courses on addiction.
/
Cambridge Fundamentals of Neuroscience in Psychology
neuroscience, these books will serve as ideal primers for students and other
/
The Neuroscience of
Addiction
/
University Printing House, Cambridge CB2 8BS, United Kingdom
One Liberty Plaza, 20th Floor, New York, NY 10006, USA
477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia
314 –321, 3rd Floor, Plot 3, Splendor Forum, Jasola District Centre,
New Delhi – 110025, India
79 Anson Road, #06– 04/06, Singapore 079906
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781107127982
DOI: 10.1017/9781316412640
© Francesca Mapua Filbey 2019
This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception
and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements,
no reproduction of any part may take place without the written
permission of Cambridge University Press.
First published 2019
Printed in the United Kingdom by TJ International Ltd, Padstow Cornwall
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library.
/
To David: thank you for your love and support. To Colin: thank
/
/
Table of Contents
1 What is Addiction? 1
Learning Objectives 1
Introduction 1
The Phenomenology of Substance Use Disorders 4
The Demography of Addiction 5
The Stigma of Addiction 5
Summary Points 14
Review Questions 14
Further Reading 14
Spotlight 15
References 16
Biochemical Imaging 27
Limitations of Neuroimaging Research 28
Summary Points 29
Review Questions 29
Further Reading 29
Spotlight 1 30
Spotlight 2 32
References 32
/
viii Table of Contents
5 Intoxication 64
Learning Objectives 64
Introduction 64
Drug Pharmacodynamics 66
Actions of Addictive Drugs 66
Brain Mechanisms of Intoxication: Evidence From Neuroimaging
Pharmacological Studies 68
Modulators of Intoxication: Challenges in Human Research 73
Summary Points 75
Review Questions 76
Further Reading 76
/
Table of Contents ix
Spotlight 76
References 78
6 Withdrawal 81
Learning Objectives 81
Introduction 81
Summary Points 91
Review Questions 92
Further Reading 92
Spotlight 1 93
Spotlight 2 94
References 94
7 Craving 98
Learning Objectives 98
Introduction 98
Cue-Elicited Craving Paradigms and Associated
Neural Mechanisms 99
8 Impulsivity 114
Learning Objectives 114
Introduction 114
Neuropharmacology of Impulsivity 116
/
x Table of Contents
10 Conclusions 148
Learning Objectives 148
Introduction 148
Risk Factors Inform Better Prevention and Intervention 149
Addiction Endophenotypes 150
Sex Differences in Addiction 155
The Question of Causality 156
General Conclusions 157
Summary Points 159
Review Questions 159
Further Reading 160
Spotlight 1 161
Spotlight 2 162
References 162
Glossary 165
Index 173
Color plate section found between pages 172 and 173
/
List of Plates
/
List of Figures
/
List of Figures xiii
/
xiv List of Figures
/
List of Figures xv
/
List of Tables
/
Preface
brain mechanisms for diseases during the “Decade of the Brain ” in the
1990s has led to greater attention on the role of the brain in addiction.
fi
Neuroscience research has made signi cant progress toward our under-
which, in turn, has helped de-stigmatize addiction and get help to those
who need it. However, to date, this information remains largely con ned fi
to scienti fic outlets resulting in a lag in dissemination to students and the
general community. This may contribute to the lack of emphasis on
the prevalence of addiction and its high co-morbidity with other diseases
and disorders. The need for this book is further highlighted by the recent
science of addiction that caters to both students and the general public.
Approach
This book has been written to fill a void in the areas of behavioral
tools to study addiction, rather than to explain it. It is also written for a
fic inquiry and public interest in the addicted brain have grown, so too
ti
has the need for a comprehensive and accessible textbook that communi-
cates extant neuroscience research on this topic. This book will serve as
students and educated lay people are the target audience for this book. It
psychology, with interest in the adolescent brain. This book can serve
/
xviii Preface
Features
Each chapter contains comprehensive figures that best illustrate con-
cepts or challenging topics. Each figure is referred to in the
/
Preface xix
corresponding text. Summary Points are provided at the end each chap-
ter to help focus the reader on the most important points and to reinforce
the gist of each chapter. Review Questions are also provided to chal-
lenge the reader’ s understanding of each chapter. These questions are
related to the important points of the chapter. The chapters also have a
Further Reading section that directs readers to supplemental materials
that could facilitate further learning. The Spotlight sections take current
issues and integrate these timely topics with constructs from the chapter.
These spotlights help put constructs into a real-world perspective that is
aimed to stimulate critical thinking in readers.
/
/
C H A P T ER O N E
What is Addiction?
Learning Objectives
• fi
Be able to describe the clinical de nition of addiction.
Introduction
According to the World Health Organization, there were 2 billion alco-
hol users, 1.3 billion smokers and 185 million drug users in the year 2000.
This figure contributed to 12.4% of all deaths worldwide that year.
Addiction does not discriminate. It affects both sexes, all races and all
ages. However, the highest rate of addiction is in the adolescent to
emerging-adult populations (ages 12 29 years) (UNODC, 2012). The
high rate of substance use initiation during this period has the potential
–
to change the tone for how the brain develops, given that this age period
is when the brain undergoes critical maturation processes. Figure 1.1
illustrates brain development as a process consisting of gray matter
reductions and cortical thinning that is then followed by increased white
matter volume, connectivity and organization through adolescence and
young adulthood (Giorgio , 2010; Gogtay , 2004; Hasan ,
2007; Lebel , 2010; Shaw , 2008).
et al. et al. et al.
pathological
/
2 What is Addiction?
HA B
J I C
K E F
N D
Q M G
P L
O
5 G
1.0
C 0.9
B Age
0.8
A
H 0.7
I
J 0.6
rettam ya rG
20 0.5
K
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
20 years of age. (From Gogtay et al., 2004. © 2004 National Academy of Sciences, USA.)
fi
(A black and white version of this gure will appear in some formats. For the color version,
/
Introduction 3
Table 1.1 2017 Schedule of Drugs according to the US DEA. The DEA
fi
classi es drugs into five distinct categories or schedules depending on the
drug ’ s acceptable medical use and the drug ’s abuse or dependency potential.
Schedule I drugs have the highest potential for abuse and the potential to
Drug fi fi
Classi cation meaning (de ned by the Drugs, substances,
Cannabis
Ecstasy
Methaqualone
Peyote
Methamphetamine
Methadone
Dilaudid
Demerol
OxyContin
Fentanyl
Dexedrine
Adderall
Ritalin
Anabolic Steroids
Testosterone
Valium
Ativan
Ambien
Tramadol
/
4 What is Addiction?
/
The Stigma of Addiction 5
/
6 What is Addiction?
Social solutions have also largely failed to remediate those who are
addicted, primarily because they do not address the underlying etiology.
Because of the stigma of addiction, those with addiction: 1) do not seek
the necessary treatment; 2) do not receive the necessary social support;
or 3) receive largely ineffective treatment that does not address the
underlying mechanisms of addiction.
The Diagnosis of Addiction
The clinical diagnoses of mental health disorders rely on classification
systems that have been developed over centuries. These classification
systems differ based on their purpose for classification (clinical, research
or administrative objectives), as well as emphasis on discerning features
of diagnostic categories (phenomenology versus etiology). The two most
prominent systems are the
(DSM) and the (ICD).
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
fi
The ICD, developed by the World Health Organization, published the
Disorders International Classi cation of Diseases
fi rst section for mental health disorders in 1949 within its 6th edition.
Based on this, the American Psychiatric Association Committee on
Nomenclature and Statistics developed the 1st edition of the DSM in
1952. The DSM then became the first official manual of mental disorders
to focus on clinical use. The DSM-5, which was published in 2013 and
implemented in 2014, is the most recent version.
In terms of the diagnosis of addiction, the DSM-5 classifies the diag-
nosis of SUDs based on evidence of impaired control, social impairment,
risky use and pharmacological criteria. The major modification from
DSM-IV to DSM-5 is the combination of the categorical symptoms in
DSM-IV into a continuum in DSM-5 (Table 1.2). Thus, rather than
dimorphic diagnoses of substance abuse and dependence, a unidimen-
sional diagnosis of SUD is evaluated on a scale from mild to severe
depending on the number of symptoms presented. This decision was
/
The Diagnosis of Addiction 7
fi
Table 1.2 Modi cations to addiction diagnosis from DSM-IV to DSM-5.
DSM-IV DSM-IV
Tolerance X X
Withdrawal X X
of use
fi
ful ll important role obligations
/
8 What is Addiction?
Computer
Lever
(a)
Drug
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
Saline
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
models, animals continuously perform an action (e.g. pressing a lever) in order to receive a
/
A Brain Disease Model of Addiction 9
/
10 What is Addiction?
/
A Brain Disease Model of Addiction 11
GLU
5-HT
Amygdala
GLU
Opioid DA
GABA Prefrontal
DA
GABA cortex
Opioid
GABA
DA
GABA
DA Nucleus accumbens
Locus NE
5-HT ceruleus
Opioid
Raphe nucleus
Opioids, ethanol,
Ventral barbiturates, benzodiazepines
tegmental area
Figure 1.3 Sites of action of various drugs on the mesocorticolimbic reward system.
Although the pathways primary neurotransmitter is dopamine (DA), this circuit is innervated
’
/
12 What is Addiction?
/
Non-Drug Addictions 13
2010). These studies suggest that the reward system is responsible for
neural adaptations as a consequence of these compulsive behaviors.
Pitchers (2010) reported neural adaptations in the form of increased
et al.
/
14 What is Addiction?
Summary Points
• Both the animal and human literature support the notion that addiction is a
brain disorder stemming from the positive reinforcing mechanisms in the
mesolimbic pathway.
• Chronic use leads to neuroadaptation, primarily within this pathway, that
results in the behavioral symptoms of addiction.
• These adaptations also lead to changes in other brain systems, including
the stress system.
• Through this cycle, addiction becomes a chronic, relapsing disorder.
• More recently, non-drug addictions have been identified, with evidence
showing parallel neural mechanisms to those of substance-based
addictions.
Review Questions
•How are the five categories in the DEA s classification of substances
’
delineated?
•What are the current (i.e. DSM-5) primary symptoms of SUD according to
clinical guidelines?
•What are the seminal animal studies that have helped shape our under-
standing of addiction as a brain disease?
•How is dopamine critical in the processes related to addiction?
Further Reading
Babor, T. F. (2011). Substance, not semantics, is the issue: comments on the
proposed addiction criteria for DSM-V. Addiction, 106(5), 870–872; discus-
sion 895–877. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03313.x
Barnett, A. I., Hall, W., Fry, C. L., Dilkes-Frayne, E. & Carter, A. (2017). Drug
and alcohol treatment providers’ views about the disease model of
/
Spotlight 15
Burrows, T., Kay-Lambkin, F., Pursey, K., Skinner, J. & Dayas, C. (2018). Food
addiction and associations with mental health symptoms: a systematic
review with meta-analysis. J Hum Nutr Diet , 31(4), 544–572. doi:10.1111/
jhn.12532
Diana, M. (2011). The dopamine hypothesis of drug addiction and its poten-
tial therapeutic value. Front Psychiatry , 2, 64. doi:10.3389/
fpsyt.2011.00064
Grant, J. E. & Chamberlain, S. R. (2016). Expanding the definition of addic-
tion: DSM-5 vs. ICD-11. CNS Spectr, 21(4), 300–303. doi:10.1017/
S1092852916000183
Hou, H., Wang, C., Jia, S., Hu, S. & Tian, M. (2014). Brain dopaminergic
system changes in drug addiction: a review of positron emission tomog-
raphy findings. Neurosci Bull, 30(5), 765–776. doi:10.1007/s12264-014-
1469-5
Lewis, M. D. (2011). Dopamine and the neural“now”: essay and review of
addiction: a disorder of choice. Perspect Psychol Sci, 6(2), 150–155.
doi:10.1177/1745691611400235
Singer, M. (2012). Anthropology and addiction: an historical review.Addic-
tion, 107(10), 1747
–1755. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03879.x
Spotlight
The magic in the mushrooms remains unknown
A 2015 report published by theCanadian Medical Association Journalpointed
to several small studies demonstrating that psychedelic drugs such as LSD and
3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) may be effective in reducing
symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) anxiety, as well as addic-
tion (Tupper et al., 2015). A small 2014 Swiss study, for instance, found that
people with terminal illness treated with a combination of LSD and psycho-
therapy had lower rates of anxiety (Gasser et al., 2014). A US study involving a
small group of patients also found that MDMA, more commonly known as
ecstasy, can greatly reduce symptoms of PTSD. However, many caution of the
negative side effects of psychedelics on mood and cognition, as well as
sensory processing and perception. For instance, LSD, psilocybin (obtained
from magic mushrooms) and mescaline can cause psychosis and/or
hallucinations (Figure S1.1).
/
16 What is Addiction?
fi
Since the 1950s, the therapeutic bene ts of psychedelics have always been
argued. However, how psychedelics affect the brain remains unknown. Fur-
fi
be used in addition to the risks and bene ts associated.
Stephen Kish, who studies the use of ecstasy in the treatment of PTSD,
interactions with their therapists (Kish et al., 2010). However, he also notes
The biggest concern in these studies is the risk that people would self-
medicate with psychedelic drugs. The fact remains that the forms available on
the street are unlikely to be pure and could lead to serious health problems
References
. Vital
Statistics for U.S. Adults: National Health Interview Survey, 2012
Health Statistics, Series 10, No. 260. Hyattsville, MD: National Center
For Health Statistics.
Bowman, E. M., Aigner, T. G. & Richmond, B. J. (1996). Neural signals in
the monkey ventral striatum related to motivation for juice and
/
References 17
doi:10.1056/NEJMra023160
Carelli, R. M. (2002). The nucleus accumbens and reward:
neurophysiological investigations in behaving animals. Behav Cogn
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.014
Filbey, F. M., Ray, L., Smolen, A., (2008). Differential neural response
et al.
/
18 What is Addiction?
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., (2004). Dynamic mapping of human
et al.
appi.ajp.163.2.303
Hasan, K. M., Sankar, A., Halphen, C., (2007). Development and
et al.
1735 1739.
–
ADM.0b013e31819aa621
Holden, C. (2001). Behavioral addictions: do they exist?
‘ ’ , 294(5544),
Science
0443.2008.02297.x
Kim, J. E., Son, J. W., Choi, W. H., (2014). Neural responses to various
et al.
S0006-3223(01)01079-4
Kish, S. J., Lerch, J., Furukawa, Y., (2010). Decreased cerebral cortical
et al.
j.1360-0443.2006.01586.x
/
References 19
doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.491883
Kuhn, S. & Gallinat, J. (2014). Brain structure and functional connectivity
associated with pornography consumption: the brain on porn. JAMA
Psychiatry, 71(7), 827 834. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.93
–
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.072
Leith, N. J. & Kuczenski, R. (1982). Two dissociable components of
behavioral sensitization following repeated amphetamine
administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 76(4), 310 315.
