill building (e.g., some types of neighborhood
meeting schedules and adult leaders, take place in specific con- recreation centers). Another unanswered question is, then, whether
texts, and are focused on building skills. Hence, the core features feelings at home offer a valid explanation for youths’ choices of
of extracurricular or structured activities are similar in Sweden and leisure activities that are structured in many respects but in which
North America. As such, the conditions for youths who participate adults are not present or play a less dominant role or youths’
in structured activities seem to be the same in Sweden as in North choices of activities that involve adults but no structure. A third
America. Structured activities are adult-controlled environments in unanswered question is whether the links to adjustment are differ-
both places. It seems reasonable to assume, therefore, that the ent in this more complex pattern of youths’ leisure activities. These
processes we have proposed generalize beyond Sweden to North are questions for future research.
America. The study has several strengths, however. One is that we used
Some caution should be taken in interpreting causality in our longitudinal data on participation in both structured activities and
results. First, we tested an idea that youths’ feelings at home guide the unstructured activity of hanging out on the streets. We were
their choices in the leisure context; however, the reverse is also therefore able to examine why youths dropped out of structured
possible. That is, youths’ leisure choices might influence parent– activities and began to spend their time hanging out on the streets
adolescent relationships and, in turn, the adolescents’ feelings at and what happened over time. Another strength is that we used
home. For instance, parents might react negatively to a youth’s independent information from peers concerning their involvement
decision to drop out of structured activities, and these reactions in structured activities and therefore eliminated the risk of certain
might change the youth’s feelings at home. This does not appear to perceptual biases in the data. Previous research has shown that
206 PERSSON, KERR, AND STATTIN

youths’ reports of their peers’ behaviors tend to inflate similarity activities in middle childhood: Links to well-being. Journal of Commu-
(Iannotti et al., 1996; Kandel, 1985). Thus, by using independent nity Psychology, 31, 641– 659.
data from peers, we removed the potential risk of youths misre- Fredricks, J. A., Alfeld-Liro, C. J., Hruda, L. Z., Eccles, J. S., Patrick, H.,
porting their peers’ involvement in structured activities. & Ryan, A. M. (2002). A qualitative exploration of adolescents’ com-
Structured leisure activities are generally regarded as an impor- mitment to athletics and the arts. Journal of Adolescent Research, 17,
68 –97.
tant context in youth development. We started out by asking why
Gilman, R. (2001). The relationship between life satisfaction, social inter-
some youths stay in structured activities, whereas others quit, est, and frequency of extracurricular activities among adolescent stu-
switch to unstructured activities, or avoid structured activities dents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 749 –767.
altogether. Past research has mainly theorized about mechanisms Gilman, R., Meyers, J., & Perez, L. (2004). Structured extracurricular
involving reinforcement. In this study, we have focused on ado- activities among adolescents: Findings and implications for school psy-
lescents’ relationships with important others and how these rela- chologists. Psychology in Schools, 41, 31– 41.
tionships might guide their choices in the leisure context. Parents Gould, D., Feltz, D., Horn, T., & Weiss, M. (1982). Reasons for attrition
and peers are important persons in adolescents’ life, and both seem in competitive youth swimming. Journal of Sport Behavior, 5, 155–165.
to play a part in youths’ leisure choices. Thus, adolescents’ rela- Gould, D., Feltz, D., & Weiss, M. (1985). Motives for participating in
tions towards others, even to those beyond the leisure context competitive youth swimming. International Journal of Sport Psychol-
itself, seem to be involved when they choose where to spend their ogy, 16, 126 –140.
Guillet, E., Sarrazin, P., Carpenter, P. J., Trouilloud, D., & Cury, F. (2002).
free time.
Predicting persistence or withdrawal in female handballers with social
exchange theory. International Journal of Psychology, 37, 92–104.
References Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of Cali-
fornia Press.
Agnew, R., & Petersen, D. M. (1989). Leisure and delinquency. Social
Huebner, A. J., & Mancini, J. A. (2003). Shaping structured out-of-school
Problems, 36, 332–350.
time use among youth: The effects of self, family, and friend systems.
