You are on page 1of 14

1398 JSLHR

JSLHR,, Volume
Volume 41,
41, 1398–1411,
1398–1411, December
December 1998
1998

Identifiers of Predominantly
Spanish-Speaking Children With
Language Impairment

María Adelaida Restrepo


The University of Georgia The purpose of this study was to identify a set of measures that would discrimi-
Athens nate 31 predominantly Spanish-speaking children with normal language (NL
children) from 31 children with language impairment (LI children). The LI
children were identified as such by experienced, bilingual (Spanish/English),
ASHA-certified, speech-language pathologists who were currently seeing the
children in their caseloads. Children ranged in age from 5 to 7 years and were
matched for age, gender, and school. Additionally, nonverbal cognitive
measures assured that they did not differ significantly intellectually. Measures of
vocabulary, novel bound-morpheme learning skills, and language form were
randomly administered to all children. Further, parents responded to questions
about their perceptions of their children’s speech and language skills and family
history of speech and language problems. A stepwise discriminant analysis
indicated that 4 measures discriminated the groups of children with a sensitivity
of 91.3% and a specificity of 100% (p < .0001): parental report of the child’s
speech and language skills, number of errors per T-unit, mean length per T-unit,
and family history of speech and language problems. A second discriminant
analysis indicated that the sensitivity and specificity could be maintained when
only the first 2 measures were included. Confirmatory discriminant analyses of
the 2- and 4-measure models indicated that the discriminant accuracy was
stable on an independent sample.
KEY WORDS: SLI, Spanish, identification, language, measures

C
hildren with specific language impairment (SLI; hereafter LI)
are described as having a “significant language deficit in the face
of normal nonverbal intelligence, adequate auditory acuity, and
the absence of gross neurological disabilities” (Leonard, Sabbadini,
Leonard, & Volterra, 1987, p. 234). The most consistent finding of inves-
tigations of LI children is that these children have a disproportionate
“language-form” deficit characterized by morphosyntactic problems such
as bound-morpheme omissions and substitutions, tense agreement prob-
lems, and shorter mean length of utterance when compared with age-
and language-matched controls (e.g., Dromi, Leonard, & Shteiman, 1993;
Lahey, 1988; Leonard, 1992; Leonard et al., 1987; Steckol & Leonard,
1979). Research on the linguistic characteristics of LI children that speak
Spanish as their primary language (SS; heretofore referred as SSLI chil-
dren), while acquiring English as a second language, is, however, very
limited. In this study, the term “primarily Spanish-speaking children”
refers to those children in the United States that speak Spanish at home,
attend bilingual education with Spanish emphasis in language arts, and

Journal of
1398 Journal
Speech,
of Speech,
Language,
Language,
and Hearing
and Hearing
ResearchResearch 1092-4388/98/4106-1398 ©1998, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1399

are learning English as a second language. These chil- Identification of English-Speaking LI


dren are fluent in Spanish, using it both at home with (ESLI) Children
parents and siblings and at school for academic purposes
and friendships. The children’s English skills are lim- Only a few measures have been validated for the
ited expressively, although they may have functional purpose of identifying ESLI children (e.g., Dunn, Flax,
receptive language skills. Sliwinski, & Aram, 1996; Gavin, Klee, & Membrino,
1993; Plante & Vance, 1994, 1995; Rescorla, 1993). In
Although the need to appropriately differentiate
1984, few speech and language tests met McCauley and
SSLI children is well acknowledged, a paucity of re-
Swisher’s psychometric criteria for use in identifying
search exists regarding the validity of measures cur-
preschool children with speech and language problems.
rently available that purport to do so. This is apparent,
Ten years later, Plante and Vance (1994) concluded that
in spite of the fact that Public Law 94-142 requires that
there was little improvement in the psychometric qual-
children in need of a communication assessment be
ity of language tests. They found that of the tests that
evaluated in their native language, and that SS chil-
met five or more of the McCauley and Swisher criteria,
dren now comprise the largest group of second language
only one test adequately discriminated between pre-
learners in the U.S. (Roseberry-McKibbin & Eicholtz,
school NL and LI children (concurrent validity): the
1994). Without appropriately normed and validated
Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test–II
measures, SS children with normal language (SSNL)
(Werner & Kresheck, 1983) consistently discriminated
are at risk for overidentification, whereas the risk for
between preschool NL and LI children with a high level
under identification resides with SSLI children (Kayser,
of accuracy (90% or above).
1995; Langdon, 1992; Roseberry-McKibbin & Eicholtz,
1994). Morphosyntactic measures of spontaneous language
may be useful for the identification of ESLI children
In the absence of valid language measures to iden-
(Dunn et al., 1996; Gavin et al., 1993). Gavin et al. (1993)
tify SSLI children (Anderson, 1996; Langdon, 1992;
entered a set of indices derived from the language
Roseberry-McKibbin & Eicholtz, 1994), clinicians work-
samples of 24 ESNL children and 24 ESLI preschool
ing with these children may use tests that are not ap-
children into a discriminant analysis. A set of four vari-
propriate for this population (cf. Anderson, 1996; Ortiz,
ables (verb-phrase errors, number of single-word utter-
1987; Wilkinson & Ortiz, 1986). In the past, this prob-
ances, age, and number of three-element noun phrases)
lem has been addressed with suggestions that clinicians
accurately discriminated 91% of the ESNL and ESLI
use dynamic assessment, test modifications, and adap-
children in their sample. The discriminant model was
tations or develop local norms when they use tests that
then validated on another sample of ESLI and ESNL
are not appropriately normed for the child under assess-
children with a discriminant accuracy of 86%.
ment, such as an SS child (Erickson & Iglesias, 1986;
Evard & Sabers, 1979; Harris, 1985; Kayser, 1989, 1995; Parental report has also been validated as a mea-
Taylor & Payne, 1983). Such modifications and adapta- sure for identification of LI children (Rescorla, 1993) and
tions invalidate the norm-referenced interpretations has been found to be an accurate source of information
provided with the tests. Developing local norms alone to describe a child’s current language skills (e.g., Dale,
does not validate a test because there needs to be dem- 1991; Dale, Bates, Reznick, & Morriset, 1989; Gutierrez-
onstration that a test has high discriminant accuracy Clellen, Palacios, & Thal, 1992; Hadley & Rice, 1991).
for validity to be assumed. Rescorla (1993) conducted a series of validity studies on
a parent survey for 1- and 2-year-old ES children, the
Despite the fact that there are several types of test
Language Development Survey (Rescorla, 1989). She
validity measures (e.g., construct, content, and criterion
found that the survey’s sensitivity (LI children classi-
validity; Anastasi, 1988), the use of concurrent validity
fied as LI) ranged from .75 to .90, and the specificity
is thought to be essential to determine whether or not a
(NL children classified as NL) from .85 to .98.
measure is appropriate for the correct identification of
an LI child. One type of concurrent validity is discrimi- In addition to the few validated measures for iden-
nant accuracy. Discriminant accuracy refers to the tifying LI children, there is considerable evidence from
measure’s ability to appropriately differentiate groups parental report that LI children have a family history
of children, such as NL versus LI (e.g., Hutchinson, of language problems, offering support to a possible bio-
1996; Plante & Vance, 1994). Language tests that have logical basis of LI (e.g., Plante, 1991; Tallal, Ross, &
a discriminant accuracy for NL and LI children above Curtiss, 1989; Tomblin, 1989). Tallal et al. (1989) found
90% can be considered “good” discriminators, and those that 77% of the LI children in their study had a reported
that discriminate with an accuracy between 80% and family history of speech, language, or academic prob-
89% can be considered “fair” discriminators (Plante & lems in at least one first degree relative. In contrast,
Vance, 1994). Tomblin (1989) found that only 53% of the LI children
in his study had a reported family history of speech and

