You are on page 1of 19

Academy of Management Journal

2010, Vol. 53, No. 3, 617635.

JOB ENGAGEMENT: ANTECEDENTS AND EFFECTS ON


JOB PERFORMANCE
BRUCE LOUIS RICH
California State University San Marcos

JEFFREY A. LEPINE
EEAN R. CRAWFORD
University of Florida

We theorize that engagement, conceptualized as the investment of an individuals


complete self into a role, provides a more comprehensive explanation of relationships
with performance than do well-known concepts that reflect narrower aspects of the
individuals self. Results of a study of 245 firefighters and their supervisors supported
our hypotheses that engagement mediates relationships between value congruence,
perceived organizational support, and core self-evaluations, and two job performance
dimensions: task performance and organizational citizenship behavior. Job involve-
ment, job satisfaction, and intrinsic motivation were included as mediators but did not
exceed engagement in explaining relationships among the antecedents and perfor-
mance outcomes.

Popular press articles and business consultants tions that emphasize affect or cognition or the mo-
have claimed that engaged employees give compa- tives for physical persistence in tasks. Yet these
nies competitive advantages (Corporate Leadership explanations do not account for the possibility that
Council, 2006; Gallup Management Journal, 2005). individuals can choose to invest their affective,
However, although scholars have made great cognitive, and physical energies simultaneously
strides over the past decade in identifying corre- into role performances and that this more holistic
lates of engagement (e.g., Harter, Schmidt, & Hayes, investment of the self into ones role represents
2002; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004), little theory or something that is distinct and fundamental (Kahn,
empirical observation accounts for the role of en- 1990, 1992).
gagement as a means through which organizations Accordingly, the purpose of this article is to draw
can create competitive advantages. In particular, from Kahns (1990) work to develop theory that
researchers have not examined the role of engage- positions engagement as a key mechanism explain-
ment as a mechanism that links employee charac- ing relationships among a variety of individual
teristics and organizational factors to employee job characteristics and organizational factors and job
performance. performance. We begin by describing research cen-
This gap in knowledge of engagement may be tered on explanations for job performance that ad-
understandable, given that the concept has a fairly dress narrower aspects of an employees self, high-
brief history and a substantial portion of this re- lighting how these perspectives may be limited
search has been grounded in theories of burnout with respect to explaining why important individ-
and employee well-being (e.g., Maslach & Leiter, ual and organizational factors impact job perfor-
1997). However, Kahn (1990) originally described mance. We then draw from Kahns theory to
engagement as a unique and important motiva- describe how engagement represents the simulta-
tional concept: the harnessing of an employees full neous investment of cognitive, affective, and phys-
self in terms of physical, cognitive, and emotional ical energies into role performance, and how such
energies to work role performances. This concep- investments may better explain relationships with
tualization not only suggests a linkage between en- two different aspects of job performance: task per-
gagement and job performance, but also represents formance and organizational citizenship behavior.
an inclusive view of the employees agentic self, We then draw from Kahns theory to identify three
and thus engagement may provide a more compre- antecedents of engagement: value congruence, per-
hensive explanation for job performance effects ceived organizational support, and core self-evalu-
than is provided by more familiar mechanisms that ations. These concepts have been previously linked
emphasize narrower aspects of the employees self. to job performance, and thus, our theorizing ex-
Researchers have focused on performance explana- tends understanding of why the relationships
617
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holders express
written permission. Users may print, download or email articles for individual use only.
618 Academy of Management Journal June

among these antecedents and job performance oc- tion supplies make people more willing to carry out
cur. We also argue that engagement plays a medi- behaviors associated with tasks that contribute to or-
ating role in relationships between the antecedents ganizational effectiveness (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993;
and the two dimensions of job performance and Judge, Bono, Thoresen, & Patton, 2001).
that this meditational role is more comprehensive Finally, there is theory and research that refers to
than research using familiar and well-researched the self primarily in terms of the physical energies
concepts, such as job involvement, job satisfaction, that are focused on specific task activities resulting
and intrinsic motivation, would suggest, as these from the need to feel competent and maintain au-
concepts rest on a relatively narrow view of the tonomy and control over courses of action. Exem-
self. Finally, we describe a study of firefighters plary is the extensive research on the concept of
designed to test our theoretical model. intrinsic motivation, which is defined as the desire
to exert effort on a task in the absence of external
constraints or contingencies (Deci, 1975; Deci &
THEORY AND HYPOTHESES
Ryan, 1985). Intrinsic motivation is promoted by
The majority of research intended to improve both work contexts and individual differences that
understanding of variability in work role perfor- foster feelings of competence, autonomy, and relat-
mances has focused on explanatory concepts that edness (Gagne & Deci, 2005), and it is argued to
emphasize relatively narrow aspects of employees influence performance because opportunities to
selves. As the first example, researchers have con- satisfy these three intrinsic needs facilitate self-
ceptualized the self in terms of cognitive energy motivation and effective regulatory functioning
that can be allocated in various work and nonwork through internalization of organizationally valued
domains according to identities individuals define goals (Baard, Deci, & Ryan, 2004).
for themselves in reference to the roles they hold Although researchers have examined each of
(Ashforth, Harrison, & Corley, 2008). As a prime these concepts in relationship with job perfor-
example, the concept of job involvement refers to mance, each focuses on a different aspect of the self
the degree to which employees relate to their jobs in explaining why individuals choose to invest
as comprising their lives in total, so that an em- themselves into their work roles. Job involvement
ployee who exhibits high job involvement identi- focuses on the cognitive energy individuals invest
fies strongly with his or her job and thinks about to maintain identities related to work. Job satisfac-
the job even when outside of work (Kanungo, tion focuses on affective reactions and the need to
1982). Job involvement is influenced by organiza- maintain happiness. Intrinsic motivation focuses
tion characteristics, supervisory behaviors, and in- on individuals effort and persistence dedicated to
dividual differences (Brown & Leigh, 1996) and is maintaining autonomy and control. When consid-
reasoned to predict job performance because indi- ered as an aggregate, these explanations comple-
viduals who identify the most strongly with their ment one another in explaining variability in per-
jobs focus their thoughts on work and interpret formance. However, this approach does not
more situations as opportunities to perform work account for the possibility that investments of cog-
role activities (Hillman, Nicholson, & Shropshire, nitive, emotional, or physical energy manifest more
2008; Kreiner, Hollensbe, & Sheep, 2006). fundamental choices to invest these three aspects
Another stream of work based on a relatively of the self in a holistic and connected manner
narrow explanation of the self is research in which (Goffman, 1961). We argue that this fundamental
it is conceptualized in terms of emotional reactions underlying mechanism is embodied by Kahns
that are associated with the human desire to fulfill (1990) engagement concept. Because this concept
subjective psychological needs or values. For ex- accounts for the simultaneous investment of avail-
ample, there is a rich history of research on the able energies into a work role, it provides a more
concept of job satisfaction, which refers to a plea- comprehensive explanation for job performance
surable or positive emotional state resulting from than do concepts that depict the self more narrowly.
the appraisal of ones job or job experience (Locke,
1976: 1300). Job satisfaction is promoted through
Kahns Engagement
favorable perceptions of job characteristics, super-
visors, and coworkers (Russell, Spitzmuller, Lin, In an article reporting results of theory-generat-
Stanton, Smith, & Ironson, 2004) and is also influ- ing ethnographic research, Kahn formally defined
enced by differences in individual personality engagement as the simultaneous employment and
(Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002). The positive feel- expression of a persons preferred self in task be-
ings associated with high job satisfaction that result haviors that promote connections to work and to
from favorable evaluations of what their organiza- others, personal presence (physical, cognitive, and
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 619

