You are on page 1of 9

TICS-1358; No.

of Pages 9

Review

Disruptions in autobiographical
memory processing in depression and
the emergence of memory
therapeutics
Tim Dalgleish and Aliza Werner-Seidler
Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK

Depression is characterized by distinct profiles of distur- given in Box 1) in the way that they process the autobio-
bance in ways autobiographical memories are repre- graphical past and outline recent advances in the transla-
sented, recalled, and maintained. We review four core tional science of memory therapeutics that seek to address
domains of difficulty: systematic biases in favor of negative these difficulties.
material; impoverished access and responses to positive
memories; reduced access to the specific details of the The four mnemonic horsemen of depression
personal past; and dysfunctional processes of rumination Sufferers of depression remember the past differently to
and avoidance around personal autobiographical material. their never-depressed peers. Their autobiographical mem-
These difficulties drive the onset and maintenance of de- ory processing is compromised in at least four distinct but
pression; consequently, interventions targeted at these interrelated ways that combine and interact to help main-
maladaptive processes have clinical potential. Memory tain depressive episodes once they have begun, and to
therapeutics is the development of novel clinical techni- confer vulnerability to new episodes when sufferers are
ques, translated from basic research, that target memory in remission (Figure 1).
difficulties in those with emotional disorders. We discuss
prototypical examples from this clinical domain including Biased recollection of negative memories
MEmory Specificity Training, positive memory elabora- The most striking feature when engaging with individuals
tion, memory rescripting, and the method-of-loci (MoL). in the grip of depression is the pervasively negative tone
that sounds throughout their discourse about the past.
Recollecting and reflecting upon our autobiographical past Depression appears to be characterized by a systematic
defines human mental life. Personal memories are the autobiographical recollection bias that favors negative
currency of social discourse, they mold and shape our experiences [4,5], with faster access to negative personal
emotions, help us plan our future, and provide candidate memories when prompted and a greater tendency to gen-
solutions for the problems that we face. Our library of erate negative memories when recall is unconstrained [6].
autobiographical memories defines who we are, scaffolding Unbidden intrusive memories of negative, often traumatic,
our sense of self across time [1]. Systematic difficulties in past experiences also characterize the disorder [7]. This
the recollection of these past experiences, especially of facilitated negative retrieval is likely to be complemented
emotionally evocative events, are a cardinal feature of by biases at encoding as a function of selective attention to
affective disorders, and range from intrusive flashbacks negative personal experiences [4] and skewed interpreta-
of trauma in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) to tion of ambiguous personal scenarios in favor of negative
ruminations upon overgeneral negative personal themes resolutions [8]. Such pervasive preferential access to neg-
in depression. These patterns not only define the mental ative personal memories in depression contributes to the
lives of many patients but also drive the onset and main- felt sense of a profoundly negative self, world, and future
tenance of disorder [2,3]. Consequently, emerging clinical that depressed individuals describe [9].
interventions that target and reverse these disrupted
memory processes have enormous potential. In this review Impoverished positive memories
we discuss the often toxic problems faced by individuals The day-to-day recollection of self-affirming positive
with clinical depression (a description of depression is experiences has been identified as a core adaptive emotion
regulation strategy to counteract downturns in negative
Corresponding author: Dalgleish, T. (tim.dalgleish@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk). affect [10,11]. The biased recollection of negative memories
Keywords: autobiographical memory; depression; overgeneral memory bias; suppres-
sion; method-of-loci. in depression, described above, is also accompanied by
1364-6613/
impoverished access to such positive autobiographical past
ß 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.010 events [4]. Even when positive memories are successfully
brought to mind, their recollection appears to have little
Trends in Cognitive Sciences xx (2014) 1–9 1
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

Box 1. The nature of depression and cognitive theories of the disorder


What is depression? Cognitive models of depression
In psychiatric terms, depression (defined as presence of a major Cognitive approaches to depression provide a framework for under-
depressive episode; MDE) is defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical standing the psychological mechanisms involved in the onset, main-
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) [70] as the presence of at least tenance, and recurrence of the disorder. Perhaps the most influential
five symptoms from a set of nine, one of which must be consistent cognitive model was provided by Beck who proposed that depression
depressed mood or anhedonia. Additional symptoms may include results from the activation of underlying dysfunctional schemas* that
changes in appetite or sleeping pattern, restlessness, fatigue, feelings represent negative mental constructions about the self, the world, and
of guilt or worthlessness, impaired concentration, and suicidal the future (‘the negative cognitive triad’ [62]). Schemas are claimed to
ideation. To fulfill criteria for an MDE the constellation of symptoms drive negative thinking and other cognitive biases and distortions
must be present for most of the day, nearly every day for a 2 week (including biases in autobiographical memory processing) that serve to
period or longer, and represent a change from previous levels of maintain schema integrity and thereby support and consolidate the
functioning. Symptoms must also interfere with functioning in social, cognitive foundations that drive the disorder [62]. Beck’s cognitive
occupational, or other important domains, or cause considerable model and other related approaches [63] provide an overarching
distress to the individual. framework in which biases and distortions in a cognitive process such
Epidemiological studies have established depression as a major as memory are conceptualized as ‘rational’ and adaptive processes that
public health concern that tends to run a chronic course [71]. The are servicing maladaptive and dysfunctional underlying schemas.
lifetime prevalence rate is approximately 16% [72], with more than Cognitive behavior therapy (CBT), the intervention paradigm derived
80% of individuals experiencing multiple episodes [71]. Each from such cognitive formulations of depression, seeks to identify and
recurring episode predicts a higher risk of future recurrence [73], modify biases in thinking and cognitive processing and to reorganize
even after successful treatment of the current episode [74,75]. and shift underlying schema to make them more adaptive and
Depression currently costs the UK exchequer more than £9 billion functional. The nascent memory therapeutics discussed here fall under
annually [76] and is predicted by the World Health Organisation (see the umbrella of this broad CBT approach.
http://www.who.int/whosis/whostat/2008/en/index.html) to be the
second leading cause of disability worldwide by 2020 [77]. Depression
is a growing problem at both individual and societal levels, under- *
Schemas are proposed to be cognitive structures which provide an internal repre-
scoring the need for more effective interventions not only to target the sentation of aspects of the self, the world, and of others, that derive from a lifetime of
acute phase of disorder but also to reduce the likelihood of relapse/ experience in encountering relevant exemplars from these categories and that shape
recurrence. the encoding, organization, and retrieval of information [62].