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


1400 JSLHR, Volume 41, 1398–1411, December 1998

language problems in at least one first-degree relative. SSNL children. Vocabulary, bound-morpheme generali-
Though these differences may be related to whether or zation, morphosyntactic measures, and parental inter-
not the investigators included academic skills (Tallal et view concerning family history and perception of LI were
al., 1989) as part of the criteria used for identifying a posi- chosen as measures for discriminant analyses with SS
tive history, the possibility of such factors contributing to children. However, because there is no existing gold stan-
overall identification of language problems remains good. dard for SSLI children, the validation of these measures
with an independent measure was not possible, result-
Characteristics of SSLI Children ing in an unavoidable circularity. The solution adopted
was to employ stringent selection criteria for subject
Children’s short-term language acquisition may inclusion. The clinical judgments of certified, bilingual
identify SSLI children. For example, ESLI children have (Spanish-English), speech-language pathologists were
problems learning novel vocabulary (e.g., Dollaghan, used to identify a group of moderate to severe SSLI chil-
1987; Oetting, Rice, & Swank, 1995) and generalizing dren for comparison with a matched group of SSNL chil-
bound morphemes to familiar and unfamiliar words (e.g., dren on the measures of interest. An exploratory
Connell, 1987; Connell & Stone, 1992; Roseberry & stepwise discriminant analysis was used to select the
Connell, 1991; Swisher, Restrepo, Plante, & Lowell, measures that most accurately and efficiently discrimi-
1995). Peña, Quinn, and Iglesias (1992) used vocabu- nated between the two groups of SS children. A confir-
lary learning for identification of SSLI children. They matory discriminant analysis was then used to deter-
administered the Expressive One Word Picture Vocabu- mine whether the variables selected through the
lary Test (EOWPVT; Gardner, 1979) to a group of SSNL exploratory analysis accurately predicted group mem-
children and a group of SSLI children, which did not bership on a second, independent sample of children.
differentiate the groups. Subsequently, they taught the
children naming skills through a mediated learning
paradigm and were again administered the EOWPVT. Method
Peña, Quinn, and Iglesias (1992) found that such com-
Participants
bination of mediated learning and the vocabulary mea-
sure could differentiate SS children at risk for language Sixty-two SS children (5;0 to 7;1) were selected from
problems and those with NL. preschool, kindergarten, and first-grade classes in which
In a study by Roseberry and Connell (1991), a novel the primary language of instruction was Spanish. There
bound-morpheme learning paradigm (Connell, 1987) were 42 boys and 20 girls from two school districts in
involving a bound morpheme that indicated part of an Tucson, Arizona. Thirty-one SS children (M = 6;1, SD =
object has been used to differentiate SSLI and SSNL 0;8) had a diagnosis of moderate to severe LI, using the
children. These investigators correctly classified 85% of criteria to be described below. A comparison group, 31
the SS children in the study as NL or LI, based on the SSNL children (M = 6;2, SD = 0;7) were identified as
SS children’s performance on the generalization of the having NL; each SSNL child was matched to an SSLI
bound morpheme. SSLI children also may have child according to age (±3 months) and gender. In all
morphosyntactic deficits characterized by errors of num- but one case, children were also matched for school and,
ber, tense, person, and gender agreement, as well as whenever possible, for classroom (87% of the total num-
omission and substitution of pronouns, articles, and ber of children). According to the school records for the
prepositions (Ambert, 1986; Juarez, 1983; Merino, 1983). free lunch program, all but 2 of the children in the study
For example, 5- to 8-year-old SSLI children obtained were from low income families. The 2 remaining chil-
significantly lower scores than controls in gender and dren were from middle income backgrounds. Income did
number agreement, verb tenses, plurals, and pronouns not appear to impact performance on the study’s mea-
(Merino, 1983). Anderson (1996) found that a structured sures during the study. All the families had Mexican-
spontaneous language elicitation task yielded more American backgrounds.
morphosyntactic forms than the Spanish Structured Pho- All children demonstrated hearing within normal
tographic Expressive Language Test–Preschool (SSPELT– limits as measured by a pure-tone hearing screening
P; Werner & Kresheck, 1989) from preschool-age SSNL (ANSI, 1969) on the day testing began. Teacher report
children. However, no concurrent validity is yet available. indicated the absence of physical, social, or behavioral
problems for all participants (see Appendix A). Additional
criteria included cognition within the normal range and
Summary and Rationale for the Spanish as the child’s primary language.
Present Study
The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children–
The purpose of this study was to identify a set of Nonverbal Scale (KABC–NVS; Kaufman & Kaufman,
measures that would discriminate SSLI children from 1983) was administered to ensure that nonverbal IQ was