Anderson, J. C., Funk, J. B., Elliot, R., & Hull Smith, P. (2003). Parental
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 453– 463.
support and pressure and children’s extracurricular activities: Relation-
Iannotti, R. J., Bush, P. J., & Weinfurt, K. P. (1996). Perceptions of friends’
ships with amount of involvement and affective experience of partici-
use of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana among urban schoolchildren: A
pation. Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 241–257.
longitudinal analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 21, 615– 632.
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a ne-
Kandel, D. B. (1985). On processes of peer influences in adolescent drug
glected construct. Child Development, 67, 3296 –3319.
use: A developmental perspective. Advances in Alcohol and Substance
Brustad, R. J., Babkes, M. L., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Youth in sport:
Abuse, 4, 139 –163.
Psychological considerations. In R. Singer, H. Hausenblas, & C. Janelle
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and
(Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 604 – 636). New York: Wiley.
several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinter-
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of
pretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 36, 366 –380.
youth in our time. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Carpenter, P. J., & Scanlan, T. K. (1998). Changes over time in the Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2003). Parenting of adolescents: Action or reac-
determinants of sport commitment. Pediatric Exercise Science, 10, 356 – tion? In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family
365. dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships (pp. 121–151).
Cooper, H., Valentine, J. C., Nye, B., & Lindsay, J. J. (1999). Relationships Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
between five after-school activities and academic achievement. Journal Kerr, M., Stattin, H., Biesecker, G., & Ferrer-Wreder, L. (2003). Relation-
of Educational Psychology, 91, 369 –378. ships with parents and peers in adolescence. In R. M. Lerner, M. A.
Dishion, T. J., French, D. C., & Patterson, G. R. (1995). The development Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6.
and ecology of antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychology (pp. 395– 422). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Pakalniskiene, V. (in press). What do parents do
(pp. 421– 471). New York: Wiley. when faced with adolescent problem behavior? In M. Kerr, H. Stattin, &
Dishion, T., Nelson, S. E., & Bullock, B. M. (2004). Premature adolescent R. Engels (Eds.), What can parents do? New insights into the role of
autonomy: Parent disengagement and deviant peer process in the am- parents in adolescent problem behavior [Hot Topics in Developmental
plification of problem behaviour. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 515–530. Research series]. London: Wiley.
Duncan, S. C. (1993). The role of cognitive appraisal and friendship Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Trost, K. (1999). To know you is to trust you:
provisions in adolescents’ affect and motivation toward activity in Parents’ trust is rooted in child disclosure of information. Journal of
physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 3, 314 – Adolescence, 22, 737–752.
323. Klint, K. A., & Weiss, M. R. (1986). Dropping in and dropping out:
Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, volunteering, Participation motives of current and former youth gymnasts. Canadian
basketball, or marching band: What kind of extracurricular involvement Journal of Applied Sport Science, 2, 106 –114.
matters? Journal of Adolescent Research, 14, 10 – 43. Klint, K. A., & Weiss, M. R. (1987). Perceived competence and motives
Eccles, J. S., Barber, B. L., Stone, M., & Hunt, J. (2003). Extracurricular for participating in youth sports: A test of Harter’s competence motiva-
activities and adolescent development. Journal of Social Issues, 59, tion theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 55– 65.
865– 889. Landers, D. M., & Landers, D. M. (1978). Socialization via interscholastic
Fincham, F. D., Grych, J. H., & Osborne, L. N. (1994). Does marital athletics: Its effects on delinquency. Sociology of Education, 51, 299 –
conflict cause child maladjustment? Directions and challenges for lon- 303.
gitudinal research. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 128 –140. Larson, R. W. (1994). Youth organizations, hobbies, and sports as devel-
Fletcher, A. C., Elder, G. H., & Mekos, D. (2000). Parental influences on opmental contexts. In R. K. Silberiesen & E. Todt (Eds.), Adolescence in
adolescent involvement in community activities. Journal of Research on context (pp. 47– 65). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Adolescence, 10, 29 – 48. Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. M. (1983). Socialization in the context of
Fletcher, A. C., Nickerson, P., & Wright, K. L. (2003). Structured leisure the family: Parent-child interaction. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hether-
STAYING IN STRUCTURED ACTIVITIES 207

ington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, Johnston, L. D. (1996). Routine activities and deviant behavior. Amer-
personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 1–101). New York: ican Sociological Review, 61, 635– 655.