beneficial impact on mood for those with a history of attention to the discrepancy between these states, thus
depression [12,13], and may even be detrimental [13]. This spawning rumination that itself contributes to worsening
failure to improve mood may reflect the quality of recol- mood [13]. There is some evidence supporting this account,
lected positive memories. For example, there is evidence with data showing that preventing rumination by instruct-
that, when in a sad mood, individuals with a history of ing depressed and formerly depressed individuals to focus
depression recall positive memories that are markedly less on the concrete and specific details of the memory leads to
vivid [14] and less emotionally intense [15] than those improved mood following memory recall [16].
retrieved by never-depressed peers. The lack of salient
phenomenological memory features may very well make Categorical memory
it more challenging for positive memory recall to confer A third striking feature of autobiographical recollection in
emotional benefits (although whether this is also the case depression is the focus on ‘categorical’ aspects of the past.
in currently depressed groups awaits confirmation). An- In contrast to individual autobiographical episodes, these
other possibility is that reflecting on a past happier time, categorical recollections comprise general, often negative-
and making comparisons to current low mood, draws ly-valent, themes that capture repetitions and regularities
across many personal experiences. There is now good
evidence that such categorical processing tends to override
Biased recollecon Impoverished access to detailed memories of specific individual events in
of negave memories posive memories depression [3]. For example, on the widely-used autobio-
graphical memory test (AMT) [17] – where specific, de-
tailed memories are prompted by cue words – a depressed
responder would be more likely to generate a categorical
memory to a cue such as ‘party’ (e.g., ‘Every birthday party
Depression I’ve ever hosted has been a disaster!’) as opposed to the
requisite recollection of a specific festive occasion.
One likely reason why categorical autobiographical rec-
ollection is so potent in depression is the highly consoli-
dated nature of negative categorical themes that underpin
the disorder – an idea that lies at the heart of cognitive
Altered relaonship
Overgeneral memory to emoonal theories of depression with their emphasis on the role of
memories negative schemas (Box 1). We used a novel ‘life chapters’
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences task to elucidate these emotionally salient categorical
autobiographical themes in depression [18]. Participants
Figure 1. The four mnemonic horsemen of depression. These four processes,
characteristic of depression, interact and combine in ways that maintain
constructed autobiographical timelines, dividing their
depression and elevate the risk of recurrence even after remission. life into discrete chapters (e.g., ‘student days’; ‘intimate
2
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

relationships’), and subjectively allocated positive and opposed to verbal narratives [24]. With image-based mem-
negative descriptive information to each chapter. Those ories, depressed individuals have a greater tendency to
with depression showed increased coherence and repeti- adopt an observer perspective [25,26] – seeing themselves
tion of negative information across chapters, and reduced in the situation, but from the point-of-view of an outsider.
coherence and repetition of positive information, whereas This has been found to be the case for both negative and
their never-depressed peers showed the opposite pattern. positive personal memories, and adopting an observer
Greater depression entrenchment, indexed by number of perspective generally reduces the affect associated with
prior episodes, was associated uniquely with lack of posi- the memory in the short-term [27,28]. For distressing
tive coherence, suggesting that a paucity of positive auto- memories, it seems that adopting an observer perspective
biographical themes is a cognitive marker of depression per se may be counterproductive in the longer-term be-
recurrence. cause it mitigates successful emotional processing of the
There is now substantive evidence that the relative memory [24,29]. Similarly, in the case of positive memo-
difficulty encountered by depressed individuals in acces- ries, simply adopting an observer perspective may be
sing specific autobiographical memories, and their procliv- unhelpful because it reduces the affective impact associat-
ity for dwelling on these categorical aspects of the personal ed with the memory [27,28]. One strong motivating factor
past, is related to poorer prognosis [19]. One reason for this for such attempted (if unsuccessful) avoidance of the recol-
is that overgeneral recall can spur ruminative processes lection of distressing aspects of the past is the fact that,
focused on the categorical themes that have been accessed. when autobiographical memories do come to mind in de-
Rumination then consolidates these themes, further en- pression, they have a greater tendency to spawn rumina-
hancing the tendency towards overgeneral memory (e.g., tive processes. These are either focused on the memories
[20]). Another reason is that fluent processing of specific themselves or on relating those memories to the wider
autobiographical memories is a core aspect of successful depressogenic categorical themes running through the
daily cognitive functioning involved in planning, problem- individual’s autobiography, referred to in the preceding
solving, and social discourse (Table 1). Consequently, im- section [18,30].
pairment across these diverse cognitive operations has
adverse downstream consequences concerning the course The CaR-FA-X model of autobiographical memory
of depressive disorder [3]. processing in depression
The CaR-FA-X model [3] (Figure 2) operationalizes the core
Alterations in the mental relationship to emotional disruptions in autobiographical memory processing asso-
autobiographical memories ciated with depression that we have outlined above. Within
The recollection of emotional past events can sometimes be the model, difficulties accessing specific episodes from the
painful, particularly for those struggling with mental past are characterized as resulting from the capture (Ca) of
health difficulties. Explicit and implicit efforts to avoid memory search efforts by highly consolidated categorical
or suppress distressing personal memories and/or the depressogenic themes, which then form the focus of mal-
emotions associated with them, perhaps unsurprisingly, adaptive cycles of rumination (R). The likelihood of such
have been shown to be more prevalent in depression capture is exacerbated by ingrained functional avoidance
[21,22]. However, such efforts are also more likely to be (FA) of specific details of past upsetting autobiographical
counterproductive, with greater intrusion of the unwanted events that itself diverts processing to the categorical level
memories following attempted suppression in individuals of autobiographical representation. The ability to override
high in depressive affect relative to their less-depressed these dysfunctional processing imperatives is compro-
peers [23]. As well as being counterproductive, suppression mised as a function of the relatively impoverished execu-
focused on a given target upsetting personal memory tive (X) control that characterizes those suffering from
appears to enhance subsequent access to other upsetting depression [4,31].
memories in depressed individuals, suggesting that sup-
pression is a particularly toxic way of relating to the Memory therapeutics and depression
personal past when depressed [23]. These advances in our understanding of the key roles
Subtler forms of mental avoidance also appear to be at that maladaptive aspects of autobiographical memory pro-
play in the way that those with depression relate to mem- cessing play in depression, and the CaR-FA-X framework
ories that have been recollected, especially when those that integrates them together, have prompted practition-
memories are recalled in the form of mental images as er-researchers to explore new avenues of therapeutic