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1401

within the normal range. Scores ranged from 77 to 115 Table 1. Means and standard deviations of participants’ character-
for the SLI group (M = 93.9, SD = 11.57) and 82 to 119 istics by group.
for the NL group (M = 99, SD = 7.87). A wider range
than the arbitrary cutoff score of 85 nonverbal IQ was Characteristic SSNL SSLI
used because there is no evidence that children with Age 6;2 (0;7) 6;1 ( 0;8)
normal nonverbal IQ between 70 and 85 perform differ-
Kaufman Assessment Battery
ently from those with nonverbal IQ > 85 (Dale & Cole,
for Children—NVS* 99.00 (7.87) 93.90 (11.57)
1991; Fey, Long, & Cleave, 1994; Plante, 1998). Further-
Teachers’ questionnaire
more, a restricted IQ range affects the external validity
Spanish* 65.09 (3.61) 62.55 (5.79)
of the study by excluding children with normal nonver-
bal IQ that would otherwise qualify (Fey et al., 1994; Teachers’ questionnaire
English 25.16 (13.34) 21.29 (13.84)
Plante, 1998). To eliminate the possibility of including
children with mental retardation, the cutoff score for Home bilingual usage
normal nonverbal IQ plus the standard error of mea- estimate—Spanish 21.48 (4.78) 24.32 (7.35)
surement was used. Home bilingual usage
estimate—English 3.90 (3.92) 4.54 (4.48)
Spanish Language Note. SSNL = Spanish-speaking children with normal language; SSLI =
A number of variables played a role in determining Spanish-speaking children with language impairment; NVS =
that all children used Spanish as their primary language Nonverbal Scale.
in both home and school environments: (a) classroom *p < .05.
placement, (b) language-arts instruction in Spanish, (c)
The Home Bilingual Usage Estimate (HBUE; Skoczylas, Selection of SSLI Children
1971), and (d) a teachers’ questionnaire (Appendix A).
All children came from bilingual classes where content Because there is no agreed-upon best practice for
instruction was in Spanish. To qualify for these class- the identification of SSLI children in the United States,
rooms, all children had shown primary Spanish profi- there is no measure available to provide external vali-
ciency on the Language Assessment Scale (LAS; Duncan dation to the identification of the SSLI children. How-
& DeAvila, 1985), a measure of both Spanish and En- ever, clinical judgment is reliable for both the identifi-
glish abilities, and on the school’s own parent-home- cation of a clinical population and the severity of a
language-use questionnaire. Another criterion that en- disorder (e.g., Lewis, 1995). Teacher perception has also
sured Spanish was the primary home language was the been found to be accurate for the identification of chil-
HBUE, a structured questionnaire completed by the dren at risk for language or learning disabilities
children’s parents. There were no significant differences (Frontera & Horowitz, 1995; Salvesen & Undheim,
[t(60) = 2.00, p = .07; t(60) = 2.00, p = .54] between the 1994). For this study, rigorous criteria, reflecting both
SSNL and SSLI groups in either Spanish scores or En- clinical and teacher judgments, were employed as the
glish scores (see Table 1). gold standard.
A teachers’ questionnaire was also developed (see Only children that were identified by ASHA-certified,
Appendix A). A portion of the questionnaire examined Spanish-English bilingual, speech-language pathologists
the teacher’s perception of the child’s English and Span- (SLPs) were included in the SSLI group. Interviews with
ish proficiency and use across several settings and each SLP regarding the method of identification used to
interactors. A 3-point scale was used. To qualify as a determine the diagnosis of SSLI revealed that they re-
participant, children had to demonstrate a 10-point lied on multiple sources of information. Measures in-
Spanish > English difference by teacher report. There cluded the Pruebas de Expresión Oral y de Percepción
were no significant differences between the groups for de la Lengua Española (Mares, 1981), a norm-referenced
the English skills [t(60) = 1.76, p = .267], though a sig- measure for Mexican- American children; informal mea-
nificant difference did emerge for the Spanish score [t(60) sures of auditory comprehension and oral expression;
= 2.000, p = .042]. This latter difference was not sur- language sample analyses of language form, content, and
prising, given that lower scores for SSLI children’s gram- use in comparison to the local population; classroom
matical skills in expressive language proficiency were observation; and parent and teacher interviews.
to be expected for their primary language. Finally, all All SSLI children had been diagnosed by the SLP
children used only Spanish during the entire pretesting as having a moderate to severe language impairment in
part of the study. Table 1 shows performances by group, Spanish. This higher range of severity was chosen to
means and standard deviations for all measures used further ensure the certainty of the diagnosis. Addition-
during the qualifying part of this investigation. ally, each child had to be currently enrolled in language

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


1402 JSLHR, Volume 41, 1398–1411, December 1998

intervention with an ASHA-certified, bilingual SLP, or the child to listen carefully for later retelling. Pictures
with a native Spanish-speaking aide, with at least 3 were used for contextual support in both the telling and
years experience in the provision of services to SSLI retelling phases.
children, under the supervision of a bilingual certified Prior to analyzing the spontaneous language
SLP. Finally, the teachers’ questionnaire that was de- samples, an RA segmented transcripts into “terminable
veloped for this study included questions related to per- units” (T-units), defined as any clause and its subordi-
ceptions of the child’s language abilities. To be selected nate clauses (Hunt, 1965). The following language-form
for participation, teacher’s judgments had to reflect the measures were then applied to the samples: Develop-
perception of child difficulties in Spanish or in academic mental Assessment of Spanish Grammar (DASG;
areas. Toronto, 1976), mean length of T-unit (MLTU), and a
ratio of number of grammatical errors per total number
Selection of SSNL Children of T-units (NETU).
Most utterances in the children’s samples were in
The SSNL children were enrolled in only regular Spanish. A few T-units were excluded due to English
educational classrooms and not in any resource pro- use (2.6% of the total) words. These T-units were dis-
grams for at-risk children. Additionally, they had no lan- carded even if the rest of the sentence was in Spanish
guage, academic, physical, or behavioral problems, as because they presented a problem for coding grammati-
judged by the teachers and reported in the teachers’ cal errors. A few English words were integrated by the
questionnaires. community into their dialect such as bye-bye, baby, VCR,
and these were included. T-units with unintelligible
Measures and Procedures words were excluded from the analyses (7.07% of the
total T-units). An RA counted and averaged the number
Description of Candidate Discriminators of words per T-unit, following Gutierrez-Clellen and
All the children selected for the study participated Hofstetter’s (1994) Spanish adaptation of the Hunt
in a battery of verbal measures, and their parents re- (1965) T-unit analysis. A T-unit was defined as any main
sponded to a parent interview. Given that the measures clause and its subordinate clauses (i.e., “The cat who
selected had not been validated for the identification of ate the mouse is here”). MLTU was used because it is
SSLI children, only raw scores and raw data were used. indicative of age differences in sentence complexity in
Research assistants (RAs) conducted all testing and in- school-age SSNL children and because it better reflects
terviews in Spanish. The RAs that tested the children syntactic complexity in a highly inflected language.
and interviewed parents were native Spanish speakers The Spanish adaptation (Toronto, 1976) of the De-
attending the undergraduate program in Speech and velopmental Sentence Scoring (Lee, 1974), the Develop-
Hearing Sciences at the University of Arizona. They were mental Assessment of Spanish Grammar (DASG), was
trained by the author and kept blind to the nature of also used as language sample analysis. The DASG as-
the study and the children’s classifications until the con- signs weighted scores to sentences based on develop-
clusion of the study. All measures were given in random mental morphosyntactic hierarchies in six categories:
order, and each of the two RAs tested half SSNL and (1) indefinite pronouns and noun modifiers, (2) personal
half SSLI children. pronouns, (3) primary verbs, (4) secondary verbs, (5)
Language Measures. The language measures were conjunctions, and (6) interrogative words. Three addi-
obtained from spontaneous language samples, a stan- tional categories (possessives, plurals, and negatives)
dardized test, and language-learning tasks. An RA elic- are taken into account within the above categories. An
ited the language samples in three different contexts: RA scored the sentences and averaged the scores accord-
picture description, interview with the child, and story ing to the guidelines, except for sentences conjoined with
retelling. The RA encouraged the child to speak as freely the conjunction y (and), which were considered as two
as possible in all contexts through the use of open-ended T-units for analysis. Due to this coding variation, as well
questions, semantically related to the prior response. as the difference in the sample of children, the DASG
All language samples were audio taped and then tran- norms were not applied.
scribed by the RA who collected the samples. The third spontaneous language-form analysis was
In the interview, the RA introduced preselected top- the NETU. In this analysis, a native Spanish-speaking
ics for discussion, such as a scary event, a hospital ex- RA identified grammatical errors in each T-unit. Verb
perience, a trip to the zoo, or a favorite movie. In the or noun phrase agreement problems, use of inappropri-
story retelling format, the RA used one of two Mercer ate pronouns, word-order problems, and omission of
Meyer “frog” books: A Boy, a Dog, and a Frog (Mayer, words were considered grammatical errors. Lexical er-
1967) and Frog Where Are You? (Mayer, 1969), asking rors, phonological errors, and articulation errors were