–
YCO.0b013e32831575d9
Pitchers, K. K., Balfour, M. E., Lehman, M. N.,et al. (2010). Neuroplasticity
in the mesolimbic system induced by natural reward and subsequent
reward abstinence. Biol Psychiatry 67(9), 872 879. doi:10.1016/j.
–
biopsych.2009.09.036
Probst, C. C. & van Eimeren, T. (2013). The functional anatomy of impulse
control disorders. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep, 13(10), 386. doi:10.1007/
s11910-013-0386-8
Robinson, D. L. & Carelli, R. M. (2008). Distinct subsets of nucleus
accumbens neurons encode operant responding for ethanol versus
water. Eur J Neurosci, 28(9), 1887 1894. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-
–
9568.2008.06464.x
Shaw, P., Kabani, N. J., Lerch, J. P., et al. (2008). Neurodevelopmental
trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci , 28(14),
3586 3594. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008
–
UNODC. (2012). World Drug Report 2012. Vienna, Austria: United Nations.
Available at: www.unodc.org/documents/data-and-analysis/WDR2012/
WDR_2012_web_small.pdf
Volkow, N. D. & O Brien, C. P. (2007). Issues for DSM-V: should obesity be
’
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.5.708
/
20 What is Addiction?
/
C H A P T ER TW O
Human Neuroscience Approaches Toward the
Understanding of Addiction
Learning Objectives
• Be able to identify current neuroimaging techniques used to study addic-
tion in humans.
technique.
Introduction
Our understanding of addiction as a brain disease can be attributed
And what kind of information do they provide above and beyond what
addiction?
/
22 Human Neuroscience Approaches
inferred from these patterns that reflect neuronal factors and activities
including ionic gradients from neuronal membranes, and excitatory and
inhibitory post-synaptic potentials. EEG recordings are measured
by electrodes placed either extracranially (on the scalp) or intracranially
(via surgical placement directly on the surface of the brain) to record
the electrical voltage fluctuations generated by the brain. Currently, a
minimum of twenty-one electrodes is considered ideal for a
clinical study, although higher density array EEG systems of up to
256 electrodes are available. Currently, while electrical signals provide
high temporal resolution data regarding brain activity, the poor spatial
resolution of the two-dimensional EEG representation of a three-
dimensional brain poses limitations in interpretation of the data. Thus,
source localization is limited in extracranial EEG recordings. Further-
more, EEG recordings are the result of synchronous activity from large
samples of neurons, which conceals small activity or activity from smaller
samples of neurons.
The net effect of electrophysiological activity in the brain also gener-
ates a magnetic field that can be detected. Magnetoencephalography
(MEG) is a technique that measures the magnetic fields emitted by
electrical activity in the brain (Figure 2.1). The magnetic field emitted
by neurons passes through brain tissue and the skull with little distortion,
thereby generating better spatial localization relative to EEG, as the
scalp distorts magnetic fields less than electrical signals. Although the
brain s magnetic field is 10 15 Tesla (T; i.e. 100 million times smaller than
’
–
/
Measuring the Brain’s Electrical Activity 23
/
24 Human Neuroscience Approaches
Precess
ion
Applied
magnetic field
Figure 2.2 Mechanisms of MRI. The MRI signal stems from the circling or precession of the
fi
spinning protons around the axis of the magnetic eld (center arrow).
/
Visualizing the Brain’s Structure and Function 25
(From https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:US_Navy_030819-N-9593R-228_Civilian_technician,_
Jose_Araujo_watches_as_a_patient_goes_through_a_Magnetic_Resonance_Imaging,_(MRI)_
DTI indexes can quantify the length of fi ber bundles (e.g. tractography),
as well as the directionality (e.g. fractional anisotropy) and diffusivity (e.g.
trace) of water molecules through brain tissue. High fractional anisotropy
and low diffusivity re flect healthy white matter (Figure 2.4).
In addition to structural information, MRI also enables functional
imaging that offers dynamic physiological information of the brain. Func-
tional MRI (fMRI) paradigms provide near real-time information
regarding task-induced as well as resting baseline state neural activation.
The fundamental element of fMRI scans is the blood oxygenated level-
dependent (BOLD) signal. Originally discovered by Seiji Ogawa in 1990,
the BOLD signal refers to the in vivo change of blood oxygenation that
leads to variation in the magnetic signal detectable with MRI. The BOLD
signal therefore provides information on brain regions that have
increased oxygenation as the result of being active and requiring more
energy. It is therefore anindirect measure of neural function and relies on
assumptions regarding underlying neuronal activity. fMRI also includes
perfusion techniques (with or without endogenous or exogenous con-
trast), regional cerebral blood flow and cerebrospinal fl uid pulsation
measurements, as well as phase contrastflow measurements.
/
26 Human Neuroscience Approaches
l1
l2 l1 l2
l3
l3
Isotropic Anisotropic
Figure 2.4 Gray matter has predominantly isotropic (soccer ball-shaped) water
diffusion, while dense white matter tracks have highly anisotropic (rugby ball-shaped)
fi
diffusion of water pointing in the direction of the ber bundle. The measure most
measure gives a value of between 0 and 1 to indicate the fraction of diffusion that is in the
directions. Other measures that can be used are axial diffusivity (AD), radial diffusivity
(RD) and mean diffusivity (MD). (From Whitfordet al., 2011.) (A black and white version of
this figure will appear in some formats. For the color version, please refer to the plate
section.)
/
Biochemical Imaging 27
Biochemical Imaging
0 0
5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 0.0 5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 0.0
15000 15000
Alcohol HIV+Alcohol
NAA NAA
10000 10000
Cr Cr
Cho Cho
5000 5000
0 0
5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 0.0 5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 0.0
Figure 2.5 MRS image of a 34-year-old man with human immunodeciency virus (HIV) fi
infection and alcohol dependence. The brain images show the parietal-occipital cortical
fi
region (in white) sampled by MRS for metabolite quantication. The graphs below show the
MRS spectra of various brain metabolites in people with HIV infection alone, alcoholism
alone, co-morbid HIV infection and alcoholism, and control subjects with neither condition.
The peak representing the metaboliteN -acetylaspartate (NAA) shows a signi cant de cit in fi fi
the HIV plus alcoholism group compared with the other groups. Cho, choline; Cr, creatine.
(From Rosenbloom et al., 2010. © 2010 Alcohol Research: Current Reviews, USA.)
/
28 Human Neuroscience Approaches
/
Further Reading 29
reported brain volume differences between users and non-users may not
alteration that puts one at risk for cannabis use. In short, much work
remains to be done in this area, but the existing literature points to a very
Summary Points
• Advancements in neuroscience techniques have paved the way for the
understanding that addiction is a brain disorder.
• Neuroimaging techniques provide the ability to measure the electrophysio-
logical, functional, structural and biochemical composition of the brain.
• Brain imaging techniques provide evidence for associations between brain
structure and function and behavioral symptoms of addiction.
• Understanding neural mechanisms underlying behavioral symptoms of
addiction is important in identifying potential targets for therapeutic
interventions.
• Future research should focus on determining the exact relationship
between changes in the brain and exposure to substances.
Review Questions
• How have neuroimaging advancements informed our understanding of
addiction?
• How is EEG different from MEG?
• What are the various techniques that can be used during MRI?
• What can PET tell us that is different from MRI?
• What chemicals can we measure using MRS?
• What is the definition of “resting state ” in neuroimaging terms?
• What are the limitations should we keep in mind when interpreting neuro-
imaging findings?
Further Reading
Garrison, K. A. & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Neuroimaging and biomarkers in
addiction treatment. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 16(12), 513. doi:10.1007/
s11920-014-0513-5
/
30 Human Neuroscience Approaches
Liu, P., Lu, H., Filbey, F. M.,et al. (2014). MRI assessment of cerebral oxygen
metabolism in cocaine-addicted individuals: hypoactivity and dose depend-
ence. NMR Biomed, 27(6), 726–732. doi:10.1002/nbm.3114
McClure, S. M. & Bickel, W. K. (2014). A dual-systems perspective on addic-
tion: contributions from neuroimaging and cognitive training.Ann N Y
Acad Sci, 1327(1), 62–78. doi:10.1111/nyas.12561
Stapleton, J., West, R., Marsden, J. & Hall, W. (2012). Research methods and
statistical techniques in addiction. Addiction, 107(10), 1724–1725.
doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03969.x
Yalachkov, Y., Kaiser, J. & Naumer, M. J. (2012). Functional neuroimaging studies
in addiction: multisensory drug stimuli and neural cue reactivity. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev, 36(2), 825–835. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.004
Spotlight 1
Love on the brain
Advancements in neuroimaging technology have demonstrated that the brain
functions via well-orchestrated, interconnected networks of brain regions.
These intrinsically linked brain networks simultaneously activate when
we are “at rest ” or not performing a specific task. There is growing
research in how these “resting-state ” networks may be related to individ-
ual factors.
/
Spotlight 1 31
Figure S2.1 What does 45 years of love look like in the brain?
Research has widely accepted that feelings of love are rewarding and are
as our feelings of love changes, so do the brain regions that underlie these
within the reward, motivation and emotion regulation network (dorsal anter-
ior cingulate cortex, insula, caudate, amygdala and nucleus accumbens) was
pared with those who were not in love (ended romantic relationship recently/
/
32 Human Neuroscience Approaches
Spotlight 2
be used to support the individual in order to prevent (or delay) the potential
make the ability to predict and prevent disorders a reality. Recently, the
National Institutes of Health (NIH) funded a historic study called the Adoles-
that has the ultimate goal of using advanced brain imaging to map brain
determine how these factors may be associated with how the brain develops.
Children from this study will be tested yearly over a 10-year period to identify
risk factors and protective factors, mental health issues and addiction. The
ability to predict will ultimately lead to better outcomes for our children.
Figure S2.2 fi
Associating the brain with behavior began with the eld of phrenology.
From www.pexels.com/photo/photo-of-head-bust-print-artwork-724994/.
References
/
References 33
/
C H A PT E R T H R E E
Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
Learning Objectives
• “
Be able to explain the difference between wanting ” and “liking” in the
Introduction
The National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) in the USA defines drug
addiction as a “chronic, often relapsing brain disease.” In this vein,
multiple models of addiction have been proposed to explain the link
between brain mechanisms and observable behavioral symptoms of
addiction. These conceptual or theoretical models have advanced neuro-
science research in addiction by providing a working framework that can
be tested and elaborated upon. This chapter will describe some of the
predominant models including the incentive-sensitization theory, the
allostasis theory, the impaired response inhibition and salience attribu-
tion (iRISA) syndrome model and the cue-elicited craving model.
Initial theories on substance use assumed that the pleasurable effects
of the drug instigate drug consumption, and that dependence develops
out of a persistent drive to obtain these positive effects. These initial
theories, however, failed to incorporate other aspects that occur during
the progression of the disorder, such as tolerance and withdrawal. The
idea of withdrawal during addiction suggests a shift in the progression of
the disorder from one that is initially driven by positive incentives to one
that is negatively reinforced, such as to avoid the withdrawal symptoms
following cessation of drug use. Such a shift would suggest neural
/
The Incentive-Sensitization Theory 35
/
36 Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
process is complicated by the fact that, with repeated drug use, tolerance
develops whereby greater amounts of the drug are needed in order to
achieve the same hedonic state. Interestingly, however, according to the
opponent-process theory, tolerance is not the result of habituation to the
positive effects but rather a sensitization to the negative effects. Thus,
the opponent-process theory suggests that repeated drug use leads to
larger effects of the opponent process, while the hedonic state becomes
smaller. Continued drug use is therefore motivated by the need to avoid
these negative states (see Chapter 6).
Koob & Le Moal (1997) extended this model to incorporate the
neurobiological adaptations that underlie this dysregulation in homeo-
stasis (Figure 3.1). They described three stages of addiction: 1) binge
intoxication, followed by 2) the withdrawal/negative affect, and then by
3) preoccupation/anticipation that would be likely to resume the cycle.
/
The Allostatic Model: Dysregulation in Homeostasis 37
cu p ati on
eoc /
Pr
i cip a ti on
an t
Persistent desire
Preoccupation with
Larger amounts taken
obtaining persistent physical/
ADDICTION
t iW
n
no
ge
eg
h
it
i ta
rd
ac
n
ev
iB
ix
w
o
l/
a
a
tn
ff
c
e
i
t
Tolerance
withdrawal
or recreational activities
/
38 Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
In 2002, Goldstein and Volkow proposed one of the first models that
integrate the behavioral, cognitive and emotional features in existing
models of addiction. Their model, the iRISA syndrome model, is based
predominantly on neuroimaging findings in cocaine-using populations
and highlights the important role of the PFC neurocircuitry in moder-
ating clusters of interconnected behaviors (Figure 3.2): drug intoxica-
tion, drug craving, compulsive drug administration and drug
withdrawal. The specific PFC regions include dorsal PFC subregions
(the dorsolateral PFC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and inferior
frontal gyrus) that are involved in higher-order control or “ cold ” pro-
cesses. Ventral PFC subregions (the medial orbitofrontal cortex, ven-
tromedial PFC and rostroventral anterior cingulate cortex) are
involved in more automatic, emotion-related processes or “hot ”
processes.
The iRISA model proposes that drug intoxication, which is tradition-
ally viewed as the result of neural changes in subcortical regions, is also
accompanied by increased dopamine levels in frontal regions as well as
activation in the PFC and anterior cingulate gyrus. Furthermore, the
patterns of activation are associated with the subjective perception of
intoxication, the reinforcing effects of the drug or enhanced mood. Drug
craving – a conditioned response to drugs that involves memory pro-
cesses – is also suggested to be associated with activation in the orbito-
frontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Greater activation in these
regions has been demonstrated across different substance-abusing popu-
lations and via different drug cue modalities (e.g. visual, tactile, gusta-
tory). Similar to intoxication, activation in these prefrontal regions also
correlates with self-reports of craving. Compulsive drug administration
that occurs during the shift from the hedonic state to the negative state
/
The iRISA Syndrome Model 39
Dorsal PFC
(“cold” functions)
Ventral PFC
(“hot” functions)
Figure 3.2 The iRISA model depicting the interactions between the PFC and subcortical
regions in drug users and non-users. Drug-related neuropsychological functions (e.g.
incentive salience, drug wanting, attention bias and drug seeking) that are regulated by
these subregions are represented by darker shades and non-drug-related functions (e.g.
sustained effort) are represented by lighter shades. Thick arrows depict increases in input
and the sizes of circles demonstrate the balance between drug- and non-drug-related
functions.
(Adapted from Goldstein & Volkow, 2011.)
/
40 Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
2008).
The Future of Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
/
The Future of Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction 41
Daily smoking
Abstainers
Any illicit
Cannabis
Lowest SES
Ecstasy
Highest SES
Meth/amphetamines
Cocaine
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
Percentage
Figure 3.3 Daily smoking, risky alcohol consumption and illicit drug use by people with the
lowest and highest socioeconomic status (SES), in Australians aged 14 years or older, in
2013.