Table 1. Putative functions of specific memories and examples of impairment in affective contexts
Function of specific memories Examples of impairment in depression
Problem solving Reduced specificity is associated with impaired problem-solving in depression [78,79] and moderates
the effect of a negative mood disturbance on problem-solving performance in previously suicidal
patients [80].
Planning Reduced specificity is associated with difficulty in imagining specific future events [81] with implications
for effective future day to day planning [82].
Emotional processing Reduced specificity is associated with high scores on emotional avoidance measures [83,84] and predicts
affective reaction to failure [85,86].
Reducing rumination Reduced memory specificity is causally related to greater ruminative processing [87,88]

3
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

and sensory detail. For example, Werner-Seidler and


Capture and Moulds [16] showed that, for individuals with depression,
ruminaon
focusing on these detailed aspects of their memories (the
moment-to-moment experience of the memories), instead
Funconal
of processing memories in an abstract way (reflecting on
Non-specific Consequences
avoidance autobiographical the causes, meanings, and consequences), enhanced the
E.g., impaired
memory problem-solving
positive emotional impact of the memories. Commensurate
with these findings is work from the imagery domain
Execuve showing that, when positive material is elaborated using
capacity imagery, the impact on emotion is amplified [45,46]. These
and control
studies provide proof-of-principle that those with depres-
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences sion can derive affective benefits from suitably elaborated
positive material.
Figure 2. The CaR-FA-X model of autobiographical memory processing in
depression. Three processes contributing to overgeneral memory – capture and An entirely different challenge, however, is to facilitate
rumination (CaR), functional avoidance (FA), and impaired executive capacity and day-to-day access to such elaborated memories when they
control (X) – can each have effects on cognition and behavior (e.g., problem-
solving), either independently or through their individual or combined effect on
are most needed (e.g., in the service of emotion regulation).
autobiographical memory. Reproduced from [3], with permission, copyright ß One candidate solution is the MoL technique – a mnemonic
(2007) American Psychological Association. device that relies on spatial relationships between familiar
locations or routes to scaffold recollection of previously
intervention which focus on changing patients’ processing identified and stored information (Box 2). In the first study
of, and relationship to, their personal past. These emerging investigating the utility of the MoL in depression [47] we
interventions sit against a broader backdrop of cognitive asked participants to generate and then elaborate a set of
approaches to the treatment of depression and other emo- positive and self-affirming memories before practicing
tional disorders which have historically focused on patient recalling the memories. Participants were divided into
autobiographies as a rich source of therapeutic material two practice conditions, using either a chunking-and-re-
(Box 1). We discuss here novel memory-based techniques hearsal technique or the MoL, and completed a week of
addressing the four highlighted core domains of autobio- retrieval training with their allocated method until they
graphical memory disturbance in depression. could recall all their identified memories without error. On
the crucial surprise recall test after a further week (during
Changing negative memory biases which no further practice took place), recollection
Cognitive bias modification (CBM) refers to a new range of remained at ceiling only in the MoL group. These initial
interventions that seek to shift the pervasive maladaptive data suggest that the MoL has promise as a technique to
cognitive biases associated with clinical disorders through allow easy access to sufficiently elaborated positive and
systematic, computerized training to recalibrate underly- self-affirming memories in depression.
ing processes in favor of positive or benign material, rather
than negative information [32]. To date, CBM has been MEmory Specificity Training (MEST)
applied most effectively in anxiety [33]. However, recently The wealth of basic research suggesting a relative difficulty
the focus has shifted to evolving and evaluating CBM in accessing specific autobiographical memories in depres-
procedures in depression [34]. CBM for depression that sion [3] suggests an elegantly simple intervention – train-
has targeted initial attention to [35,36], and interpretation ing patients to become more specific in their emotional
of [37–40], emotional events has produced mixed results recollection (Box 3). To this end, Raes and colleagues [48]
[34]. However, CBM procedures that have directly targeted evaluated the effect of a group-based MEST program with
memory biases [41] by training depressed individuals to depressed inpatients in an uncontrolled trial. The retrieval
process mildly upsetting events in a specific and concrete style of participants became significantly more specific
way potentially show more promise, with the CBM parti- (Figure I in Box 3), and improvements in specificity were
cipants experiencing decreased depressive symptoms rel- significantly associated with reported improvements in
ative to a control group [42]. processes important in day-to-day cognition (Table 1) in-
Explicit rescripting of negative autobiographical mem- cluding rumination, cognitive avoidance, and problem-
ories to reduce their distressing impact, an approach with a solving skills.
history of successful application in anxiety syndromes [43], Building on this platform, we conducted the first random-
also shows potential as a clinical tool in depression. Wheat- ized controlled trial (RCT) of MEST (versus a no-interven-
ley et al. [44], in a clinical case series, reported benefits of tion control) with a sample of bereaved, depressed Afghan
rescripting in the form of reduced symptoms of depression adolescent refugees living in Iran; importantly, this RCT
and a decreased frequency of intrusive recollections of the also included a 2 month follow-up [49] (Figure I in Box 3).
rescripted events. MEST successfully enhanced memory specificity, and spec-
ificity gains mediated subsequent improvements in depres-
Enhancing the recollection of positive autobiographical sive symptoms for adolescents in the MEST group, which
memories were reduced into the non-clinical range. There is similar
The affective impact of recalled positive and self-affirming promising evidence for MEST as an intervention for suf-
memories can be enhanced through elaboration processes ferers of PTSD [50]. These early-stage trials suggest that,
targeted at enriching the memories with affective, visual, despite its simplicity, MEST is an efficacious intervention
4
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