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1403

excluded because the focus was on morphosyntactic repeat the affixed nouns. Once the child finished imi-
skills. Cohesion errors across sentences were not counted tating the affixed nouns, the RA asked the child to choose
in this analysis because it is difficult to determine four of the nine cards and elicited the affixed noun, giv-
whether the problem is due to morphosyntactic, prag- ing specific feedback regarding the accuracy of the re-
matic, or developmental difficulties at this age; only er- sponse of the first two cards and neutral feedback on
rors of agreement within a sentence were included. The the last two cards.
investigator, who is also a native Spanish speaker, The RA presented three picture pairs, a whole ob-
checked the RA’s coding of grammatical errors. When ject and a half object, of words used during the NVL
disagreements between the RA and the author were task. The RA also presented a novel word (with a pic-
encountered, they were discussed until agreement was ture pair) that was not presented during the NVL task,
obtained. A second RA added and averaged the total for a total of four items. The RA labeled the whole-ob-
number of grammatical errors per T-unit to use for the ject picture and asked the child to label the half-object
statistical analyses. picture. Each card was presented twice, for a total of
The Spanish Structured Photographic Expressive eight possible correct responses.
Language Test–II (SSPELT–II; Werner & Kresheck, Children’s phonological approximations of the stems
1989) is a formal test without norms that measures ex- and morphemes were considered correct. The correct
pressive morphosyntactic skills. The RA provided a se- stem with the correct bound morpheme, the correct stem
ries of photographs to the child and asked the child to with an approximation of the bound morpheme, and an
complete a sentence, to respond to a question, or to de- approximation of the stem with the correct bound mor-
scribe it. The child was praised for attention to the task, pheme were considered correct responses. The use of a
but no feedback was provided relating to the accuracy bare stem or a completely different stem with a correct
of the responses on the test. bound morpheme were not considered correct responses.
Two learning measures were developed to assess the Parent Interview. The parent interview elicited a
child’s novel vocabulary learning (NVL) and novel bound- family history of educational, speech, and language prob-
morpheme generalization (NBMG). The NVL measure lems (FHSLP) and a report of the child’s educational,
was designed to teach three novel nouns (pefo, duco, tijo) speech, or language problems (PRSLP; see Appendix B
that corresponded to three novel objects depicted in line for the English translation). All questions were in yes/
drawings on cards. The novel words were in CVCV or- no format. The FHSLP measure contained 21 questions
der to resemble a common word form in Spanish. Three addressing whether the child’s family members (i.e., sib-
native Spanish speakers verified (100% agreement) that lings, parents, uncles, aunts, cousins, and grandparents)
the novel words were phonetically consistent with Span- had a history of speech and language deficits, educa-
ish and that they were indeed novel. tional problems, learning or reading disabilities, and
The novel vocabulary was introduced in four ways attention deficit or hyperactivity problems. The PRSLP
in fixed order: modeling, imitation, identification, and measure contained 29 questions that asked whether the
naming. However, only scores on the naming portion of parents thought their child had current problems with
the NVL were used for the statistical analyses of the speech, expressive language, receptive language, learn-
study because of earlier suggestions that LI children ing, or attention, in comparison with other children the
typically comprehend novel words (e.g., Dollaghan, same age.
1987). Scores on the naming task indicated the total tri-
als to criterion of three out of four consecutive correct Reliability
responses for each novel word.
Reliability between at least two examiners was cal-
The NBMG learning measure adapted from the culated as percent of agreement (point-to- point) for each
Roseberry and Connell (1991) task involved learning a measure, the transcription, and the T-unit segmenta-
novel bound morpheme and generalizing it to new novel tion. Twenty percent of the data were randomly selected
nouns. The bound morpheme was /-ono/, which indicated and reanalyzed by a second examiner (see Table 2 for a
half of an object. To introduce the bound morpheme, the summary of the reliability for each measure, the tran-
RA first showed the child a set of nine line drawings scription, and the T-unit segmentation by group).
representing familiar objects and asked the child to la-
bel the objects. If the child did not name any of the ob-
jects with the form expected, the RA provided the name Results
of the noun for the child. After the RA presented the
nine nouns, the RA showed the child the set of nine cards To identify the set of measures that best discrimi-
with the drawings of the whole object and half the ob- nated between the two groups, exploratory discriminant
ject in each card and asked the child to listen and then analyses were run on 23 matched pairs (exploratory data

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


1404 JSLHR, Volume 41, 1398–1411, December 1998

Table 2. Point-to-point reliability on language and cognitive discriminant accuracy could not be improved. Because
measure, on transcription, and on terminable unit segmentation by it is possible that the discriminant accuracy of the model
group. can be maintained with fewer than four measures, the
PRSLP and NETU were entered alone into a discrimi-
Measure SSNL SSLI nant analysis to determine whether the sensitivity and
Nonverbal vocabulary learning 99.22% 99.44% specificity of the four-measure model were maintained.
Using these two measures only, the discriminant accu-
Novel bound morpheme generalization 99.04% 99.04%
racy remained; specificity was 100% and sensitivity was
Mean length of T-unit 100.00% 100.00%
91.3%. These results indicate that the same level of dis-
Developmental assessment of Spanish criminant accuracy that the four-measure model pro-
grammar 89.50% 84.86% duced could be maintained with the two-measure model,
Number of error per T-unit 100.00% 95.23% although efficiency could not be improved with fewer
Kaufman Assessment Battery for than the two measures. See Table 5 for the sensitivity
Children—NVS 97.91% 97.91% and specificity for each of the measures.
Parent report of speech and language
problems 98.85% 97.00%
Confirmatory Discriminant Analyses
Family history of speech and language
problems 99.57% 98.91% Results of the confirmatory discriminant analyses
Spanish Structured Photographic indicated that the two- and four-measure discriminant
Expressive Language Test–II 99.00% 89.79%
Transcription 90.20% 85.77% Table 3. Means and standard deviations for each measure by
group.
T-unit segmentation 100.00% 100.00%

