You are on page 1of 16

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association

2011, Vol. 101, No. 6, 1262–1277 0022-3514/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024407

Suspicious Spirits, Flexible Minds: When Distrust Enhances Creativity

Jennifer Mayer and Thomas Mussweiler


University of Cologne

Intuitively, as well as in light of prior research, distrust and creativity appear incompatible. The social
consequences of distrust include reluctance to share information, a quality detrimental to creativity in
social settings. At the same time, the cognitive concomitants of distrust bear resemblance to creative
cognition: Distrust seems to foster thinking about nonobvious alternatives to potentially deceptive
appearances. These cognitive underpinnings of distrust hold the provocative implication that distrust may
foster creativity. Mirroring these contradictory findings, we suggest that the social versus cognitive
consequences of distrust have diverging implications for creativity. We address this question in Study 1
by introducing private/public as a moderating variable for effects of distrust on creativity. Consistent with
distrust’s social consequences, subliminal distrust (vs. trust) priming had detrimental effects on creative
generation presumed to be public. Consistent with distrust’s cognitive consequences, though, an opposite
tendency emerged in private. Study 2 confirmed a beneficial effect of distrust on private creative
generation with a different priming method and pointed to cognitive flexibility as the mediating process.
Studies 3 and 4 showed increased category inclusiveness versus increased remote semantic spread after
distrust priming, consistent with enhanced cognitive flexibility as a consequence of distrust. Taken
together, these results provide evidence for the creativity-enhancing potential of distrust and suggest
cognitive flexibility as its underlying mechanism.

Keywords: creativity, cognitive flexibility, distrust, suspicion, priming

Creativity—the generation of work that is both novel and ap- performance crucially depends on its social reception: An idea is
propriate (e.g., Sternberg & Lubart, 1999)—is an important skill creative only if the social environment considers it to be so (e.g.,
for coping with many different circumstances in life. Innovations Simonton, 1997). In light of the fine line between novelty and
in science or economy depend heavily on people tapping their nonsense, displaying creative behavior often means going out on a
creative potential. More mundane problems also often demand limb and running the risk of social ridicule. In addition, people
leaving the beaten track to find creative solutions. The significance sharing their creative ideas run the risk of others stealing their most
of creativity in various domains is reflected in a broad scientific novel and useful ones.
interest in factors that foster or hinder creative performance (e.g., The risky business of creativity (cf. George & Zhou, 2007;
see Hennessey & Amabile, 2010, for an overview). Intrapsychic Glover, 1977; Runco, 2007) is therefore likely to be particularly
variations in affect, cognition, and motivation as well as important sensitive to the perceived benevolence of the social surroundings.
interpersonal factors like cooperation and conflict (Carnevale & This interplay of expected versus doubted benevolence and the
Probst, 1998; De Dreu & Nijstad, 2008) or social power (Galinsky, willingness to take risks is reflected in the concepts of trust and
Magee, Gruenfeld, & Whitson, 2008) have been studied with distrust (e.g., Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 2007; Simpson, 2007).
respect to their influence on creativity. Interpersonal factors or, When people expect others to be by and large benevolent toward
more generally, social context might play a particularly important them, they trust others and become willing to take risks (cf.
role in creativity. For one, creativity is highly desired in interactive Schoorman et al., 2007). The more people doubt benevolent in-
contexts like the classroom or work teams. Furthermore, creative tentions, however, the more risk adverse they become. The ex-
pression of creative ideas is thus likely to differ depending on
whether spirits are trustful or suspicious.

This article was published Online First July 11, 2011.


Suspicious Spirits in Creativity Research
Jennifer Mayer and Thomas Mussweiler, Department of Psychology,
University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany. Research aimed directly at the link between distrust and cre-
This research was supported by a Gottfried-Wilhelm-Leibniz Award by ativity is rather scarce and predominantly originates from organi-
the German Science Foundation as well as by a European Young Investi- zational psychology. The (mostly correlational) data point to a
gator Award from the European Science Foundation to Thomas Muss- negative effect of distrust on creativity (e.g., Dakhli & De Clercq,
weiler. We would like to thank the members of Social Cognition Cologne
2004; Ekvall, 1996; Ekvall & Ryhammar, 1999; Klimoski &
for their input. This research was conducted in the context of Jennifer
Mayer’s doctoral dissertation at the University of Cologne, Cologne,
Karol, 1976). Distrust is seen as disruptive of a supporting and
Germany. cooperative team climate and, therefore, is seen as disruptive of
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jennifer creativity and innovation as well (e.g., Amabile & Gryskiewicz,
Mayer, Department Psychologie, Universität zu Köln, Richard-Strauss-Str. 1989; Hunter, Bedell, & Mumford, 2007). Research on factors
2, 50931 Köln, Germany. E-mail: jennifer.mayer@uni-koeln.de related to the concepts of trust and distrust strongly supports this

1262
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1263

view: For example, psychological safety (i.e., the expectation that or, more generally, the truth might be. To do so, not only are
others will respond favorably when one expresses concerns or people in a state of distrust likely to engage in mind reading, but
proposes new ideas) is seen as conducive to creativity in the they also appear to consider possible alternative scenarios
workplace (cf. Carmeli, Reiter-Palmon, & Ziv, 2010; Hennessey, (Chiappe et al., 2004; Fein, Hilton, & Miller, 1990; Marchand &
2003). Psychological safety, in turn, is certainly associated with Vonk, 2005; Schul, 1993; Schul et al., 1996). Moreover, being
trust rather than with distrust. skilled deceivers themselves, people are aware that for deception
Nevertheless, it is especially in situations of potential danger to be successful, it is crucial to make the true motive as nonobvious
that a problem must be solved on the spot, often in an unusual or as possible. In the case of impending deception, people therefore
novel way. Creativity as a means of problem solving might be know that they are well advised to look for uncommon, nonroutine
especially important in such situations. Researchers have begun to occurrences. Research by Schul et al. (2008) supports this hypoth-
take a closer look at the potentially beneficial influence of chal- esis by showing that participants primed with distrust were partic-
lenging or threatening states on creativity. Along the way, the ularly sensitive to nonroutine contingencies. In summary, suspi-
understanding of the factors that hinder and foster creativity has cious spirits are likely to interpret information from multiple
become considerably more complex. On the intrapersonal level, it perspectives, which tend to be remote, uncommon ones. In contrast
has been demonstrated that not only positive but also negative to the notion that distrust is necessarily detrimental to creativity,
affective states can foster creativity (De Dreu, Baas, & Nijstad, these characteristics perfectly mirror preconditions for thinking
2008). With regard to interpersonal factors, effective lying, for outside the box—a common metaphor of creative cognition.
instance, has been shown to be positively correlated with perfor- How may these cognitive consequences of distrust influence
mance in common measures of creative ideation (Walczyk, Runco, creative thinking? To answer this question, it is important to
Tripp, & Smith, 2008). Furthermore, it has been shown that identify elements of creative cognition. According to the dual
conflict is conducive to creativity if creative performance is func- pathway to creativity model (De Dreu et al., 2008), cognitive
tional in the sense that it is related to handling the conflict at hand flexibility and persistence are the two critical processes of creative
(De Dreu & Nijstad, 2008). cognition. Using the flexibility pathway, novel ideas originate
In light of the latter findings, a creativity-enhancing effect of a from changing perspectives (set breaking; e.g., Smith & Blanken-
negative and threat-associated state like distrust seems at least not ship, 1991) and flat associative hierarchies (Mednick, 1962). This
out of the question. The cognitive characteristics of distrust even kind of creative cognition becomes manifest in the use of many
cast considerable doubt on a unilaterally detrimental influence of different cognitive categories as well as in the breadth and inclu-
distrust on creativity, as we delineate in the following. siveness of the categories themselves. Using the persistence path-
way, original ideas are discovered by systematically and persever-
The Creative Mind of Suspicious Spirits ingly exploring few cognitive categories in depth. This alternative
process of creative cognition usually results in prolonged effort
According to the high prevalence of lying and attempts at and increased time on task.
deception documented in research (e.g., DePaulo & Kashy, 1998; How can these pathways of creative cognition be linked to
DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendol, Wyer, & Epstein, 1996), distrust distrust? On the one hand, preconditions of the persistence path-
constitutes a ubiquitous occurrence in everyday life (cf. Kramer, way map nicely onto the affective–motivational properties of
1998). It is readily evoked by subtle cues of unreliability, by a distrust, which are comparable with those of other states involving
single questionable action, or by an isolated instance of betrayal threatening circumstances: Compared with trust and neutral con-
(cf. Marchand & Vonk, 2005). The resulting mind-set is, broadly ditions, distrust is associated with a more negative affective state
defined, characterized by doubts about whether appearances can be that also involves a higher level of arousal (cf. Schul et al., 2008).
taken at face value (cf. Schul, Mayo, & Burnstein, 2008). The case Negative activating affective states, in turn, have been shown to
of misleading appearances usually implies a disadvantage for the have the potential to enhance creativity through the persistence
deceived individual. Walking away from every potentially suspi- pathway (De Dreu et al., 2008). Furthermore, distrust can be seen
cious character or disbelieving any potentially invalid information, as associated with an avoidance motivational orientation (Muss-
however, would not be a well-adapted option. Individuals might weiler & Burgmer, 2011). Avoidance orientations, in turn, have
miss out on opportunities, if the other person were actually trust- been connected to narrow and systematic processing (cf. Friedman
worthy or the information were true (Fetchenhauer & Dunning, & Förster, 2000), which is consistent with findings of elaborate
2009). processing in a distrust mind-set (e.g., Chiappe et al., 2004; Hilton
A better strategy would be to invest in more elaborate informa- et al., 1993; Schul et al., 1996) and, therefore, again, the persis-
tion processing (Chiappe et al., 2004; Hilton, Fein, & Miller, 1993; tence pathway.
Schul, Burnstein, & Bardi, 1996) and to prepare for both appear- On the other hand, people in a distrust mind-set not only
ances being true and appearances not being true. Schul, Mayo, and elaborate more, but they do so in a specific way: They seem to
Burnstein (2004) concluded that, if the latter was true, then the entertain multiple interpretations of potentially invalid information
opposite of any given information should be readily available in rather than to elaborate intensely on that information within only
situations of distrust. In line with this possibility, they demon- one interpretative frame (Fein et al., 1990; Schul et al., 1996). The
strated that a state of distrust indeed involves a basic tendency to latter is consistent with category diversity and is thus indicative of
activate incongruent cognitions. The effects of distrust, however, cognitive flexibility. A comparable basic tendency to consider
are unlikely to be limited to the mere tendency to consider oppo- alternatives is evoked by counterfactual thinking (Galinsky &
sites. Instead, it seems reasonable that distrust should induce Moskowitz, 2000), the process of mentally creating alternative
individuals to try and figure out what a potentially hidden motive outcomes to a given event (cf. Epstude & Roese, 2008). Counter-
1264 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