Box 2. The method-of-loci (MoL)


The MoL is an ancient mnemonic device that relies on memorized features of the images used to combine the loci with the material to
spatial relationships between loci (e.g., locations on a familiar route be remembered are crucially important – the more salient, vivid, and
such as the journey to work) that are used to arrange and recollect bizarre the imagery, the easier the material is to recollect [94].
memorial content. The basic method uses visual imagery to mentally The MoL is versatile; it can be readily used for the temporary
associate each to-be-remembered piece of information with one of storage of information (e.g., a shopping list), but also lends itself to
the loci along the route (e.g., a particular building). By this means, the construction of more permanent mental repositories supporting
material can be recollected simply by mentally retracing the route and access to the target material on repeated future occasions. Such
using the loci and associated imagery on this imagined journey as repositories are commonly known as ‘memory palaces’ [95,96]
prompts to cue the information from memory. because they often comprise elaborate and beautiful fictitious
The MoL was first described in Roman rhetorical treatises (e.g., locations that the recollector has imagined solely for the purposes
[89]). It remains the favored strategy adopted by contemporary of information storage. Memory palaces of this kind have a long
memory champions to store exceptionally large amounts of informa- pedigree dating back to medieval times [97].
tion, such as recalling the value of P to thousands of decimal places Our study utilizing the MoL to improve access to positive and self-
[90]. affirming memories in depression [47] extends the technique to
The MoL can be easily acquired by non-experts, and dramatically clinical application; a worked example of this approach is illustrated in
improves memory performance in naı̈ve participants [91–93]. The Figure I.

Elaborate Combine loci Retrieve


Idenfy memories to Idenfy loci with memories by
memories to Idenfy a
include rich along the memories mentally
be familiar route
and specific route using vivid navigang
remembered details imagery along route

The birth of my I was overcome The daily The telephone Instead of the By simply
first son with emoon – I commute to work box at the end telephone imagining my
remember his of the street booth, I see that cycle to work, I
skin was so so my infant son is pass the
as I held him for holding the telephone booth
the first me phone receiver and see my son
chang away gossiping away
into the
receiver...
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences

Figure I. Using the method-of-loci (MoL) to enhance recollection of positive autobiographical memories in depression [47].