Note. SSNL = Spanish-speaking children with normal language; SSLI = Measure SSNL SSLI
Spanish-speaking children with language impairment; NVS =
Spanish Structured Photographic
Nonverbal Scale.
Expressive Language Test–II 44.06 (3.99) 31.55 (9.82)
Parent report of speech and
set), randomly selected from the 31 matched pairs. Con- language problems 4.7 (2.76) 17.48 (6.54)
firmatory discriminant analyses were then run on the Family history of speech and
remaining eight matched pairs (confirmatory data set) language problems 9.93 (7.34) 23.39 (13.20)
to validate the results on an independent sample from Number of errors per T-unit 0.09 (0.05) 0.39 (0.21)
which the original discriminant function was derived.
Mean length of T-unit 5.64 (.87) 4.51 (1.00)
Developmental assessment of
Exploratory Discriminant Analyses Spanish grammar 5.36 (1.01) 3.97 (1.19)
Novel vocabulary learning 22.74 (11.24) 26.19 (13.52)
The children’s scores on SSPELT–II, MLTU, NETU,
DASG, PRSLP, FHSLP, NVL, and NBMG were included Novel bound morpheme
generalization 4.35 (3.32) 2.80 (3.41)
in the stepwise discriminant analysis. Table 3 shows the
means and standard deviations for each measure by the Note. SSNL = Spanish-speaking children with normal language; SSLI =
two groups of children. The discriminant analysis iden- Spanish-speaking children with language impairment.
tified four measures that best discriminated between
SSNL and SSLI children: PRSLP, NETU, MLTU, FHSLP
(ordered by the degree of variance; see Table 4). Table 4. Results of the stepwise discriminant analysis.

The discriminant model classified 21 of 23 SSLI as


Individual Total
LI, indicating that the model had a sensitivity of 91.3%; Measure variance variance
whereas it classified all of 23 SSNL as NL, indicating a
specificity of 100%. The total average squared canoni- Parent report of speech and
cal correlation of the model was .79 (p > .0001). To test language problems .57** .57**
the remaining measures for their potential contribution Family history of speech and
to the model’s discriminant accuracy, these measures language problems .15** .72**
were added individually by removing the two measures Number of errors per T-unit .04* .76*
that added the least variance. This left the PRSLP and Mean length of T-unit .03* .79*
NETU that accounted for 72% of the variance. When
the remaining measures were added individually, the *p < .05. **p < .0001.

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1405

Table 5. Sensitivity and specificity of each measure. MLTU, and FHSLP—were entered into the stepwise
analysis. This three-measure model discriminated with
Characteristic Sensitivity Specificity a sensitivity of 83.9% and a specificity of 100% of the
total sample (91.9% accuracy). The total average squared
Spanish Structured Photo-
graphic Expressive
canonical correlation of the model was .71 (p > .0001).
Language Test–II 65.22% (15/23) 91.30% (21/23) The results indicate that the same three measures in-
volved in the initial analysis are still valid even when
Parent report of speech and
language problems 73.91% (17/23) 95.65% (22/23)
the variance of PRSLP is removed from the analysis,
thus confirming the value of the results.
Family history of speech and
language problems 73.91% (17/23) 91.30% (21/23)
Mean length of T-unit 69.57% (16/23) 86.96% (20/23)
Discussion
Number of errors per T-unit 69.57% (16/23) 100% (23/23)
Developmental assessment of
The present study was designed to identify a set of
Spanish grammar 65.22% (15/23) 91.30% (21/23) measures that would discriminate between SSNL and
SSLI children with a high level of accuracy. A stepwise
Novel vocabulary learning 43.48% (10/23) 69.57% (16/23)
discriminant analysis indicated that four of eight mea-
Novel bound morpheme
sures accounted for 79% of the variance in the model
generalization 65.22% (15/23) 60.87% (14/23)
and successfully discriminated between the groups. They
were: parental report of their child’s speech and language
models correctly classified 7 of 8 SSLI children (sensi- problems (PRSLP), family history of the same (FHSLP),
tivity of 87.5%) and all 8 of the SSNL children (specific- mean length of T-unit (MLTU), and number of errors
ity of 100%). Table 6 provides the sensitivity and speci- per T-unit (NETU). Results from a sample different from
ficity of the models, and Table 7 the weights and the one in which the discriminant functions were de-
constants for the two- and four-measure discriminant rived validated these findings.
models. To control for possible bias of the PRSLP, given The high level of discriminant accuracy was main-
that parents knew of the child’s diagnosis, a stepwise tained when a discriminant model was tested with the
discriminant analysis was run without this measure. two measures that accounted for the most variance of
Results indicated that only three measures—NETU, the four-measure model (72%): PRSLP and NETU. The

Table 6. Sensitivity and specificity of the two discriminant models.

Exploratory data set Confirmatory data set

Model Sensitivity Specificity Sensitivity Specificity

PRSLP 91.3% 100% 87.5% 100%


Number of errors per T-unit (21/23) (23/23) (7/8) (8/8)
PRSLP
Number of errors per T-unit 91.3% 100% 87.5% 100%
Mean length of T-unit (21/23) (23/23) (7/8) (8/8)
FHSLP

Note. PRSLP = parent report of speech and language problems; FHSLP = family history of speech and language
problems.

Table 7. Discriminant functions for the two- and four-measure models.

Model Discriminant function

Four-measure model DF score = –.2084969 + PRSLP(.1019379) + NETU(4.0810516) +


FHSLP(.0429730) + MLTU(–.5324122)
Two-measure model DF score = –2.6891650 + PRSLP(.1447095) + NETU(4.3891402)

Note. DF = discriminant function; PRSLP = parent report of speech and language problems; NETU = number of
error per terminable units; MLTU = mean length of terminable unit; FHSLP = family history of speech and
language problems.