/
42 Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
Summary Points
Review Questions
Further Reading
Bickel, W. K., Mellis, A. M., Snider, S. E.,et al. (2018). 21st century neurobe-
havioral theories of decision making in addiction: review and evaluation.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 164, 4–21. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2017.09.009
/
Spotlight 43
Di Chiara, G., Bassareo, V., Fenu, S., (2004). Dopamine and drug
addiction: the nucleus accumbens shell connection. ,
et al.
Garcia Pardo, M. P., Roger Sanchez, C., de la Rubia Orti, J. E. & Aguilar Calpe,
–
Lewis, M. D. (2011). Dopamine and the neural now : essay and review of
–
Addiction –
Spotlight
Is addiction a moral failing?
An alarming report from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
(CDC) in 2016 stated that ninety-one Americans die from opioid over dose
every day. This figure is higher than deaths from car accidents or gun
homicides. In opioid addiction, which has now reached epidemic proportions
(i.e. six out of ten drug overdose deaths are due to opioids), 80% developed
their addiction after being prescribed opioid medication for pain (Figure S3.1).
In other words, in these cases, addiction began with a medical prescription. In
turn, the opioid epidemic in the USA has led to muchfinger pointing, with
blame put on pharmaceutical companies for creating and aggressively
marketing these highly addictive drugs and on physicians who have heavily
prescribed the drugs (perhaps not knowing the high risk of addiction).
However, the public health response to the opioid epidemic is unlike that of
past drug epidemics. Specifically, treatment rather than criminal justice
options are provided to those who have opioid addiction. This humane
/
44 Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
/
References 45
the approach taken in Poland under their“treat rather than punish” principle.
grams and the quality of life of those undergoing treatment, harm reduction
The response to the opioid epidemic in the USA can hopefully lead a
change in how addiction is addressed, i.e. by making sure that those with
we remove the stigma of addiction and accept that addiction can happen to
anyone.
References
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2014). National Drug Strategy
Household Survey detailed report: 2013. Drug statistics series no. 28.
Canberra, Australia: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.
Anagnostaras, S. G., Schallert, T. & Robinson, T. E. (2002). Memory
processes governing amphetamine-induced psychomotor sensitization.
Neuropsychopharmacology , 26(6), 703–715. doi:10.1016/S0893-133X
(01)00402-X
Boileau, I., Dagher, A., Leyton, M., (2006). Modeling sensitization to
et al.
/
46 Brain-Behavior Theories of Addiction
/
C H A P T ER F O U R
From the Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to
Systems
Learning Objectives
• fi
Be able to understand the notion of the reward de ciency syndrome.
Introduction
This chapter will describe the first ecological stage of the progression of
addiction: initial motivation to use drugs. This chapter will explain the
cliché that “drugs hijack the brain” by discussing the various neuroima-
ging studies that demonstrate this phenomenon.
/
48 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
(a)
Speaker
Signal lights
Pellet dispenser
Lever
Dispenser tube
Food cup
Electric grid
To shock
generator
(b) Suspending
elastic band
Lever
Lever press
activates
stimulator
Stimulator
Figure 4.1 Lever press (a) and intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) (b) are two examples of
experimental paradigms used to study reward and motivation in animals. (a) Animals learn
to press a lever to receive rewards (e.g. food, water, sexual mates, drugs). (b) In ICSS,
animals receive electrical stimulation directly into reward areas of the brain, without the
in fluence fi
of speci c incentives. These animal paradigms have implicated a role of the
of Behavior
/
Reward and Motivational Systems 49
Orbitofrontal cortex
Nucleus accumbens
Mesolimbic dopamine pathway
Mesocortical dopamine pathway
Ventral tegmental
area (VTA)
Figure 4.2 The brain’s reward system lies in the mesocorticolimbic pathway, which is
regulated by dopamine. This pathway has dopamine cell bodies in the ventral tegmental
area and projects to the nucleus accumbens and areas in the prefrontal cortex, particularly
/
50 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
Saline Amphetamine
Figure 4.3 Camera lucida drawings of medium spiny neurons in the shell (top) and core
subregions – the shell and the core. The shell is interconnected with the
hypothalamus and ventral tegmental area (VTA), while the core has
innervations with the anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex. An
interesting finding in animal studies was that different subsets of neurons
in the nucleus accumbens respond differentially to encoding “natural”
rewards such as water versus cocaine (Carelli , 2000). Given theet al.
/
Predicting Rewards 51
event occurs, projections from the VTA release dopamine, which trig-
gers a behavioral response to the motivational event. This process leads
to cellular changes that establish learned associations for highly desirable
stimuli. Over time, repeated exposure to the same motivational event no
longer leads to the same level of dopamine released in response to the
event; however, the conditioned stimuli predicting the event continue to
trigger the release of dopamine (see Chapter 7 for further details).
Unlike the shell of the nucleus accumbens, its core has projections to
PFC areas including the anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex.
These connections underlie the motivation for rewarding stimuli,
thereby contributing to response selection and adaptive learning. Studies
have illustrated that the magnitude of change in metabolic activity in
both the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices correlates with the
intensity of the self-reported cue-induced craving. Drug specifi city of
increased prefrontal activity is illustrated by studies demonstrating
reduced prefrontal activity during biologically relevant rewards, such as
sexually evocative cues and also during decision-making tasks that typic-
ally elicit a prefrontal response (Garavan et al., 2000). Thus, dysregula-
tion in the anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex is not only critical
for cue-elicited motivation but also in decision making (i.e. cognitive
control) over drug seeking (discussed in Chapter 8).
/
52 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
Dopamine transporter
blocked by cocaine
Dopamine
Transmitting
neuron
Dopamine receptor
Intensity of effect
Receiving Cocaine
neuron
Figure 4.4 The release of dopamine signals reward. This illustrates mechanisms by which
synaptic cleft.
Human research has also provided evidence for the important role of
dopamine during reward and motivation. These studies showed that
large and fast increases in dopamine levels that are longer in duration
and more intense than those induced by dopamine cell firing to other
salient events underlie the development of drug addiction. Higher and
longer dopamine release potentiates the threshold required for motiv-
ational events to activate dopamine neurons, thereby requiring more
potent stimuli to reach the prior levels of dopaminergic signaling.
Decreases in dopamine release and in dopamine D2 receptors in the
striatum also occur following drug use. For example, positron emission
tomography (PET) with the D2/3 dopamine receptor ligand antagonist
[ C]raclopride in combination with methylphenidate (a dopamine reup-
11
take inhibitor, the same as cocaine) showed that methamphetamine
abusers had 24% lower levels of dopamine transporters in the striatum
compared with people who never used the drug (Volkow , 2001).
These reductions in striatal extracellular dopamine levels are associated
et al.
with reduced activity of the orbitofrontal cortex and the cingulate gyrus.
/
Final Common Pathway: All Drugs Lead to One 53
11
Interestingly, PET [ C]raclopride studies have also shown that, in
response to drug-related stimuli (drug cues), these hypoactive prefrontal
regions become hyperactive proportionally to the subjective desire for
the drug or craving, and may be the mechanism by which “drugs hijack
the brain” (discussed further in Chapter 7). Specifically, dopamine
release was related to increased motivation, despite the absence of a
reward (Volkow et al., 2001).
So far, we have discussed how dopamine is critical for acute reward and
reinforced learning that leads to addiction. Although, in general, dysfunc-
tion of the dopaminergic circuitry may be the neural substrate for the
development and maintenance of addiction, an important note is that end-
stage addiction is primarily due to neural adaptations in glutamatergic
projections from the PFC to the nucleus accumbens. Alterations in excita-
tory input lead to a reduction in the capacity of the PFC to initiate
behaviors in response to natural rewards and to provide executive control
over drug seeking (lack of control or impulsivity is discussed further in
Chapter 8). The hyper-responsivity of the PFC to rewarding stimuli leads
to increased glutamatergic input in the nucleus accumbens, where excita-
tory synapses have a reduced capacity to regulate neurotransmission.
/
54 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
Nucleus
Ventral Prefrontal
accumbens
pallidum cortex
core
Ventral
Basolateral
tegmental
amygdala
area
Extended amygdala
Stress
Figure 4.5 According to Kalivas and Volkow (2005), the projection from the PFC to the
fi
nucleus accumbens core to the ventral pallidum is a nal common pathway for drug seeking
by increases in dopamine release (via stress, a drug-associated cue or the drug itself) in
the PFC.
/
Is Addiction a Reward Deficiency Syndrome? 55
/
56 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
/
Role of Memory Systems 57
(a) (b)
Palatable
food /FR 5
??
Lab chow /
free access
(c) (d)
Figure 4.6 Experiments on the effects of dopamine depletion on effort. In these studies,
animals select between high-effort conditions where highly palatable food reward is
fi
accessible through lever pressing (with xed ratios) or low-effort conditions where less
preferred food reward (lab chow) is freely available (a, b). Untreated rats prefer the highly
palatable food and lever press, and eat little of the freely available chow (c). This
demonstrates preference for high effort/high reward during normal dopamine levels. In
contrast, dopamine-depleted rats (through dopamine antagonists) shift their choice from the
high-effort condition (lever pressing) to the low-effort condition (freely available chow) (d).
/
58 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
Summary Points
• The mesocorticolimbic pathway underlies reward and motivation
processes.
behavior.
perceived gain.
and memory.
Review Questions
• What are the brain regions within the mesocorticolimbic pathway and what
• What has been referred to as the primary reward center of the brain?
• What are the three ways that induce drug reinstatement in animals?
• fi
What is the premise behind the theory of reward de ciency syndrome?
/
Further Reading 59
Further Reading
Ekhtiari, H., Nasseri, P., Yavari, F., Mokri, A. & Monterosso, J. (2016).
Neuroscience of drug craving for addiction medicine: from circuits to
therapies. Prog Brain Res, 223, 115–141. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.10.002
Filbey, F. M. & DeWitt, S. J. (2012). Cannabis cue-elicited craving and the
reward neurocircuitry. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 38(1),
30–35. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.11.001
Filbey, F. M. & Dunlop, J. (2014). Differential reward network functional
connectivity in cannabis dependent and non-dependent users. Drug
Alcohol Depend, 140, 101–111. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.04.002
00081-2
Gu, X. & Filbey, F. (2017). A Bayesian observer model of drug craving.JAMA
Psychiatry, 74(4), 419–420. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3823
Heinz, A., Beck, A., Mir, J., et al. (2010). Alcohol craving and relapse predic-
tion: imaging studies. In C. M. Kuhn & G. F. Koob, eds.,Advances in the
Neuroscience of Addiction, 2nd edn. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press,
pp. 137–162.
Robinson, T. E. & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: an
incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev, 18(3),
247–291. doi:10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P
Sinha, R. (2009). Modeling stress and drug craving in the laboratory: implica-
tions for addiction treatment development. Addict Biol, 14(1), 84–98.
doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00134.x
Wise, R. A. (1988). The neurobiology of craving: implications for the under-
standing and treatment of addiction. J Abnorm Psychol, 97(2), 118–132.
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.118
/
60 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
Spotlight
Motivated to predict future drug abuse
Early intervention for substance use disorder is key to treatment success and is
the reason why much research is dedicated toward identifying ways to predict
addiction can inform targeted treatment. For example, knowing the mechanisms
that led to the disorder can lead to timely and effective interventions.
fi
drug addiction could be identi ed using brain response patterns in 14-year-
olds with high novelty seeking. Novelty seeking is an attribute that promotes
fi
independence and is therefore bene cial during adolescence. This is why
although novelty seeking has also been associated with later development
drugs. The question then becomes, what makes novelty seeking in adoles-
cence a risk for drug addiction? To answer this question, Büchelet al. (2017)
predicted drug abuse at age 16. Using the monetary incentive delay task
(Figure S4.1), which measures the response to monetary gains, the research-
ers found that the 14-year-old adolescents who showed reduced motivational
activity during monetary gain were more likely to abuse drugs by the time
fi
they were 16 years old. In other words, insuf cient activation in motivation
(a)
/
References 61
(b)
Magnitude cue shapes:
250 ms
1.75–14 s
160–260 ms
+ ~2–14 s
Win
1s
$0 $1 $10
Magnitude
cue + +
2.12 s
Target Hit?
+
1s
250 ms
+ 0–12 s
1.75–14 s
Hit/win (+$0.00)
+
160–260 ms
cue $10.00
+ ~2–14 s
Magnitude 1s Feedback
cue + $ 2.12 s ITI
Target Win? ITI
+
1s
$ 0–12 s
Hit/win (+$1.00)
cue $11.00 +
Feedback
ITI
Figure S4.1 (a) Sensation and novelty seeking are characteristic of adolescence. (b)
Schematic of the monetary incentive delay task. This is a widely utilized task to
measure brain responses during motivated behavior. In this task, participants win or
avoid losing money if they are able to press a button while the target (the white
square in this illustration) is present. The task not only provides researchers with the
ability to measure responses during monetary wins and losses but is also able to
determine if the magnitude of the reward (i.e. different amounts of money: $0, $1
or $10 in this illustration) influences response. ITI, intertrial interval.
References
/
62 Motivation to Initiate Drug Use to Recreational Drug Use
Büchel, C., Peters, J., Banaschewski, T., et al. (2017). Blunted ventral striatal
responses to anticipated rewards foreshadow problematic drug use in
novelty-seeking adolescents. Nat Commun, 8, 14140. doi:10.1038/
ncomms14140
Carelli, R. M., Ijames, S. G. & Crumling, A. J. (2000). Evidence that separate
neural circuits in the nucleus accumbens encode cocaine versus
“natural” (water and food) reward. J Neurosci , 20(11), 4255–4266.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04255.2000
Dobryakova, E., DeLuca, J., Genova, H. M. & Wylie, G. R. (2013). Neural
correlates of cognitive fatigue: cortico-striatal circuitry and effort-
reward imbalance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 19(8), p. 849–853.
doi:10.1017/S1355617713000684
Filbey, F. M., Claus, E. D. & Hutchison, K. E. (2011). A neuroimaging
approach to the study of craving. In: Adinoff, A. & Stein, E., eds.
Neuroimaging in Addiction. London: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 133 –156.