emotional) and active, full performances (1990: gregated value to an organization of the set of be-
700). In engagement, organization members har- haviors that an employee contributes both directly
ness their full selves in active, complete work role and indirectly to organizational goals (Borman &
performances by driving personal energy into phys- Motowidlo, 1993; Campbell, 1990). We chose a be-
ical, cognitive, and emotional labors. Engaged in- havioral conceptualization of job performance be-
dividuals are described as being psychologically cause engagement is a concept that reflects human
present, fully there, attentive, feeling, connected, agency, and thus it is appropriate to focus on con-
integrated, and focused in their role performances. sequences that are largely under an employees
They are open to themselves and others, connected volitional control. Moreover, because behavioral
to work and others, and bring their complete selves performance has multiple dimensions, this per-
to perform (Kahn, 1992). Kahn noted that engage- spective can provide insight into the specific types
ment is observed through the behavioral invest- of employee behaviors that transmit the effects of
ment of personal physical, cognitive, and emo- engagement to more objective outcomes, such as
tional energy into work roles (Kahn, 1992). People productivity, efficiency, and quality.
exhibit engagement when they become physically Kahn (1990) did not explicitly outline a relation-
involved in tasks, whether alone or with others; are ship between engagement and job performance.
cognitively vigilant, focused, and attentive; and are However, we have strong theoretical reasons to be-
emotionally connected to their work and to others lieve that such a link exists. At a general level,
in the service of their work (Kahn, 1990). Put sim- employees who are highly engaged in their work
ply, engagement involves investing the hands, roles not only focus their physical effort on the
head, & heart (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995: 110) pursuit of role-related goals, but are also cogni-
in active, full work performance. tively vigilant and emotionally connected to the
Kahns engagement concept is motivational be- endeavor (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995; Kahn,
cause it refers to the allocation of personal re- 1990). In contrast, employees who are highly dis-
sources to role performance and also to how in- engaged in their work roles withhold their physi-
tensely and persistently those resources are applied cal, cognitive, and emotional energies, and this is
(Kanfer, 1990). Engagement, however, subsumes reflected in task activity that is, at best, robotic,
the traditional focus on physical or cognitive effort passive, and detached (Goffman, 1961; Hochschild,
allocated to specific tasks or sets of tasks, as it 1983; Kahn, 1990).
reflects bringing forth increasing depths of the self At a more specific level, theoretical research has
in the service of ones broadly defined role. In other linked investments of the three energies of en-
words, although individuals can be involved in gagement to job performance. First, investment of
their work roles physically, cognitively, or emo- physical energy into work roles contributes to
tionally, engagement is maintaining these involve- organizational goals because it facilitates the ac-
ments simultaneously in a connected rather than complishment of organizationally valued behaviors
fragmented manner (Kahn, 1992). Rather than the at increased levels of effort over extended periods
summation of the various energies that can be of time (Kahn, 1990, 1992). Because peoples work
brought to a role, engagement reflects their com- roles are defined largely by behavioral expectations
monalitya common cause of the investment of of others in their organization (Katz & Kahn, 1978),
the various energies. Thus, from the perspective of investments of physical energy toward role accom-
Kahn, job engagement is best described as a multi- plishment should result in a greater likelihood of
dimensional motivational concept reflecting the si- meeting these expectations, and thus, judgments
multaneous investment of an individuals physical, that the role holder is a positive contributor to the
cognitive, and emotional energy in active, full work organization. Brown and Leigh (1996) found in
performance. In even more direct terms, engage- multiple samples that employees who worked
ment is a multidimensional motivational construct harder exhibited higher levels of job performance.
of the latent form with dimensions serving as indi- Second, investment of cognitive energy into
cators of the higher-order engagement concept work roles contributes to organizational goals be-
(Law, Wong, & Mobley, 1998). cause it promotes behavior that is more vigilant,
attentive, and focused (Kahn, 1990). Weick and
Roberts (1993) used the term heedfulness as a
Job Performance Consequences of Engagement
label for behaviors that possess this same set of
As stated previously, the overarching purpose of characteristics, and those authors noted that when
this article is to provide insight into the role that heedfulness declines because of reductions in in-
engagement plays in relationships with job perfor- vestments of cognitive energy, performance decre-
mance. Here we define job performance as the ag- ments result from failures to see, to take note of, or
620 Academy of Management Journal June

to be attentive to ones work role. In their research most common label for these performance behav-
on flight deck operators on an aircraft carrier, iors is organizational citizenship behavior (OCB;
Weick and Roberts observed that as the degree of Organ, 1988). To the extent that engaged employees
heedfulness increased, crucial operational errors invest themselves more fully while at work than do
decreased. those who are less engaged, they should be more
Finally, investments of emotional energy into willing to step outside the bounds of their formally
work roles contribute to organizational goals in a defined jobs and engage in acts that constitute OCB.
number of related ways (Kahn, 1990). Those who Moreover, to the extent that engagement is reflected
invest emotional energy into their roles enhance by heedfulness and connectedness to ones work
performance through the promotion of increased (Kahn, 1992), it may foster a mental frame in which
connection among coworkers in pursuit of organi- ones role is perceived to include a wider array of
zational goals (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995). In- behaviors that could ultimately benefit the organi-
vestments of emotional energies also help individ- zation. Indeed, Kahn (1990, 1992) argued that the
uals meet the emotional demands of their roles in a physical, cognitive, and emotional energies of en-
way that results in more complete and authentic gagement foster active, complete role performances
performance (Kahn, 1990, 1992). through behavior that is extra conscientious, inter-
Relationships with task performance. Individ- personally collaborative, innovative, and involved.
ual job performance consists of distinct sets of ac-
tivities that contribute to an organization in differ- Hypothesis 2. Job engagement is positively re-
ent ways (Campbell, 1990). Accordingly, it is lated to organizational citizenship behavior.
important to consider how different aspects of job
performance might be influenced by engagement.
Antecedents of Job Engagement
The first narrow aspect of job performance is task
performance, defined as those activities that are Kahn (1990) assumed that individuals percep-
directly involved in the accomplishment of core job tions of their work contexts and their own individ-
tasks, or activities that directly support the accom- ual characteristics foster psychological conditions
plishment of tasks involved in an organizations that directly influence the willingness to person-
technical core (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993). Be- ally engage in work roles. This general causal flow
haviors that comprise task performance are estab- is similar to Hackman and Oldhams (1980) notion
lished and central to any given job; there is consen- that job characteristics impact critical psychologi-
sus about what they are; and they are relatively cal states that influence peoples internal work mo-
static over time (Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1991). Because tivation. Kahn suggested three direct psychological
engaged individuals invest their physical, cogni- conditions for engagement, each of which can be
tive, and emotional energies into their work roles, thought of in terms of a question people ask them-
they should exhibit enhanced performance because selves prior to choosing to personally engage or
they work with greater intensity on their tasks for disengage from their role: (1) How meaningful is it
longer periods of time, they pay more attention to for me to bring myself into this performance? (2)
and are more focused on responsibilities, and they How safe is it to do so? and (3) How available am I
are more emotionally connected to the tasks that to do so? Kahn also theorized that characteristics of
constitute their role. employees and organizations drive beliefs regard-
ing these three questionswhich we will refer to as
Hypothesis 1. Job engagement is positively re-
psychological meaningfulness, safety, and avail-
lated to task performance.
ability. Specifically, perceptions of organizational
Relationships with organizational citizenship and work factors related to tasks and roles are the
behavior. Job performance not only includes task primary influences on psychological meaningful-
performance, but also the less formal emergent ness; perceptions of social systems related to sup-
behaviors that contribute to organizations less di- port and relationships are the primary influences
rectly (Motowidlo, Borman, & Schmit, 1997). These on psychological safety; and self-perceptions of
types of behaviors, which include helpfulness, confidence and self-consciousness are the primary
sportsmanship, conscientiousness, and civic virtue influences on psychological availability. In the
(Organ, 1988), do not contribute directly to an or- present research, we consider a focal antecedent
ganizations technical core, but rather, they contrib- from each of these categories: value congruence,
ute to the organization by fostering a social and perceived organizational support, and core self-
psychological environment conducive to the ac- evaluations. These three antecedents have been
complishment of work involved in the organiza- previously linked to job performance, and thus we
tions technical core (Motowidlo et al., 1997). The can consider the degree to which engagement
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 621