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


1406 JSLHR, Volume 41, 1398–1411, December 1998

level of accuracy was true for both the exploratory and Gavin et al.’s (1993) report that the number of verb-
confirmatory data sets. These results fall within the cri- phrase errors per utterance accounted for the most vari-
terion for acceptable discriminant accuracy for identify- ance in a discriminant model of language indices that
ing LI children (Plante & Vance, 1994). Accordingly, a discriminated between ESNL and ESLI groups. In ad-
rate of identification below an 80% level would have “se- dition, it supports the findings of Dunn et al. (1996) that
rious social consequences” (p. 21). structural errors are contributors to the discrimination
The four measures found to be accurate discrimina- of ESLI and ESNL groups.
tors between the SSNL and SSLI groups involved two As expected on the MLTU measure, the SSLI group
procedural contexts: parental interview and spontane- used less complex sentences than the SSNL group, con-
ous language-form analysis. Although standardized sistent with a report by Linares-Orama and Sanders
measures do not appear to be a requirement of appro- (1977). Linares-Orama and Sanders used MLU in mor-
priate diagnoses, parents appear to be a valuable source phemes to describe the language of preschool children,
of information for identifying SSLI children. These re- and this study used MLTU to describe the language of
sults provide support for Kayser’s (1995) suggestion that, young school-age children. Different measures of syn-
in addition to obtaining case histories, clinicians should tactic complexity may be more appropriate for different
interview parents about their children’s language skills age groups of SS children. MLU in morphemes might
in a systematic manner. Parental report may be espe- identify preschool SSLI children, whereas MLTU might
cially important when assessing children for whom there best identify school-age SSLI children.
are no appropriate norm-referenced measures, such as Research on SSLI children should continue to iden-
SS children. tify the types of language deficits that characterize these
Several factors may have contributed to the discrimi- children so that the understanding of the nature of SSLI
nant accuracy of the questionnaire. Parents have been can be enhanced and identification improved. For ex-
found to be quite accurate at assessing current language ample, Restrepo (1997) found that article errors are one
skills (Dale et al., 1989). Furthermore, the specificity of of the most common types of errors in SSLI children.
the questions (e.g., noun-phrase agreement skills, abil- These errors occur mainly in gender agreement and not
ity to follow directions, and ability to retrieve words) in the use of definite versus indefinite articles, or in
and the requirement of some direct comparisons of their number agreement. Therefore, SSLI children may pro-
children’s abilities to peers, may have led the parents to duce more errors in the noun system than ESLI chil-
more accurate determination of normalcy or atypicality dren who produce more errors in the verb system (Gavin
in relation to the child’s peers. et al., 1993). Because Spanish is a more inflected language
Because of the possible bias in the PRSLP, (e.g., than English, and agreement occurs in verb and noun
parents of SSLI children already knew their child’s di- phrases, it could be hypothesized that errors in both the
agnosis) a stepwise discriminant analysis with the whole noun and verb phrase contributed to the discrimination
sample was run to determine if, by removing the vari- between the SSNL and SSLI groups in the present study.
ance of this measure, the same measures would discrimi- Nevertheless, research is needed to understand noun
nate and, if so, with what accuracy. The results indi- phrase status in SSLI children’s language.
cated that the same measures, NETU, MLTU, and In contrast to the present findings for learning tasks,
FHSLP were entered into the model. Furthermore, the Roseberry and Connell (1991) demonstrated that an
level of discrimination was still high (91.9% accuracy), English bound-morpheme-learning measure was useful
indicating that the remaining measures are necessary in classifying 85% of SSNL and SSLI children into the
for the accurate identification of SSLI children. appropriate groups. It may be that some SSNL children
Several researchers of ESLI children have docu- showed early difficulties in vocabulary learning or that
mented the increased prevalence of positive family his- the learning measures in the current study did not tax
tories for speech, language, and academic problems (e.g., the children’s language system enough to produce group
Plante, 1991; Tallal et al., 1989; Tomblin, 1989). In the differences. ESLI children perform similarly to controls
present study, results of the FHSLP measure also sug- when required to imitate affixed words while learning a
gested that SS children with family histories positive bound morpheme. However, when imitation is not re-
for these problems are also likely to be at greater risk quired, the groups differ (Connell, 1987; Connell & Stone,
for LI than those without such histories. 1992). These findings suggest a primary role of imitation
in learning morphemes. In the current study, required
From the spontaneous language-form analysis, the
imitation during the vocabulary and the bound-morpheme
NETU measure of morphosyntactic errors per T-unit
tasks may have limited discriminability of these measures.
yielded results consonant with findings of ESLI children.
That is, the language of SSLI is characterized by a In summary, parent interviewing and language sam-
morphosyntactic deficit. This finding is compatible with pling procedures, as used in this study, currently appear

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1407

to be the clinician’s best tools for identification of SSLI practice for teachers and administrators (pp. 15–33). New
children. Replication of these findings with other groups York: American Library Publishing.
of SS children and the continuation of data-based ap- American National Standards Institute. (1969). Specifi-
proaches to the identification of LI in all children is cer- cations for audiometers. New York: Author.
tainly warranted. Consequences for misclassified chil- Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (6th ed.). New
dren are serious (cf. Plante & Vance, 1994). A child with York: Macmillian.
LI that is not identified misses the available opportuni- Connell, P. (1987). A comparison of modeling and imitation
ties for academic and communication support that can teaching procedures on language-disordered children.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 105–113.
promote success in school. On the other hand, a child
who is mislabeled LI will likewise miss the opportuni- Connell, P. J., & Stone, C. A. (1992). Morpheme learning of
children with specific language impairment under
ties and resources for academic success. Both can result controlled instructional conditions. Journal of Speech and
in children whose emotional and developmental well Hearing Research, 35, 844–852.
being are compromised. Dale, P. S. (1991). The validity of a parent report measure of
The results of this study confirm that well trained vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. Journal of Speech
and qualified, bilingual speech-language pathologists and Hearing Research, 34, 565–571.
accurately identify SSLI children with moderate to se- Dale, P., Bates, E., Reznick, S., & Morriset, C. (1989).
vere deficits, despite the absence of validated measures. The validity of a parent report instrument of child
language at twenty months. Journal of Child Language,
However, the multiple qualitative methods they use (e.g.,
16, 239–250.
interviews, informal assessment, and observation) are
Dale, P. S., & Cole, K. N. (1991). What’s normal? Specific
not efficient and are not widely used. Furthermore, not
language impairment in an individual differences perspec-
all SS children have fully proficient bilingual clinicians tive. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the
available for evaluations. These findings point to the Schools, 22, 80–83.
most efficient approaches to valid assessment and cor- Dollaghan, C. (1987). Fast mapping in normal and lan-
rect identification of SSLI children by all speech-lan- guage impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
guage pathologists, even those who have only a transla- Disorders, 34, 218–222.
tor available to them. Dromi, E., Leonard, L. B., & Shteiman, M. (1993). The
grammatical morphology of Hebrew-speaking children
with specific language impairment: Some competing
Acknowledgments hypotheses. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36,
760–771.
This article was adapted from the doctoral dissertation
Duncan, S. E., & DeAvila, E. (1985). Preschool Language
completed by the author at the University of Arizona,
Assessment Scale. Monterey, CA: CTB, McGraw-Hill.
Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, under Linda
Swisher. The research was supported in part by the Tucson Dunn, M., Flax, J., Sliwinski, M., & Aram, D. (1996).
Scottish Rite Charitable Foundation, by the U.S. Depart- The use of spontaneous language measures as criteria
ment of Education Grant H029D20070, and by the National for identifying children with specific language impair-
Multipurpose Research and Training Center Grant DC- ment: An attempt to reconcile clinical and research
01409 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other incongruence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
39, 643– 654.
Communication Disorders. The author would like to thank
especially Linda Swisher and Elena Plante for their support Erickson, J. G., & Iglesias, A. (1986). Speech and language
and advice during the dissertation process and Ana Dokken disorders in Hispanics. In O. Taylor (Ed.), Nature of
for her assistance in the data collection and transcription. In communication disorders in linguistically diverse popula-
addition, the author would like to thank Marilyn Newhoff for tions (pp. 181–218). San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.
her great support editing the manuscript. The author also Evard, B. L., & Sabers, D. L. (1979). Speech and language
thanks the staff, students, and their families that partici- testing with distinct ethnic-racial groups: A survey of
pated in this study from the Tucson Unified and Sunny Side procedures for improving validity. Journal of Speech and
Unified School Districts and the undergraduate students Hearing Disorders, 54, 271–281.
that contributed to data collection and analysis. Fey, M. E., Long, S. H., & Cleave, P. (1994). Reconsidera-
tion of IQ criteria in the definition of specific language
impairment. In Watkins, R. & Rice, M. (Eds.), Specific
References language impairments in children (pp. 161–178). Balti-
more: Brookes.
Anderson, R. (1996). Assessing the grammar of Spanish-
speaking children: A comparison of two procedures. Frontera, L., & Horowitz, R. (1995). Reading and study
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, behavior of fourth grade Hispanic students: Can teachers
27, 333–344. assess risk? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17,
1, 100–120.
Ambert, A. (1986). Identifying language disorders in
Spanish-speakers. In A. C. Willig & H. F. Greenberg Gardner, M. F. (1976). The Expressive One Word Picture
(Eds.), Bilingualism and learning disabilities: Policy and Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy
Publications.