that can alleviate depression (and other disorders), consis- depression and has a strong database of supportive outcome
tent with other novel depression interventions that focusing data [56]. Although not targeted at memories per se, MBCT
on enhancing concrete processing of personal material [42]. seeks to enhance affective executive control over mental life,
including memory for the past, through the cultivation of
Changing relationships to memories mindfulness skills that promote the ability to psychological-
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) interventions for emo- ly ‘step back’ from distressing mental content.
tional disorders, including depression, that seek to change MBCT is a complex and intensive intervention requir-
the way recipients relate to aspects of their mental life, ing highly trained therapist input. More recently,
such as their memories of the past, instead of trying to researchers have endeavored to distil core cognitive ele-
modify or challenge mental content itself, have been ments of MBCT into simpler cognitive training protocols.
deemed the ‘third wave’ of behavior therapy [51]. Such Kross and colleagues investigated the benefits of self-dis-
treatments include acceptance and commitment therapy tancing (SD) – the process of intentionally stepping back
[52], dialectic behavior therapy [53], and, of most relevance from an experience to reflect on it and reappraise it from
to depression, rumination-focused cognitive behavioral the perspective of a distanced observer. It is important to
therapy (RF-CBT [54]) and mindfulness-based cognitive note how this more reflective process is distinct from
therapy (MBCT [55]). simply automatically adopting an observer perspective
RF-CBT is an adapted form of CBT designed to reduce upon personal memories – something that by itself appears
residual symptoms of depression by directly targeting to be counterproductive (as discussed above [24,29]). Kross
ruminative processes that we know are intimately linked and colleagues have shown in a novel series of studies that
to disruptions in autobiographical memory. There is pre- analyzing the meaning of memories and experiences (e.g.,
liminary support for this intervention, with RF-CBT prov- thinking about why they may have occurred) from a self-
ing efficacious in reducing symptoms relative to treatment- distanced perspective can reap mental health benefits in
as-usual, although these effects need to be replicated in individuals with depression [57]. Similar work in our own
trials involving an active control group [54]. laboratory has examined perspective broadening (PB) –
MBCT is a group intervention targeted at preventing the capacity to frame experiences within wider contexts.
relapse in currently well patients with recurrent Preclinical work across four studies showed that a single
5
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

Box 3. MEmory Specificity Training (MEST)


MEST is designed to enhance the specificity with which depressed
participants retrieve personal events from autobiographical memory 1
with the aim of counteracting the difficulties associated with the
disorder in accessing specific personal information [48,49]. MEST is
0.9 Key:

Memory specificity
delivered in small groups led by trained therapists and comprises the 0.8
following five-session structure: MEST (Raes pilot)
Session 1: psychoeducation about memory difficulties common in 0.7
MEST (Neshat-Doost trial)
depression is presented and discussed, and impairment in retrieving
0.6
specific memories is provided as the rationale for MEST. As a group,
Control (Neshat-Doost
participants practice recalling specific memories in response to 0.5 trial)
positive and neutral cues, with demonstrations and support from
therapists. For homework, participants identify specific memories in 0.4
response to 10 cue words, and identify an additional specific event 0.3
that has happened to them each day to bring to the subsequent Pre Post
session.
Session 2: the second session commences with a brief summary of
session 1 followed by a homework review. The remainder of the 40
session follows the same format as session 1, focusing on memory

Depressive symptoms
retrieval for positive and neutral words. Additionally, participants are 35 Key:
encouraged and assisted in retrieving multiple distinct memories in
response to the same cue. For homework, participants identify 30 MEST - BDI-II scores
(Raes Pilot)
specific memories in response to 10 cue words and identify two
additional specific events each day. 25 MEST - MFQ scores
Session 3: in the third session, negative cues are introduced and (Neshat-Doost trial)
participants practice retrieving specific memories in response to 20
Control - MFQ scores
negative and positive cue words. As in session 2, participants retrieve (Neshat-Doost trial)
multiple memories of a different nature in response to the same cue. 15
Homework is identical to that of session 2 with the inclusion of
negative cue words. 10
Pre Post Follow-up
Sessions 4–5: the fourth session involves further practice of
memory retrieval in response to neutral words, and the fifth and TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences
final session involves retrieval to a variety of cues (e.g., positive,
Figure I. The top panel shows mean of level specificity as measured by the
negative, and neutral), concluding with a summary and review of the autobiographical memory test (AMT) [17] at pre-training and post-training.
program. Bottom panel shows mean depression scores as measured by the Beck
The impact of MEST on memory specificity and depressive Depression Inventory (BDI-II) for the single-group, uncontrolled pilot trial in the
symptoms in the two published studies in depression is illustrated Raes et al. study (personal communication) and by the Mood and Feelings
in Figure I. Questionnaire (MFQ) in the Neshat-Doost et al. controlled trial [49].

session of PB training reduced negative affect and psycho- [61]. These initial studies suggest that affective executive
physiological responses to distressing film clips and upset- training has potential to accrue transferable benefits to
ting personal memories and reduced subsequent memory skills relevant to patients with affective disorders.
intrusions [58].
Concluding remarks and future perspectives
Affective executive training Cognitive theories of depression [62,63] emphasize the key
Central to the CaR-FA-X framework is the idea that a core role played by biases and perturbations across core cogni-
barrier faced by sufferers of depression when using auto- tive faculties in the onset and maintenance of the disorder.
biographical memory techniques in the clinic is a lack of Central to these is maladaptive processing of the emotional
executive capacity to work in complex ways with memories autobiographical past. We have reviewed four domains of
that are often emotive and resource-demanding [31]. To try such autobiographical memory dysfunctionality: system-
to address this we developed an affective variant of an atic biases in favor of negative material, impoverished
established working memory (WM) training protocol [59] access and responses to positive memories, reduced access
that aimed to augment executive control in such affective to the specific details of the personal past (overgeneral
contexts, and we used it to train affective WM over 20 days memory), and dysfunctional processes of rumination and
in healthy participants. We found that those exposed to avoidance around emotive personal autobiographical
training, unlike placebo-trained controls, showed en- themes. These difficulties drive the onset and maintenance
hanced performance on a measure of affective attention of depression; consequently, interventions targeted at
– the emotional Stroop task [60] – as well as on a quintes- these maladaptive processes have considerable clinical
sential index of affective executive control – the ability to potential. Memory therapeutics is the development of
downregulate negative affect in response to viewing dis- novel intervention techniques, translated from basic re-
tressing film clips [61]. Regulatory gains were associated search to autobiographical memory, that target memory
with greater activity in the frontoparietal brain network as difficulties in those with emotional disorders. We have
well as in other neural regions implicated in affective presented prototypical examples from this clinical domain
control, notably the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex including MEST, positive memory elaboration, memory
6
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