Wiley. Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., Alfeld-Liro, C., Fredricks, J. A., Hruda, L. Z., &
Magnusson, D., Dunér, A., & Zetterblom, G. (1975). Adjustment: A lon- Eccles, J. S. (1999). Adolescents’ commitment to developing talent: The
gitudinal study. New York: Wiley. role of peers in continuing motivation for sports and the arts. Journal of
Mahoney, J. L. (2000). School extracurricular activity participation as a Youth and Adolescence, 28, 741–763.
moderator in the development of antisocial patterns. Child Development, Persson, A., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2004). Why a leisure context is linked
71, 502–516. to normbreaking for some girls and not others: Personality characteris-
Mahoney, J. L., & Cairns, R. B. (1997). Do extracurricular activities tics and parent-child relations as explanations. Journal of Adolescence,
protect against early school dropout? Developmental Psychology, 33, 27, 583–598.
241–253. Rodin, J., & Langer, E. J. (1977). Long-term effects of a control-relevant
Mahoney, J. L., Larson, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Lord, H. (2005). Organized intervention with the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and
activities as developmental contexts for children and adolescents. In J. L. Social Psychology, 35, 897–902.
Mahoney, R. W. Larson, & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Organized activities as Sarrazin, P., Vallerand, R., Guillet, E., Pelletier, L., & Cury, F. (2002).
contexts of development: Extracurricular activities, after-school and Motivation and dropout in female handballers: A 21-month prospective
community programs (pp. 3–22). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. study. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 395– 418.
Mahoney, J. L., Schweder, A. E., & Stattin, H. (2002). Structured after- Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state
school activities as a moderator of depressed mood for adolescents with of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177.
detached relations to their parents. Journal of Community Psychology, Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and
30, 69 – 86. death. San Francisco: Freeman.
Mahoney, J. L., & Stattin, H. (2000). Leisure activities and adolescent Smith, A. L. (1999). Perceptions of peer relationships and physical activity
antisocial behavior: The role of structure and social context. Journal of participation in early adolescence. Journal of Sports & Exercise Psy-
Adolescence, 23, 113–127. chology, 21, 329 –350.
Mahoney, J. L., Stattin, H., & Lord, H. (2004). Unstructured youth recre- Stattin, H., Kerr, M., Mahoney, J., Persson, A., & Magnusson, D. (2005).
ation centre participation and antisocial behaviour development: Selec- Explaining why a leisure context is bad for some girls and not for others.
tion influences and the moderating role of antisocial peers. International In J. L. Mahoney, R. W. Larson, & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Organized
Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 553–560. activities as contexts of development: Extracurricular activities, after-
Mahoney, J. L., Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (2001). Youth recreation school and community programs (pp. 211–235). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
centre participation and criminal offending: A 20 year longitudinal study U.S. Department of Education. (1995). The condition of education. Wash-
of Swedish boys. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, ington, DC: Government Printing Office.
509 –520. Weiss, M., Kimmel, L. A., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Determinants of sport
Marsh, H. W. (1992). Extracurricular activities: Beneficial extension of the commitment among junior tennis players: Enjoyment as a mediating
traditional curriculum or subversion of academic goals? Journal of variable, Pediatric Exercise Science, 13, 131–144.
Educational Psychology, 84, 553–562. Weiss, M., & Smith, A. L. (2002). Friendship quality in youth sport:
McNeal, R. (1995). Extracurricular activities and high school dropouts. Relationship to age, gender, and motivational variables. Journal of
Sociology of Education, 68, 62– 80. Sports & Exercise Psychology, 24, 420 – 437.
McNeal, R. (1998). High school extracurricular activities: Closed struc-
tured and stratifying patterns of participation. Journal of Education Received July 14, 2005
Research, 91, 183–191. Revision received July 11, 2006
Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J. K., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Accepted August 1, 2006 䡲

You might also like