Garavan, H., Pankiewicz, J., Bloom, A., et al. (2000). Cue-induced
cocaine craving: neuroanatomical speci ficity for drug users and drug
stimuli. Am J Psychiatry, 157(11), 1789– 1798. doi:10.1176/appi.
ajp.157.11.1789
Kalivas, P. W. & Volkow, N. D. (2005). The neural basis of addiction: a
pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry, 162(8),
1403 –1413. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403
Kelley, A. E. (2004a). Memory and addiction: shared neural circuitry and
molecular mechanisms. Neuron, 44(1), 161–179. doi:10.1016/j.
neuron.2004.09.016
(2004b). Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive
behavior and reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev , 27(8),
765 –776. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.015
Olds, J. & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical
stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain.J Comp
Physiol Psychol, 47(6), 419–427. doi:10.1037/h0058775
Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (1997). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates ,
3rd edn. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Robinson, T. E. & Kolb, B. (1997). Persistent structural modifications in
nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex neurons produced by
previous experience with amphetamine. J Neurosci, 17(21), 8491 –8497.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08491.1997
Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Farrar, A. & Mingote, S. M. (2007). Effort-
related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated
forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 191(3), 461–482.
doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0668-9
/
References 63
/
CHAPTER F IVE
Intoxication
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Drug intoxication refers to the immediate effects of the drug and occurs
during consumption of a drug in a large enough dose to produce signi fi-
cant behavioral, physiological or cognitive impairments. It is these intoxi-
cating effects that drive initial drug use. When drugs and alcohol are
consumed, a cascade of short- and long-term effects follows. Although
some of the effects of intoxication are pleasant and desired, other effects
can be aversive (Figure 5.1).
For example, alcohol intoxication or the state of being “drunk ” mani-
fests as facial flushing, slurred speech, unsteady gait, euphoria, increased
activity, volubility, disorderly conduct, slowed reactions, impaired judge-
ment and motor incoordination, insensibility and stupefaction. Under-
standing the effects of intoxication on the brain can inform how this
process contributes to drug addiction. This chapter will discuss the
mechanisms that underlie these intense feelings of pleasure that occur
while taking some of the most common substances of abuse including
alcohol, nicotine, cannabis and cocaine.
According to the ICD-10, “ intoxication is a condition that follows the
administration of a psychoactive substance and results in disturbances in
the level of consciousness, cognition, perception, judgement, affect, or
behavior, or other psychophysiological functions and responses ” (World
/
Introduction 65
/
66 Intoxication
Drug Pharmacodynamics
To begin to understand the speci fic effects of addictive drugs on the
brain and behavior, it is fi rst important to understand the principles of
pharmacodynamics. Pharmacodynamics refers to the mechanisms of
drugs at both organ and cellular levels. It also refers to dose– effect
relationships, as well as interactive effects of drugs. The majority of
drugs interact with target biomolecules, such as enzymes, ion channels
and transporters through receptor binding. Receptors are macromol-
ecules located on the cell surface whose function is to recognize drug
signals and initiate a response (i.e. transduction). Drugs can be classified
based on the receptor ’s response to them (see Figure 5.2): agonists
activate receptors; antagonists block the action of an agonist on the
receptor; inverse agonists activate receptors to produce an effect in the
opposite direction of an agonist; partial agonists activate a receptor but
only at a submaximum level while also blocking the action of a full
agonist; and ligands have selective binding to specifi c receptors or sites.
There are four classes of receptor that can transduce a signal to a
response: G protein-coupled receptors, ion-channel receptors, enzyme-
linked receptors and receptors of gene expression.
/
Agonistic drug effects Antagonistic drug effects
enzymes
of the neurotransmitter
of neurotransmitters by blocking
degradation or reuptake
particularly α4β2 but not α4β9 and α4β10 receptors, where it acts as a
receptor antagonist. α4β 2 receptors are present on dopamine neurons,
and may be the mechanism through which nicotine exerts its reinforcing
effects. Activation of nAChRs leads to increased acetylcholine, which
modulates other neurotransmitter functions and is associated with
increased memory, muscle contractions, sweat and saliva secretions,
and decrease heart rate. Sedatives or depressants, such as alcohol, bar-
biturates and benzodiazepines, increase dopamine indirectly through
their effects on γ -aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptors, which decrease
the excitability of neurons. This action promotes decreased brain func-
tion, inducing sleepiness and reducing anxiety, alertness, memory and
muscle tension. Sedative-anesthetic drugs such as phencyclidine (PCP)
and ketamine are N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor (a type of
glutamatergic receptor) antagonists. The primary effect is increased
excitatory transmission, which leads to visual and auditory distortions
(hallucinations), as well as perceptual changes at higher doses (dissoci-
ations or feelings of detachment). Opiates such as morphine, heroin and
hydrocodone bind to μ -opioid receptors present on dopamine and
GABA neurons, thus regulating dopamine function. μ -Opioid receptor
binding leads to sedation, increasing sleepiness and reducing anxiety and
pain. Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in cannabis is a partial agonist at
cannabinoid 1 (CB1) receptors that modulate dopamine cells and post-
synaptic dopamine signaling. The effects of THC on CB1 receptors
include increased hunger, happiness and calmness, but it can also lead
to unusual thoughts and feelings. Moreover, the modulatory role of CB1
receptors on dopamine functioning provides a possible mechanism
through which THC may increase the reinforcing effects of other drugs
of abuse, such as alcohol, nicotine, cocaine and opioids.
/
Brain Mechanisms of Intoxication 69
/
70 Intoxication
kBq/mL MRI
0
0.0 Cigarette 0.1 Cigarette 0.3 Cigarette 1.0 Cigarette 3.0 Cigarette
(b) (c)
kB q V /f
s p
10
10
0 0
Figure 5.3 PET studies to determine the effects of nicotine administration. (a) Nicotine
intake leads to dose-dependent occupancy ofα4β2* nAChRs (noted by progressively
decreasing nAChR binding in blue with increased dose). (b) Low-nicotine cigarettes result in
26% and 79% α4β2* nAChR occupancies. (c) Moderate second-hand smoke exposure
results in 19% occupancy of α4β2* nAChRs in smokers (shown) and non-smokers (not
shown). 2-FA, 2-[18F]fluoro-3-(2( )-azetidinylmethoxy) pyridine; MRI, magnetic resonance
S
imaging. (From Jasinska , 2014.) (A black and white version of thisfigure will appear in
et al.
some formats. For the color version, please refer to the plate section.)
of cigarette smoke exposure and β2* nAChR occupancy (Figure 5.3).
They further noted that β2* nAChR binding lasted for up to 3.1 h after
exposure, suggesting long-lasting saturation of β2* nAChRs. Similar
prolonged effects123on β2* nAChR occupancy has been reported using
the chemical 5-[ I]iodo-85380 to quantify nAChRs during SPECT
(Esterlis , 2010). They found 67Æ9% (range 55 80%) receptor
occupancy after subjects had smoked to satiety (~2.4 cigarettes). Of note,
et al. –
/
Brain Mechanisms of Intoxication 71
/
72 Intoxication
several brain regions including the insula, lateral prefrontal cortex and
parietal lobe. Similar dose-related decreases in neural activation in
driving-associated brain regions that correlated with driving perform-
ance have also been reported (Meda , 2009). An example of a
virtual reality driving simulator device is shown in Figure 5.4. Meda
et al.
/
Modulators of Intoxication: Challenges in Human Research 73
et al.
Insula
Claustrum
Putamen
Caudate nucleus
Internal capsule
Globus pallidus
Thalamus
Corpus callosum
Lateral ventricle
Choroid plexus
Fornix
Third ventricle
Medial medullary lamina
Intermediate mass
Third ventricle
Optic tract
Corpora mamillaria
Amygdaloid nucleus
Figure 5.5 (a) Position of the amygdala (arrow). (b). Response in brain regions to
fi
emotional faces during alcohol intoxication. Asterisks indicate statistically signi cant
(Part (b) from Gilman et al., 2008. © 2008 Society for Neuroscience, USA.)
/
74 Intoxication
(b)
Alcohol neutral
* * * * *
0.08 *** ***
Placebo fearful
e n i l es a b ot ev it a l er
0.06
Placebo neutral
0.04
*
0.2
–0.02
–0.04
–0.06
–0.08
–0.1
Nucleus accumbens Putamen Caudate
(left) (right) (left) (right) (left) (right)
Alcohol neutral
Placebo neutral
0.02
0.15
0.1
0.05
–0.05
–0.1
Amygdala Fusiform gyrus Lingual gyrus
several factors that interact with the mechanisms that underlie intoxi-
cation. These factors could be: 1) context dependent, e.g. rate of
consumption, concentration or potency of the drug; 2) individual char-
acteristics, e.g. sex, age or genetics; or 3) state dependent, e.g. expect-
ancies, or adaptations to substance use (e.g. tolerance) (see Spotlight
on how these factors pose challenges for drug policies). The speed with
which a drug acts depends on the dose taken, the mode of adminis-
tration, and the rate of clearance to and from the brain. Intravenous
/
Summary Points 75
delivery leads to the fastest drug effects because the drug reaches the
brain more quickly. The response to drugs is also related to previous
drug experiences. For example, the magnitude of intoxication (i.e. the
increase in dopamine) attenuates with greater severity of substance
use. Acute administration of methylphenidate, for example, increased
levels of glucose metabolism in prefrontal-striatal areas in active
cocaine abusers with low D2 receptor availability (Volkow
et al., 1999) but decreased it in non-addicted individuals (Volkow
et al., 2005). Individual differences in personality traits as well as drug
expectancies – the expected effect of a drug – can also in fluence
intoxicated behavior and may interfere with the pharmacodynamic
properties of drugs. Females are also typically more sensitive to
intoxicating effects of drugs, perhaps due to general differences in
body weight, percentage body fat or rate of renal clearance of
unchanged drug (which is decreased in females due to a lower glom-
erular filtration rate or flow rate of fl uid through the kidney). Similar
age effects may be due to a reduction in renal and hepatic clearance
with increasing age. Last, dopamine sensitivity based on underlying
genetic factors can also influence the response to the intoxicating
effects of drugs. This notion suggests that genetic variations in the
dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2) allele may lead to hypersensitiv-
ity of dopamine release, leading to increased likelihood of relapse
(Blum et al., 2009). In other words, dopaminergic agonists may result
in stronger activation of brain reward circuitry in those who carry the
DRD2 A1 allele compared with the DRD2 A2 allele because those
with the A1 allele have signifi cantly lower D2 receptor density (see
reward deficiency syndrome in Chapter 4).
Summary Points
• fi
The speci city of drug targets lead to the varied intoxication effects.
• Levels of intoxication are due to many factors that are: 1) context depend-
/
76 Intoxication
Review Questions
•Describe the specific mechanisms leading to various intoxicating effects of
each drug class type.
•What can factors that influence differences in intoxication effects be
categorized into?
•In general, what do EEG studies show in terms of changes in brain electro-
physiology during intoxication?
•How is cerebral blood flow impacted during intoxication?
•What happens to glucose and acetate during intoxicated states?
•Describe the neural underpinnings of intoxicated driving.
•What mechanisms underlie the emotional symptoms during intoxication?
Further Reading
Calhoun, V. D., Pekar, J. J. & Pearlson, G. D. (2004). Alcohol intoxication
effects on simulated driving: exploring alcohol-dose effects on brain acti-
vation using functional MRI. Neuropsychopharmacology , 29(11),
2097 2017. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300543
–
Hsieh, Y. J., Wu, L. C., Ke, C. C., (2018). Effects of the acute and chronic
et al.
Volkow, N. D., Kim, S. W., Wang, G. J., (2013). Acute alcohol intoxication
et al.
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., (2000). Cocaine abusers show
et al.
Spotlight
Buzz Kill
The legalization of cannabis for recreational use in California made the state
the world’s largest cannabis market. One of the challenges this brings is to
/
Spotlight 77
(From https://www.pexels.com/photo/auto-automobile-blur-buildings-532001/.)
/
78 Intoxication
References
Anderson, B. M., Stevens, M. C., Meda, S. A., (2011). Functional
et al.
/
References 79
cerebral blood flow in subjects with low and high responses to alcohol.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res , 35(6), 1034–1040. doi:10.1111/j.1530-
0277.2011.01435.x
/
80 Intoxication
Volkow, N. D., Hitzemann, R., Wolf, A. P., et al. (1990). Acute effects of
ethanol on regional brain glucose metabolism and transport.
Psychiatry Res, 35(1), 39– 48. doi:10.1016/0925-4927(90)90007-S
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., et al. (1999). Blockade of striatal
dopamine transporters by intravenous methylphenidate is not
sufficient to induce self-reports of “high ”. J Pharmacol Exp Ther ,
288(1), 14– 20.
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Ma, Y., et al. (2005). Activation of orbital and
medial prefrontal cortex by methylphenidate in cocaine-addicted
subjects but not in controls: relevance to addiction. J Neurosci, 25(15),
3932 –3939. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0433-05.2005
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Franceschi, D., et al. (2006). Low doses of
alcohol substantially decrease glucose metabolism in the human brain.
Neuroimage , 29(1), 295–301. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.004
Volkow, N. D., Kim, S. W., Wang, G. J., et al. (2013). Acute alcohol
intoxication decreases glucose metabolism but increases acetate
uptake in the human brain. Neuroimage, 64, 277– 283. doi:10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2012.08.057
Wang, G. J., Volkow, N. D., Franceschi, D., et al. (2000). Regional brain
metabolism during alcohol intoxication. Alcohol Clin Exp Res , 24(6),
822 –829.
World Health Organization (2004). ICD-10, 2nd edn. Geneva: World Health
Organization.
/
C H A P T ER SI X
Withdrawal
Learning Objectives
of withdrawal.
withdrawal.
withdrawal symptoms.
Introduction
/
82 Withdrawal
/
What Does Withdrawal Look Like? 83
Table 6.1 Drug speci ficity and timing of acute withdrawal symptoms.
Physical and
Paranoia Violence
energy Suicide
diarrhea syndrome
Delirium syndrome
Death
Diarrhea syndrome
Goose bumps
Runny nose
Teary eyes
Yawning
days Appetite
disturbance
Sleep
disturbance
Nausea
Dif ficulty
concentrating
Nystagmus
Diarrhea
/
84 Withdrawal
Physical and
Nicotine 1 –2 days –
1 10 Irritability Insomnia
Depression Dizziness
fi
Dif culty Nausea
Increased Tremors
Irritability Insomnia
Anger Dysphoria
Nervousness Craving
Restlessness Shakiness
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
de fined by the time it takes for the concentration of the drug in the
plasma or the total amount in the body to be reduced by 50%. In other
words, after one half-life, the concentration of the drug in the body will
be half of the starting dose. For example, as illustrated in Figure 6.1,
research suggests that, although the half-life of cannabis is highly vari-
able, it is typically ~3–4 days. Unlike cannabis, other drugs have shorter
half-lives, leading to faster onset of withdrawal symptoms following
discontinued use, e.g. the half-life of heroin is 12 h, opiates is 8 h, alcohol
/
Acute Withdrawal Symptoms and Associated Neural Mechanisms 85
men with alcohol use disorder (~10% of the general population) than
women (3 5%) (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism,
2006).
–
/
86 Withdrawal
/
Protracted Withdrawal Symptoms 87
30
) g/ n im/ lm( FBC c im a l a ht n i e g n a hC
15
–15
–1 0 1 2 3
Figure 6.2 Change in CBF in the thalamus from baseline to overnight abstinence and
subjective withdrawal from nicotine as measured by the Minnesota withdrawal score from
baseline to withdrawal.