factual thinking, in turn, has been shown to foster creativity The latter is highly relevant with regard to the social conse-
(Markman, Lindberg, Kray, & Galinsky, 2007) and to be helpful in quences of distrust: Although the cognitive characteristics of the
overcoming functional fixedness (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). accompanying mind-set may foster individual creativity, distrust
The latter is indicative of set breaking and, therefore, again, of will keep people from sharing ideas—and probably the more
cognitive flexibility. Consistent with the general possibility of unusual and possibly crazy ones in particular—in order to protect
increased cognitive flexibility in states involving potential threat, themselves against exploitation and ridicule (Amabile & Gryskie-
De Dreu and Nijstad (2008) showed that participants in a compet- wicz, 1989; Edmondson, 2004; Ekvall, 1996; Ekvall & Ryham-
itive conflict mind-set exhibited increased, targeted category di- mar, 1999; Klimoski & Karol, 1976; Petermann, 1992; Rotenberg
versity as well as category inclusiveness for conflict-related ma- et al., 2010; Schoorman et al., 2007; West, 2002; see also De
terial. Recently, a mind-set quite similar to the one provoked by Cremer, 1999). Potentially creativity-enhancing effects of distrust
impending deceit (i.e., a competitive mind-set that also involves and other threat-involving interpersonal factors are therefore un-
the inclination to engage in mind reading) has been shown to likely to surface in public settings. Consistently, findings of
enhance executive functioning in terms of cognitive flexibility heightened creativity in a conflict mind-set or related to effective
more generally (Ybarra, Winkielman, Yeh, Burnstein, & Ka- lying are characterized by the relative privacy of the creative act:
vanagh, 2011). Finally, a distrust mind-set has been shown to Competitive negotiation tactics are not disclosed to the other party
foster sensitivity to nonroutine occurrences (Schul et al., 2008), for scrutiny (De Dreu & Nijstad, 2008) nor will a liar reveal his or
consistent with an increased accessibility of uncommon, remote her (creative) considerations in coming up with an effective lie.
alternatives. The latter is consistent with flat associative hierar- Social climate might therefore be less important to creativity in
chies (Mednick, 1962) as another manifestation of the flexibility private, making room for the potentially beneficial cognitive con-
pathway. sequences of a distrusting state of mind.
Valid reasons for assuming both enhanced persistence and en-
hanced cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-set strongly impli- Current Research
cate the possibility of a creativity-enhancing effect of distrust.
In accordance with the diverging social and cognitive conse-
Social Versus Private Creativity quences of distrust, we predict a detrimental influence of distrust
on creative performance when the task involves public sharing of
The cognitive consequences of distrust strongly suggest a ben- creative ideas. In cases where the task does not require sharing
eficial effect of suspicious spirits on creative performance. Yet, ideas, however, we hypothesize the opposite to be true, namely,
this seems at odds with findings from a review of the research on increased creativity in a distrusting state of mind. The following
the link between distrust and creativity, which indicates a detri- four studies set out to investigate these influences of distrust on
mental effect. A closer look at the two contradicting lines of creativity. Study 1 aims at disentangling the differential influence
research reveals a possible moderator: Most research hinting at of distrust on creativity depending on whether public information
detrimental effects of distrust and distrust-related variables on sharing is inherent to the creativity task. Studies 2 through 4 focus
creativity refers to something one might term public creativity (i.e., on the hypothesized beneficial influence of the cognitive conse-
creative tasks performed in group settings). For this kind of cre- quences of distrust on creative cognition in private.
ativity, it is crucial not only to come up with creative ideas but to For these private situations, distrust as we spontaneously picture
share them as well. Consequently, creativity in public not only it—suspicion focused at a specific person in a particular situa-
depends on how creative the individual is but is also crucially tion—might not be the most relevant form of distrust. Yet, dis-
conditional upon the degree to which individual-level creativity trusting states are not restricted to instances of actual interactions
comes into effect (e.g., in a group setting; Taggar, 2002). For with potentially untrustworthy individuals. Rather, the ubiquity of
individual creativity to play out in a group setting, a positive, distrust and its significance in preventing harm due to impending
benevolent, and psychologically safe group climate seems to be deception make it likely that an integrated mental representation
beneficial: Groups consisting of members with prosocial (rather has evolved, comprising the respective affective, cognitive, and
than proself) motives exhibit increased creativity (Bechtoldt, De behavioral tendencies (Cosmides & Tooby, 2005; see also Schul et
Dreu, Nijstad, & Choi, 2010). Similarly, individuals’ agreeable- al., 2008). In the case of such an integrated distrust representation,
ness and conscientiousness contribute significantly to creative even subtle distrust-related cues should be capable of evoking the
performance on the group level (Taggar, 2002). Because group whole mental representation, that is, a distrust mind-set (cf. Schul
climate also reacts upon individual level creativity, a creativity- et al., 1996, 2004, 2008). Consequently, a distrust mind-set is also
enhancing social environment needs to encourage constructive, likely to be triggered by cues that are typically seen as being
task-related criticism and must not be too soothing either. Consis- related to deception, for example, certain facial or vocal features
tently, stimulation brought on by changing group composition (see DePaulo et al., 2003; Schul et al., 2004; Zebrowitz, Voinescu,
(Nemeth & Ormiston, 2007) or by a preceding competitive nego- & Collins, 1996) and distrust-related semantic cues (see Friesen &
tiation (Beersma & De Dreu, 2005) has been shown to foster group Sinclair, 2011; Rotenberg et al., 2010). Therefore, the concept is
creativity. In sum, for public creativity to be successful, social also amply relevant to a wide array of private situations, in which
climate needs to stimulate individual creativity without preventing we expect the creativity-enhancing effects of a distrust mind-set to
idea sharing. If idea sharing is hindered (e.g., because of social emerge.
anxiousness), public creative performance decreases even though With our operationalizations of distrust, we intended to (a)
private performance is not necessarily diminished (e.g., Camacho closely model the form of distrust relevant to private creativity and
& Paulus, 1995). (b) instill distrust in a pure fashion (“mere state of distrust”; Schul
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1265

et al., 2004, p. 669) that does not simultaneously involve additional conceivable but not central to our hypothesis. The remaining
social factors, such as, for example, decreased liking of a specific measures of flexibility and persistence allow for an exploratory
target (cf. Rule et al., 2010; Tyler, Feldman, & Reichert, 2006). investigation as to which one of the two alternative pathways to
Consequently, we chose to manipulate distrust in a subtle way. creativity proposed by De Dreu et al. (2008) may be the critical
This approach has been taken not only in previous research on one.
distrust (Rotenberg et al., 2010; Schul et al., 2004, 2008), but also
in research on other important interpersonal variables like social
Methods
power (Galinsky et al., 2008). Specifically, Studies 1, 3, and 4 used
a subliminal priming procedure embedded in a lexical decision Design and participants. Study 1 is based on a 2 (type of
task. Study 2 used a supraliminal priming procedure, namely a priming: distrust vs. trust) ⫻ 2 (context of idea generation: public
scrambled sentences task. Both procedures are widely used for vs. private) between-subjects factorial design. We recruited 68
concept activation in an unobtrusive manner (cf. Bargh & Char- students as participants and randomly assigned them to one of the
trand, 2000). four experimental conditions. Participants were approached on a
As our dependent measures, we used three tasks that tap differ- campus of the University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany, and
ent aspects of creative thinking. In Studies 1 and 2, creativity was offered chocolate and coffee vouchers as compensation. Three
assessed by one of the most widely used creativity measures, participants were excluded because of data collection irregulari-
namely, a prototypical idea-generation task. Tasks of that kind ties, leaving a final sample of 65 participants (44 female, 21 male).
allow not only for the investigation of originality as the core Materials and procedure. When the experimenter and the
component of creativity, but also for a preliminary exploration of participant arrived at the lab, they found a female confederate
the pathways thereto (i.e., of cognitive flexibility and persistence). waiting in front of the lab door, pretending to be another partici-
In Studies 3 and 4, we took a closer look at the role of the pant. Inside the lab, the experimenter ushered the participant and
flexibility pathway, for which the cognitive consequences of dis- the confederate into two adjacent, sound insulated cubicles and
trust suggest a particularly important role. We did so by measuring asked them to sit down in front of a 75-Hz computer monitor.
category inclusiveness and flat associative hierarchies, respec- After giving their consent, participants proceeded to the first
tively, as two central manifestations of cognitive flexibility. All task, which was introduced as a lexical decision task. This task
studies were computer based, and all participants were native served as a means to subliminally and repeatedly present partici-
German speakers. pants with either the German verb misstrauen [to distrust] or the
verb vertrauen [to trust] (cf. Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Bargh, & van
Study 1 Knippenberg, 2000; Mussweiler & Förster, 2000). Such a proce-
dure has been shown to yield reliable differences in a trust game,
Study 1 was designed to examine whether distrust and trust have indicating that compared with the trust condition, participants in
differential effects on creativity depending on whether creative the distrust condition trusted less (Posten & Mussweiler, 2011).
ideas will be made public. To that end, we first induced a distrust Instructions to the task pointed out that participants were to
mind-set or a trust mind-set in participants by means of a sublim- focus on the center of the screen where letter strings would appear.
inal priming procedure. Subsequently, we asked participants to Their task was to decide whether any given letter string constituted
work on a creativity task. One group of participants believed their a proper German word. Each of the 26 trials began with the
performance would remain private, whereas another group of presentation of a fixation string (XWXXWWXXWX) for 3,000 ms.
participants were told that their performance was made public to a Without the awareness of participants, the fixation string was
participant next door. In accordance with prior research, we replaced by the prime word (13 ms), which was immediately
expected distrust to have a detrimental effect on creativity if masked by the fixation string again (506 ms; cf. suggestions by
creative performance required public sharing. We did, however, Bargh & Chartrand, 2000). Next, the target letter string overwrote
expect this disadvantage to vanish and the beneficial cognitive the fixation string and remained on the screen until participants
consequences of distrust to take effect when creative performance indicated their decision by pressing either a blue computer key (for
remained private. proper German words) or a yellow key (for nonwords). The first
Creative performance was measured by means of an idea- two trials served as practice trials, and instead of the prime word,
generation task, which allows for the investigation of different a blank screen was presented. The subsequent 24 experimental
aspects of creativity. Idea-generation tasks are typically scored for trials each contained the same prime word (i.e., misstrauen [to
quantity (ideational fluency), quality (originality), and diversity of distrust] in the distrust condition or vertrauen [to trust] in the trust
production (flexibility; e.g., Plucker & Renzulli, 1999). Addition- condition). The target letter strings were proper German words in
ally, from the latter two measures an indicator of persistence can one of the practice trials and 18 of the experimental trials (e.g.,
be calculated (cf. De Dreu et al., 2008; Nijstad, Stroebe, & Jacke [jacket], kämmen [to comb]) and were nonsense letter strings
Lodewijkx, 2002). Out of these indicators, originality certainly in the remaining seven trials (e.g., grompem, Bealk).
constitutes the core component of creativity. Therefore, the hy- After completion of the priming task, participants continued on
pothesized differential influences of distrust compared with trust to a second, ostensibly unrelated study. This study consisted of an
depending on public versus private performance concerns first and idea-generation task and constituted the dependent measure of
foremost the originality of solutions. Quality of solutions (origi- creativity. In addition, instructions for the task served as a means
nality) may come along with quantity (e.g., Diehl & Stroebe, 1987; to introduce the second experimental factor, that is, the context of
Hocevar, 1979; Osborn, 1963), but this need not be the case. A idea generation (public vs. private). Participants were told that we
conceptually similar pattern for ideational fluency is therefore were interested in how the simultaneous processing of ideas pro-
1266 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