serves as an important mechanism through which thy, secure, predictable, and clear in terms of be-
the effects of the antecedents are transmitted. havioral consequences. Kahn suggested that em-
Value congruence. The experience of psycholog- ployees experience psychological safety, in part, as
ical meaningfulness involves a sense of return on a result of supportive management and supportive
investments of the self in role performance (Kahn, and trusting interpersonal relationships with oth-
1990). Individuals who experience meaningfulness ers in their organization. Individuals with trusting
tend to feel worthwhile, useful, valuable, and able interpersonal relationships in supportive organiza-
to give themselves to their work role and to others tional environments are able to take risks, expose
(Kahn, 1990). According to Kahn (1990, 1992), one their real selves, and try and perhaps fail without
important influence of meaningfulness is the con- fearing the consequences (Kahn, 1990). Kahn fur-
gruence between the behaviors expected by an or- ther suggested that individuals feel safer when they
ganization and the behaviors that individual em- have some control over their work and that mana-
ployees value as a part of their own self-images. gerial reluctance to loosen its control sends a mes-
That is, when employees find that their roles call sage that employees are not to be trusted and should
for behaviors that are congruent with how they like fear overstepping their boundaries. Thus, supportive
to see themselves (their preferred self-images), they management and interpersonal relationships foster
are more likely to find their roles inviting, valuable, feelings of psychological safety that increase willing-
and worthwhile and more willing to fully engage ness to engage fully in work roles.
themselves (Kahn, 1992). When individuals find Perceived organizational support, a concept that
that their role expectations pull for behaviors that reflects the type of support Kahn (1990) discussed,
they feel are inappropriate for their preferred self- develops through employee interactions with or-
images, they feel devalued, taken advantage of, and ganizational agents such as supervisors and reflects
less willing to give themselves to their work roles employees beliefs concerning the extent to which
(Kahn, 1990, 1992). the organization they work for values their contri-
Because organizational values are communicated butions and cares about their well-being (Eisen-
to organization members in terms of what behav- berger, Huntington, Hutchison, & Sowa, 1986). Em-
iors are appropriate and expected for their work ployees who perceive high organizational support
roles (Chatman, 1989; Ravlin & Meglino, 1987), and have positive and secure expectations concerning
because personal values reflect, in part, behavioral the organizations likely reaction to employees
standards and desires involved in ones self-image contributions as well as their mistakes, and thus
(Cable & Edwards, 2004; Locke, 1976), the role of they have less reason to fear incurring damaging
value congruence becomes clear. That is, when in- consequences for their self-images, statuses, or ca-
dividuals believe that their personal values are con- reers as a result of investing themselves fully into
gruent with those of the organization for which their work roles (Edmondson, 1999). When per-
they work, they perceive that organizational role ceived organizational support is low, employees
expectations are congruent with their preferred are unsure of what to expect, fear that they may
self-images (Chatman, 1989; Kahn, 1990, 1992; suffer for their personal engagement, and choose to
Kristof, 1996), and thus they should find more guard their selves by withdrawing from their roles
meaningfulness in their work, and in turn, exhibit (Kahn, 1990). This reasoning is consistent with re-
higher engagement. Research has supported the search showing positive relationships between per-
idea that perceived value congruence facilitates in- ceptions of various forms of support in an organi-
dividuals making greater personal investments in zation and conceptualizations of job engagement
the pursuit of organizational goals because of the similar to Kahns (e.g., Bakker, Demerouti, &
experienced meaningfulness of their work roles Schaufeli, 2005; Nembhard & Edmondson, 2006;
(Brown & Leigh, 1996; May, Gilson, & Harter, 2004); Saks, 2006).
however, no research has examined the relation-
Hypothesis 4. Perceived organizational sup-
ship between perceived value congruence and
port is positively related to job engagement.
Kahns conceptualization of engagement.
Core self-evaluations. Psychological availability
Hypothesis 3. Perceived value congruence is
is described as individuals readiness to personally
positively related to job engagement.
engage at a particular moment (Kahn, 1990). Indi-
Perceived organizational support. The experi- viduals who are psychologically available perceive
ence of psychological safety is described as feeling themselves to be ready and prepared to put their
able to invest oneself without fear of negative con- physical, cognitive, and emotional energies into
sequences (Kahn, 1990). Individuals feel safe in role performance, and thus, they tend to exhibit
organizational contexts perceived to be trustwor- higher engagement in role performance contexts.
622 Academy of Management Journal June

One of the key influences on availability is an in- performance, and we argue here that engagement
dividuals having a general level of confidence in plays an important role in explaining these
his or her own abilities, status, and self-conscious- relationships.
ness that leaves more or less room for investments As we noted earlier, scholars have offered expla-
of self in role performances (Kahn, 1990). Kahn nations of job performance rooted in theories that
(1992) further suggested that this type of confi- define human agency more narrowly than does en-
dence is a relatively stable individual difference, gagement, and these narrower explanations could
and it operates in such a way that the more gener- also account for relationships between our anteced-
ally confident the individual feels about his or her ents and outcomes. However, whereas concepts
capabilities and status, the more likely the individ- such as job involvement, job satisfaction, and in-
ual is to feel available and prepared to engage fully trinsic motivation suggest performance may be en-
in his or her role. hanced through different aspects of the self that
To a large degree, the confidence that Kahn dis- operate with relative independence, Kahn (1990)
cussed is reflected in the concept of core self- theorized that there is a unique aspect of human
evaluations, a contemporary construct defined as agency that functions in a more holistic, consistent,
individuals appraisals of their own worthiness, and connected manner. To be engaged in a job is
effectiveness, and capability as people (Judge, not just being cognitively attentive to the job, or
Locke, & Durham, 1997). People with high core feeling and expressing positive emotions on the
self-evaluations are well adjusted, positive, self- job, or doing specific job tasks simply for the sake
confident, and efficacious, and they believe in their of doing them. Instead, engagement reflects the si-
own agency (Judge, Erez, Bono, & Thoresen, 2003). multaneous investment of cognitive, emotional,
Individuals with high core self-evaluations ap-
and physical energies in such a way that one is
praise demands more positively, have greater abil-
actively and completely involved in the full perfor-
ity to cope with these demands effectively, and
mance of a role. For these reasons, we expect value
thus have more resources available to invest in the
congruence, perceived organizational support, and
performance of their work roles (Judge & Hurst,
core self-evaluations to affect job performance
2007). Because individuals with high core self-
through investments of the self as reflected by en-
evaluations tend to feel more capable of dealing
gagement, even when job involvement, job satisfac-
with work demands, they should also perceive a
tion, and intrinsic motivation are considered as
higher level of availability to invest themselves into
mediators.
their roles, and thus, core self-evaluations should
be positively related to job engagement. Although
core self-evaluations have been linked to motiva- Hypothesis 6a. Job engagement mediates the
tional concepts such as goal setting and overall task relationship between value congruence and
motivation (e.g., Erez & Judge, 2001), no research task performance.
has linked the concept to job engagement. Hypothesis 6b. Job engagement mediates the
relationship between value congruence and
Hypothesis 5. Core self-evaluations are posi-
and OCB.
tively related to job engagement.
Hypothesis 7a. Job engagement mediates the
relationship between perceived organizational
Mediating Role of Job Engagement support and task performance.
To this point, we have argued that value congru- Hypothesis 7b. Job engagement mediates the
ence, perceived organizational support, and core relationship between perceived organizational
self-evaluations promote the simultaneous invest- support and OCB.
ment of cognitive, emotional, and physical energy
into a work role, and this investment, in turn, trans- Hypothesis 8a. Job engagement mediates the
lates into superior work role performance. In other relationship between core self-evaluations and
words, we have implicitly described a model in task performance.
which engagement mediates relationships among Hypothesis 8b. Job engagement mediates the
its antecedents and job performance activities. In-
relationship between core self-evaluations
deed, researchers have found that value congru-
and OCB.
ence (Kristof-Brown, Zimmerman, & Johnson,
2005), perceived organizational support (Rhoades Hypothesis 9. Job engagement mediates rela-
& Eisenberger, 2002), and core self-evaluations tionships among the antecedents and perfor-
(Judge & Bono, 2001) are positively linked to job mance outcomes when job involvement, job
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 623

satisfaction, and intrinsic motivation are con- a set of existing scales with items that perfectly
sidered as additional mediators. fit the definitions of the dimensions, we were able
to compile a list of items that we felt we could use,
and we supplemented this list with items we wrote
METHODS to fill out the content domain of each dimension.
Sample and Procedures Our review of the literature for existing measures
that might tap Kahns (1992) physical engagement
Participants included 245 full-time firefighters dimension revealed that Brown and Leighs (1996)
and their supervisors employed by four municipal- measure of work intensity, which the authors
ities. Participants were predominately male (87%) defined as the energy exerted per unit of time
and Caucasian (88%) and had completed at least an (1996: 362), came the closest. However, we signif-
associates degree (61%). On average, they were 39 icantly modified the items in Brown and Leighs
years old (s.d. 8.71) and had 11.5 years tenure scale to promote greater conceptual correspon-
(s.d. 8.86). There were no differences among the dence with Kahns conceptualization of the physi-
fire departments in variables that could result in cal engagement dimension. For example, three of
spurious relationships. We distributed and col- the five original items began with the phrase when
lected surveys during working hours. We told par- I work or when theres a job to be done, and we
ticipants that the survey was designed to measure felt this wording created some ambiguity. Specifi-
job attitudes and that their responses to the survey cally, the phrase could be interpreted as limiting
would be kept confidential. the focal situation to those particular, and perhaps
rare, circumstances in which an individual is
present at work for a full day, but only chooses to
Measures
perform role-related activities for a small portion of
Participants rated their own job engagement, job time, during which he or she accomplishes the
involvement, job satisfaction, intrinsic motivation, activitieswith high intensity simply to avoid being
value congruence, perceived organizational sup- reprimanded or fired for not being productive. In
port, and core self-evaluations using a five-point our opinion, this interpretation is not consistent
Likert scale that ranged from strongly disagree (1) with engagement as conceptualized by Kahn, and
to strongly agree (5). so the wording of these items was revised.
Job engagement. Most existing measures of en- To measure the emotional aspect of engagement,
gagement have been criticized for not fully reflect- we drew from Russell and Barretts (1999) research
ing Kahns (1990) conceptualization as the degree on core affect, defined as a somewhat generalized
to which individuals invest their physical, cogni- emotional state consisting of two independent di-
tive, and emotional energies into their role perfor- mensionspleasantness (feeling positive) and acti-
mance (Newman & Harrison, 2008). The most pop- vation (a sense of energy). To be consistent with
ular measure of engagement is the Utrecht Work Kahns description of this aspect of engagement, we
Engagement Scale (UWES; Schaufeli & Bakker, wrote items that refer to emotions that reflect both
2003); however, it includes items that confound high pleasantness and high activation (enthusiasm,
engagement with the antecedent conditions sug- excitement, energy, interest). Further, in keeping
gested by Kahn. For instance, the UWES includes with research on emotions (defined as affective
items that tap respondent perceptions of the level states directed toward something specific [Frijda,
of meaningfulness and challenge of work. Thus, 1993]), we also had the items refer to feelings asso-
although the UWES has gained in popularity as ciated with a particular target, which in the context
interest in engagement has grown, we needed to of the present research was a respondents work
develop a measure that maps more precisely onto role. Measuring work-related emotion within a seg-
Kahns conceptualization. ment of the core affect domain has precedent in the
To this end, we followed practices described by organizational behavior and applied psychological
scholars who have discussed the process of devel- literatures. For example, Bono, Foldes, Vinson, and
oping and validating measures of constructs (e.g., Muros (2007) measured positive workplace emo-
Schwab, 1980). We first searched the literature for tions using a measure that combined scores on
scales and items that fit the definitions of the three items that referred to the degree of enthusiasm,
engagement dimensions as described by Kahn happiness, and optimism experienced at work.
(1990). Our goal was to measure the three dimen- Finally, for the cognitive aspect of engagement,
sions of engagement in such a way that the com- we drew from Rothbards (2001) measure of engage-
monality of those dimensions would adequately ment, which includes both attention (level or
reflect job engagement. Although we did not locate amount of focus and concentration) and absorption
624 Academy of Management Journal June