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


1408 JSLHR, Volume 41, 1398–1411, December 1998

Gavin, W. J., Klee, T., & Membrino I. (1993). Differentiat- Linares-Orama, N., & Sanders, L. J. (1977). Evaluation of
ing specific language impairment from normal language syntax in three-year-old Spanish-speaking Puerto Rican
development using grammatical analysis. Clinical children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20,
Linguistics & Phonetics, 7, 191–206. 350–357.
Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., & Hofstetter, R. (1994). Syntac- McCauley, R. J., & Swisher, L. (1984). Psychometric
tic complexity in Spanish narrative: A developmental review of language and articulation tests for preschool
study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49,
645–654. 34–42.
Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., Palacios, M., & Thal, D. (1992). Mares, S. (1981). A formal assessment tool for Hispanic
The validity of parental report with Spanish-speaking children: Prueba de Expresión Oral para la Lengua
toddlers. Poster presented at the American Speech- Española. Paper presented at the American Speech-
Language-Hearing Association Annual Convention, San Language-Hearing Association Annual Convention, Los
Antonio, TX. Angeles, CA.
Hadley, P., & Rice, M. (1991). Conversational responsive- Mayer, M. (1967). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York: Pied
ness of speech- and language-impaired preschoolers. Piper Books.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1308–1317. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog where are you? New York: Pied Piper
Harris, G. A. (1985). Considerations in assessing English Books.
language performance of Native American populations. Merino, B. J. (1983). Language development in normal and
Topics of Language Disorders, 5, 42–52. language handicapped Spanish-speaking children.
Hunt, K. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three Hispanic Journal of the Behavioral Sciences, 5, 379–400.
grade levels. National Council of Teachers of English Oetting, J. B., Rice, M. L, & Swank, L. K. (1995). Quick
(Research Report No. 3). Champaign, IL: National Council incidental learning (QUIL) of words by school-age children
of Teachers of English. with and without SLI. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Hutchinson, T. (1996). What to look for in the technical Research, 38, 434–445.
manual: Twenty questions for users. Language, Speech, Ortiz, A. (1987). Communication disorders among limited
and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 109–121. English proficient Hispanic students. Bilingual Special
Juarez, M. (1983). A comparison of the oral language skills Education Newsletter, 5, 1–5.
of language-impaired and normally-developing Spanish- Peña, E. D., Quinn, R., & Iglesias, A. (1992). The applica-
English bilingual children. Unpublished doctoral disserta- tion of dynamic methods to language assessment: A
tion, University of Texas, Austin. nonbiased procedure. The Journal of Special Education,
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman 26, 269–279.
Assessment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN: Plante, E. (1998). Criteria for SLI: The Stark & Tallal
American Guidance Service. legacy and beyond. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Kayser, H. (1989). Speech and language assessment of Hearing Research, 41, 951–957.
Spanish-English speaking children. Language, Speech, Plante, E. (1991). MRI findings in the parents and siblings
and Hearing Services in Schools, 20, 226–244. of specifically language-impaired boys. Brain and Lan-
Kayser, H. (1995). Assessment of speech and language guage, 41, 67–80.
impairments in children. In H. Kayser (Ed.), Bilingual Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool
speech and language pathology: An Hispanic focus (pp. language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech,
243–264). San Diego, CA: Singular. and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 15–24.
Lahey, M. (1988). Language disorders and language Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1995). Diagnostic accuracy of two
development. New York: Macmillian. tests of preschool language. American Journal of Speech-
Langdon, H. W. (1992). Speech and language assessment of Language Pathology, 4, 70–76.
LEP/bilingual Hispanic students. In H. L. Langdon & L. Restrepo, M. A. (1997, April). Grammatical errors in
L. Cheng (Eds.), Hispanic children and adults with Spanish-speaking children with language impairment who
communication disorders (pp. 201–271). Gaithersburg, are learning English as a second language. Paper pre-
MD: Aspen. sented at the International Symposium on Bilingualism,
Lee, L. L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, Newcastle upon Tyne, England.
IL: Northwestern University Press. Rescorla, L. (1993). Use of parental report in the identifica-
Leonard, L. B. (1992). Specific language impairment in three tion of communicatively delayed toddlers. Seminars in
languages: Some cross-linguistic evidence. In P. Fletcher & Speech and Language, 14, 264–277.
D. Hall (Eds.), Specific speech and language disorders in Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey.
children. (pp. 118–126). San Diego, CA: Singular. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587–599.
Leonard, L. B., Sabbadini, L., Leonard, J. S. , & Roseberry, C. A., & Connell, P. J. (1991). The use of an
Volterra, V. (1987). Specific language impairment in invented language rule in the differentiation of normal
children: A cross-linguistic study. Brain and Language, 32, and language-impaired Spanish-speaking children.
233–252. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 596–603.
Lewis, K. E. (1995). Do SSI-3 scores adequately reflect Roseberry-McKibbin, C. A., & Eicholtz, G. E. (1994).
observations of stuttering behavior? American Journal of Serving children with limited English proficiency in the
Speech-Language Pathology, 4, 46–48.