vided by other people—a characteristic of brainstorming in The flexibility index was calculated by averaging the numbers of
groups—influenced generating ideas oneself. The instructions fur- different categories per participant that resulted from the ratings of
ther read that there were two different roles: creating ideas while the two independent coders. Persistence is characterized by an
processing ideas of others versus creating ideas undisturbed. Ac- in-depth exploration of categories. The more ideas participants
tually, the computer assigned all participants the latter role. De- generate within any tackled category, the higher their persistence.
pending on condition, however, participants received different Accordingly, we derived a measure of average within-category
information about what would happen to the ideas they were about fluency by dividing the number of unique ideas (ideational flu-
to generate. In the public condition, instructions explained that the ency) by the number of categories these ideas were derived from
person in the adjacent cubicle (the confederate) was assigned the (flexibility; De Dreu et al., 2008; Nijstad et al., 2002).
role of “generating ideas while processing the ideas of others.”
Participants were told that this would be achieved by transferring Results
every idea they generated to the other person’s computer screen,
implying that their ideas would be made public to the other person. In the main analysis, we submitted the originality measure to a
In the private condition, instructions did not mention the other 2 (type of priming: distrust vs. trust) ⫻ 2 (context of idea gener-
person but simply stated that the participant’s ideas would be ation: public vs. private) analysis of variance (ANOVA). Analo-
saved on a server. gous analyses were performed for the remaining three indicators.
Next, participants received detailed instructions for the idea- Main analysis: Originality. Inspection of Table 1 reveals
generation task, a so-called alternative uses task (Guilford, 1967), that the mean originality scores are in line with the predicted
in which participants were told that they were to generate as many pattern: In the public condition, participants primed with distrust
alternative uses for an everyday object as they could think of. achieved lower originality scores than participants primed with
Before beginning idea generation, participants were asked to click trust. In the private condition, participants primed with distrust
on a button to check whether the person in the adjacent cubicle was tended to exhibit higher originality scores compared with partici-
ready to begin (public condition) versus whether the connection to pants primed with trust. This pattern was borne out in a significant
the server was working properly (private condition). This was done interaction effect, F(1, 61) ⫽ 5.89, p ⫽ .02, ␩2p ⫽ .09 (all other
to lend credibility to the cover story. After a few seconds, partic- Fs ⬍ 1, ns).
ipants read that the person in the adjacent cubicle (vs. the server) Supplementary analyses: Ideational fluency, flexibility, and
had confirmed the connection. On the next screen, participants persistence. Our hypotheses for ideational fluency were con-
were presented with the object for which they should generate ceptually parallel to those for originality. The pattern of means
alternative uses (a newspaper; cf. Wallach & Kogan, 1965) as well (Table 1) is consistent with this reasoning: In the public condition,
as an input field to type in their ideas. participants primed with distrust tended to achieve lower fluency
A final questionnaire asked for demographic data and tested for scores than participants primed with trust, whereas the opposite
awareness of the true purpose of the study as well as for awareness tendency resulted from the private condition. The respective two-
of the prime word in the priming procedure. None of the partici- way interaction did not reach significance, F(1, 61) ⫽ 2.07, p ⫽
pants indicated awareness of the true purpose of the study or of the
prime word. At the end, participants were thanked and handed
their compensation. Table 1
Measures and dependent variables. Indicators of creative Means and Standard Deviations of Mean Originality of Ideas,
performance were derived from ideas generated in 2 min (cf. Number of Ideas (Fluency), Number of Categories (Flexibility),
Gilhooly, Fioratou, Anthony, & Wynn, 2007; Schoppe, 1975). In and Within-Category Fluency (Persistence) as a Function of
a first step, ideas were coded for ideational fluency, originality, Type of Priming (Distrust vs. Trust) and Context of Idea
and flexibility. In a second step, a measure of persistence was Generation Condition (Public vs. Private) in Study 1
calculated from the fluency and flexibility measures.
The number of nonredundant ideas constituted the measure of Context of idea generation
ideational fluency. For the originality measure, two independent Public Private
coders rated every idea on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (not at
all creative) to 5 (very creative). Interrater reliability was good Measure Distrust Trust Distrust Trust
(intraclass correlation coefficient ⫽ .78). Ratings of the two coders
Originality
were averaged per idea. The mean of these values for each par- M 1.41a 1.68b 1.58a,b 1.43a
ticipant’s set of ideas constituted the originality measure. Flexibil- SD 0.44 0.40 0.29 0.22
ity is reflected in the number of different semantic categories from Fluency
which ideas are derived. The more categories participants use, the M 6.12a 7.13a,b 8.07b 7.41a,b
higher their cognitive flexibility. To create a category system, the SD 2.23 2.03 3.06 1.91
Flexibility
first author reviewed ideas generated by a different group of 28 M 4.50a 5.63b 5.27a,b 4.94a,b
participants and extracted 14 categories (e.g., crafting, hygiene, SD 1.27 1.63 1.40 1.29
weapon) complemented by a miscellaneous category for ideas that Persistence
did not fit any of the semantic categories. Two independent coders M 1.37a,b 1.28b 1.52a 1.53a
SD 0.37 0.18 0.38 0.35
assigned a category to each idea. Interrater reliability was excellent
for single ideas (Cohen’s ␬ ⫽ .94) and for the number of different Note. Within each row, means not labeled with the same superscript
categories per participant (intraclass correlation coefficient ⫽ .98). differed according to the respective simple-contrast analysis at p ⬍ .05.
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1267

.16. A trend toward a main effect for context of idea generation certainly involves a higher level of privacy in the private condition
emerged, F(1, 61) ⫽ 3.74, p ⫽ .06, ␩2p ⫽ .06. No main effect for than in the public condition where ideas ostensibly were trans-
type of priming emerged, F ⬍ 1, ns. ferred to another person. Nevertheless, participants in the private
For flexibility, inspection of the means in Table 1 reveals a condition may well have assumed that their ideas would be ana-
pattern parallel to the one for originality: In the public condition, lyzed by the experimenter at some later point in time and that they
participants primed with distrust derived their ideas from fewer would not remain so private after all. This may have prevented a
different categories than participants primed with trust. In the potential creativity-enhancing effect of distrust to become readily
private condition, participants primed with distrust tended to ex- apparent.
hibit higher flexibility scores compared with participants primed
with trust. As for originality, this pattern resulted in a significant Study 2
interaction effect, F(1, 61) ⫽ 4.35, p ⫽ .04, ␩2p ⫽ .07 (all other
We designed Study 2 to examine the effects of distrust under
Fs ⬍ 1.33, ps ⬎ .25). For the persistence measure, inspection of
conditions of true privacy. Accordingly, participants were not
the means in Table 1 suggests a pattern different from those
made to believe that their ideas would be transferred to a server
observed for the other indicators. In both the public and the private
and were thus likely to experience a heightened sense of privacy so
conditions, persistence was similar for participants primed with
that the creativity-enhancing effects of distrust may become ap-
distrust and for participants primed with trust. Independent of type
parent.
of priming condition, persistence values were higher for partici-
We used the same creativity measure—an idea-generation
pants in the private condition (combined M ⫽ 1.53, SD ⫽ 0.36)
task—as in Study 1 but used a different priming manipulation for
than for participants in the public condition (combined M ⫽ 1.33,
generalizability purposes. To that end, we adapted another widely
SD ⫽ 0.29), F(1, 61) ⫽ 6.09, p ⫽ .02, ␩2p ⫽ .09. No other
used priming procedure, namely, a scrambled sentences task (Srull
significant effects emerged, all Fs ⬍ 1.
& Wyer, 1979), to induce distrust versus trust. Furthermore, we
added a neutral control group. So far, trust manipulations have
Discussion repeatedly yielded results similar to those of the control conditions
in experimental studies (Klimoski & Karol, 1976; Schul et al.,
Results of Study 1 are generally in line with our hypotheses.
2004; cf. Schul et al., 2008), most likely because without reasons
Effects of trust and distrust on originality crucially depended on
for doubt, people typically have trust in others (e.g., Berg, Dick-
whether creative behavior was carried out publicly or privately. In
haut, & McCabe, 1995; McKnight, Cummings, & Chervany, 1998;
the public condition, we demonstrated that distrust is detrimental
cf. Gilbert, 1991). In light of this default to trust, we expected
to creativity: Participants who were told that their ideas would be
similar levels of creativity in both the trust and control conditions.
made public to another person produced less original ideas when in
Our theoretical analysis, however, holds that— compared with the
a distrust mind-set than when in a trust mind-set. This was not the
trust and control conditions— distrust enhances creativity.
case, however, for participants in the private condition whose ideas
were ostensibly merely saved to a special server and, thus, re-
Methods
mained private. In line with previous findings (e.g., Diehl &
Stroebe, 1987; Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2007), a conceptu- Design and participants. We recruited 60 psychology stu-
ally similar pattern for ideational fluency emerged. The same was dents as participants and offered them partial course credit as
true for category diversity as the indicator of flexibility: In the compensation. They were randomly assigned to either a distrust
public condition, distrust yielded less category diversity than trust, condition, a trust condition, or a control condition. Three partici-
which was not the case in the private condition. Results from the pants were excluded because of data collection irregularities, leav-
persistence measure indicated no differences for participants ing a final sample of 57 participants (39 female, 18 male).
primed with trust and participants primed with distrust regardless Materials and procedure. On arrival in the lab, participants
of context of idea generation. The parallel between originality and were told that they would be given two short experimental tasks.
flexibility measures, and the missing parallel between originality The first of these was the scrambled sentences priming task (Srull
and persistence measures, might be taken as preliminary evidence & Wyer, 1979). Instructions to this task pointed out that, in each
for cognitive flexibility as the critical factor with regard to the trial, five words would be presented in random order. Four out of
influence of (dis)trust on creativity. the five words formed a meaningful sentence. Participants were
In sum, the results provide initial support for our hypothesis by asked to quickly form the correct sentence and to type it into the
revealing that distrust is not necessarily detrimental to creativity. computer. Seven out of a total of 15 trials were identical for all
On a descriptive level, results actually imply a reversal in the three conditions. These trials did not contain prime words and were
pattern of means in the private condition: Originality, fluency, and chosen to be unrelated to distrust and trust. In the control condi-
flexibility scores of participants primed with distrust tended to be tion, the same was true for the remaining eight trials. In the distrust
higher than those of participants primed with trust. In simple effect and the trust conditions, these remaining trials contained prime
analyses, these differences did not reach statistical significance (cf. words closely related to distrust and trust, respectively (see Table
superscripts in Table 1). This may be due to the moderate level of 2; for a similar procedure, cf. Friesen & Sinclair, 2011). The
privacy evoked by our operationalization of the private context of second, ostensibly unrelated, task was the alternative uses task, our
idea generation: For reasons of experimental control, the private creativity measure. Instructions were mostly similar to the ones
condition had to be modeled closely after the public condition. To described in Study 1 with the critical exception that this time no
that end, participants in the private condition were informed that mention was made that participants’ ideas were transferred to a
their ideas would be saved on a server. This operationalization server or another person.
1268 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