(level of engrossment or the intensity of the focus thus the items did not appear to reflect a single
and concentration). Although Rothbard distin- engagement factor. Next, we specified a three-factor
guished between these two facets in her analyses, model in which we loaded each item onto its cor-
the dimensions were strongly related, and they had responding engagement dimension. Results of this
remarkably similar zero-order relationships with CFA indicated that this model fit the data well both
other variables in her study. For these reasons, and in an absolute sense (2[132] 302.67, CFI .98,
also because we had no theoretical predictions re- SRMR .05, RMSEA .08) and as compared to the
garding functional differences between the two di- alternative one-factor model (2[3] 704.54, p
mensions, we did not attempt to maintain the dis- .001). The strong interrelationships among the
tinction. Rather, we refined six items from three engagement dimensions (average r .65) sug-
Rothbards scale to promote conceptual consis- gested a commonality indicative of a higher-order
tency with Kahns description of the cognitive as- factor (Kline, 2005; Law et al., 1998). Accordingly,
pect of engagement. we specified an additional model in which we
We first administered our initial 18-item job en- loaded the three first-order engagement dimensions
gagement scale to a convenience sample of 117 onto a second-order engagement dimension. Be-
individuals who were employed full-time in a va- cause the number of estimated endogenous rela-
riety of occupations and organizations. We re- tionships and degrees of freedom in this model are
ceived completed surveys from 84 participants (a the same as those for the model with three corre-
72 percent response rate). About half of the partic- lated engagement dimensions, the fit statistics of
ipants were female, and on average, they were 28 the second-order model indicated exactly the same
years old with 4.6 years of full-time work experi- good fit with the data. However, the second-order
ence. Participants rated the job engagement items factor loadings for the physical, cognitive, and
on a scale that ranged from strongly disagree (1) emotional dimensions were all positive, strong,
to strongly agree (5). We submitted the data to an and statistically significant (.89, .64, and .90, re-
exploratory factor analysis using principal axis fac- spectively), as were the factor loadings on the in-
toring with an oblique rotation. Three factors were dividual items (shown in the Appendix). Thus, in
extracted with eigenvalues greater than 1.00. The keeping with Kahns theorizing, specifying engage-
emotional engagement factor accounted for the larg- ment as a second-order factor was supported.
est amount of variance (57%), followed by the phys- For the main study involving the firefighters, we
ical (11.46%) and cognitive (6.26%) factors. With submitted the engagement items to CFA and again
one exception, factor loadings of items to their cor- found support for the structure of our engagement
responding scale were greater than .71, and there measure as consisting of three first-order factors
was no cross-loading greater than .30. We modified that in turn load on a second-order factor. We con-
the item with a low factor loading (.59) for the ducted analyses that mirrored what we described
subsequent data collection. The items for each di- in the previous paragraph. The hypothesized sec-
mension were averaged and formed reliable scales ond-order model fit the data well (2[132] 391.90,
(internal consistency reliabilities ranged from .89 CFI .97, SRMR .05, RMSEA .09), signifi-
to .94). The strong correlations among the scales cantly better than the one-factor alternative (2[3]
(r .63.74) supported their aggregation to an over- 1,073.12, p .001). The first-order (see the Appen-
all job engagement scale, which was also reliable dix) and second-order factor loadings (.90, .72, .79)
from an internal consistency standpoint (.95). from the hypothesized model were strong, statisti-
We then cross-validated the job engagement scale cally significant, and similar to the cross-validation
(after modifying the one potentially problematic sample in size.
item) in a sample of 180 employees of a skilled Other self-report measures. We measured value
care nursing facility. Participants in this sample congruence using three items from Caldwell, Chat-
were predominately female (81%) and Caucasian man, and OReilly (1990) that focus on the align-
(73.3%), had completed at least an associates de- ment of employee values with organizational val-
gree (55.5%), and on average were 43 years in age ues. We assessed perceived organizational support
(s.d. 15.5). To assess the structure of the engage- with a 6-item scale developed by Eisenberger,
ment scale in this sample, we specified a series of Armeli, Rexwinkel, Lynch, and Rhodes (2001). We
models and tested them using confirmatory factor used a 12-item measure of core self-evaluations
analysis (CFA). We first fit the data to a one-factor from Judge and colleagues (Judge et al., 2003). We
model in which all 18 items loaded on a single measured job involvement using Kanungos (1982)
latent variable. The results of this model indicated 10-item scale. We measured job satisfaction using
poor fit to the data in an absolute sense (2[135] Cammann, Fichman, Jenkins, and Kleshs (1983)
1,007.21, CFI .89, SRMR .12, RMSEA .24); 3-item scale of general or overall job satisfaction.
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 625

Finally, we measured intrinsic motivation with the tural models in which all of the relationships with job
corresponding 4 items from the Situational Motiva- engagement are embedded.
tion Scale (Guay, Vallerand, & Blanchard, 2000).
Supervisor-report measures. Participants su-
Measurement Model
pervisors completed a job performance question-
naire that included 5 items from Williams and Following convention, we assessed the fit of our
Andersons (1991) task performance scale, and Lee data to a measurement model prior to assessing
and Allens (2002) 16-item OCB scale. Supervisors substantive relationships. Model 1 is the proposed
scored the items using a five-point Likert scale that measurement model, for which we loaded each
ranged from strongly disagree (1) to strongly individual item onto its respective higher order
agree (5). To assess whether the ratings lacked factor (value congruence, perceived organizational
independence, we calculated intraclass correlation support, core self-evaluations, job engagement, job
coefficients, or ICC1s, using the between- and with- involvement, job satisfaction, intrinsic motivation,
in-supervisor variance components estimated from task performance, organizational citizenship be-
random coefficient models. Supporting indepen- havior), which we allowed to correlate. In keeping
dence, the ICC1s for both task performance and with the previously discussed CFA results, we
OCB were very small, and not close to reaching loaded the engagement items onto the respective
statistical significance (ICC1task performance .01, first-order factor, which were then loaded onto a
2[110] 94.96, p .85; and ICC1OCB .05, second-order factor. We scaled each latent variable
2[110] 107.79, p .54). by setting a factor loading of an indicator to 1.00.
To account for variance due to measurement arti-
facts, we allowed error variances from the two neg-
RESULTS
atively worded job involvement items to correlate.
Table 1 reports descriptive statistics and correla- Similarly, to account for item wording similarity,
tions among all study variables. As shown in the we allowed error variances for two of the job satis-
table, the study variables all possess an acceptable faction items to correlate.
degree of internal consistency reliability. Of more As the fit statistics in Table 2 indicate, model 1 fit
substantive interest, job engagement is associated the data well in an absolute sense (2[2,809]
with the conceptual antecedents and consequences 5,097.90, CFI .95, SRMR .10, RMSEA .06),
in the manner that we hypothesized. That is, indi- and the loadings of the items onto their respective
viduals reported they were more engaged in their latent variables were statistically significant and
jobs when they also reported higher levels of value strong (average estimate/standard error 11.70;
congruence, perceived organizational support, and average loading .70). We compared model 1 to
core self-evaluations. Also, individuals reporting more parsimonious nested alternatives that com-
higher levels of engagement tended to receive bined latent constructs to assess the discriminant
higher supervisor ratings of task performance and validity of the latent variables, and in no case did
organizational citizenship behavior. Although these an alternative model fit the data as well as the
zero-order correlations are meaningful and provide hypothesized model. Because of our focus on en-
preliminary support for our theorizing, we formally gagement in this study, we report results of models
tested our hypotheses by specifying a series of struc- in which we combined job engagement with job

TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Key Study Variablesa

Variable Mean s.d. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Job engagement 4.32 0.49 (.95)