rescripting, and the MoL, that have promise either as 11 Rusting, C.L. and DeHart, T. (2000) Retrieving positive memories to
regulate negative mood: consequences for mood-congruent memory. J.
stand-alone low-intensity interventions for patients with
Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 737–752
milder forms of depression or as adjuncts to existing evi- 12 Joormann, J. and Siemer, M. (2004) Memory accessibility, mood
dence-based interventions. regulation, and dysphoria: difficulties in repairing sad mood with
In terms of the trajectory of intervention development happy memories? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113, 179–188
[64], these techniques in autobiographical memory thera- 13 Joormann, J. et al. (2007) Mood regulation in depression: differential
effects of distraction and recall of happy memories on sad mood. J.
peutics are at an early stage. Although data from explor-
Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 484–490
atory trials (e.g., MEST [48–50]) are encouraging, there 14 Werner-Seidler, A. and Moulds, M.L. (2011) Autobiographical memory
is now a mandate for later-stage trials to provide defini- characteristics in depression vulnerability: Formerly depressed
tive evidence of the clinical utility, feasibility, and cost- individuals recall less vivid positive memories. Cogn. Emot. 25,
effectiveness of these nascent approaches. There is also a 1087–1103
15 Werner-Seidler, A. and Moulds, M.L. (2012) Characteristics of self-
need to build on emergent techniques that are still at the defining memory in depression vulnerability. Memory 20, 935–948
preclinical stage to explore their therapeutic utility in 16 Werner-Seidler, A. and Moulds, M.L. (2012) Mood repair and
patients (e.g., [65]). processing mode in depression. Emotion 12, 470–478
Disturbances in autobiographical memory processing 17 Williams, J.M.G. and Broadbent, K. (1986) Autobiographical memory
are of course not limited to depression, and there is good in suicide attempters. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95, 144–149
18 Dalgleish, T. et al. (2011) The structure of past and future lives in
basic research evidence for the transdiagnostic relevance depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 1–15
[66] of the major domains of difficulty reviewed here; for 19 Kleim, B. and Ehlers, A. (2008) Reduced autobiographical memory
example, sufferers of PTSD also struggle to access specific specificity predicts depression and posttraumatic stress disorder after
details of the personal past [67] and there is encouraging recent trauma. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 76, 231–242
20 Watkins, E.R. and Teasdale, J.D. (2001) Rumination and overgeneral
evidence that memory specificity training delivers thera-
memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking.
peutic benefits in this population [50]. Similarly, individuals J. Abnorm. Psychol. 110, 353–357
with social anxiety disorder show impoverished recollection 21 Beblo, T. et al. (2012) Increased suppression of negative and positive
of positive personal experiences [68], and there is scope here emotions in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 141, 474–479
to apply techniques to assist the elaboration and retention 22 Sumner, J.A. (2012) The mechanisms underlying overgeneral
autobiographical memory: an evaluative review of evidence for the
of memories for positive past events.
CaR-FA-X model. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 34–48
This focus on working clinically to target underlying 23 Dalgleish, T. and Yiend, J. (2006) The effects of suppressing a negative
transdiagnostic processing difficulties that cut across autobiographical memory on concurrent intrusions and subsequent
traditional nosological divisions, rather than clusters of autobiographical recall in dysphoria. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 467–473
surface signs and symptoms, reflects a broader paradig- 24 Holmes, E.A. and Mathews, A. (2010) Mental imagery in emotion and
emotional disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 349–362
matic shift in our approaches to understanding mental 25 Bergouignan, L. et al. (2008) Field perspective deficit for positive
health problems. Exemplified most clearly in the Nation- memories characterizes autobiographical memory in euthymic
al Institute of Mental Health research domain criteria depressed patients. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 322–333
[69], this sea-change in the way that we seek to classify 26 Lemogne, C. et al. (2006) Episodic autobiographical memory in
psychopathology provides an excellent context for the depression: Specificity, autonoetic consciousness, and self-
perspective. Conscious. Cogn. 15, 258–268
translation of novel process-focused interventions from 27 Nigro, G. and Neisser, U. (1983) Point of view in personal memories.
basic research. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 467–482
28 McIsaac, H.K. and Eich, E. (2002) Vantage point in episodic memory.
References Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 146–150
1 Conway, M.A. and Pleydell-Pearce, C.W. (2000) The construction of 29 Kuyken, W. and Moulds, M.L. (2009) Remembering as an observer:
autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol. Rev. how is autobiographical memory retrieval vantage perspective linked
107, 261–288 to depression? Memory 17, 624–634
2 Dalgleish, T. (2004) Cognitive approaches to posttraumatic stress 30 Watkins, E.R. (2008) Constructive and unconstructive repetitive
disorder (PTSD): the evolution of multi-representational theorizing. thought. Psychol. Bull. 134, 163–206
Psychol. Bull. 130, 228–260 31 Dalgleish, T. et al. (2007) Reduced specificity of autobiographical
3 Williams, J.M.G. et al. (2007) Autobiographical memory specificity and memory and depression: the role of executive control. J. Exp.
emotional disorder. Psychol. Bull. 133, 122–148 Psychol. Gen. 136, 23–42
4 Gotlib, I.H. and Joormann, J. (2010) Cognition and depression: current 32 MacLeod, C. et al. (2009) Whither cognitive bias modification research?
status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285 Commentary on the special section articles. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118,
5 Watts, F.N. et al. (1990) Memory deficit in clinical depression: 89–99
processing resources and the structure of materials. Psychol. Med. 33 Hallion, L.S. and Ruscio, A.M. (2011) A meta-analysis of the effect of
20, 345–349 cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol. Bull.
6 Lloyd, G.G. and Lishman, W.A. (1975) Effect of depression on the speed 137, 940–958
of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychol. Med. 5, 173– 34 Browning, M. et al. (2013) The use of cognitive bias modification and
180 imagery in the understanding and treatment of depression. Curr. Top.
7 Patel, T. et al. (2007) Intrusive images and memories in major Behav. Neurosci. 14, 243–260
depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2573–2580 35 Baert, S. et al. (2010) Attentional bias training in depression:
8 Dearing, K.F. and Gotlib, I.H. (2009) Interpretation of ambiguous therapeutic effects depend on depression severity. J. Behav. Ther.
information in girls at risk for depression. J. Abnorm. Child Exp. Psychiatry 41, 265–274
Psychol. 37, 79–91 36 Wells, T.T. and Beevers, C.G. (2010) Biased attention and dysphoria:
9 Beck, A.T. (1976) Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders, manipulating attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms.
Meridian Cogn. Emot. 24, 719–728
10 Erber, R. and Erber, M. (1994) Beyond mood and social judgment: 37 Yiend, J. et al. (2013) Modifying interpretation in a clinically depressed
mood incongruent recall and mood regulation. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 24, sample using cognitive bias modification errors: a double blind
79–88 randomised controlled trial. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2, 1–14