Table 2
German Prime Words (Uninflected Forms) and Their General-Sense English Equivalent as Used
in the Experimental Trials of the Distrust and Trust Conditions in Study 2

Type of priming

Distrust Trust

German English German English


prime words equivalents prime words equivalents

misstrauen to distrust vertraut trusting


vorgetäuscht feigned glaubwürdig credible
zweifeln to doubt authentisch authentic
Argwohn suspicion Verlass dependence
unsicher uncertain sicher certain
fraglich questionable zuverlässig reliable
verdächtig suspicious ehrlich honest
sich täuschen to be mistaken solidarisch showing solidarity

Note. Prime words were embedded in a scrambled sentences task.

A final questionnaire asked participants to provide some demo- persistence scores in the distrust condition). Yet, the contrast
graphic data. A concluding question probed for suspicion of the comparing the distrust condition with the other two conditions was
true purpose of the study. None of the participants indicated not significant (|t| ⬍ 1).
awareness. At the end, participants were thanked and given their Mediation analysis. The parallel pattern of results for orig-
course credit as compensation. inality and flexibility and the lack of such a parallel pattern for
Measures and dependent variables. Measures of ideational originality and persistence suggest that flexibility is the more
fluency, originality, flexibility, and persistence were derived as important pathway to creativity in the present context. In a next
described in Study 1. Interrater reliabilities were comparable with step, therefore, we tested whether flexibility mediated the effect of
the ones in Study 1 for the originality ratings (intraclass correlation the priming manipulation on originality (see Figure 1 for the
coefficient ⫽ .74) as well as for the flexibility coding (Cohen’s mediational model and the respective regression coefficients).
␬ ⫽ .91 for single ideas; intraclass correlation coefficient ⫽ .98 for Following recent recommendations (Preacher & Hayes, 2004;
the number of different categories per participant). Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010), we conducted a bootstrapping anal-
ysis (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Results revealed that, indeed,
Results flexibility mediated the effect of type of priming on originality,
yielding a point estimate of .029 for the indirect effect with a 95%
Our hypothesis specifically predicts differing scores for the confidence interval (CI) ranging from .006 to .061.
distrust condition compared with the other two conditions. There-
fore, we used the corresponding contrast to analyze the originality Discussion
scores as well as the other indices. Means and standard deviations
for all four measures are given in Table 3. We set up Study 2 to investigate the influence of distrust on
Main analysis: Originality. As expected, participants in the creative performance in a mostly nonpublic situation and with a
distrust condition achieved the highest originality scores out of different priming method. As expected, distrust priming yielded
the three priming conditions. The planned contrast comparing the increased originality, ideational fluency, and flexibility compared
distrust condition (M ⫽ 1.48, SD ⫽ 0.32) to the other two condi- with trust and neutral control conditions. No reliable differences
tions (combined M ⫽ 1.32, SD ⫽ 0.21) was statistically reliable, emerged for the persistence measure. Thus, Study 2 provides
t(54) ⫽ 2.21, p ⫽ .02, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.66.1 evidence for an actual creativity-enhancing effect of distrust under
Supplementary analyses: Ideational fluency, flexibility, and conditions of true privacy. To determine the overall reliability of
persistence. For ideational fluency, a pattern similar to the one this effect, we used a meta-analytic procedure (cf. Rosenthal,
observed in the originality measure emerged. Participants primed 1991) for the contrast comparing the distrust and trust conditions
with distrust produced more ideas (M ⫽ 8.81, SD ⫽ 2.66) com- in Studies 1 (private condition only) and 2. Results of these
pared with participants in the other two conditions (combined M ⫽
6.71, SD ⫽ 1.94), t(54) ⫽ 3.28, p ⫽ .001, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.99. In 1
line with the notion that cognitive flexibility may be critical for the All p values of reported t tests and meta-analytic procedures involving
hypothesized differences are one tailed. Although we had clear directional
influence of distrust on creativity, participants primed with distrust
predictions and many authorities recommend the use of one-tailed signif-
derived their ideas from more categories (M ⫽ 6.06, SD ⫽ 1.60) icance tests in such cases, this practice is controversial (e.g., Abelson,
than participants in the trust and control conditions (combined 1995). We adopted Abelson’s (1995) recommended compromise between
M ⫽ 4.91, SD ⫽ 1.39), t(54) ⫽ 2.66, p ⫽ .005, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.81. the two extreme positions in this debate, constructing a null-hypothesis
For the persistence measure, on a descriptive level, a pattern rejection region of 5% in the theoretically expected tail and 0.5% in the
similar to those for the other measures emerged (i.e., highest unexpected tail.
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1269

Table 3 of priming on flexibility or, conversely, flexibility mediating the


Means and Standard Deviations of Mean Originality of Ideas, effect of priming on fluency. Additional bootstrapping analyses
Number of Ideas (Fluency), Number of Categories (Flexibility), revealed a significant indirect effect for either of the two models:
and Within-Category Fluency (Persistence) as a Function of Fluency mediated the effect of priming on flexibility (point esti-
Type of Priming in Study 2 mate ⫽ .294 and 95% CI [.108, .517]), and flexibility mediated the
effect of priming on fluency (point estimate ⫽ .352 and 95% CI
Type of priming [.087, .666]). Therefore, the data presented so far do not unequiv-
Measure Distrust Control Trust
ocally demonstrate that a distrust mind-set does indeed come along
with an increase in cognitive flexibility.
Originality In the following two studies, we aim at exploring more directly
M 1.48a,† 1.36b,† 1.29b the potential of cognitive flexibility as the underlying mechanism
SD 0.32 0.24 0.18
of the creativity-enhancing effect of distrust demonstrated earlier.
Fluency
M 8.81a 6.76b 6.65b According to the dual pathway to creativity model (De Dreu et al.,
SD 2.66 1.79 2.13 2008), manifestations of the flexibility pathway include (a) the use
Flexibility of many different categories, (b) breadth and inclusiveness of the
M 6.06a 4.79b 5.05b categories themselves, and (c) flat associative hierarchies.
SD 1.60 1.10 1.65
Persistence Whereas Studies 1 and 2 provided evidence for category diversity,
M 1.49a 1.43a 1.37a Studies 3 and 4 used measures to investigate the other two facets
SD 0.38 0.31 0.33 of the flexibility pathway—inclusiveness of categories and flat
associative hierarchies.
Note. Within each row, means not labeled with the same superscript
differed according to the respective simple-contrast analysis at p ⬍ .05.
Means that are labeled with different superscripts and also share an Study 3
additional superscript (†) differed at p ⫽ .06.
In an attempt to corroborate our preliminary findings on cogni-
tive flexibility, we chose to use the category inclusion task (Isen &
analyses confirmed that, compared with trust priming, distrust Daubman, 1984; Rosch, 1975) that has been widely used in cre-
priming reliably increased originality (Z ⫽ 2.45, p ⫽ .007), flu- ativity research (e.g., Carnevale & Probst, 1998; De Dreu &
ency (Z ⫽ 2.54, p ⫽ .005), and flexibility (Z ⫽ 1.89, p ⫽ .03). Nijstad, 2008; Friedman & Förster, 2000; Rietzschel, De Dreu, &
Furthermore, the mediation analysis suggests flexibility as the Nijstad, 2007). This measure captures cognitive flexibility through
cognitive mechanism that may be responsible for the creativity- breadth and inclusiveness of categories, which, in turn, is seen as
enhancing effect of distrust. conducive to producing original ideas (Murray, Sujan, Hirt, &
The latter finding has to be taken with caution, however: Flex- Sujan, 1990). The category inclusion task asks participants to rate
ibility indices in idea-generation tasks are disputable because of the goodness of fit of several exemplars (e.g., car, wheelchair,
their possible contamination by fluency (e.g., Hocevar, 1979; camel) to a superordinate category (e.g., vehicle). Seeing highly
Plucker & Renzulli, 1999): The more ideas individuals produce, and moderately typical exemplars (e.g., car, wheelchair) as good
not only are they more likely to run out of trivial responses representatives of their category is not presumed to require cog-
(quantity breeds quality; e.g., Diehl & Stroebe, 1987; Osborn, nitive flexibility (Isen & Daubman, 1984). These exemplars
1963), but they are also more likely to change to a different clearly belong to the category. The extent to which atypical ex-
category to find further nonredundant solutions. At the same time, emplars (e.g., camel) are included in the category, however, re-
cognitive flexibility is thought of “as a precursor of the production quires looking at these exemplars in a different way than usual and,
of many (ideational fluency) and original responses” (De Dreu et therefore, presupposes cognitive flexibility. Following this logic,
al., 2008, p. 740). Following this reasoning, enhanced fluency cognitive flexibility is captured by goodness-of-fit ratings for
could be contaminated by the increased accessibility of many atypical exemplars.
different categories rather than vice versa. These two possibilities To manipulate trust and distrust, we used the subliminal priming
imply mediational models with either fluency mediating the effect procedure from Study 1 and added a neutral control condition. We