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1409

schools: A national survey. Language, Speech, and Hearing mental language impairment. Journal of Speech and
Services in Schools, 25, 156–164. Hearing Disorders, 54, 287–295.
Salvesen, K. A., & Undheim J. O. (1994) Screening for Toronto, A. S. (1976). Developmental assessment of
learning disabilities with teacher rating scales. Journal of Spanish grammar. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Learning Disabilities, 27, 60–66. Disorders, 41, 150–171.
Skoczylas, R. V. (1971). Home Bilingual Usage Estimate. Werner, E. O., & Kresheck, J. D. (1983). Structured
Gilroy, CA: Author. Photographic Expressive Language Test–II. DeKalb, IL:
Steckol, K. F., & Leonard, L. B. (1979). The use of Janelle.
grammatical morphemes by normal and language- Werner, E. O., & Kresheck, J. D. (1989). Spanish Struc-
impaired children. Journal of Communication Disorders, tured Photographic Expressive Language Test–II. DeKalb,
12, 291–301. IL: Janelle.
Swisher, L., Restrepo, M. A., Plante, E., & Lowell, S. Wilkinson, C. Y., & Ortiz, A. A. (1986). Reevaluation of
(1995). Effect of implicit and explicit “rule” presentation learning disabled Hispanic students: Changes after three
on bound-morpheme generalization in specific language years. Bilingual Special Education Newsletter, 5, 1–6.
impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38,
168–173.
Received August 19, 1997
Tallal, P., Ross, R., & Curtiss, S. (1989). Familial aggrega-
tion in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Accepted April 13, 1998
and Hearing Disorders, 54, 167–173. Contact author: M. Adelaida Restrepo, PhD, Department of
Taylor, O. L., & Payne, K. T. (1983). Culturally valid Communication Sciences and Disorders, 557 Aderhold
testing. Topics of Language Disorders, 3, 8–20. Hall, The University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602. Email:
arestrep@coe.uga.edu
Tomblin, J. B. (1989). Familial concentration of develop-

Appendix A. Questionnaire for teachers about the child’s language at home and at school.

Name of the child __________________________ School/grade _______________________


Age of the child ___________________________ Teacher ______________________

Use refers to how much the child uses each language. Give English Spanish
information on each language, English and Spanish. Mark for Expressive Language Use Prof Use Prof
Use:
1. Speaks with you in class 012 012 012 012
0 - never uses the indicated language (Expressive Language),
2. Speaks with aides 012 012 012 012
hears it very little (Receptive Language)
3. Speaks with best friend 012 012 012 012
1 - uses the indicated language a little (Expressive Language),
4. Speaks with classmates 012 012 012 012
hears it sometimes (Receptive Language)
5. Speaks outside class 012 012 012 012
2 - uses the indicated language most of the time (Expressive
Language), hears it most of the time (Receptive Language) 6. Speaks with parents 012 012 012 012
7. Speaks with brothers/sists 012 012 012 012
Proficiency (Prof) refers to how well the child speaks each
language. Give information on each language. Mark for Prof: 8. Speaks with other family 012 012 012 012
0 - cannot speak the indicated language has a few words or 9. Speaks it in a program after 012 012 012 012
phrases (Expressive Language), cannot produce sentences, school or at the babysitter
only understands a few words (Receptive Language)
Receptive Language
1 - limited proficiency with grammatical errors, limited
1. Listens to you in class 012 012 012 012
vocabulary (Expressive Language), understands the
general idea of what is being said (Receptive Language) 2. Listens to aides 012 012 012 012
2 - good proficiency with few grammatical errors, good 3. Listens to best friend 012 012 012 012
vocabulary (Expressive Language), understands most of 4. Listens to classmates 012 012 012 012
what is said (Receptive Language) 5. Listens outside class 012 012 012 012
6. Listens to parents 012 012 012 012
7. Listens to brothers/sists 012 012 012 012
8. Listens to other family 012 012 012 012
9. Listens to in a program after 012 012 012 012
school or at the babysitter

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


1410 JSLHR, Volume 41, 1398–1411, December 1998

Do you think the child has language Teacher’s Comments


problems? YES/NO
Do you think the child has academic
or learning problems? YES/NO
Do you think the child has behavioral
or social problems? YES/NO
Do you think the child has physical
problems? YES/NO

Appendix B. Translation of the parent interview.

Today’s date: ______ Code :____ Date of Birth __/__/__ Age of the child: ________
Child’s name: _____________ Name of the informant ________ Relation to the child: ____________________

Parental report of the child’s speech or 13. Does your child have difficulty finding the
exact words to express him/herself? yes no
language problems
14. Does your child have difficulty explaining or
1. In comparison with other children of the same
describing things? yes no
age, do you think that your child has problems
expressing him/herself or being understood? yes no 15. Is it difficult for your child to tell you what he/
she did during the day? yes no
2. In comparison with children of the same age, do
you think that your child has speech problems? yes no 16. Is your child frustrated because he/she can not
talk well? yes no
3. Do your family or friends think that your child is
delayed in language? yes no 17. Do you or your child’s siblings have to repeat
what you say to him or her with more frequency
4. For his/her age or in comparison with other
than to other children? yes no
children, does your child have difficulty
producing correct phrases? yes no 18. Do you have to repeat questions or directions
to your child more than to other children? yes no
5. Do your family or friends think that your child
is difficult to understand? yes no 19. Does your child understand most of what he/
she is told? yes no
6. For his/her age, does your child produce very
short phrases? yes no 20. Do you think that your child has difficulty
learning new words? yes no
7. Do you think that your child has problems with
his/her grammar? yes no 21. In comparison with children the same age, is it
difficult for your child to learn new ideas? yes no
8. When your child talks about the same person,
does he/she have difficulty using the correct 22. In comparison with children the same age,
pronoun like he, she, they all the time in his/her does your child have a very low or limited
conversation? yes no vocabulary? yes no
9. When your child talks about something that 23. Do you think that your child has a learning
happened, does he/she have difficulty problem? yes no
explaining when this happened or uses words 24. Does your child have dyslexia? yes no
in different times? For example, talking about 25. For his/her age, does your child have difficulty
yesterday the child say “falls” instead of “fell” yes no paying attention for a long period? yes no
10. Does you child use correct phrases almost all 26. Is your child hyperactive? yes no
the time? yes no
27. Does your child have difficulty attending to an
11. When your child talks, does he/she have activity or game? yes no
difficulty differentiating whether he/she is
28. For his/her age, does your child have difficulty
talking about a man or a woman? yes no
pronouncing words? yes no
12. In comparison with other children of the same
29. Is your child’s pronunciation easy to
age, does your child use many words that are
understand? yes no
too general and not descriptive such as this,
that, thing? yes no

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research


Restrepo: Identifiers of Spanish-Speaking Children With LI 1411

History of speech and language problems in the family


Has or had any of the child’s (brothers, sisters) any of the following?
bro/sist father mother par/fath par/moth
30. normal language development yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
31. problems of attention or hyperactivity yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
32. difficulties in school or learning yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
33. dyslexia or a problem learning to read yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
34. speech or pronunciation problems yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
35. language problems, like in phrases, words, and
grammar yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
36. special education classes yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
37. speech and language therapy yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
38. program for children with speech, language or
learning problems yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
39. omits words or parts of words in his/her phrases
after age 3 yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
40. problems producing correct sentences yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
41. problems making his/herself understood yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
42. problems expressing ideas with words yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
43. problems following directions yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
44. problems understanding questions yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
45. problems understanding what he/she is told yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
46. problems producing certain sounds yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
47. problems reading or learning to read yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
48. stuttering after 4 years of age yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
49. difficulty learning English yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
50. repeated one or more grades yes no yes no yes no yes no

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research

You might also like