2. Job involvement 2.75 0.64 .47* (.86)
3. Job satisfaction 4.36 0.62 .56* .48* (.83)
4. Intrinsic motivation 3.76 0.52 .35* .42* .37* (.70)
5. Value congruence 3.75 0.70 .44* .27* .44* .19* (.70)
6. Perceived organizational support 3.30 0.90 .45* .48* .64* .34* .52* (.92)
7. Core self-evaluations 3.88 0.39 .34* .19* .24* .14* .21* .21* (.72)
8. Task performance 4.27 0.55 .35* .26* .29* .21* .21* .23* .12 (.90)
9. Organizational citizenship 4.04 0.53 .35* .23* .30* .18* .29* .32* .14* .63* (.93)

a
n 245. Coefficient alpha reliabilities are on the diagonal in parentheses.
* p .05
626 Academy of Management Journal June

TABLE 2
Measurement Modela

Structure 2 df CFI SRMR RMSEA 90% CI 2(df)b

Model 1: Nine factors 5,097.90 2,809 .95 .10 .06 .06.06


Model 2: Eight factors, JE & JI combined 5,176.07 2,810 .95 .18 .06 .06.06 78.17 (1)
Model 3: Eight factors, JE & JS combined 5,113.65 2,810 .95 .13 .06 .06.06 15.75 (1)
Model 4: Eight factors, JE & IM combined 5,170.11 2,810 .95 .16 .06 .06.06 72.21 (1)
Model 5: Eight factors, JE & VC combined 5,151.37 2,810 .95 .15 .06 .06.06 53.47 (1)
Model 6: Eight factors, JE & POS combined 5,130.49 2,810 .95 .16 .06 .06.06 32.59 (1)
Model 7: Eight factors, JE & CSE combined 5,200.98 2,810 .95 .16 .06 .06.06 103.08 (1)
Model 8: Eight factors, TP & OCB combined 5,140.81 2,810 .95 .17 .06 .06.06 42.91 (1)

a
n 245. JE, job engagement; JI, job involvement; JS, job satisfaction; IM, intrinsic motivation; VC, value congruence; POS, perceived
organizational support; CSE, core self-evaluations; TP, task performance; OCB, organizational citizenship behavior; CFI, comparative fit
index; SRMR, standardized root-mean-square residual; RMSEA, root-mean-square error of approximation; 90% CI, 90% RMSEA confi-
dence interval. All 2 and 2 values are p .001.
b
2 tests relative to model 1.

involvement (model 2), job satisfaction (model 3), Therefore, we specified a second structural model,
intrinsic motivation (model 4), value congruence nested within the first, in which we eliminated the
(model 5), perceived organizational support (model six direct effects of the independent variables on
6), and core self-evaluations (model 7). To support the dependent variables. This model also fit the
the discriminant validity of the supervisor-rated data well (2[2,823] 5,238.43, CFI .95, SRMR
constructs, we also report a comparison of the fit of .11, RMSEA .06). In fact, the fit of this model to
the data to a model in which we combined task the data was the same as the fit of the first model
performance and OCB (model 8). The statistics and (2[6] 7.90, p .25). Thus, the second model
fit indexes in Table 2 show that none of the alter- was superior to the first because it was more parsi-
native models fit the data as well as our hypothe- monious and fit the data equally well.
sized model. Chi-square difference tests indicated The standardized path estimates from the second
statistically significant differences in model fit fa- model, depicted in Figure 1, indicate support for
voring model 1. In sum, this analysis supported the Hypotheses 1 and 2 in that the paths from job
adequacy of the measures testing substantive engagement to task performance and OCB were
relationships. positive and statistically significant ( .25 and
.27, respectively). In other words, supervisors of
Tests of Substantive Relationships individuals who indicated they were highly en-
gaged reported that these individuals had higher
In keeping with the theory we outlined earlier,
levels of task performance and OCB. Hypotheses 3,
we specified a model in which value congruence,
4, and 5 were also supported, as the paths to en-
perceived organizational support, and core self-
gagement from value congruence, perceived organ-
evaluations predicted job engagement, job involve-
ment, job satisfaction, and intrinsic motivation, izational support, and core self-evaluations were
which in turn predicted task performance and or- positive and statistically significant ( .35, .37,
ganizational citizenship behavior. Because theory and .36, respectively). Individuals reported being
and empirical research indicate that task perfor- more highly engaged when they perceived higher
mance and OCB are reflections of a broader job value congruence, organizational support, and core
performance construct (e.g., Hoffman, Blair, Me- self-evaluations.
riac, & Woehr, 2007), we allowed the disturbance The path estimates in Figure 1 also reveal that
terms on these latent variables to correlate. We first although job involvement, job satisfaction, and in-
specified a model that included these relationships trinsic motivation were predicted by the indepen-
together with direct effects of the independent vari- dent variables, these three variables did not have
ables on the dependent variables. any statistically significant relationships with the
This first structural model fit the data well in an two job performance outcomes. Although job in-
absolute sense (2[2,817] 5,230.53, CFI .95, volvement, job satisfaction, and intrinsic motiva-
SRMR .11, RMSEA .06). However, there were tion appear to have meaningful relationships
no statistically significant direct relationships be- with job performance when considered alone in a
tween the independent and dependent variables. zero-order sense, they appear to have little pre-
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 627

FIGURE 1
Structural Model with Engagement and Other Affective-Motivational Statesa

Job .25*
Engagement
.35*
.27*
Value Congruence .06
Task
Performance
.22*
.03
.07
Job
Involvement

.37* .05
Perceived
.48*
Organizational
Support
.62* .14
.38* Job
Satisfaction
.15

.11 Organizational
.36* Citizenship
.15* Behavior
Core
Self-Evaluations .02
.15
Intrinsic
Motivation .02

a
Standardized paths. Only relationships among the highest-order latent variables shown.
* p .05

dictive relevance when considered along with when the zero-order relationship between an inde-
engagement. pendent and dependent variable is nonsignificant,
Thus far, our analyses support the mediating role as might be the case when the independent variable
of engagement in relationships between value con- is more distal or when the effect of the independent
gruence, perceived organizational support, and variable is masked by offsetting indirect effects of
core self-evaluations and the two aspects of job other mediators (Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger, 1998;
performance. To more thoroughly examine media- Shrout & Bolger, 2002).
tion, we examined the pattern of indirect relation- In Table 3 we report Sobel (1982) tests of statis-
ships attributable to the various mediators. Mean- tical significance using unstandardized estimates
ingful indirect relationships may be observed even and corresponding standard errors for indirect ef-

TABLE 3
Tests of Indirect Relationships through Engagement and Other Affective-Motivational Statesa

Indirect Effect Through

Job Job Job Intrinsic


Relationship Engagement Involvement Satisfaction Motivation

Value congruence3Task performance 0.12* 0.01 0.04 0.00


Value congruence3Organizational citizenship 0.11* 0.00 0.04 0.00
Perceived organizational support3Task performance 0.08* 0.03 0.08 0.01
Perceived organizational support3Organizational citizenship 0.07* 0.02 0.06 0.00
Core self-evaluations3Task performance 0.18* 0.02 0.04 0.01
Core self-evaluations3Organizational citizenship 0.16* 0.01 0.04 0.00