Figure 1. Flexibility mediates the effect of type of priming on originality (Study 2). Paths are labeled with
standardized regression coefficients. Simultaneous regression coefficients are given in parentheses.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
1270 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

expected participants primed with distrust to give higher typicality Results and Discussion
ratings to atypical exemplars than participants in the trust and
We hypothesized that distrust induces cognitive flexibility rep-
control conditions, thus exhibiting increased cognitive flexibility. resented by increased category inclusiveness. We therefore ex-
pected (a) participants in the distrust condition to rate the weak
Methods exemplars as more typical than participants in the other two
conditions and (b) ratings for the remaining exemplars (high
Design and participants. Study 3 is based on a 3 (type of typicality index) not to differ for the distrust condition compared
priming: distrust vs. control vs. trust) ⫻ 2 (typicality: high vs. low) with the other two conditions. To test these predictions, we sub-
mixed model design, with the last factor being manipulated within jected the typicality ratings to a 3 (type of priming: distrust vs.
participants. We recruited 56 students (40 female, 16 male) as control vs. trust) ⫻ 2 (typicality: high vs. low) repeated measures
participants and randomly assigned them to one of the three ANOVA with typicality as a within-subjects factor. To test our
priming conditions. Participants were approached at a campus of specific hypothesis that participants primed with distrust showed
the University of Cologne and offered a chocolate bar as compen- increased category inclusiveness (i.e., higher typicality ratings for
sation for participation in two short experimental tasks. the weak exemplars compared with the other two conditions) we
Materials and procedure. On arrival in the lab, participants calculated the respective planned contrast, as well.
were led to separate cubicles and asked to sit down in front of a Confirming the nature of the typicality indices, a significant
60-Hz computer monitor. After giving their consent, participants main effect emerged for typicality, F(1, 53) ⫽ 1,065.67, p ⬍ .001:
read that the study was about categorization and that they would Independent of type of priming, participants rated high-typicality
work on two different categorization tasks consecutively. The first exemplars (combined M ⫽ 7.56, SD ⫽ 1.00) as more typical than
task was labeled as a task requiring categorization of whether or low-typicality exemplars (combined M ⫽ 2.83, SD ⫽ 1.27). The
not a letter string was a proper German word. The task consisted main effect for type of priming was not significant, F(2, 53) ⫽
1.98, p ⫽ .15. In line with our hypotheses, we obtained a signif-
of the subliminal priming procedure described in Study 1, com-
icant interaction effect, F(2, 53) ⫽ 3.36, p ⫽ .04, ␩2p ⫽ .11,
plemented by a control condition.2 In the control condition, the
indicating that high- and low-typicality indices were differentially
fixation string (XWXXWWXXWX) was substituted for the prime
affected by the priming conditions (see Figure 2).
word.
For high-typicality exemplars, ratings in the distrust condition
After finishing the priming task, participants proceeded to the
(M ⫽ 7.57, SD ⫽ 1.11) did not differ significantly from those in
category inclusion task (adapted from Friedman & Förster, 2000;
the control condition (M ⫽ 7.37, SD ⫽ 1.12), in the trust condition
cf. Isen & Daubman, 1984; Rosch, 1975). In this task, four (M ⫽ 7.71, SD ⫽ 0.79), or in the control and trust conditions
category names (Fortbewegungsmittel [vehicle], Möbel [furni- combined (M ⫽ 7.56, SD ⫽ 0.96), all |t|s ⬍ 1. As expected,
ture], Gemüse [vegetable], Kleidung [clothing]) were presented however, for exemplars low in typicality, participants primed with
followed by 12 exemplars of each. Categories were presented in distrust exhibited higher ratings (M ⫽ 3.35, SD ⫽ 1.41) than
random order. The 12 exemplars within each category were pre- participants in the control and trust conditions (combined M ⫽
sented one at a time in a fixed order and differed in typicality from 2.56, SD ⫽ 1.13), t(53) ⫽ 2.37, p ⫽ .01, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.66. Simple
high (e.g., car for the vehicle category) through moderate (wheel- effect analyses revealed that ratings in the distrust condition tended
chair) to low (camel). Participants indicated for each exemplar to be higher than those in the trust condition (M ⫽ 2.80, SD ⫽
how typical it was for the named category on a 10-point scale 1.07), t(53) ⫽ 1.41, p ⫽ .08, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.45, and were
ranging from 0 (atypical) to 9 (typical). significantly increased compared with ratings in the control con-
A final questionnaire asked for demographic data and tested for dition (M ⫽ 2.27, SD ⫽ 1.16), t(53) ⫽ 2.65, p ⫽ .005, Cohen’s
awareness of the true purpose of the study as well as for awareness d ⫽ 0.86.
of the prime word in the priming procedure. None of the partici- In accordance with prior interpretations of the category inclu-
pants indicated awareness of the true purpose of the study or of the sion task, these results support the idea of increased cognitive
prime word. At the end, participants were thanked and handed flexibility under distrust. One may, however, still argue that in-
their compensation. creased category inclusiveness of exemplars low in typicality is
Measures and dependent variables. Typicality ratings in brought on by increased persistence (De Dreu et al., 2008). By
the category inclusion task constituted the critical dependent mea- persevering and systematically working through possibilities, par-
sure. In a first step, we defined the typicality of each exemplar with ticipants may end up concluding that weak exemplars do not fit the
the help of the data provided by participants in the control group. category so badly after all. If perseverance and careful systematic
To that end, we subjected each exemplar to a t test probing for processing were responsible for increased category inclusiveness
deviation from its respective category mean. Exemplars that in the distrust condition, this should be accompanied by increased
either significantly exceeded their category mean (strong ex- response latencies (cf. De Dreu et al., 2008). To test for this
emplars) or did not significantly differ from their category possibility, we averaged reaction times for exemplars high in
mean (moderate exemplars) were averaged across the four typicality and for exemplars low in typicality, respectively. Reac-
tion times were subjected to the same ANOVA as the typicality
categories and formed the high typicality index (Cronbach’s
␣ ⫽ .90). Exemplars with means significantly lower than their
respective category mean were categorized as weak exemplars 2
Because of the different monitor refresh rate, prime word presen-
and averaged across the four categories to form the low typi- tation was 16 ms. Presentation time was, therefore, slightly longer than
cality index (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .80). in Study 1.
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1271

are markedly more similar in their associative strength to the


stimulus. If associative hierarchies are flat rather than steep, re-
mote associations should come to mind more easily.
To measure associative hierarchies, we used materials from a
direct/indirect priming procedure developed by Kiefer, Weisbrod,
Kern, Maier, and Spitzer (1998). In this task, participants react to
target words that are preceded by a prime word. The semantic
association of the prime–target pairs varies from proximal to
remote to unrelated. Typically, that is, under neutral conditions,
individuals show faster reactions in proximal trials than in remote
trials (e.g., Kiefer et al., 1998), a pattern consistent with steep
associative hierarchies. Increased remote semantic spread is re-
flected in relatively faster reactions to remotely associated prime–
target pairs. If reactions to proximally related trials are not af-
fected, this would further imply a diminished discrepancy in
response latencies to proximally and remotely related trials. The
latter is conceptually consistent with flat associative hierarchies
and, in turn, with increased cognitive flexibility. Building on the
data presented so far, we expected participants primed with distrust
to exhibit a pattern consistent with flat associative hierarchies, that
is, a pattern of increased remote semantic spread. As the results of
both Studies 2 and 3 were largely in line with the notion that trust
Figure 2. Mean typicality ratings as a function of type of priming
and control conditions produce similar results, we chose to contrast
condition (between factor) and typicality of exemplars (within factor) in
Study 3. Error bars represent one standard error.
the distrust condition with a trust condition only. Consistent with
the conceptualization of trust as the default (cf. Berg et al., 1995;
McKnight et al., 1998), we expected to find the typical pattern of
ratings. This analysis revealed only a significant main effect for prolonged response latencies in remote trials in the trust condition.
typicality, F(1, 53) ⫽ 14.09, p ⬍ .001, indicating that, independent For reasons of compatibility with the direct/indirect priming pro-
of type of priming, participants took longer to rate exemplars low cedure, we used a slightly modified version of our subliminal
in typicality than they took to rate exemplars high in typicality (all trust/distrust priming (cf. Studies 1 and 3).
other Fs ⬍ 1.27, all other ps ⬎ .29). Inspection of the means
further revealed that, descriptively, mean response latencies in the Methods
distrust condition were lowest among the three priming conditions
for both high-typicality exemplars and low-typicality exemplars. Design and participants. Study 4 is based on a 2 (type of
Therefore, it seems unlikely that persistence was responsible for priming: distrust vs. trust) ⫻ 2 (semantic distance: proximal vs.
increased category inclusion in the distrust condition. remote) mixed design, with the last factor manipulated within
In conclusion, we consider the results of Study 3 as evidence for participants. We recruited 48 individuals on a University of Co-
increased category inclusiveness and therefore as evidence for logne campus as participants and randomly assigned them to one
increased cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-set. A meta- of the two priming conditions. Participants were offered 5€ (ap-
analytical procedure (cf. Rosenthal, 1991) further strengthens this proximately $6.50 at the time) as compensation. One participant
contention by attesting a reliable increase in measures of cognitive was excluded because of data collection irregularities, leaving a
flexibility in the distrust condition compared with the control final sample of 47 participants (36 female, 11 male).
condition across Studies 2 and 3 (Z ⫽ 3.62, p ⬍ .001). Study 4 was Materials and procedure. On arrival in the lab, participants
designed to test whether these results can be conceptually repli- were led to separate cubicles and asked to sit down in front of an
cated when looking at the third indicator of cognitive flexibility, 85-Hz computer monitor. After giving their consent, participants
namely, flat associative hierarchies. worked on a lexical decision task. This task combined our sub-
liminal priming procedure with a direct/indirect priming paradigm.
The material for the direct/indirect priming paradigm (Kiefer et al.,
Study 4
1998) consisted of a total of 270 word pairs. Each pair consisted of
Flat associative hierarchies are considered another manifestation a prime word and a target letter string. Primes were all proper
of the flexibility pathway to creativity (De Dreu et al., 2008). German words and so were half of the target letter strings. The
Associative hierarchies differ with regard to their patterns of the remaining target letter strings constituted meaningless yet pro-
associative strength of close and remote associations (Mednick, nounceable nonwords. Pairs with proper German words as targets
1962). In the case of steep associative hierarchies, close associates differed with respect to the semantic relatedness of prime and
(e.g., chair) to a given stimulus (e.g., table) come to mind much target: proximal (e.g., König–Krone [king– crown]), remote (e.g.,
more easily than relatively remote ones (e.g., leg or food; example Bier–Traube [beer– grape]), and unrelated (e.g., Bleistift–Uhr
from Mednick, 1962, p. 223). Put differently, there is a steep [pencil– clock]), with each category being represented by 45 pairs.
decline in associative strength from close to remote associates. In Participants worked through 132 trials drawn randomly from the
the case of flat associative hierarchies, close and remote associates pool of 270 prime–target pairs. This procedure yields, on average,
1272 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