Neural Mechanisms
/
88 Withdrawal
cocaine patients with less than 1 week s abstinence described above was
’
/
Electrophysiological Mechanisms of Withdrawal 89
of fast β power
Relapse-prone
group
Current density
2
[uAmm/mm ]
0.00597
0.00199
0.000996
0
Abstinence-prone
group
Figure 6.3 Fast β power can be a predictor of relapse in polysubstance users during a
3-month abstinence. BEM, boundary element method; CSD, current source density. (From
fi
Bauer, 2001. © 2001 Springer Nature, USA.) (A black and white version of this gure will
appear in some formats. For the color version, please refer to the plate section.)
P300 latencies and decreases in N100 and P300 amplitudes have been
reported in those with alcohol use disorder (Porjesz , 1987).
A reduced P300 amplitude is a consistent finding during cocaine (Good-
et al.
/
90 Withdrawal
/
Summary Points 91
ACC
Stimulus value Craving
Action value/cost + Thal
Anticipation/availability dlPFC DS
GP
Context +
vlPFC NAC
Craving External
Action inhibition vmPFC Action B context
Emotion control
– NS
T HPC
Stress
Incentive Affective
to action state
Figure 6.4 Neuroadaptations between the reward and stress systems during withdrawal.
ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; BNST, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; CeA, central
nucleus of the amygdala; DS, dorsal striatum; dlPFC, dorsolateral PFC; GP, globus pallidus;
HPC, hippocampus; NAC, nucleus accumbens; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; Thal, thalamus;
vIPFC, ventrolateral PFC; vmPFC, ventromedial PFC.
(Modified from George & Koob, 2013.)
• Acute withdrawal symptoms begin within hours or days after last use of the
/
92 Withdrawal
Review Questions
• What are the individual differences that contribute to the highly variable
presentation of withdrawal symptoms?
• What is the primary determinant of the timeline of drug withdrawal
effects?
• How does dopamine depletion result in withdrawal symptoms?
• How do between-system changes contribute to withdrawal?
Further Reading
De Biasi, M. & Dani, J. A. (2011). Reward, addiction, withdrawal to nicotine.
Annu Rev Neurosci, 34, 105–130. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-
113734
Filbey, F. M., Dunlop, J. & Myers, U. S. (2013). Neural effects of positive and
negative incentives during marijuana withdrawal.PLoS One, 8(5), e61470.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061470
George, O., Koob, G. F. & Vendruscolo, L. F. (2014). Negative reinforcement
via motivational withdrawal is the driving force behind the transition to
addiction. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 231(19), 3911–3917. doi:10.1007/
s00213-014-3623-1
Myers, K. M. & Carlezon, W. A., Jr. (2010). Extinction of drug- and
withdrawal-paired cues in animal models: relevance to the treatment of
addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 35(2), 285–302. doi:10.1016/j.
neubiorev.2010.01.011
Negus, S. S. & Banks, M. L. (2018). Modulation of drug choice by extended
drug access and withdrawal in rhesus monkeys: implications for negative
reinforcement as a driver of addiction and target for medications devel-
opment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 164, 32–39. doi:10.1016/j
.pbb.2017.04.006
Piper, M. E. (2015). Withdrawal: expanding a key addiction construct.Nico-
tine Tob Res, 17(12), 1405 –1415. doi:10.1093/ntr/ntv048
/
Spotlight 1 93
Spotlight 1
Figure S6.1 Babies have to be weaned from opiates when born from opiate-using
mothers.
(From https://pixabay.com/en/baby-crying-cry-crying-baby-cute-2387661/.)
While the circumstances that lead to opiate addiction for these women vary,
the effects of exposure to drugs in the womb are the same. Most of these
infants are born prematurely and suffering from withdrawal, a condition called
neonatal abstinence syndrome (NAS). Withdrawal symptoms in babies with
NAS are similar to those experienced by adults. These include excessive crying,
vomiting, diarrhea, muscle twitches and seizures (Figure S6.1).
Fortunately, there is awareness of this problem, and programs have been
developed to provide support for these women. Such programs provide
women with clinicians to help manage their medication-assisted therapy,
and support so that they are able to take care of their families through
childcare and education. Such programs have led to reductions in the infants
length of stay in neonatal intensive care units, e.g. by 33% in Texas (Cleve-
’
land , 2015).
et al.
/
94 Withdrawal
Spotlight 2
than not, researchers have begun to ask whether addictive processes may be
involved in the use of these devices and their applications (Figure S6.2).
away from internet use, and found similarities with withdrawal symptoms
from drug addiction. They discovered that people who spend an extended
amount of time on the internet experience increased heart rate and rises in
blood pressure after they stop using the internet. The study was based on
–
144 adults aged 18 33. The authors warn that these physiological changes
may lead to anxiety as well as to hormonal imbalances. Only time will tell
what the long-term effects of excessive electronic device use on public health
and society will be, but government organizations are already feeling the
shut down internet access across the entire country to support students
(From https://pixabay.com/en/facebook-social-media-addiction-2387089/.)
References
/
References 95
/
96 Withdrawal
/
References 97
/
C H A PT E R S EV EN
Craving
Learning Objectives
• Be able to understand the problems in the conceptualization of craving.
craving.
• “
Be able to explain what is meant by the statement that drugs hijack”
the brain.
separate processes.
Introduction
Craving is often defined as a strong subjective desire to use alcohol or
drugs. Historically, there has been debate with regard to the conceptual-
ization and measurement of craving (see reviews by Tiffany & Conklin,
2000; Tiffany , 2000). Craving can be measured in terms of physical
et al.
historically been criticized for its subjective nature, which does not
prospectively predict drug-use behavior (Tiffany , 2000). There were
et al.
/
Cue-Elicited Craving Paradigms 99
also concerns with regard to the ecological validity of the use of subject-
ive measures in laboratory settings that question their accuracy, reliabil-
ity and validity. Translating cue-elicited craving to classical approaches
from animal models has also been a challenge. For example, subjective
craving is not easily discerned in animals; thus, direct translation of the
multidimensional construct of craving may not be possible from animal
models to humans.
As discussed in Chapter 4, findings from the animal literature have
shown that the motivation to use drugs is linked to the actions of drugs
on the mesocorticolimbic pathways in the brain, which are the neural
substrates that putatively underlie the attribution of incentive salience
to alcohol and other drugs of abuse (Berridge & Robinson, 1998;
Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Wise, 1988). Recently, scientists have begun
to use neuroimaging approaches to focus on the neurobiology of craving
in humans. The use of more objective neuroimaging techniques allevi-
ates the burden of proof on subjective responses, thus addressing some
of the limitations of accuracy and validity in behavioral investigations.
Neuroimaging approaches also allow greater consistency between
animal and human models because of the focus on neurobiology.
This chapter will focus on the various techniques that demonstrate the
presence of cue-elicited craving across different substances and that led
to the addition of craving as a primary symptom for the diagnosis of a
substance use disorder (SUD) in the DSM-5.
Cue-Elicited Craving Paradigms and Associated
Neural Mechanisms
/
100 Craving
pictures. Videos have also been utilized to study craving (Wrase et al.,
2001) and limbic areas (Myrick et al., 2004). A study by Franklin et al.
/
Neurophysiological Underpinnings of Craving 101
OR
No urge 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Extremely
20 s 5 s 20 s
Figure 7.1 Cue-elicited craving paradigm using tactile cannabis cue paraphernalia, a
neutral object (pencil) and appetitive non-drug reward cues (fruit, not shown).
/
102 Craving
“evocative scripts” that described the context where drug use occurs in
the individuals, in combination with videos and paraphernalia related to
cocaine, elicited activation of the lateral amygdala, an important area for
emotion regulation. These findings replicated earlier reports of the
involvement of areas in the limbic system important for processing
emotion and memory in response to cocaine cues (Childress et , al.
/
Do Drugs Hijack the Reward Circuitry of the Brain? 103
/
104 Craving
“Unseen” cue
paradigm
Null
+
Sexual
Neutral
Aversive
Cocaine
Figure 7.2 Cue-elicited craving paradigm. A study by Childresset al. (2008) found a greater
/
Greater Craving or Greater Attention? 105
Fixation cross
+ Target stimulus
5
0
0
m
s
Masking stimulus
3
3
m
s
Fixation cross
4
6
7
m
2 s
0
0
0
m
s
1
+
0
0
0
m
s
Figure 7.3 Representative trial from the backward-masked cue task. In each trial,
participants were presented with the following visual stimuli in immediate succession:
crosshair (500 ms); target stimulus (33 ms); masking stimulus (467 ms); crosshair (1000 ms).
Target images were presented from one of four categories: cocaine (shown), neutral, sexual
and aversive.
The idea presented by Daglish (2003) that the ability of drug cues to
et al.
masked cues have shed some light on this topic and support the notion
that craving is implicit, i.e. occurs subconsciously and only occasionally
intrudes into conscious awareness (Tiffany & Wray, 2012). These studies
utilized backward-masked images of cocaine, sexual, aversive and neu-
tral cues and presented them rapidly (i.e. 33 ms) (Figure 7.3). Backward
masking presents a masked stimulus immediately after another brief
/
106 Craving
Neuromolecular Mechanisms
The idea that craving occurs after the drug is consumed suggests the
occurrence of neural adaptations following drug exposure. One of the
cellular changes triggered by drug use is increased dendritic structure via
increased dendritic spine density in the nucleus accumbens and PFC.
Nestler and colleagues suggested that these dendritic alterations are
mediated by transformation of FBJ murine osteosarcoma viral oncogene
homolog B (FosB) to Δ FosB (Figure 7.4) (Nestler, 2001; Nestleret al.,
Repeated drug
exposure
(e.g. via
neurotrophic
factors, FosB,
CDK5?)
Figure 7.4 Regulation of the dendritic structure by drugs of abuse. Expansion of a neuron
’s
dendritic tree and spine density occurs after chronic exposure to a drug of abuse in the
nucleus accumbens and PFC, mediated by ΔFosB and the consequent induction of CDK5.
(From Nestler et al., 2001. © 2001 Springer Nature, USA.)
/
Review Questions 107
Summary Points
• The conceptualization of craving has been advanced by neuroimaging
techniques.
systems.
• Patterns of brain response to drug cues are greater than those to natural
rewards and are correlated with subjective craving as well as with indices of
addiction severity.
drug cues.
• ΔFosB mediates the neural changes, including craving, that occur following
drug exposure.
Review Questions
• What were the criticisms in the conceptualization of craving?
• What are the wider systems that integrate to underlie craving in response
to drug cues?
/
108 Craving
Further Reading
Ekhtiari, H., Nasseri, P., Yavari, F., Mokri, A. & Monterosso, J. (2016). Neuro-
science of drug craving for addiction medicine: from circuits to therapies.
Prog Brain Res , 223, 115–141. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.10.002
Filbey, F. M. & DeWitt, S. J. (2012). Cannabis cue-elicited craving and the
reward neurocircuitry. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry , 38(1),
30–35. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.11.001
Filbey, F. M., Schacht, J. P., Myers, U. S., Chavez, R. S. & Hutchison, K. E.
(2009). Marijuana craving in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , 106(31),
13016–13021. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903863106
Grant, S., London, E. D., Newlin, D. B., et al. (1996). Activation of memory
circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ,
93(21), 12040–12045.
Gu, X. & Filbey, F. (2017). A Bayesian observer model of drug craving. JAMA
Spotlight
Drug Cravings Persist in Death
The presence of mutated ΔFosB protein weeks after the drug-use event
suggests that craving persists for weeks, even after cessation of use.
/
Spotlight 109
Austria, made headlines in 2016 when they published their research ndings fi
on evidence that drug craving persists in the dead (Seltenhammer et al. ,
2016). In their study, they examined tissue samples from the nucleus accum-
bens of fifteen deceased heroin addicts and fifteen non-drug users. They
measured levels of ΔFosB and found that accumulation of the protein was
still detectable 9 days after death. The scientists referred to this effect as
“dependence memory. ” From this finding, the scientists inferred that ΔFosB
persists even longer in living individuals, perhaps as long as months. This
DGip
(d) (e)
(f) (g)
fi –
magni cation (d g), DGip, infrapyramidal blade of the dentate gyrus; DGsp,
suprapyramidal blade of the dentate gyrus. (From Nishijimaet al. , 2013.) (A black and
white version of this figure will appear in some formats. For the color version, please
/
110 Craving
Δ
for addiction (Figure S7.1). The scientists suggest that activation of FosB can
References
Bonson, K. R., Grant, S. J., Contoreggi, C. S., (2002). Neural systems
et al.
limbic activation by “unseen” drug and sexual cues. PLoS One , 3(1),
e1506. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001506
Daglish, M. R. & Nutt, D. J. (2003). Brain imaging studies in human addicts.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol , 13(6), 453–458. doi:10.1016/j.
euroneuro.2003.08.006
Daglish, M. R., Weinstein, A., Malizia, A. L., (2003). Functional
et al.
/
References 111
Filbey, F. M., Dunlop, J., Ketcherside, A.,et al. (2016). fMRI study of neural
sensitization to hedonic stimuli in long-term, daily cannabis users.Hum
Brain Mapp, 37(10), 3431–3443. doi:10.1002/hbm.23250
Franken, I. H., Stam, C. J., Hendriks, V. M. & van den Brink, W. (2003).
Neurophysiological evidence for abnormal cognitive processing of
drug cues in heroin dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 170(2),
205 –212. doi:10.1007/s00213-003-1542-7
Franklin, T. R., Wang, Z., Wang, J., et al. (2007). Limbic activation to
cigarette smoking cues independent of nicotine withdrawal: a
perfusion fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(11), 2301–2309.
doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301371
George, M. S., Anton, R. F., Bloomer, C.,et al. (2001). Activation of
prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on
exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 58(4), 345–352.
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.345
Heinze, M., Wolfling, K. & Grusser, S. M. (2007). Cue-induced auditory
evoked potentials in alcoholism. Clin Neurophysiol, 118(4), 856–862.
doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.12.003
Herning, R. I., Guo, X., Better, W. E.,et al. (1997). Neurophysiological signs
of cocaine dependence: increased electroencephalogram beta during
withdrawal. Biol Psychiatry, 41(11), 1087– 1094. doi:10.1016/S0006-
3223(96)00258-2
Herrmann, M. J., Weijers, H. G., Wiesbeck, G. A.,et al. (2000). Event-
related potentials and cue-reactivity in alcoholism. Alcohol Clin Exp
Res , 24(11), 1724– 1729. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.12.003
Herrmann, M. J., Weijers, H. G., Wiesbeck, G. A., Boning, J. & Fallgatter,
A. J. (2001). Alcohol cue-reactivity in heavy and light social drinkers as
revealed by event-related potentials. Alcohol Alcohol , 36(6), 588–593.
doi:10.1093/alcalc/36.6.588
Knott, V., Cosgrove, M., Villeneuve, C., et al. (2008a). EEG correlates of
imagery-induced cigarette craving in male and female smokers. Addict
Behav , 33(4), 616–621. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.11.006
Knott, V. J., Naccache, L., Cyr, E., et al. (2008b). Craving-induced EEG
reactivity in smokers: effects of mood induction, nicotine dependence
and gender. Neuropsychobiology, 58(3– 4), 187–199. doi:10.1159/
000201716
Kuhn, S. & Gallinat, J. (2011). Common biology of craving across legal and
illegal drugs – a quantitative meta-analysis of cue-reactivity brain
response. Eur J Neurosci, 33(7), 1318–1326. doi:10.1111/j.1460-
9568.2010.07590.x
Liu, X., Vaupel, D. B., Grant, S. & London, E. D. (1998). Effect of cocaine-
related environmental stimuli on the spontaneous
/
112 Craving
with cue evoked emotional states and heroin in opiate addicts. Drug
/
References 113
/
C H A PT E R E I G H T
Impulsivity
Learning Objectives
• fi
Be able to explain the challenges in de ning impulsivity as a unitary
construct.
consequence of addiction.
to impulsivity.