a
n 245.
* p .05
628 Academy of Management Journal June

fects through job engagement, job involvement, job tended to be helpful, courteous, and involved in
satisfaction, and intrinsic motivation. In support of organizational matters. The degree to which en-
Hypotheses 6a and 6b, value congruence exhibited gagement predicts such a wide array of behavioral
statistically significant indirect effects on both task activities is noteworthy, and research could de-
performance and OCB through engagement. In sup- velop and test more nuanced theoretical explana-
port of Hypotheses 7a and 7b, perceived organiza- tions of why this might be true. For example, per-
tional support exhibited statistically significant in- haps engagement increases the breadth of the
direct effects on both task performance and OCB activities that individuals consider to be part of
through engagement. Finally, in support of Hypoth- their roles, and accordingly, engaged individuals
eses 8a and 8b, core self-evaluations exhibited sta- do not distinguish among activities that reflect task
tistically significant indirect effects on both task performance and OCB when they make choices
performance and OCB through engagement. These about how to allocate their physical, cognitive, and
indirect relationships were obtained in models that emotional energies at workthey simply throw
included job involvement, job satisfaction, and in- their full selves into their roles, which they under-
trinsic motivation, and thus Hypothesis 9 was sup- stand to include any activity that could potentially
ported. Finally, as shown in the third, fourth, and contribute to their effectiveness.
fifth columns of Table 3, no statistically significant Of course, this issue raises the question of
indirect relationships can be attributed to job in- whether there may be circumstances in which we
volvement, job satisfaction, or intrinsic motivation. might expect to find differences in relationships
between engagement and the two types of perfor-
mance criteria. For example, differences in rela-
DISCUSSION tionships could appear in job contexts where dis-
tinctions among the behavioral elements that
Theoretical Contributions
constitute task performance and OCB are large. The
Our primary theoretical contribution is that we line between whether helping behavior directed
extended Kahns (1990) theory by considering the toward a fellow firefighter is task performance or
degree to which engagement serves as an important citizenship behavior may be blurred because the
mechanism through which the antecedents of en- behavior likely contributes to the employing organ-
gagement impact job performance. We argued that ization both directly (through task accomplishment
Kahns theory provides for a more complete repre- that requires cooperation and teamwork) and indi-
sentation of the self in terms of the energies that rectly (by fostering a positive social-psychological
individuals invest in their roles, and in doing so, climate). In such contexts, and as we noted in the
engagement provides a more comprehensive expla- previous paragraph, it may be that the relationships
nation of mediation than do mechanisms that con- with engagement would be similar because the em-
cern narrower aspects of the self. In support of this ployees see their role as including both elements.
argument, we found statistically significant indi- In contrast, we may observe differences in relation-
rect relationships through engagement between ships with engagement in contexts where the line
each of the antecedents and each of the outcomes, between task performance and OCB is easier to
and these relationships emerge in models that also appreciate. In a commission-based sales job, for
include job involvement, job satisfaction, and in- example, helping behavior directed toward another
trinsic motivation as mediators. In fact, our results salesperson would more likely be viewed as some-
indicate that engagement fully accounts for the re- thing clearly outside the bounds of normal role
lationships between the antecedents and the per- activities that primarily involve customer interac-
formance outcomes. Although we certainly could tion. With such a possibility in mind, researchers
have examined the mediating role of additional could test theory that explains how relationships
variables in our research, no remaining direct ef- between job engagement and job performance de-
fects with engagement were included in the model, pend on the nature of job performance.
and accordingly, there were no remaining effects Our research also provides a better understand-
for any additional variables to mediate. ing of the etiology of job engagement. Kahn (1990)
Our research also illuminates the nature of the suggested that engagement is rooted in the psycho-
behavioral contributions to their organizations logical conditions of meaningfulness, safety, and
made by employees as a function of their job en- availability and that perceptions of self and of work
gagement. More specifically, firefighters in our context cause these psychological conditions. We
study who were engaged not only invested their applied this framework and identified three ante-
energy into executing the tasks involved in fighting cedents of engagement that we considered in our
fires and dealing with other emergencies, but also research: value congruence, perceived organization-
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 629

al support, and core self-evaluations. Ours is the be supplemented with concepts that are found to be
first research to link this particular set of anteced- conceptually unique. For example, it could be worth-
ents to Kahns engagement construct, and our re- while to examine the influence of self-regulation on
sults illustrate that each antecedent has a unique the three psychological conditions. Feedback indicat-
effect on engagement and that all these effects are of ing negative discrepancies between goals and role
near-equal magnitude. We note that the patterns of performance creates a sense of challenge and an in-
relationships among the antecedents and the other centive for investment of the self in a role, and in
mediators are different from the pattern of relation- turn, this should foster a greater sense of meaningful-
ships with engagement. All three antecedents pre- ness. The same negative feedback, however, may be
dict job satisfaction; however, the effect of per- threatening to an individuals self-image and may
ceived organizational support is stronger than the therefore reduce feelings of psychological safety. Fu-
effects of value congruence and core self-evalua- ture research could directly examine these relation-
tions. Job involvement is only predicted by value ships, as well as possible solutions to the implied
congruence and perceived organizational support. dilemma. As one idea, Kahn noted that psychological
Intrinsic motivation is only predicted by perceived safety could be promoted in nonthreatening contexts
organizational support. Although we did not antic- in which there is consistency, predictability, and re-
ipate these specific patterns of relationships, the spect. Accordingly, it is possible that the literature on
differences in these patterns do provide further interactional justice, defined as the degree to which
support for the distinctiveness of engagement rela- information is communicated with truthfulness, re-
tive to job involvement, job satisfaction, and intrin- spect, politeness, and dignity (e.g., Bies & Moag,
sic motivation. 1986), could be applied to develop a more elaborate
model of the roles that goal setting and self-regulation
play in the process of engagement.
Implications for Future Research
As mentioned in our introduction, although claims
Although the majority of research on engagement abound that employee engagement creates competi-
has been grounded in the literature on burnout and tive advantages for organizations, little theory or em-
employee well-being and has emphasized relation- pirical observation accounts for the means by which
ships with antecedent conditions, our research ex- engagement creates this competitive advantage. Our
plicitly positions engagement as a motivational research provides one answer in that employees who
concept and emphasizes relationships with behav- exhibited higher levels of engagement were found to
ioral consequences. This emphasis is consistent contribute to their organizations with higher levels of
with Ashforth and Humphreys (1995) argument individual task performance and OCB. Future re-
that because engagement accounts for the simulta- search is needed to examine other means by which
neous expression of both strong motivation and engagement contributes to performance advantages
psychological involvement, it is an important mo- for organizations. One potential avenue is to examine
tivational concept with great potential to improve whether engagement manifests itself as a property of
understanding of the mechanisms through which work groups and teams. For example, it is possible
contextual perceptions and behavioral tendencies that work teams develop a characteristic or conta-
ultimately influence behavior and performance. To gion level of employee engagement, so that some
tap this potential, however, research should begin teams members consistently invest their full selves
to focus more explicitly on the development and into their work team roles, while other teams mem-
testing of theory regarding how engagement fits bers do not. This collective engagement might facili-
into other theories of motivation (Kanfer, 1990). tate development of common purpose and cohesive-
For example, because Kahn defined engagement as ness, even connecting the individuals in a kind of
the degree to which people choose to invest their full group mind, which then is mobilized toward the
selves into role-related activities, it could be worth- teams pursuit and achievement of organizational
while to explore implications of engagement in the goals (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995). As teams be-
context of existing cognitive choice theories of moti- come increasingly common as a unit of work organi-
vation (e.g., Naylor, Pritchard, & Ilgen, 1980). In zation, the operation of engagement at the team or
Kahns view, choices to engage are a function of three work group level represents an important means by
psychological conditions that follow from questions which organizations can develop performance advan-
that organization members seem to unconsciously tages. Such research would also inform prior empir-
ask themselves in each situation. Researchers could ical work that has linked engagement to unit-level
examine how these questions correspond to expect- performance (Harter et al., 2002; Salanova, Agut, &
ancies and appraisals from theories of motivation Peiro, 2005; Salanova, Llorens, Cifre, Martnez, &
(e.g., Vroom, 1964), and perhaps Kahns theory could Schaufeli, 2003).
630 Academy of Management Journal June

Limitations cedent concepts with fairly direct relevance to


managers regarding which specific aspects of the
Although the findings of this study are generally
employees and their work contexts could be
supportive of our hypotheses, our research design
shaped to enhance engagement.
had limitations that could be addressed in future
Fourth, although we used multiple data sources,
research. First, the degree to which our results would
our research was cross-sectional, and so any infer-
generalize to other employees and jobs is unknown.
ences regarding causality are limited. Although we
For example, the physical nature of the firefighting
had strong theoretical and logical reasons to presume
job may have helped respondents to distinguish be-
causal ordering, which was subsequently reflected in
tween physical and cognitive energies in a way that
would be more difficult for knowledge workers. our structural equation modeling, we caution readers
Although this may be a valid concern, the factor that alternative causal models are plausible, and
structure of the engagement scale with firefighters these could be examined in future research. For ex-
was similar to that for employees in the skilled nurs- ample, to fully understand how job engagement and
ing facility. Moreover, we were more interested in the job performance are related, researchers could con-
commonality of the factors, rather than in loadings of sider the impact that performance has on job engage-
the first-order factors. ment. Along these same lines, although we consid-
Second, although the majority of research on en- ered job involvement, job satisfaction, and intrinsic
gagement is cross-sectional, including the current motivation as mechanisms through which the ante-
research, Kahn originally described engagement in cedents could impact the criteria, we did not consider
terms of dynamic moments, ebbs and flows, and alternative roles that these concepts might play. For
calibrations of self-in-role (1990: 694). Kahn dis- example, relative to engagement, these variables
tinguished engagement from concepts such as in- could be more distally related to the performance
volvement and commitment, designating the latter criteria. Such a structure would be consistent with
as more generalized states of which organization the idea that job engagement is the motivational con-
members maintain average levels over time and cept most proximally related to behavioral outcomes
specifying the former, engagement, as instead refer- and that positive attitudes such as satisfaction and
ring to specific fluctuations of psychological pres- involvement are reasons why an individual might
ence in particular moments and situations. Kahn become more engaged (Rothbard, 2001). Research fo-
himself called for future research that would de- cused directly on this issue is necessary to help re-
velop dynamic process models explaining how the searchers understand more clearly how job attitudes,
antecedent conditions described above combine to engagement, and job performance are interrelated.
produce moments of personal engagement (Kahn, Fifth, the antecedents and mediators in our model
1990: 717-718). Future research should begin to were all from self-report measures, and therefore, it is
employ experience-sampling, within-subjects de- likely that method variance inflated the relationships
signs and use multilevel modeling to develop and among these variables. However, our primary focus
test models capturing the variance in individuals was on substantive relationships with job perfor-
engagement in their work roles over time. mance variables that were not from the same source
Third, we did not measure three concepts (i.e., and, maybe more important, the tests of mediation
meaningfulness, safety, and availability) that, we were dependent on a fairly complex pattern of rela-
presumed, underlie the relationships among our tionships among the variables that would be very
antecedents and job engagement. Several measures difficult to explain away with method variance alone.
of these concepts exist (e.g., Edmonson, 1999; May Nevertheless, future research could be designed to
et al., 2004), and if we had included them in our reduce reliance on self-reports. It may be possible, for
study, we could have conducted a more direct test example, to obtain ratings of job engagement from
of Kahns theory. We note, however, that the link- peers who work closely with focal employees.
ages among these concepts and engagement have Finally, we conceptualized and measured job
been established theoretically (Kahn, 1990) and performance as behavior, and therefore, it is un-
empirically (May et al., 2004), and so our approach known whether engagement has effects on objec-
of using the previous research as the foundation for tive measures of job performance such as produc-
specifying our model is reasonable. Moreover, to tivity, efficiency, or quality. Although these types
the extent that Kahn positioned the antecedents we of objective measures may be less appropriate for
considered as more distal causes of engagement firefighters, given the nature of the work outcomes
than the three psychological conditions, our study for which they are responsible, it would be worth-
may provide a more conservative test of our med- while to consider their use in research in other
itation hypotheses. Finally, we focused on ante- work contexts. In fact, future research could exam-
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 631