22 trials per category of semantic distance (proximal, remote,


unrelated). Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation
string (XQFBZRMQWGBX). After 494 ms, the fixation string was
briefly (12 ms, subliminal) replaced, by either the word misstrauen
(in the distrust condition) or the word vertrauen (in the trust
condition). The same fixation string was then presented again for
another 494 ms. Next, the prime word was presented for 153 ms
(supraliminal). This prime word was replaced by a blank screen
(94 ms) that was, in turn, followed by the target letter string. The
target remained on the screen until participants indicated whether
or not it constituted a proper German word by pressing the appro-
priate key (for procedures, see Kiefer et al., 1998; Topolinski &
Deutsch, 2009).
A final questionnaire asked for demographic data and tested for
awareness of the true purpose of the study as well as for awareness
of the prime word in the subliminal priming procedure. None of
the participants indicated awareness of the true purpose of the
study or of the prime word. At the end, participants were thanked
and handed their compensation.
Measures and dependent variables. For our purposes, only
trials with real German words as targets were of interest (cf. Kiefer
Figure 3. Mean reaction times as a function of type of priming and
et al., 1998; Topolinski & Deutsch, 2009). From these trials, in a semantic distance in Study 4. Error bars represent one standard error.
first step, incorrect responses (1.82% of the data) and response
latencies exceeding 2,000 ms (0.46% of the data) were dropped.
Additionally, response latencies longer than twice the individual Subjecting the corresponding error rates to the same ANOVA did
mean per trial category (proximal, remote, unrelated) were not not yield any significant effects (all Fs ⬍ 2.3, all ps ⬎ .13).
considered (1.01% of the data; cf. Kiefer et al., 1998). The re- These results shed additional light on the cognitive mechanisms
maining response latencies were averaged per trial category. that underlie the creativity-enhancing effect of distrust: Distrust-
primed individuals exhibited increased remote semantic spread
Results and Discussion and thereby an activation pattern in accordance with flat associa-
tive hierarchies. Flat associative hierarchies, in turn, are considered
Responses to proximally and remotely associated prime–target a manifestation of the flexibility pathway to creativity (De Dreu et
pairs constitute our main variable of interest; responses to unre- al., 2008). We, therefore, interpret this finding as yet another
lated prime–target pairs served as a baseline. For baseline trials, indicator of increased cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-set.
mean response latencies in the distrust condition (M ⫽ 638 ms, A meta-analytic procedure (Rosenthal, 1991) corroborates the
SD ⫽ 141 ms) and in the trust condition (M ⫽ 657 ms, SD ⫽ 153 increase in cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-set: Across
ms) did not differ significantly and neither did the error rates, all Studies 1 (private condition only) through 4, distrust priming
Fs ⬍ 1. yielded reliably increased flexibility indices compared with trust
In the main analysis, we subjected mean response latencies to priming (Z ⫽ 2.63, p ⫽ .004). A parallel analysis comparing the
proximally and remotely related prime–target pairs to a 2 (type of distrust condition to both control conditions (i.e., to the trust
priming: distrust vs. trust) ⫻ 2 (semantic distance: proximal vs. conditions in Studies 1 and 4; to the combined trust and control
remote) repeated measures ANOVA with semantic distance as a conditions in Studies 2 and 3) also reveals significantly increased
within-subjects factor. Inspection of the means in Figure 3 reveals cognitive flexibility after distrust priming (Z ⫽ 2.89, p ⫽ .002).
a pattern of means consistent with our predictions. In the trust
condition, the typical pattern indicating steep associative hierar-
General Discussion
chies emerged: Response latencies for remotely related trials (M ⫽
610 ms, SD ⫽ 131 ms) were prolonged compared with proximally At first sight, it might seem plausible to assume that distrust
related trials (M ⫽ 574 ms, SD ⫽ 118 ms), F(1, 45) ⫽ 8.93, p ⫽ thwarts creativity—an intuition that is also consistent with empir-
.005, ␩2p ⫽ .17, for the simple effect. In line with our predictions, ical evidence so far. Contained, however, in recent findings on the
participants primed with distrust, however, did not show the typ- cognitive concomitants of distrust (Schul et al., 1996, 2004, 2008)
ical pattern of prolonged response latencies in remotely related is the provocative implication that distrust may foster rather than
trials. Rather, they were as fast in reacting to remotely related hinder creativity. To solve the thereby emerging contradiction, we
prime–target pairs (M ⫽ 570 ms, SD ⫽ 92 ms) as to proximally proposed that whether creative thoughts remain private or are
related ones (M ⫽ 574 ms, SD ⫽ 126 ms), F ⬍ 1, ns, for the simple made public constitutes a critical moderating variable: In public,
effect, indicating flat associative hierarchies. This pattern of means the social consequences of distrust are likely to prove detrimental
was borne out in a statistically reliable two-way interaction, F(1, to creativity. In private, however, these social consequences are
45) ⫽ 5.71, p ⫽ .02, ␩2p ⫽ .11. In addition, a trend for a main effect incapacitated, so that the beneficial cognitive consequences of
for semantic distance emerged, F(1, 45) ⫽ 3.53, p ⫽ .07, ␩2p ⫽ .07. distrust may develop their creativity-enhancing potential. In line
The main effect for type of priming was not significant, F ⬍ 1. with this reasoning, our results replicated earlier findings of a
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1273

detrimental influence of distrust on creativity when ideas gener- interest in creativity research. Respective differences are therefore
ated were allegedly made public to another participant. When ideas often discussed as alternative explanations in creativity research.
remained private, however, inducing a distrust mind-set proved With regard to hedonic tone and arousal, it is important to keep
beneficial to creative performance. The present findings also shed in mind that our manipulations were subtle and specifically de-
light on the cognitive mechanism underlying this creativity- signed not to produce substantial differences in hedonic tone and
enhancing effect of distrust. Our studies provided sound evidence arousal. Consistently, additional tests of our priming procedures
that a distrust mind-set is accompanied by increased cognitive revealed no significant differences between the distrust condition,
flexibility in terms of category diversity, category inclusiveness, the trust condition, and the neutral condition in explicit measures
and flat associative hierarchies. Cognitive flexibility also mediated of mood and arousal.3 Second, even if our priming procedures had
the increase in originality after distrust priming under conditions of changed affective states to a certain degree not captured by explicit
true privacy. The counterintuitive finding of enhanced creativity in measures, the data are still unlikely to be the result of these
a distrust mind-set, its specific association with cognitive flexibil- variations alone. If anything, affective qualities relevant to distrust
ity, and the moderating influence of public versus private contexts yield creativity by increased persistence, not by increased cogni-
have important implications for research on both creativity and tive flexibility (Baas et al., 2008; De Dreu et al., 2008). However,
distrust. Before discussing these implications in detail, together our data reveal increased cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-
with avenues for future research, we first address the question of set, whereas measures of persistence remained unaffected.
the broader applicability of our findings as well as potential With regard to the qualities of distrust in terms of action orien-
alternative explanations. tations, avoidance orientations have mostly been shown to be
negatively related to creativity (e.g., Friedman & Förster, 2000).
Applicability of the Presented Findings and Notably, however, we demonstrate an increase in creativity after
Alternative Explanations distrust priming. Recent research further suggests that the specific
combination of hedonic tone, arousal, and action orientation of a
The methods used to elicit distrust in our studies may seem far particular state plays a critical role for creativity (Baas et al., 2008;
from how one usually pictures the natural emergence of distrust. Baas, De Dreu, & Nijstad, 2011). For other states that share the
Yet, we propose that our priming manipulations closely mirror negative, activating, avoidance-related affective properties of dis-
how distrust emerges in a wide array of real world settings. trust (e.g., anxiety), evidence is somewhat mixed with regard to
Specifically, whenever there is no prior information available their overall influence on creativity and yet quite consistent with
about the trustworthiness of a person or a source of information, regard to their negative influence on cognitive flexibility (Baas et
subtle cues may easily trigger a distrust mind-set. This follows al., 2008, 2011). However, as mentioned before, our data show
from the nature of deceptive attempts, in which deceivers often go enhanced cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-set.
to great lengths to hide their true motives. Therefore, we can In sum, it seems unlikely that our findings are the result of
expect deceptive intentions to show up primarily in unintentionally differences in affective states, approach versus avoidance orienta-
disclosed cues that are easily overlooked and often ambiguous tions, or a combination of both.
with respect to their informational value (Anderson, DePaulo,
Ansfield, Tickle, & Green, 1999; DePaulo et al., 2003). The
Theoretical Implications and Avenues for Future
activation of a distrust mind-set triggered by such subtle cues—
and not necessarily accompanied by conscious awareness—applies
Research
to many real world settings. Our findings extend previous research on distrust and creativity
As has been shown for distrust (Friesen & Sinclair, 2011; in several important ways. First, our work contributes to the
Rotenberg et al., 2010; Schul et al., 1996, 2004, 2008) but also for research on factors that foster or hinder creativity. Creativity is
many other variables (e.g., Corcoran, Hundhammer, & Muss- predominantly perceived as a positive and desirable quality that
weiler, 2009; De Dreu & Nijstad, 2008; Markman et al., 2007), produces positive and socially desirable outcomes (Mumford &
once elicited, the respective mind-set can linger and influence Gustafson, 1988). By showing that a negatively valued state like
behavior unrelated to the eliciting circumstances. It has further distrust has the potential to foster creativity, our research contrib-
been shown that mind-sets can reliably be instantiated by primes utes to a more complex understanding of creativity. Our findings
semantically related to the targeted concept (e.g., Damisch, Sto- therefore add to the growing body of research investigating the
berock, & Mussweiler, 2010; Galinsky et al., 2008; Mussweiler &
Förster, 2000). Taking these considerations into account, we feel
3
confident that the effects observed in our experimental setting are We recruited 47 people at a University of Cologne campus. They
applicable to a wide array of naturally occurring instances. worked on the subliminal priming procedure (cf. Study 3) and afterward
Apart from the question of the ecological validity of our meth- answered three questions assessing hedonic tone and three questions as-
ods to elicit a distrust mind-set, one might consider qualities of the sessing arousal. Separate three-level ANOVAs for the hedonic tone and
mind-set other than the targeted cognitive ones as influential to our arousal scales revealed no significant differences between the three con-
ditions (distrust, control, trust), all Fs(2, 46) ⬍ 1.5, all ps ⬎ .23. A different
results. As mentioned earlier, a distrust mind-set is being associ-
(online) sample of 43 participants first worked on the scrambled sentences
ated with negative, activating, avoidance-oriented qualities (cf. task (cf. Study 2) and afterward filled in a German questionnaire assessing
Mussweiler & Burgmer, 2011; Schul et al., 2008). Hedonic tone mood, alertness, and calmness (Steyer, Schwenkmezger, Notz, & Eid,
and activation (cf. Baas, De Dreu, & Nijstad, 2008), as well as 1997). Separate three-level ANOVAs for all three scales again revealed no
action orientation in terms of approach versus avoidance (e.g., significant differences between the three conditions (distrust, control,
Friedman & Förster, 2000), in turn, have been of prominent trust), all Fs ⬍ 1 (cf. Mussweiler & Burgmer, 2011, for the latter data set).
1274 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