7
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

38 Lang, T.J. et al. (2012) Cognitive bias modification using mental imagery 61 Schweizer, S. et al. (2013) Training the emotional brain: improving
for depression: developing a novel computerized intervention to change affective control through emotional working memory training. J.
negative thinking styles. Eur. J. Pers. 26, 145–157 Neurosci. 33, 5301–5311
39 Williams, A.D. et al. (2013) Combining imagination and reason in the 62 Beck, A.T. (1972) Depression: Causes and Treatment, University of
treatment of depression: A randomized controlled trial of internet- Pennsylvania Press
based cognitive bias modification and internet-CBT for depression. J. 63 Power, M.J. and Dalgleish, T. (2008) Cognition and Emotion: From
Consult. Clin. Psychol. 8, 793–799 Order to Disorder. (2nd edn), Taylor Francis
40 Micco, J.A. et al. (2013) Efficacy of interpretation bias modification 64 Craig, P. et al. (2008) Developing and evaluating complex
in depressed adolescents and young adults. Cogn. Ther. Res. 37, interventions: the new Medical Research Council guidance. BMJ
1–14 337, 979–983
41 Watkins, E.R. et al. (2009) Concreteness training reduces dysphoria: 65 Küpper, C. et al. (2014) Direct suppression as a mechanism for
proof-of-principle for repeated cognitive bias modification in controlling unpleasant memories in daily life. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 55–64 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036518
42 Watkins, E.R. (2012) Guided self-help concreteness training as an 66 Watkins, E. et al. (2004) Cognitive Behavioural Processes Across
intervention for major depression in primary care: a Phase II Psychological Disorders, Oxford University Press
randomised controlled trial. Psychol. Med. 42, 1359–1371 67 Moore, S.A. and Zoellner, L.A. (2007) Overgeneral autobiographical
43 Holmes, E. et al. (2007) Imagery rescripting in cognitive behaviour memory and traumatic events: an evaluative review. Psychol. Bull.
therapy: images, treatment techniques and outcomes. J. Behav. Ther. 133, 419–437
Exp. Psychiatry 38, 297–305 68 Kashdan, T.B. et al. (2011) Whether, how, and when social anxiety
44 Wheatley, J. et al. (2007) ’I’ll believe it when I can see it’: imagery shapes positive experiences and events: a self-regulatory framework
rescripting of intrusive sensory memories in depression. J. Behav. and treatment implications. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 786–799
Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 371–385 69 Sanislow, C.A. et al. (2010) Developing constructs for psychopathology
45 Holmes, E.A. et al. (2009) Developing interpretation bias modification research: research domain criteria. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119,
as a ‘cognitive vaccine’ for depressed mood: imagining positive events 631–639
makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally. J. Abnorm. 70 American Psychiatric Association (2012) Diagnostic and Statistical
Psychol. 118, 76–88 Manual of Mental Disorders. (5th edn), American Psychiatric
46 Holmes, E.A. et al. (2006) Positive interpretation training: effects of Publishing
mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood. Behav. Ther. 71 Judd, L.L. (1997) The clinical course of unipolar major depressive
37, 237–247 disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54, 989–991
47 Dalgleish, T. et al. (2013) Method-of-Loci as a mnemonic device to 72 Kessler, R.C. et al. (1994) Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-
facilitate access to self-affirming personal memories for individuals III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the
with depression. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 1, 156–162 National Comorbidity Survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 8–19
48 Raes, F. et al. (2009) Reducing cognitive vulnerability to depression: a 73 Solomon, D.A. et al. (2000) Multiple recurrences of major depressive
preliminary investigation of MEmory Specificity Training (MEST) in disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 229–233
inpatients with depressive symptomatology. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. 74 Frank, E. et al. (1990) 3-year outcomes for maintenance therapies in
Psychiatry 40, 24–38 recurrent depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47, 1093–1099
49 Neshat-Doost, H.T. et al. (2013) Enhancing autobiographical memory 75 Kupfer, D.J. et al. (1992) 5-year outcome for maintenance therapies in
specificity through cognitive training: an intervention for depression recurrent depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 469–477
translated from basic science. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 1, 84–92 76 The Kings Fund (2008) Paying the Price: The Cost of Mental Health
50 Moradi, A. et al. (2014) A pilot randomized control trial investigating Care in England to 2026, The Kings Fund