Introduction
/
Introduction 115
/
116 Impulsivity
Visual
loop Motor
loop
Executive
loop
Motivational
loop
Neuropharmacology of Impulsivity
/
Is Impulsivity Pre-existing or Drug Induced? 117
/
118 Impulsivity
(a) (b)
Impulsivity Sensation seeking
100 30
*
*
25
80 *
20
60
15
40
10
20 5
0
0
(c) (d)
40 10
*
*
8
*
)ES 1 ±( ero c s nae M
30 *
)ES 1 ± ero c s nae M
20
*
10
2
0
0
Figure 8.3 Study in stimulant-dependent individuals, their non-using siblings and non-
using controls demonstrating that impulsivity traits (but not sensation seeking) may be a
predisposing factor for stimulant dependence. The results show measurement of impulsivity
traits using BIS-11 (a, c) and sensation-seeking personality traits using the Sensation-
Seeking Scale Form V (SSS-V) (b, d). SE, standard error; *, signi cant difference at P fi < 0.05.
studies have also found associations between genes that regulate the ser-
otonergic system (tryptophan hydroxylase 1 and 2, serotonin transporter),
the dopaminergic system (dopamine transporter, monoamine metabolism
pathway) and the noradrenergic system (dopamine β -hydroxylase) and
impulsive personality. Together, these studies suggest that impulsivity is
heritable and could be an endophenotype for addiction.
Notably, the study by Ersche et al. (2010) also reported that those with
stimulant abuse had impulsivity even greater than their siblings, suggest-
ing that exposure to drugs may exacerbate an already elevated level of
/
Is Impulsivity Pre-existing or Drug Induced? 119
X Go
700 ms
+
300 ms
Y 700 ms
Go
+
300 ms
X
3
Go
700 ms
+
300 ms
X
4
700 ms
No go
+
300 ms
312
Figure 8.4 Illustration of a go/no go test. A response is made for every go condition (i.e.
each visual presentation of “X ” and “ Y” ) but not for no go conditions (i.e. consecutive
presentations of “X”).
/
120 Impulsivity
2010). Support for this also exists in the animal literature. For
example, in a study on rats differentially characterized on impulsivity
and sensation seeking, it was found that high-sensation-seeking rats
were more sensitive to cocaine and acquired cocaine self-
administration more rapidly compared with the high-impulsive rats
that did not acquire cocaine self-administration as rapidly. However,
the high-impulsive rats exhibited greater cocaine-seeking behavior
despite mild foot-shock punishment (Belin et al., 2008). This drug-
seeking behavior despite negative consequences, in this case foot
shock, is considered risky decision making.
A widely utilized task to evaluate risky decision making in humans is
the Iowa gambling task (IGT) (Bechara et al., 1994). The IGT is a
computerized card game that measures sensitivity to rewards and losses.
During the IGT, participants must weigh expected but uncertain rewards
and penalties, for example taking bigger risks for greater rewards or
smaller risks for lesser rewards. Using the IGT, neuroimaging studies
have shown that the right ventromedial PFC is engaged during decision
making, although activation in the left ventromedial PFC is associated
/
Inhibitory Control 121
(2008) in lesion patients, for example, found dissociable roles for the
ventromedial PFC and insula where the ventromedial PFC played a role
in the regulation of decision making during trials with known outcome
probabilities (see Figure 8.5), while the insula had a specifi c role only at
more unfavorable odds, confirming the specificity of the insula during
affective decision making.
Inhibitory Control
Another aspect of impulsivity is the ability to stop an action that has
either already been initiated or is in the choice selection phase. Imagine
the effort required to release the gas pedal when driving through a
stoplight that has just turned from green to yellow. This action requires
a similar process of inhibiting a pre-potent response (i.e. stepping on the
gas pedal). As introduced earlier, some of the widely used tasks to
measure inhibitory control are the SSRT task and the go/no go test.
Whereas the SSRT involves the cancellation of an already selected
response (“ action cancellation” ) the go/no go test implicates action
restraint. An animal analogue of this paradigm is the 5CSRTT, where
animals are trained to detect brief visual targets to earn food. Anticipa-
tory responses that occur prior to the onset of the visual signals are
considered premature responses.
The circuit that underlies inhibitory control includes the right infer-
ior frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the pre-
supplementary and motor cortex, as well as the basal ganglia and
projections to the subthalamic nucleus (Aron et al., 2007) (Figure 8.6).
Critics of this right-lateralized model argue for the additional contri-
butions of left hemispheric regions. Some also suggest that, given that
response inhibition during the SSRT task is in response to an external
cue, the described processes may be predominantly attention driven.
Last, despite the prevailing argument that inhibitory control is exerted
top-down by cortical mechanisms, there is growing evidence that
neural circuitry involving both cortical and subcortical mechanisms
are implicated, particularly within the basal ganglia. Moreover,
the possibility exists for impulsivity to be caused by chemical dys-
modulation, not only of cortical processes but also at the level of the
striatum.
/
122 Impulsivity
3 2 1 1 2 3
IN
IN
VMPF
VMPF
1 2 3
1 2 3 4 5 >
# of overlaps
Healthy controls
80 Lesion controls
VMPF
70 Insula
60
50
teB %
40
30
20
10
9 to 1 8 to 2 7 to 3 6 to 4
Chance of winning
Figure 8.5 Ventromedial PFC lesions lead to risky decision making. A studies found that
twenty patients with ventromedial PFC (VMPF) lesions (left side) exhibited greater betting
behavior compared with forty-one non-lesion controls, thirteen patients with insula lesions
/
Delay Discounting of Reward 123
“Stopping” impulsivity
PFC
SNc
RIFG/OFC
Caudate-
putamen
Raphe
ACC
GP Th
LC
dPM
SMA/pre-SMA
STN
M1
Figure 8.6 Schematic of the stop circuit. Inhibitory control depends on the interactions
between PFC areas (cortical motor areas: M1, primary motor cortex; SMA/pre-SMA,
supplementary motor area; dPM, dorsal pre-motor area), the right inferior frontal gyrus
(RIFG), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and striatal
regions including the dorsal striatum (caudate-putamen), globus pallidus (GP) and
subthalamic nucleus (STN), which project via the thalamus (Th) to the PFC. The PFC and
striatal networks are modulated by midbrain dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra
pars compacta (SNc)/ventral tegmental area, serotonergic neurons in the raphé nuclei
/
124 Impulsivity
Now Later
(immediate) (delayed)
E.g. $2 in 5 s E.g. $5 in 10 s
$ $
$ $
“Waiting” impulsivity
PFC
HC
AMG
ACC
VTA
PLd
NAcb core
PLv
Raphe
IL
LC NAcb shell
Figure 8.8 Schematic of the wait circuit. Delay discounting of reward depends on top-down
PFC interactions with the hippocampus (HC), amygdala (AMG) and structures in the ventral
striatum, including the nucleus accumbens core (NAcb core) and shell (NAcb shell). The
anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsal and ventral prelimbic cortex (PLd and PLv), and
infralimbic cortex (IL) make distinct contributions to waiting via topographically organized
inputs to the NAcb. VTA, ventral tegmental area; LC, locus coeruleus.
/
Review Questions 125
answered. It is also likely that, while impulsivity may be a risk factor that
• fi
Risky decision making is de ned as persistence despite the potential for
negative consequences.
impulsivity.
Review Questions
• How can studies such as the one described by Erscheet al. (2010) decipher
/
126 Impulsivity
Further Reading
Beaton, D., Abdi, H. & Filbey, F. M. (2014). Unique aspects of impulsive traits
in substance use and overeating: specific contributions of common assess-
ments of impulsivity. Am J Drug Alcohol , 40(6), 463–475.
Abuse
doi:10.3109/00952990.2014.937490
Crews, F. T. & Boettiger, C. A. (2009). Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for
addiction. Pharmacol Biochem , 93(3), 237–247. doi:10.1016/j.
Behav
pbb.2009.04.018
Ding, W. N., Sun, J. H., Sun, Y. W., (2014). Trait impulsivity and impaired
et al.
doi:10.1186/1744-9081-10-20
Filbey, F. M. & Yezhuvath, U. S. (2017). A multimodal study of impulsivity and
body weight: integrating behavioral, cognitive, and neuroimaging
approaches. , 25(1), 147–154. doi:10.1002/oby.21713
Obesity (Silver Spring)
Filbey, F. M., Claus, E. D., Morgan, M., Forester, G. R. & Hutchison, K. (2012).
Dopaminergic genes modulate response inhibition in alcohol abusing adults.
Addict Biol, 17(6), 1046–1056. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00328.x
Hu, Y., Salmeron, B. J., Gu, H., Stein, E. A. & Yang, Y. (2015). Impaired
functional connectivity within and between frontostriatal circuits and its
association with compulsive drug use and trait impulsivity in cocaine addic-
tion. JAMA Psychiatry , 72(6), 584–592. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1
Jupp, B. & Dalley, J. W. (2014). Convergent pharmacological mechanisms in
impulsivity and addiction: insights from rodent models. Br J Pharmacol,
171(20), 4729–4766. doi:10.1111/bph.12787
McHugh, M. J., Demers, C. H., Braud, J., (2013). Striatal-insula circuits in
et al.
eatbeh.2015.06.007
/
Spotlight 127
–
J Subst Abuse Treat, 47(1), 58 72. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.01.008
–
lar level. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1121, 639 655. doi:10.1196/annals.1401.024
Spotlight
Why So Impulsive?
Teenagers are universally viewed as an impulsive population. Before the advent
their brains) are more or less how they will be for the rest of their lives. However,
research has shown that the teenage brain is still developing, with areas
for impulse control and decision making – the PFC – being the last to develop
(Figure S8.1). The brain, in essence, develops from the back to the front.
/
128 Impulsivity
multiple years during adolescent development noted that the brain continues
period occur within the white matter tracts that connect different brain
regions. Thus, these frontal control areas are not accessed as rapidly. This leads
References
Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and
impulse control. , 82(4), 463–496. doi:10.1037/h0076860
Psychol Bull
Aron, A. R., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S., Frank, M. J. & Poldrack, R. A. (2007).
Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. ,
J Neurosci
Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. (2008).
High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking.
Science , 320(5881), 1352 –1355. doi:10.1126/science.1158136
Clark, L., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., (2008). Differential effects of
et al.
/
References 129
/
CHAPTER NINE
Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries on Prevention
and Intervention Approaches
Learning Objectives
treatment.
ioral approaches.
Introduction
Because the effects of addiction have such high social implications, it has
historically been viewed primarily as a social problem (i.e. “ disordered
will ”) rather than a medical/health problem. This misconception has
contributed to the current lack of successful approaches to the preven-
tion and intervention of addiction. Over the last two decades, and partly
due to the “Decade of the Brain” in 1990– 2000, a greater scientifi c
understanding and public awareness of addiction as a chronic brain
disease emerged. Thus, current effective treatment programs are based
on the understanding that addiction is a treatable disease that affects
brain function, and that treatment must be individualized and address
other possible mental disorders. As discussed in Chapter 5, although
drugs of abuse have different mechanisms of action, neuroscientifi c
research, particularly in vivo human neuroimaging studies, has provided
evidence that they all alter the brain’s dopaminergic signaling in the
mesolimbic reward system. Dysfunction in this system leads to alter-
ations in reward-processing, motivational and goal-directed behaviors
as well as inhibitory control, as discussed throughout this book. These
are therefore key brain regions and processes that can be targeted in
therapeutic interventions.
/
Introduction 131
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
Figure 9.1 Relapse rates for drug-addicted patients compared with those suffering from
diabetes, hypertension and asthma. Relapse is common and similar across these illnesses (as
is adherence to medication). Thus, drug addiction should be treated like any other chronic
/
132 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
Vocational
services
Mental
Family
health
services
services
Assessment
Evidence-Based treatment
Clinical/case management
Legal Medical
Recovery support programs
services Continuing care services
HIV/AIDS Educational
services services
Figure 9.2 Components of comprehensive drug addiction treatment. The best treatment
programs provide a combination of therapies and other services to meet the needs of the
individual patient.
Pharmacological Approaches
/
Pharmacological Approaches 133
/
134 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
-12R L -9 -6 +3 +6
T value
-3.20
-5.00
+24 +39 +42 +45 +48
Figure 9.3 Following methadone-assisted therapy (MAT), long-term abstinent heroin users
(mean length of abstinence, 193 days) had a greater decreased response in striatal areas
compared with short-term abstinent heroin users (mean length of abstinence, 23 days)
during a cue-induced craving task. (From Liet al. , 2013.) (A black and white version of this
figure will appear in some formats. For the color version, please refer to the plate section.)
/
Behavioral Approaches 135
receptors. The balance of GABA and glutamate tone may be the mech-
anism that leads to its therapeutic effects. Acamprosate has been shown
to reduce craving, leading to dose-dependent effects on decreasing alco-
hol consumption, increasing rate of treatment completion and maintain-
ing abstinence. Using magnetoencephalography (see Chapter 2) in
alcohol-dependent participants, it was found that acamprosate decreased
the arousal level during alcohol withdrawal, as indicated by α slow-wave
index measurement, in the parietotemporal regions (Boeijinga et al.,
2004). This finding is in line with the notion that acamprosate modulates
neuronal hyperexcitability of acute alcohol withdrawal, acting through
glutamatergic neurotransmission.
The aldehyde dehydrogenase inhibitor disul fi ram is an alcohol-
aversive agent that has also been used to treat alcohol use disorder as
a deterrent. Disulfiram markedly alters the metabolism of alcohol, which
leads to increased blood acetaldehyde concentrations. This accumulation
of acetaldehyde leads to aversive effects such asflushing, systemic vaso-
dilation, respiratory diffi culties, nausea, hypotension and other symp-
toms (i.e. acetaldehyde syndrome). In contrast to anti-craving
medications, disulfiram does not modulate neurobiological reward mech-
anisms but rather works by producing an aversive reaction to alcohol. As
a deterrent, the therapeutic effect of disulfiram in supporting abstinence
is mediated through its psychological effects, i.e. the expectancy effect
due to anticipation of the aversive reaction. Evidence for this comes
from a meta-analysis, which showed that the significant therapeutic
effects of disulfiram are greater in open-label trials (Skinner et al., 2014).