creativity-enhancing potential of unpleasant or threatening states regard to the specific requirements in situations of impending
(e.g., Baas et al., 2008; De Dreu et al., 2008; De Dreu & Nijstad, deceit, on the other hand, cognitive flexibility seems highly func-
2008). tional and, therefore, quite likely to have evolved as an integral
More specifically, we identified distrust as a literally two-faced part of the distrust mind-set: Because the deceiver will carefully
predictor with the potential of both fostering and hindering cre- hide his or her true motives, the targeted victim is unlikely to
ative performance. Thereby, our research makes a significant con- quickly and unambiguously figure out whether ulterior motives
tribution to creativity research beyond earlier research on, for exist and, if so, what they might be. Consequently, it should be
example, counterfactual thinking (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; more adaptive not to commit to a particular alternative and explore
Kray, Galinsky, & Wong, 2006; Markman et al., 2007): Although it in depth early on, because this particular alternative might be
both the distrust and counterfactual mind-sets entail a basic ten- wrong. Considering many and quite different perspectives, in
dency to flexibly consider alternatives, they do not share the contrast, increases the likelihood of eventually figuring out the
differential effects of fostering creative performance in private carefully hidden truth. Only when clear-cut evidence for a partic-
while diminishing it in public. These conditional effects are char- ular alternative is presented or, alternatively, when several ambig-
acteristic of a distrust mind-set with its diverging cognitive versus uous pieces of evidence add up to implicate a particular alternative
social consequences. Other than in a distrust mind-set, a counter- does it seem safe to pursue this idea in more depth. The emergence
factual mind-set has been shown to foster rather than hinder the of increased cognitive flexibility in a distrust mind-set— despite
sharing of unique information (Galinsky & Kray, 2004). the contradicting effects of the accompanying affective–
Future research will have to clarify whether the diverging ef- motivational qualities—thus seems easily attributable to the func-
fects on creativity that we observed for distrust may also apply to tional aspect of protecting against impending deceit. The idea that
other trust/distrust-related variables. Extrapolating from our re- functional aspects of a particular state may predominate other
search, it may well be the case that some of the positive effects tendencies associated with a particular state has, for example, been
observed for trust-related variables (e.g., Bechtoldt et al., 2010; shown in the research on social conflict and creativity (De Dreu &
Carmeli et al., 2010) are restricted to group settings, that is, to Nijstad, 2008): Even though conflict has traditionally been related
creative performance in public. It may well be that the presence of to threat rigidity and low levels of cognitive flexibility (Carnevale
others provides the activating component (e.g., Mullen, Bryant, & & Probst, 1998), cognitive flexibility can still be fueled if it proves
Driskell, 1997) that has been shown to be critical for fueling functional for handling the conflict at hand (De Dreu & Nijstad,
cognitive flexibility and creativity in positive affective states (De 2008).
Dreu et al., 2008). Our data would be largely consistent with this In much the same way, distrust may—through the mechanism of
contention: Within the trust condition of Study 1, we observed cognitive flexibility— have beneficial effects. Suspicious spirits
increased creativity (i.e., originality) in the public context com- have flexible and—as a consequence— creative minds.
pared with the private context (see Table 1).
Although the creativity-enhancing effects of trust-related vari-
ables may at times be restricted to public settings, the complete References
opposite seems likely for variables that share distrust’s context
Abelson, R. P. (1995). Statistics as principled argument. Hillsdale, NJ:
sensitivity. Creative performance in situations associated with Erlbaum.
threat coming from the social environment, more generally, may Amabile, T. M., & Gryskiewicz, N. D. (1989). The Creative Environment
be observed exclusively in private. Future research may look into Scales: Work Environment Inventory. Creativity Research Journal, 2,
whether, for example, findings of targeted creativity in a conflict 231–253. doi:10.1080/10400418909534321
mind-set (De Dreu & Nijstad, 2008) disappear if the creativity task Anderson, D. E., DePaulo, B. M., Ansfield, M. E., Tickle, J. J., & Green,
involves public sharing of ideas. Considering a potentially mod- E. (1999). Beliefs about cues to deception: Mindless stereotypes or
erating influence of whether creative performance is carried out untapped wisdom? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 67– 89. doi:
publicly or privately may also be helpful in investigating so-called 10.1023/A:1021387326192
malevolent creativity (Cropley, Kaufman, & Cropley, 2008), that Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of
25 years of mood– creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or
is, creativity that yields creative but socially undesirable end
regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779 – 806. doi:10.1037/
products. Even though we usually do not think of lying (Walczyk a0012815
et al., 2008) or terrorist attacks (cf. Cropley et al., 2008) in terms Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2011). When prevention
of creativity, they often meet the criteria by being novel and promotes creativity: The role of mood, regulatory focus, and regulatory
appropriate means to a specific end. Because instances of malev- closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 794 – 809.
olent creativity are subject to social disapproval, creativity in this doi:10.1037/a0022981
realm is likely to show up only in private. Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A
Second, our research adds to the understanding of the cognitive practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis &
consequences of distrust. We show that distrust is accompanied not C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and per-
only by increased creativity but also by cognitive flexibility man- sonality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York, NY: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
ifested in category diversity, category inclusiveness, and remote
Bechtoldt, M. N., De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., & Choi, H.-S. (2010).
semantic spread. This is of considerable significance, because the Motivated information processing, social tuning, and group creativity.
affective–motivational qualities of distrust have quite consistently Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 622– 637. doi:
been associated with a decrease rather than an increase in broad 10.1037/a0019386
and flexible processing (Baas et al., 2008, 2011; De Dreu et al., Beersma, B., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2005). Conflict’s consequences:
2008; Friedman & Förster, 2000; Isen & Daubman, 1984). With Effects of social motives on postnegotiation creative and convergent
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1275

group functioning and performance. Journal of Personality and Social nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 531–544. doi:10.1006/
Psychology, 89, 358 –374. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.358 jesp.2000.1427
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social Edmondson, A. C. (2004). Psychological safety, trust, and learning in
history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142. doi:10.1006/ organizations: A group-level lens. In R. M. Kramer & K. S. Cook (Eds.),
game.1995.1027 Trust and distrust in organizations: Dilemmas and approaches (pp.
Camacho, L. M., & Paulus, P. B. (1995). The role of social anxiousness in 239 –272). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
group brainstorming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, Ekvall, G. (1996). Organizational climate for creativity and innovation.
1071–1080. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.6.1071 European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 5, 105–123.
Carmeli, A., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Ziv, E. (2010). Inclusive leadership and doi:10.1080/13594329608414845
employee involvement in creative tasks in the workplace: The mediating Ekvall, G., & Ryhammar, L. (1999). The creative climate: Its determinants
role of psychological safety. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 250 –260. and effects at a Swedish university. Creativity Research Journal, 12,
doi:10.1080/10400419.2010.504654 303–310. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1204_8
Carnevale, P. J., & Probst, T. M. (1998). Social values and social conflict Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual
in creative problem solving and categorization. Journal of Personality thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 168 –192.
and Social Psychology, 74, 1300 –1309. doi:10.1037/0022- doi:10.1177/1088868308316091
3514.74.5.1300 Fein, S., Hilton, J. L., & Miller, D. T. (1990). Suspicion of ulterior
Chiappe, D., Brown, A., Dow, B., Koontz, J., Rodriguez, M., & Mc- motivation and the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality and
Culloch, K. (2004). Cheaters are looked at longer and remembered better Social Psychology, 58, 753–764. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.753
than cooperators in social exchange situations. Evolutionary Psychology, Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2009). Do people trust too much or too
2, 108 –120. Retrieved from http://www.epjournal.net/filestore/ little? Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 263–276. doi:10.1016/
ep021081202.pdf j.joep.2008.04.006
Corcoran, K., Hundhammer, T., & Mussweiler, T. (2009). A tool for Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2000). The effects of approach and avoid-
thought! When comparative thinking reduces stereotyping effects. Jour- ance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1008 –1011. doi:10.1016/ Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477– 492. doi:10.1037/0022-
j.jesp.2009.04.015 3514.79.4.477
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2005). Neurocognitive adaptations designed for
Friesen, J., & Sinclair, L. (2011). Distrust and simultaneous activation of
social exchange. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary
multiple categories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2,
psychology (pp. 584 – 627). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
112–118. doi:10.1177/1948550610382666
Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent
Galinsky, A. D., & Kray, L. J. (2004). From thinking about what might
creativity: A functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime.
have been to sharing what we know: The effects of counterfactual
Creativity Research Journal, 20, 105–115. doi:10.1080/
mind-sets on information sharing in groups. Journal of Experimental
10400410802059424
Social Psychology, 40, 606 – 618. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.005
Dakhli, M., & De Clercq, D. (2004). Human capital, social capital, and
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Whitson, J. A. (2008).
innovation: A multi-country study. Entrepreneurship & Regional De-
Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity,
velopment, 16, 107–128. doi:10.1080/08985620410001677835
conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Damisch, L., Stoberock, B., & Mussweiler, T. (2010). Keep your fingers
ogy, 95, 1450 –1466. doi:10.1037/a0012633
crossed! How superstition improves performance. Psychological Sci-
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Counterfactuals as behavioral
ence, 21, 1014 –1020. doi:10.1177/0956797610372631
primes: Priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alterna-
De Cremer, D. (1999). Trust and fear of exploitation in a public goods
dilemma. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on tives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 384 – 409. doi:
Diverse Psychological Issues, 18, 153–163. doi:10.1007/s12144-999- 10.1006/jesp.1999.1409
1024-0 George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2007). Dual tuning in a supportive context: Joint
De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and contributions of positive mood, negative mood, and supervisory behav-
activation level in the mood– creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to iors to employee creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 605–
creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 622. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.25525934
739 –756. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739 Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psycholo-
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Mental set and creative gist, 46, 107–119. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.46.2.107
thought in social conflict: Threat rigidity versus motivated focus. Jour- Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Diver-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 648 – 661. doi:10.1037/ gent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel
0022-3514.95.3.648 uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611– 625.
DePaulo, B. M., & Kashy, D. A. (1998). Everyday lies in close and casual doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 63–79. Glover, J. A. (1977). Risky shift and creativity. Social Behavior and
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.63 Personality, 5, 317–320. doi:10.2224/sbp.1977.5.2.317
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY:
J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social McGraw-Hill.
Psychology, 70, 979 –995. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.979 Hennessey, B. A. (2003). The social psychology of creativity. Scandina-
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, vian Journal of Educational Research, 47, 253–271. doi:10.1080/
K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 00313830308601
129, 74 –118. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74 Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of
Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming Psychology, 61, 569 –598. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100416
groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Hilton, J. L., Fein, S., & Miller, D. T. (1993). Suspicion and dispositional
Social Psychology, 53, 497–509. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.497 inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 501–512.
Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., Bargh, J. A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). On doi:10.1177/0146167293195003
the relation between associative strength and automatic behavior. Jour- Hocevar, D. (1979). Ideational fluency as a confounding factor in the
1276 MAYER AND MUSSWEILER