ine a more comprehensive meditational chain for example, emphasized the motivational nature of
whereby various performance behaviors are posi- engagement, and so we do not know whether en-
tioned as mediators between engagement and the gagement conceptualized and measured largely as
objective indicators of performance. an attitude (e.g., Harter et al., 2002) or as well-being
(Schaufeli & Bakker, 2003) would function simi-
larly in terms of enhancing job performance. Thus,
until more is known about the correspondence
Practical Contributions
among the various measures of engagement, prac-
Although our research was primarily intended to titioners should pay special attention to whether
test theoretically derived hypotheses, our findings do the measure of engagement being considered for
have practical implications. First, and at a very gen- application has shown evidence of being linked
eral level, our results suggest that practices that en- specifically to the criteria of interest.
gender engagement among employees can enhance
job performance, and these improvements in job per-
formance are likely to come in the form of both task Conclusion
performance and organizational citizenship behavior.
However, although the relevance of engagement to Kahns perspective on engagement has been cited
job performance may be important in and of itself, as providing a sound conceptual basis for research
what may be more noteworthy is the greater useful- on engagement (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995; May
ness of engagement in predicting job performance et al., 2004; Rothbard, 2001; Salanova et al., 2005;
relative to job involvement, job satisfaction, and in- Schaufeli, Salanova, Gonzalez-Roma, & Bakker,
trinsic motivation. This pattern of findings suggests 2002). We further argued here that Kahns concep-
that rather than spreading resources over various tualization of engagement is important to consider
practices aimed at assessing and improving a variety as a mechanism that transmits effects of individual
of attitudes and motivational states, it may be worth- and organizational factors to different aspects of job
while to focus resources on practices that assess and performance. Whereas previous research has fo-
enhance employee engagement. cused on mechanisms that emphasize narrow as-
Second, our research shows that higher levels of pects of the self, Kahns engagement concept ac-
value congruence, perceived organizational sup- counts for the simultaneous employment of several
port, and core self-evaluations are associated with aspects of the self in a work role and thus provides
higher levels of employee engagement. Although a more comprehensive understanding of perfor-
there is already good reason to believe in the value mance. Results of our study strongly supported a
of management practices that can increase the prev- theoretical model grounded in this idea. Relative to
alence of these factors at work, their strong impact other mechanisms that reflect narrower views of
on engagement provides an additional reason for the self, investments of the self that are reflected in
recommending these practices. Thus, for example, engagement appear to provide a more complete
staffing practices could be tailored to select em- explanation for relationships with job performance.
ployees who possess high core self-evaluations and
values that fit with those of the hiring organization.
Following their entry, the organization could then
REFERENCES
use mentoring, socialization opportunities, and an
aligned set of people management practices to com- Ashforth, B. E., Harrison, S. H., & Corley, K. G. 2008.
municate a consistent set of organizational values. Identification in organizations: An examination of
Finally, through leadership training as well as per- four fundamental questions. Journal of Manage-
ment, 34: 325374.
formance management systems that provide devel-
opmental feedback, the organization could foster Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. 1995. Emotion in the
perceptions among employees that it is supportive. workplace: A reappraisal. Human Relations, 48: 97
Our results show that managers use of practices to 125.
increase these factors can promote employee en- Baard, P., Deci, E., & Ryan, R. 2004. Intrinsic need satis-
gagement directly and enhance employee perfor- faction: A motivational basis of performance and
mance indirectly. well-being in two work settings. Journal of Applied
Finally, as we noted earlier, engagement has been Social Psychology, 34: 20452068.
conceptualized and measured in several different Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & Schaufeli, W. B. 2005. The
ways, and we believe that it is important to take crossover of burnout and work engagement among
these differences into account when interpreting working couples. Human Relations, 58: 661 689.
scores and applying them to practice. Our measure, Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. 1986. Interactional justice: Com-
632 Academy of Management Journal June

munication criteria of fairness. In R. Lewicki, M. behavior in work teams. Administrative Science


Bazerman, & B. Sheppard (Eds.), Research on nego- Quarterly, 44: 350 383.
tiation in organizations, vol. 1: 4355. Greenwich, Eisenberger, R., Armeli, S., Rexwinkel, B., Lynch, P. D., &
CT: JAI Press. Rhoades, L. 2001. Reciprocation of perceived organ-
Bono, J. E., Foldes, H. J., Vinson, G., & Muros, J. P. 2007. izational support. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Workplace emotions: The role of supervision and 86: 4251.
leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92: Eisenberger, R., Hungtington, R., Hutchison, S., & Sowa,
13571367. D. 1986. Perceived organizational support. Journal
Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. 1993. Expanding the of Applied Psychology, 71: 500 507.
criterion domain to include elements of contextual
Erez, A., & Judge, T. A. 2001. Relationship of core self-
performance. In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.),
evaluations to goal setting, motivation, and perfor-
Personnel selection in organizations: 7198. San
mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 1270
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
1279.
Brown, S. P., & Leigh, T. W. 1996. A new look at psycho-
Frijda, N. H. 1993. Moods, emotion episodes, and emo-
logical climate and its relationship to job involve-
tions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook
ment, effort, and performance. Journal of Applied
of emotions: 381 403. New York: Guilford Press.
Psychology, 81: 358 368.
Gagne, M., & Deci, E. 2005. Self-determination theory
Cable, D. M., & Edwards, J. R. 2004. Complementary and
and work motivation. Journal of Organizational Be-
supplementary fit: A theoretical and empirical inte-
havior, 26: 331362.
gration. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89: 822
834. Gallup Management Journal. 2005. Unhappy workers
are unhealthy too. http://gmj.gallup.com. Retrieved
Caldwell, D. F., Chatman, J. A., & OReilly, C. A. 1990.
October 5.
Building organizational commitment: A multi-firm
study. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Goffman, E. 1961. Encounters: Two studies in the soci-
Psychology, 63: 245251. ology of interaction. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
Cammann, C., Fichman, M., Jenkins, G. D., Jr., & Klesh, Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., & Blanchard, C. 2000. On the
J. R. 1983. Assessing the attitudes and perceptions of assessment of situational intrinsic and extrinsic mo-
organizational members. In S. E. Seashore, E. E. tivation: The Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS).
Lawler III, P. H. Mirvis, & C. Cammann (Eds.), As- Motivation and Emotion, 24: 175213.
sessing organizational change: A guide to meth- Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. 1980. Work redesign.
ods, measures, and practices: 71138. New York: Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Wiley.
Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., & Hayes, T. L. 2002. Business-
Campbell, J. P. 1990. Modeling the performance predic- unit-level relationship between employee satisfaction,
tion problem in industrial and organizational psy- employee engagement, and business outcomes: A
chology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87:
Handbook of industrial and organizational psy- 268 279.
chology, vol. 1 (2nd ed.): 687732. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press. Hillman, A. J., Nicholson, G., & Shropshire, C. 2008.
Directors multiple identities, identification, and
Chatman, J. A. 1989. Improving interactional organiza- board monitoring and resource provision. Organiza-
tional research: A model of person-organization fit. tion Science, 19: 441 456.
Academy of Management Review, 14: 333349.
Hochschild, A. R. 1983. The managed heart: Commer-
Corporate Leadership Council. 2006. The effort divi- cialization of human feeling. Berkeley: University
dend: Driving employee performance and reten- of California Press.
tion through engagement. Paper presented at the
Annual Meeting of the Society for Industrial Organi- Hoffman, B. J., Blair, C. A., Meriac, J. P., & Woehr, D. J.
zational Psychology, Dallas. 2007. Expanding the criterion domain? A quantita-
tive review of the OCB literature. Journal of Applied
Deci, E. L. 1975. Intrinsic motivation. New York: Psychology, 92: 555566.
Plenum.
Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenbeck, J. R. 1991. The structure of
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. 1985. Intrinsic motivation and work: Job design and roles. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M.
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organi-
Plenum. zational psychology, vol 2: 165207. Palo Alto, CA:
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. 1993. The psychology of Consulting Psychologists Press.
attitudes. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. 2001. Relationship of core
College Publishers. self-evaluations traitsself-esteem, generalized self-
Edmondson, A. 1999. Psychological safety and learning efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 633