Behavioral Approaches
Behavioral approaches are designed to enhance the cognitive deficits
linked to addiction, particularly prefrontal lobe functioning. Prefrontal
areas such as the orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior
cingulate cortices mediate executive functioning such as attention,
working memory, decision making, set shifting and inhibitory control,
among others. Cognitive behavioral models provide cognitive strategies
and training that increase self-control and awareness of triggers for drug
use. For example, cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) may be utilized
for the reduction of a cue-elicited craving response. The “ active ingredi-
ents” of CBT may exert their effects via strengthening aspects of execu-
tive control over behavior. Although the neural mechanisms by which
CBT exerts its therapeutic effects are still unclear, neuroimaging studies
have begun to understand that improvement of brain network function is
/
136 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
involved. For example, CBT has been shown to strengthen the network
connectivity that underlies executive functioning, such as attention
(Lewis et al., 2009). Additionally, an fMRI study investigating cue-
induced craving and using instructions based on CBT strategies to focus
on long-term consequences of tobacco use rather than short-term pleas-
urable tobacco associations found that dorsolateral PFC regions exerted
control over ventral striatal activation in the regulation of craving
(Kober et al., 2010).
Cognitive rehabilitation strategies provide intensive exposure to com-
puterized exercises that strengthen memory, attention, planning and
other executive functioning. Improvement of these cognitive skills
should therefore result in: 1) greater cognitive control over learned
behavior related to substance use; 2) decreased impulsivity; 3) improved
decision making; and 4) awareness of cognitions associated with drug
use. Neuroimaging studies suggest that cognitive rehabilitation may
normalize regional brain activation in the PFC (Wexler et al., 2000).
Bickel et al. (2011) demonstrated that focused training on computerized
memory tasks resulted in significant reductions in an aspect of impulsiv-
ity, delay discounting (i.e. preference for immediate versus delayed
rewards), among stimulant users.
Psychosocial interventions such as motivational enhancement therapy
(MET) and motivational interviewing (MI) are brief and focused inter-
ventions that aim to increase one’ s motivation to change. Research
suggests that the effi cacy of these approaches depends on age, type of
drug addiction and the goal of the intervention. For example, MET has
shown treatment success in cannabis-using adults but not consistently in
adolescents or in those using cocaine, heroin or nicotine. Feldstein
Ewing et al. (2011) suggested that MI supports a reduction in substance
use by attenuation of the response in regions in the reward pathway,
which suggest that the effi cacy of MI is in reducing the salience of drug
cues. Furthermore, they found that the active ingredient in MI, i.e. client
change talk, elicited activation in areas that underlie self-awareness– the
left inferior frontal gyrus/anterior insula and superior temporal gyri
(Feldstein Ewing et al., 2014). Contingency management (CM)
approaches have shown strong empirical support in randomized clinical
trials. CM corrects the amplified valuation of immediate reward and the
discounted value of delayed rewards (delay discounting) by reinforcing
targeted outcomes with positive incentives. Delay discounting has been
associated with poor treatment outcome for addiction and has been
shown to involve cortical and subcortical systems involved in decision
making (Balleine et al., 2007). Subcortical reward regions such as the
/
Combined Approaches 137
/
138 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
Cognitive Behavioral
enhancement treatments (CBT,
treatments CM, MI and other)
Prefrontal cortex
α 2
agonists and
NET inhibitors
DA Glu Glutamate
medications
L. Cer.
DA agonists
Opioid GABA and antagonists
agonist and
medications
antagonists
Figure 9.4 Proposed model illustrating synergistic mechanisms between behavioral and
transporter; Nac, nucleus accumbens; Glu, glutamate; VTA, ventral tegmental area; L. Cer.,
and orbitofrontal cortex (Figure 9.5). They also noted that, while there
were overlaps, behavioral interventions were more likely to modulate
the response in the anterior cingulate, middle frontal gyrus and precu-
neus/posterior cingulate cortex relative to pharmacological interven-
tions, con firming the “top-down” notion of behavioral interventions as
suggested by the model of Potenza et al. (2011). Overall, these findings
suggest a potential mechanism by which the combined use of pharmaco-
logical and cognitive-based strategies may produce synergistic (due to
their common targets) or complementary (due to their distinct targets)
therapeutic effects. The infl uences of behavioral interventions on pre-
frontal and parietal cortical regions may be important for treatment
adherence.
Treatment Outcomes
/
Treatment Outcomes 139
interventions interventions
VS VS VS
Y = 13
L R
A P
X = –3
A
ACC ACC P
X =8
MFG MFG
L R
Prec Prec
Z = 40
Figure 9.5 Common (a) and distinct (b) neural targets of pharmacological and cognitive-
cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; L, left; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; OFC, orbitofrontal
cortex; P, posterior; Prec, precuneus; R, right; VS, ventral striatum. (From Konovaet al.,
fi
2013. © 2013 Elsevier, USA.) (A black and white version of this gure will appear in some
formats. For the color version, please refer to the plate section.)
/
140 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
/
Further Reading 141
Summary Points
• Studies demonstrate that a combination of pharmacological and cognitive
approaches lead to better treatment success.
• There are three stages to the recovery from addiction: detoxification, initial
recovery and relapse prevention.
• The synergistic mechanisms in combined pharmacological and behavioral
therapies may be a combination of “top-down” mechanisms through
behavioral intervention with “bottom-up” processes in pharmacological
approaches.
Review Questions
• What are the common targets of pharmacological and cognitive therapies?
• How can neuroimaging methods lead to individualized treatment?
• What are the three primary stages of addiction intervention?
• How could behavioral and pharmacological treatment mechanisms com-
plement each other?
• What biological pathways do behavioral and pharmacological treatments
both target?
Further Reading
Bickel, W. K., Christensen, D. R. & Marsch, L. A. (2011). A review of
computer-based interventions used in the assessment, treatment, and
research of drug addiction. Subst Use Misuse , 46(1), 4–9. doi:10.3109/
10826084.2011.521066
Chung, T., Noronha, A., Carroll, K. M., et al. (2016). Brain mechanisms of
change in addictions treatment: models, methods, and emergingfindings.
Curr Addict Rep , 3(3), 332–342. doi:10.1007/s40429-016-0113-z
Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Filbey, F. M., Hendershot, C. S., McEachern, A. D. &
Hutchison, K. E. (2011). Proposed model of the neurobiological mechan-
isms underlying psychosocial alcohol interventions: the example of motiv-
ational interviewing.
J Stud Alcohol Drugs , 72(6), 903–916.
Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Filbey, F. M., Sabbineni, A., Chandler, L. D. & Hutch-
ison, K. E. (2011). How psychosocial alcohol interventions work: a prelimin-
ary look at what FMRI can tell us. Alcohol Clin Exp Res , 35(4), 643–651.
doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01382.x
/
142 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Houck, J. M., Yezhuvath, U.,et al. (2016). The impact
of therapists’ words on the adolescent brain: in the context of addiction
treatment. Behav Brain Res, 297, 359–369. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.09.041
Feldstein Ewing, S. W., McEachern, A. D., Yezhuvath, U.,et al. (2013).
Integrating brain and behavior: evaluating adolescents’ response to a can-
nabis intervention. Psychol Addict Behav, 27(2), 510–525. doi:10.1037/
a0029767
Gilfillan, K. V., Dannatt, L., Stein, D. J. & Vythilingum, B. (2018). Heroin
detoxification during pregnancy: a systematic review and retrospective
study of the management of heroin addiction in pregnancy.S Afr Med J,
108(2), 111–117. doi:10.7196/SAMJ.2017.v108i2.7801
Glasner-Edwards, S. & Rawson, R. (2010). Evidence-based practices in addic-
tion treatment: review and recommendations for public policy. Health
Policy, 97(2–3), 93–104. doi:10.1016/j.healthpol.2010.05.013
Gorsane, M. A., Kebir, O., Hache, G.,et al. (2012). Is baclofen a revolutionary
medication in alcohol addiction management? Review and recent updates.
Subst Abus, 33(4), 336 –349. doi:10.1080/08897077.2012.663326
Liu, J., Nie, J. & Wang, Y. (2017). Effects of group counseling programs,
cognitive behavioral therapy, and sports intervention on internet addiction
in East Asia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public
Health , 14(12). doi:10.3390/ijerph14121470
Spotlight 1
/
Spotlight 2 143
Figure S9.1 Peer addiction recovery specialists bring different perspective to treatment.
In the case of Lehigh Valley, each peer recovery specialist supports up to thirty
clients.
The benefits of peer counseling programs are reciprocal. The process of
providing support and managing the functional needs of others encourages
peer recovery specialists to maintain the same level of expectations for them-
selves. In short, as peer counselors encourage their clients to resist the urge to
use substances, so do they. Witnessing others overcome their addiction
through the program also keeps the peer counselors motivated and encour-
aged to continue down their path.
Spotlight 2
/
144 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
in 2010 to 25% in 2015, according to the Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention), the costs for treatment programs are expected to rise, contrib-
uting toward growing economic challenges in healthcare. For example, the
budget cuts in the Affordable Care Act’s requirement for addiction services
under Medicaid have led to a 2018 ban on drug toxicology tests that verify
adherence to treatment and abstinence during addiction treatment in Mary-
land. The Maryland Medicaid program claimed to have spent 23% of its $315
million budget for substance use treatment. Most legislators acknowledge the
opioid epidemic and advocate for more drug treatment centers but are
hindered by the associated costs. As an alternative approach, legislative
leaders, such as those in Indiana, have reached out to private foundations
to help fund more centers. Additionally, a Senate committee is considering a
bill that allows tougher penalties against drug dealers if one of their custom-
ers dies of an overdose.
Despite these costs, changes in legislation have been put in place to maxi-
mize treatment opportunities. In 2017, Jessie’s Law was passed by the Senate
ensuring that clinical providers have information on patients’ substance abuse
history1. House-passed bills would make drug treatment available in jail to
people charged with misdemeanors and would make it easier for drug coun-
selors to be licensed, to fund overdose rescue medications such as naloxone
and to study whether office-based treatment programs should be licensed.
References
drugalcdep.2008.01.009
Balleine, B. W., Delgado, M. R. & Hikosaka, O. (2007). The role of the
dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. , 27(31),
J Neurosci
1 Jessie s Law was named after Jessica Grubb who was in recovery from opioid abuse when
’
she underwent surgery. Her discharging physician did not receive the information about
her history of opioid use and erroneously discharged her with a prescription forfifty
oxycodone tablets. Jessie overdosed and died the same night.
/
References 145
Bickel, W. K., Yi, R., Landes, R. D., Hill, P. F. & Baxter, C. (2011).
Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay
discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry, 69(3), 260–265.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017
Boeijinga, P. H., Parot, P., Soufflet, L.,et al. (2004). Pharmacodynamic
effects of acamprosate on markers of cerebral function in
alcohol-dependent subjects administered as pretreatment and during
alcohol abstinence. Neuropsychobiology , 50(1), 71– 77. doi:10.1159/
000077944
Carroll, K. M., Kiluk, B. D., Nich, C., (2011). Cognitive function and
et al.
/
146 Impacts of Brain-Based Discoveries
Lewis, C. C., Simons, A. D., Silva, S. G., (2009). The role of readiness to
et al.
Li, Q., Wang, Y., Zhang, Y., et al. (2013). Assessing cue-induced brain
response as a function of abstinence duration in heroin-dependent
individuals: an event-related fMRI study. 8(5): e62911.
PLoS One
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0062911
Loughead, J., Ray, R., Wileyto, E. P., (2010). Effects of the α4β2 partial
et al.
biopsych.2010.01.016
McLellan, A. T., Lewis, D. C., O Brien, C. P. & Kleber, H. D. (2000). Drug
’
doi:10.1001/jama.284.13.1689
National Institute on Drug Abuse (2018). Treatment approaches for drug
addiction. Available at: www.drugabuse.gov/publications/drugfacts/
treatment-approaches-drug-addiction (accessed November 11, 2018).
Ooteman, W., Naassila, M., Koeter, M. W., (2009). Predicting the effect
et al.
1600.2009.00159.x
Potenza, M. N., Sofuoglu, M., Carroll, K. M. & Rounsaville, B. J. (2011).
Neuroscience of behavioral and pharmacological treatments for
addictions. Neuron , 69(4), 695 712. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.009
–
j.1521-0391.2011.00130.x
Sofuoglu, M., DeVito, E. E., Waters, A. J. & Carroll, K. M. (2013). Cognitive
enhancement as a treatment for drug addictions. ,
Neuropharmacology
doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.09.025
/
References 147
/
C H A PT E R T EN
Conclusions
Learning Objectives
• fi
Be able to summarize how neuroscienti c research has advanced our
understanding of addiction.
in addiction.
Introduction
As Chapters 1 and 9 discussed, the social implications of addiction have
led to the stigma that addiction is a social problem. This general public
opinion may originate from the more evident societal burden of addic-
tion relative to the personal burden that is usually minimized by the
sufferer. For example, approximately $67 billion is spent in the USA due
to crime, lost work productivity and social support related to addiction.
This stigma of addiction as a non-medical disorder has perpetuated in
medical settings where the training curricula continue to place little to no
emphasis on programs related to the treatment of addiction. As a result,
medical practices rarely evaluate potential substance-related problems,
which, in turn, leads to poor prognosis. The preceding chapters discussed
how the operational de fi nition of addiction has been validated by neuro-
scientifi c research in the absence of diagnostic laboratory tests or
biomarkers for substance use disorder or addiction (see Chapter 1 for
diagnostic criteria). Indeed, neuroscience research, especially with the
advancements of in vivo human imaging techniques, has provided us
/
Risk Factors Inform Better Prevention and Intervention 149
/
150 Conclusions
also been associated with a greater risk for later development of addic-
tion. Stress induces the release of central corticotropin-releasing factor
from the hypothalamus that binds to corticotropin-releasing factor
receptors in the pituitary. This interaction in the pituitary stimulates
the production of active peptides, including β-endorphin and adrenocor-
ticotropic hormone, which is carried via blood to the adrenal glands
where it induces the secretion of glucocorticoids. The glucocorticoids
are then transported by the blood to the brain, where they act on
numerous signaling systems including the dopaminergic reward system,
in addition to systems involved in physiological stress responses (e.g.
increases in blood glucose levels and blood pressure) (see Chapter 6 for
more information on neuroadaptations related to stress). This stress-
related modulation of the reward system during neurodevelopment
may therefore disrupt the maturational process of the reward system.
Indeed, pre-clinical studies in rats show that early life stress is associated
with dysregulation in midbrain circuitry (Chocyk , 2015), linked to
dysfunctions in reward-related behavior (see Spotlight 1 for more on the
et al.