measurement of originality. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, Posten, A.-C., & Mussweiler, T. (2011). Priming trust. Manuscript in
191–196. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.71.2.191 preparation.
Hunter, S. T., Bedell, K. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2007). Climate for Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for
creativity: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 69 – estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Re-
90. doi:10.1080/10400410709336883 search Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. doi:10.3758/
Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on BF03206553
categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206 – Rietzschel, E. F., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2007). Personal
1217. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1206 need for structure and creative performance: The moderating influence
Kiefer, M., Weisbrod, M., Kern, I., Maier, S., & Spitzer, M. (1998). Right of fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
hemisphere activation during indirect semantic priming: Evidence from 855– 866. doi:10.1177/0146167207301017
event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 64, 377– 408. doi: Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Relative accessi-
10.1006/brln.1998.1979 bility of domain knowledge and creativity: The effects of knowledge
Klimoski, R. J., & Karol, B. L. (1976). The impact of trust on creative activation on the quantity and originality of generated ideas. Journal of
problem solving groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 630 – 633. Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 933–946. doi:10.1016/j
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.61.5.630 .jesp.2006.10.014
Kramer, R. M. (1998). Paranoid cognition in social systems: Thinking and Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Jour-
acting in the shadow of doubt. Personality and Social Psychology nal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–233. doi:10.1037/
Review, 2, 251–275. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_3 0096-3445.104.3.192
Kray, L. J., Galinsky, A. D., & Wong, E. M. (2006). Thinking within the Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. New-
box: The relational processing style elicited by counterfactual mind-sets. bury Park, CA: Sage.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 33– 48. doi:10.1037/ Rotenberg, K. J., Addis, N., Betts, L. R., Corrigan, A., Fox, C., Hobson, Z.,
0022-3514.91.1.33 & Boulton, M. J. (2010). The relation between trust beliefs and loneli-
Marchand, M. A. G., & Vonk, R. (2005). The process of becoming ness during early childhood, middle childhood, and adulthood. Person-
suspicious of ulterior motives. Social Cognition, 23, 242–256. doi: ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1086 –1100. doi:10.1177/
10.1521/soco.2005.23.3.242 0146167210374957
Markman, K. D., Lindberg, M. J., Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2007). Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., Adams, R. B., Ozono, H., Nakashima, S.,
Implications of counterfactual structure for creative generation and Yoshikawa, S., & Watabe, M. (2010). Polling the face: Prediction and
analytical problem solving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, consensus across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
33, 312–324. doi:10.1177/0146167206296106 ogy, 98, 1–15. doi:10.1037/a0017673
McKnight, D. H., Cummings, L. L., & Chervany, N. L. (1998). Initial trust Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity: Theories and themes: Research, devel-
formation in new organizational relationships. Academy of Management opment, and practice. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Review, 23, 473– 490. doi:10.2307/259290 Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (2007). An integrative
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. model of organizational trust: Past, present, and future. Academy of
Psychological Review, 69, 220 –232. doi:10.1037/h0048850 Management Review, 32, 344 –354. doi:10.5465/AMR.2007.24348410
Mullen, B., Bryant, B., & Driskell, J. E. (1997). Presence of others and Schoppe, K.-J. (1975). Verbaler Kreativitäts-Test (V-K-T). Ein Verfahren
arousal: An integration. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Prac- zur Erfassung verbal-produktiver Kreativitätsmerkmale [The verbal cre-
tice, 1, 52– 64. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.1.1.52 ativity test. A procedure for measuring verbal-productive aspects of
Mumford, M. D., & Gustafson, S. B. (1988). Creativity syndrome: Inte- creativity]. Göttingen, Germany: Verlag für Psychologie.
gration, application, and innovation. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 27– 43. Schul, Y. (1993). When warning succeeds: The effect of warning on
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.1.27 success in ignoring invalid information. Journal of Experimental Social
Murray, N., Sujan, H., Hirt, E. R., & Sujan, M. (1990). The influence of Psychology, 29, 42– 62. doi:10.1006/jesp.1993.1003
mood on categorization: A cognitive flexibility interpretation. Journal of Schul, Y., Burnstein, E., & Bardi, A. (1996). Dealing with deceptions that
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 411– 425. doi:10.1037/0022- are difficult to detect: Encoding and judgment as a function of preparing
3514.59.3.411 to receive invalid information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Mussweiler, T., & Burgmer, P. (2011). Effects of trust and distrust on ogy, 32, 228 –253. doi:10.1006/jesp.1996.0011
approach and avoidance tendencies. Manuscript in preparation. Schul, Y., Mayo, R., & Burnstein, E. (2004). Encoding under trust and
Mussweiler, T., & Förster, J. (2000). The sex 3 aggression link: A distrust: The spontaneous activation of incongruent cognitions. Journal
perception– behavior dissociation. Journal of Personality and Social of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 668 – 679. doi:10.1037/0022-
Psychology, 79, 507–520. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.507 3514.86.5.668
Nemeth, C. J., & Ormiston, M. (2007). Creative idea generation: Harmony Schul, Y., Mayo, R., & Burnstein, E. (2008). The value of distrust. Journal
versus stimulation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 524 – of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1293–1302. doi:10.1016/
535. doi:10.1002/ejsp.373 j.jesp.2008.05.003
Nijstad, B. A., Stroebe, W., & Lodewijkx, H. F. M. (2002). Cognitive Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: A predictive and explana-
stimulation and interference in groups: Exposure effects in an idea tory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychological Review,
generation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 535– 104, 66 – 89. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.66
544. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00500-0 Simpson, J. A. (2007). Foundations of interpersonal trust. In A. W. Krug-
Osborn, A. F. (1963). Applied imagination: Principles and procedures of lanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic
creative thinking. New York, NY: Scribner. principles (2nd ed., pp. 587– 607). New York, NY: Guilford.
Petermann, F. (1992). Psychologie des Vertrauens [The psychology of Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence
trust] (2nd ed.). München, Germany: Quintessenz. of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104,
Plucker, J. A., & Renzulli, J. S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the 61– 87. doi:10.2307/1422851
study of human creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of cre- Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the
ativity (pp. 35– 61). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and
WHEN DISTRUST ENHANCES CREATIVITY 1277

implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1660 – A study of the creativity–intelligence distinction. Oxford, United King-
1672. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1660 dom: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Pros- West, M. A. (2002). Sparkling fountains or stagnant ponds: An integrative
pects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity model of creativity and innovation implementation in work groups.
(pp. 3–15). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51, 355–387. doi:
Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P., & Eid, M. (1997). Der Mehrdi- 10.1111/1464-0597.00951
mensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF) [Multidimensional Ybarra, O., Winkielman, P., Yeh, I., Burnstein, E., & Kavanagh, L. (2011).
Comfort Questionnaire]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Friends (and sometimes enemies) with cognitive benefits: What types of
Taggar, S. (2002). Individual creativity and group ability to utilize indi- social interactions boost executive functioning? Social Psychological
vidual creative resources: A multilevel model. Academy of Management and Personality Science, 2, 253–261. doi:10.1177/1948550610386808
Journal, 45, 315–330. doi:10.2307/3069349 Zebrowitz, L. A., Voinescu, L., & Collins, M. A. (1996). “Wide-eyed” and
Topolinski, S., & Deutsch, R. (2009). Phasic affective modulation of “crooked-faced”: Determinants of perceived and real honesty across the
semantic spread. Manuscript submitted for publication. life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1258 –1269.
Tyler, J. M., Feldman, R. S., & Reichert, A. (2006). The price of deceptive doi:10.1177/01461672962212006
behavior: Disliking and lying to people who lie to us. Journal of Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and
Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 69 –77. doi:10.1016/j Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer
.jesp.2005.02.003 Research, 37, 197–206. doi:10.1086/651257
Walczyk, J. J., Runco, M. A., Tripp, S. M., & Smith, C. E. (2008). The
creativity of lying: Divergent thinking and ideational correlates of the
resolution of social dilemmas. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 328 – Received November 25, 2010
342. doi:10.1080/10400410802355152 Revision received May 9, 2011
Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: Accepted May 11, 2011 䡲

You might also like