the efficacy of MEmory Specificity Training in improving 77 Murray, C.J.L. and Lopez, A.D. (1997) Alternative projections
symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 56, of mortality and disability by cause 1990-2020. Lancet 349, 1498–
68–74 1504
51 Hofmann, S.G. et al. (2010) The empirical status of the ‘new wave’ 78 Raes, F. et al. (2005) Reduced specificity of autobiographical memories:
of cognitive behavioral therapy. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 33, a mediator between rumination and ineffective problem solving in
701–710 major depression? J. Affect. Disord. 87, 331–335
52 Hayes, S.C. et al. (2006) Acceptance and commitment therapy: model, 79 Goddard, L. et al. (2001) The effects of specific retrieval instruction
processes and outcomes. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1–25 on social problem-solving in depression. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 40,
53 Linehan, M.M. (1993) Cognitive behavioural therapy of borderline 297–308
personality disorder, Guilford Press 80 Williams, J.M.G. et al. (2005) Problem solving deteriorates following
54 Watkins, E.R. et al. (2011) Rumination-focused cognitive-behavioural mood challenge in formerly depressed patients with a history of
therapy for residual depression: Phase II randomised controlled trial. suicidal ideation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 421–431
Br. J. Psychiatry 199, 317–322 81 Williams, J.M.G. et al. (1996) The specificity of autobiographical
55 Kuyken, W. et al. (2012) Does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy memory and imageability of the future. Mem. Cognit. 24, 116–125
prevent depressive relapse in routine practice settings? Br. Med. J. 82 Ajzen, I. (1998) Attitudes, Personality and Behaviour, Dorsey Press
345, e7194 83 Hermans, D. et al. (2005) Reduced autobiographical memory specificity
56 Piet, J. and Hougaard, E. (2011) The effect of mindfulness-based as an avoidant coping style. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 583–589
cognitive therapy for prevention of relapse in recurrent major 84 Kuyken, W. and Brewin, C.R. (1995) Autobiographical memory
depressive disorder: a systematic review and analysis. Clin. Psychol. functioning in depression and reports of early abuse. J. Abnorm.
Rev. 31, 1032–1040 Psychol. 104, 585–591
57 Kross, E. et al. (2012) ‘Asking why’ from a distance: its cognitive and 85 Raes, F. et al. (2003) Autobiographical memory specificity and affect
emotional consequences for people with depression. J. Abnorm. regulation: an experimental approach. Emotion 3, 201–206
Psychol. 121, 559–569 86 Raes, F. et al. (2006) Reduced autobiographical memory specificity and
58 Schartau, P. et al. (2009) Seeing the bigger picture: Training in perspective affect regulation. Cogn. Emot. 20, 402–429
broadening reduces self-reported affect and psychophysiological response 87 Crane, C. et al. (2007) The effects of analytical and experiential
to distressing films and autobiographical memories. J. Abnorm. Psychol. rumination on autobiographical memory specificity in individuals
118, 15–27 with a history of major depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 3077–3087
59 Jaeggi, S.M. et al. (2008) Improving fluid intelligence with training on 88 Raes, F. et al. (2008) Non-ruminative processing reduces overgeneral
working memory. PNAS 105, 6829–6833 autobiographical memory retrieval in students. Behav. Res. Ther. 46,
60 Schweizer, S. et al. (2011) Extending brain-training to the affective 748–756
domain: increasing cognitive and affective executive control through 89 Cicero, M.T. (2009). In (55 BCE/2001) Cicero on the Ideal Orator (De
emotional working memory training. PLoS ONE 6, e0024372 Oratore) (May, J.M. and Wisse, K., eds), Oxford University Press

8
TICS-1358; No. of Pages 9

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences xxx xxxx, Vol. xxx, No. x

90 Raz, A. et al. (2009) A slice of P: an exploratory neuroimaging study 94 von Restorff, H. (1933) Uber die Wirkung von Bereichsbildungen im
of digit encoding and retrieval in a superior memorist. Neurocase 15, Spurenfeld [The effects of field formation in the trace field].
361–372 Psychologische Forschung 18, 299–342 (in German)
91 Bellezza, F.S. and Reddy, B.G. (1978) Mnemonic devices and natural 95 Spence, J.D. (1984) The Memory Palace of Matteo Ricci, Penguin
memory. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 11, 277–280 96 Yates, F.A. (1966) The Art of Memory, Routledge and Kegan Paul
92 Groninger, L.D. (1971) Mnemonic imagery and forgetting. Psychon. 97 Carruthers, M.J. and Ziolkowski, J. (2002) The Medieval Craft of
Sci. 23, 161–163 Memory: An Anthology of Texts and Pictures, University of
93 Ross, J. and Lawrence, K.A. (1968) Some observations on memory Pennsylvania Press
artifice. Psychon. Sci. 13, 107–108

You might also like