with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta- person-group, and person-supervisor fit. Personnel
analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86:80 92. Psychology, 58: 281342.
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Thoresen, C. J., & Patton, G. K. Law, K. S., Wong, C., & Mobley, W. H. 1998. Toward a
2001. The job satisfaction-job performance relation- taxonomy of multidimensional constructs. Acad-
ship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psycho- emy of Management Review, 23: 741755.
logical Bulletin, 127: 376 407. Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. 2002. Organizational citizenship
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. 2003. behavior and workplace deviance: The role of affect
The core self-evaluations scale: Development of a and cognition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87:
measure. Personnel Psychology, 56: 303331. 131142.
Judge, T. A., Heller, D., & Mount, M. K. 2002. Five-factor Locke, E. A. 1976. The nature and causes of job satisfac-
model of personality and job satisfaction: A meta- tion. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of indus-
analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87: 530 trial and organizational psychology: 12931349.
541. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Judge, T. A., & Hurst, C. 2007. The benefits and possible Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. 1997. The truth about burn-
costs of positive core self-evaluations: A review and out. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
agenda for future research. In D. L. Nelson & C. L. May, D. R., Gilson, R. L., & Harter, L. M. 2004.The psy-
Cooper (Eds.), Positive organizational behavior: chological conditions of meaningfulness, safety and
159 174. London: Sage. availability and the engagement of the human spirit
Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., & Durham, C. C. 1997. The at work. Journal of Occupational & Organizational
dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A core eval- Psychology, 77: 1137.
uations approach. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw Motowidlo, S. J., Borman, W. C., & Schmit, M. J. 1997. A
(Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. theory of individual differences in task and contex-
19: 151188. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. tual performance. Human Performance, 10: 71 83.
Kahn, W. A. 1990. Psychological conditions of personal Naylor, J. C., Pritchard, R. D., & Ilgen, D. R. 1980. A
engagement and disengagement at work. Academy theory of behavior in organizations. New York: Ac-
of Management Journal, 33: 692724. ademic Press.
Kahn, W. A. 1992. To be fully there: Psychological pres- Nembhard, I. M., & Edmondson, A. C. 2006. Making it
ence at work. Human Relations, 45: 321349. safe: The effects of leader inclusiveness and profes-
Kanfer, R. 1990. Motivation theory and industrial and sional status on psychological safety and improve-
organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. ment efforts in health care teams. Journal of Organ-
Hough (Eds.) Handbook of industrial and organi- izational Behavior, 27: 941966.
zational psychology: 75170. Palo Alto, CA: Con- Newman, D. A., & Harrison, D. A. 2008. Been there,
sulting Psychologists Press. bottled that: Are state and behavioral work engage-
Kanungo, R. N. 1982. Measurement of job and work in- ment new and useful construct wines? Industrial
volvement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67: 341 and Organizational Psychology, 1: 3135.
349. Organ, D. W. 1988. Organizational citizenship behav-
Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. 1978. The social psychology of ior: The good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA:
organizations. New York: Wiley. Lexington Books.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. 1998. Data Ravlin, E. C., & Meglino, B. M. 1987. Effect of values on
analysis in social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. T. perception and decision making: A study of alterna-
Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psy- tive work values measures. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology: 233265. New York: McGraw-Hill. chology, 72: 666 673.
Kline, R. B. 2005. Principles and practice of structural Rhoades, L., & Eisenberger, R. 2002. Perceived organiza-
equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford tional support: A review of the literature. Journal of
Press. Applied Psychology, 87: 698 714.
Kreiner, G. E., Hollensbe, E. C., & Sheep, M. L. 2006. Rothbard, N. P. 2001. Enriching or depleting? The dy-
Where is the me among the we? Identity work namics of engagement in work and family roles.
and the search for optimal balance. Academy of Administrative Science Quarterly, 46: 655 684.
Management Journal, 49: 10311057. Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. 1999. Core affect, prototyp-
Kristof, A. L. 1996. Person-organization fit: An integra- ical emotional episodes, and other things called
tive review of its conceptualizations, measurement, emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Person-
and implications. Personnel Psychology, 49: 1 49. ality and Social Psychology, 76: 805 819.
Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. Russell, S. S., Spitzmuller, C., Lin, L. F., Stanton, J. M.,
2005. Consequences of individuals fit at work: A Smith, P. C., & Ironson, G. H. 2004. Shorter can
meta-analysis of person-job, person-organization, also be better: The abridged Job in General Scale.
634 Academy of Management Journal June

Educational and Psychological Measurement, Bakker, A. 2002. The measurement of burnout


64: 878 893. and engagement: A confirmatory factor analytic
Saks, A. M. 2006. Antecedents and consequences of em- approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3: 7192.
ployee engagement. Journal of Managerial Psychol- Schwab, D. P. 1980. Construct validity in organizational
ogy, 21: 600 619. behavior. In B.M. Staw & L.L. Cummings, (Eds.),
Salanova, M., Agut, S., & Peiro, J. M. 2005. Linking or- Research in organizational behavior, vol. 2: 3 43.
ganizational resources and work engagement to em- Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
ployee performance and customer royalty: The me- Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. 2002. Mediation in experimen-
diation of service climate. Journal of Applied tal and nonexperimental studies: New procedures
Psychology, 90: 12171227. and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7:
Salanova, M., Llorens, S., Cifre, E., Martnez, I., & 422 445.
Schaufeli, W. B. 2003. Perceived collective efficacy, Sobel, M. E. 1982. Asymptotic intervals for indirect ef-
subjective well-being and task performance among fects in structural equations models. In S. Leinhart
electronic work groups: An experimental study. (Ed.), Sociological methodology: 290 312. San
Small Groups Research, 34: 4373. Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. 2003. UWESUtrecht Vroom, V. H. 1964. Work and motivation. Oxford, U.K.:
Work Engagement Scale: Test manual. Department Wiley.
of Psychology, Utrecht University. http://www.
Weick, K. E., & Roberts, K. H. 1993. Collective mind in
schaufeli.com/. Retrieved August 8.
organizations: Heedful interrelating on flight decks.
Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. 2004. Job demands, job Administrative Science Quarterly, 38: 357381.
resources and their relationship with burnout and
Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. 1991. Job satisfaction
engagement: A multi-sample study. Journal of Or-
and organizational commitment as predictors of or-
ganizational Behavior, 25: 293315.
ganizational citizenship and in-role behaviors. Jour-
Schaufeli, W.B., Salanova, M., Gonzalez-Roma, V., & nal of Management, 17: 601 617.

APPENDIX
TABLE A1
Job Engagement Items and Factor Loadingsa

Skilled Care Nursing


Items Facility Employeesb Firefightersc

Physical engagement
I work with intensity on my job .60 .71
I exert my full effort to my job .81 .86
I devote a lot of energy to my job .88 .77
I try my hardest to perform well on my job .89 .84
I strive as hard as I can to complete my job .87 .79
I exert a lot of energy on my job .78 .67

Emotional engagement
I am enthusiastic in my job .85 .87
I feel energetic at my job .89 .90
I am interested in my job .81 .82
I am proud of my job .78 .68
I feel positive about my job .81 .87
I am excited about my job .80 .91

Cognitive engagement
At work, my mind is focused on my job .88 .80
At work, I pay a lot of attention to my job .84 .87
At work, I focus a great deal of attention on my job .92 .91
At work, I am absorbed by my job .88 .92
At work, I concentrate on my job .78 .67
At work, I devote a lot of attention to my job .78 .88

a
All factor loadings are significant at p .001.
b
n 180.
c
n 245.
2010 Rich, Lepine, and Crawford 635

Bruce Louis Rich (brich@csusm.edu) is an associate pro- of Florida. He received his Ph.D. from Michigan State
fessor in the Department of Management at California University. His research interests include team composi-
State University San Marcos. He received his Ph.D. in tion and teamwork, citizenship performance and voice,
organizational behavior and human resource manage- occupational stress, and employee engagement.
ment from the University of Florida. His research inter-
Eean R. Crawford (eean.crawford@cba.ufl.edu) is a doc-
ests include employee engagement, leadership, motiva-
toral student in organizational behavior at the War-
tion, emotions, and prosocial organizational behavior.
rington College of Business Administration, University
Jeffery A. LePine (jeffery.lepine@cba.ufl.edu) is the Dar- of Florida. His current research interests include em-
den Restaurants Professor of Management at the War- ployee engagement, team effectiveness, work stress,
rington College of Business Administration, University memory, and learning.

You might also like