/
Addiction Endophenotypes 151
0.8
e g n ar ±
0.6
2
0.4
h
0.2
Mean
)9
)7
)0
)7
)6
)9
)4
)8
)0
)2
9
7
0
5
9
5
2
1
2
6
4
5
6
2
,3
,9
,6
,2
,7
,3
,4
,4
,0
,2
(
(
(
(
(
1
(
lo
s
s
e
e
s
(
s
n
ib
e
e
n
tn
n
ta
v
ie
ia
e
n
a
lb
o
a
it
g
ik
c
n
ip
ff
c
lu
m
o
lA
o
n
o
d
O
m
a
n
C
a
m
e
G
ic
C
it
S
S
u
ll
a
H
Figure 10.1 Heritability ( 2; weighted means and ranges) of ten addictions based on a
h
/
152 Conclusions
60 (e)
n o it u b i rts i d dev res bO
(a)
50
Variability in measures of temperament and
30 stressors
20
Personality measure
60
(b)
e r usaem yt i la n os reP
50
to threat)
30
20
1.5
(c)
n o itc n uf t i uc r ic n ia r B
1.0
antagonism)
–0.5
7.0
(d)
6.0
1.0
AA AB BB
Figure 10.2 Integration of complementary technologies (e) can be used to reveal the
can then be associated with serotonin signaling (via PET) (c) and tied to variability in the
/
Addiction Endophenotypes 153
e pyt o n e h p f o yt ixe l pm oC
s isyl a n a c it e n e g d n a
Number of genes
Figure 10.3 The concept of endophenotypes is that they lie in the causal pathway between
/
154 Conclusions
30 Brain
20
10
oscillations
0
0
0 700 0 700 0
2 2
Power µv Power µv
0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60
Chromosome 7
3.5 θ
3
Cz, Max LOD=3.6 at 164 cM
Genetic
DOL
1.5
linkage
1
2MRHC
8 M RG
0.5
0
0971S7D
2081S7D
8381S7D
6482S7D
0381S7D
6403S7D
0781S7D
7971S7D
6971S7D
9971S7D
7181S7D
7482S7D
9881S7D
4081S7D
4281S7D
5081S7D
315S7D
926S7D
376S7D
718S7D
125S7D
196S7D
874S7D
976S7D
566S7D
028S7D
128S7D
894S7D
905S7D
497S7D
2YPN
CHRM2
Candidate
no xe
no xe
no xe
no xe
no xe
gene
1 2 3 81.7 kb 4 41.1 kb 5 22.6 kb 6
Coding 3’ -UTR
5’ -UTR
Sequence
SNPs
06274231 sr
8554241 sr
4754241 sr
9654241 sr
0870532 sr
734879 sr
8121 c c
5692877 sr
6468731 sr
4204281 sr
0568731 sr
7834241 sr
587 c c
0710087 sr
8585541 sr
4711602 sr
7409977 sr
6870532 sr
4508496 sr
056423 sr
156423 sr
2991918 sr
3991918 sr
8454241 sr
656423 sr
5 xe2mrh c
046423 sr
Genetic
8554241 sr
association
maetsnwod
maertspu
4-3nortn i
5-4nortn i
6-5nortn i
1no xe
5no xe
RTU ’3
6no xe
Figure 10.4 Brain EEG oscillations may be useful endophenotypes for alcohol use disorders.
(From Rangaswamy & Porjesz, 2008.) (A black and white version of thisfigure will appear in
some formats. For the color version, please refer to the plate section.)
develop decision making and inhibitory control process that would lead
/
Sex Differences in Addiction 155
5000
*** ***
**
4500
4000
3500
)3 m m( e muloV
3000
2500
2000
1500
1000
500
0
L hippocampus* R hippocampus*
Figure 10.5 Changes in brain volume may be an endophenotype for cannabis use disorder.
fi
The graph shows a signi cant difference in bilateral hippocampal volumes for cannabis users
and matched healthy controls according to genotype. *P ± 0.05 for interaction between
identify signi fi cant cognitive impairments from risk factors that com-
tive pro file could better facilitate targeted strategies that support
/
156 Conclusions
/
General Conclusions 157
birth cohort of 1037 people born between April 1972 and March 1973 in
Dunedin, New Zealand. The results of this study reported that daily
cannabis users who initiated use during adolescence had elevated risk for
psychosis as well as cognitive declines, such as a loss of 8 IQ points as
assessed from age 11 to age 38 (Figure 10.6) (Meieret al., 2012).
General Conclusions
Figure 10.6 (a) Birth cohort design. (b) The prospective study included initiation alcohol
and drug use. (c) Using a prospective, longitudinal design on a birth cohort, the Dunedin
/
158 Conclusions
(c)
0.4
One diagnosis Two diagnoses Three or more diagnoses
0.2
) stinu noit ai v ed d r adn at s ni(
QI el a c s-lluf ni egn ah C
–0.2
–0.4
–0.6
–0.8
P = 0.44 P = 0.09 P = 0.02
Cannabis Not cannabis Cannabis Not cannabis Cannabis Not cannabis
before age 18 before age 18 before age 18 before age 18 before age 18 before age 18
(n = 17) n = 57)
( (n = 12) n = 21)
( (n = 23) ( n = 14)
/
Review Questions 159
course of the addiction cycle that lead to the positive reinforcing effects
events that occur between the genetic mechanisms and the expression of
addiction to better understand factors that increase risk for, but also
throughout this book highlight some of these gaps that have current
can help change the face of and eliminate the stigma related to addiction.
Summary Points
• Advancements in neuroscience techniques have paved the way for our
interventions.
Review Questions
• How do risk factors leave the brain vulnerable to addiction?
• fi
What is the bene t of identifying endophenotypes for addiction?
/
160 Conclusions
Further Reading
Abasi, I. & Mohammadkhani, P. (2016). Family risk factors among women
with addiction-related problems: an integrative review. Int J High Risk
603–612. doi:10.1037/adb0000076
Filbey, F. M., Schacht, J. P., Myers, U. S., Chavez, R. S. & Hutchison, K. E.
(2010). Individual and additive effects of the and
CNR1 FAAHgenes on
brain response to marijuana cues. Neuropsychopharmacology , 35(4),
967–975. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.200
Ketcherside, A., Baine, J. & Filbey, F. (2016). Sex effects of marijuana on brain
structure and function. Curr Addict Rep , 3, 323–331. doi:10.1007/s40429-
016-0114-y
Konova, A. B., Moeller, S. J., Parvaz, M. A., (2016). Converging effects of
et al.
3–18. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.09.004
Prashad, S., Milligan, A. L., Cousijn, J. & Filbey, F. M. (2017). Cross-cultural
effects of cannabis use disorder: evidence to support a cultural neurosci-
ence approach. Curr Addict Rep, 4(2), 100–109. doi:10.1007/s40429-017-
0145-z
Puetz, V. B. & McCrory, E. (2015). Exploring the relationship between child-
hood maltreatment and addiction: a review of the neurocognitive evidence.
Curr Addict Rep , 2(4), 318–325. doi:10.1007/s40429-015-0073-8
/
Spotlight 1 161
Spotlight 1
parents being shot to death. This traumatic event not only changed his life
instantly in that moment but also changed its course dramatically. McDonald
was a responsible citizen and father; however, when he became involved with
deeply rooted trauma from his early childhood. He began using cannabis to
treat his post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) from the murder of his parents.
in childhood and into adulthood, such as PTSD and addiction (Figure S10.1).
(From www.pexels.com/photo/adult-alone-black-and-white-dark-551588/.)
Patients with PTSD have reported that cannabis provides relief from their
symptoms with fewer side effects than prescribed medications. To date, most
answered.
/
162 Conclusions
Spotlight 2
A Rocker s Fight Against Addiction
’
In February 2018, the musician Flea disclosed his struggles with addiction in a
fl
5168435/ ea-temptation-drug-addiction-opioid-crisis/). Flea, who is the lead
bassist for the rock band Red Hot Chili Peppers, candidly described his rst- fi
hand life experiences that contributed to his substance abuse and addiction,
and that eventually led him back to good health. Stating that drugs have been
a fixture in his life since infancy, he also described witnessing loved ones’ lives
end tragically due to addiction. He details how ful filling responsibilities as a
father was challenging yet infl uential in his fi ght against the disease and
would later help him defeat it. Alongside his personal motivation, he ascribes
tion, exercise and spiritual guidance. In the end, he claims that recognizing
and accepting the challenges of addiction“helped [him] stay away from the
“It’s always there, seducing you to come on in and get your head right,
” as he
describes repeatedly dealing with severe anxieties that challenge his sobriety.
In light of the current opioid epidemic in the USA, he recalls his own
experience with opioids and is forthright about the role that the medical
him home with a 2-month supply with instructions to take as many as four
pills per day. He described how Oxycontin removed his physical pain but also
discontinued his use of Oxycontin before his 2-month supply was depleted,
his first-hand experience has given him insight into how little we know about
pain management and how our current approaches need to be improved.
References
Bobzean, S. A., DeNobrega, A. K. & Perrotti, L. I. (2014). Sex differences in
the neurobiology of drug addiction. Exp Neurol, 259, 64– 74.
doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2014.01.022
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C. & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the
developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development?
Trends Cogn Sci, 9(3), 104–110. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011
Chocyk, A., Majcher-Maslanka, I., Przyborowska, A., Mackowiak, M. &
Wedzony, K. (2015). Early-life stress increases the survival of midbrain
/
References 163
/
164 Conclusions
/
Glossary
/
166 Glossary
brain tumors. It also serves many other functions throughout the body
including neurotransmitter synthesis, cell membrane signaling, liquid
transport and methyl group metabolism.
Classical conditioning – a mechanism of learning and memory, in which
one associates a relevant stimulus with an otherwise, non-relevant
stimulus. Typically occurs after repeated exposure to the two stimuli
together.
Cognitive behavioral model – a theory based on the assumption that mental
processes can influence emotional and behavioral (physiological)
responses.
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) – a type of therapy that seeks to help
patients recognize, avoid and cope with the situations in which they are
most likely to abuse drugs.
Computed tomography (CT) – a type of computerized X-ray imaging that
constructs a three-dimensional image from many individual cross-
sectional X-ray images, taken in succession, of an anatomical region.
Used primarily in neuroscience for structural measurements of the
nervous system.
Contingency management (CM) – a method that uses positive reinforcement
such as providing rewards or privileges for remaining drug free, for
attending and participating in counseling sessions, or for taking
treatment medications as prescribed.
Craving – the intense desire to use or obtain a substance. May be
continuous, or may occur randomly or after presentation of drug-
related cues.
Creatine – an amino acid that is utilized by cells under high-energy demand.
This metabolite is commonly targeted in magnetic resonance
spectroscopy (MRS) to examine metabolic activity in neurons of the
human brain.
Cue reactivity – a conditioned response (craving) to various stimuli that are
associated (either naturally or through repeated exposure) with drug-
seeking and drug-taking behaviors.
Delay discounting – the tendency to undervalue a reward or punishment that
is received after a delayed time period. This concept is thought to be the
underlying principle of the tendency of individuals to choose smaller,
more immediate rewards over bigger rewards that require a waiting time
for receipt.
Depressant – a substance that slows the activity of the central nervous
system, typically through activation of GABAergic neurons. This
category includes sedatives, tranquilizers and alcohol.
Diffusivity – the pattern and nature of a substance’s ability to spread (or
diffuse) throughout a system.
/
Glossary 167
/
168 Glossary
/
Glossary 169
/
170 Glossary
/
Glossary 171
/
172 Glossary
/
Index
/
174 Index
/
Index 175
/
176 Index
/
Index 177
/
178 Index
/
Index 179
/
180 Index
–
between systems adaptations, 90 91 Wray, J. M., 105
fi
de ned, 81 82–
–
electrophysiological mechanisms of, 88 90 Zubieta, J. K., 86
/
HA B
J I C
K E F
N D
Q M G
P L
O
5 G
1.0
C 0.9
B Age
0.8
A
H 0.7
I
J 0.6
r ett am y ar G
20 0.5
K
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
years of age.
l1
l2 l1 l2
l3
l3
Isotropic Anisotropic
Plate 2.4 Gray matter has predominantly isotropic (soccer ball-shaped) water diffusion,
while dense white matter tracks have highly anisotropic (rugby ball-shaped) diffusion of
/
(a) 2-FA PET imaging of nAChR occupancy from cigarette smoke exposure
kBq/mL MRI
9
0.0 Cigarette 0.1 Cigarette 0.3 Cigarette 1.0 Cigarette 3.0 Cigarette 0
(b) (c)
kBq V s/fp
10 10
0 0
MRI No smoking Q-3 Q-1 T1-weighted Control Second-hand
(0.0 ng/ml) (0.4 ng/ml) (2.6 ng/ml) MRI smoke
Relapse-prone
group
Current density
2
[uAmm/mm ]
0.00597
Left hem. 0.00490 Right hem.
0.00398
0.00299
0.00199
0.000996
0
Abstinence-prone
group
Plate 6.3 Fast β power can be a predictor of relapse in polysubstance users during a 3-month abstinence.
/
(a) (b) (c)
DGsp
DGip
(d) (e)
(f) (g)
IN IN
VMPF VMPF
1 2 3
1 2 3 4 5>
# of overlaps
Healthy controls
80
Lesion controls
VMPF
70 Insula
60
50
teB %
40
30
20
10
9 to 1 8 to 2 7 to 3 6 to 4
Chance of winning
Plate 8.5 Ventromedial PFC lesions lead to risky decision making.
/
-12R L -9 -6 +3 +6
T value
-3.20
-5.00
+24 +39 +42 +45 +48
Plate 9.3 Following methadone-assisted therapy (MAT), long-term abstinent heroin users
(mean length of abstinence, 193 days) had a greater decreased response in striatal areas
compared with short-term abstinent heroin users (mean length of abstinence, 23 days)
/
(a) Pharmacological
interventions
Cognitive-based
interventions
Conjunction
A ACC ACC P
X=8
MFG MFG
L R
Prec Prec
Z=40
Plate 9.5 Common (a) and distinct (b) neural targets of pharmacological and cognitive-
/
Controls (N=100) ERO Alcoholics (N=100) ERO
TOT Head plot θ
TOT
θ
12 power µv
2 12
Fz Fz
40
30 Brain
20
10
oscillations
0
0
0 700 0 700 0
2 2
Power µv Power µv
0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60
Chromosome 7
θ
3.5
3
Cz, Max LOD=3.6 at 164 cM
Genetic
DOL
1.5
linkage
1
2MRHC
8 M RG
0.5
0
0971S7D
2081S7D
8381S7D
6482S7D
0381S7D
6403S7D
0781S7D
7971S7D
6971S7D
9971S7D
7181S7D
7482S7D
9881S7D
4081S7D
4281S7D
5081S7D
315S7D
926S7D
376S7D
718S7D
125S7D
196S7D
874S7D
976S7D
566S7D
028S7D
128S7D
894S7D
905S7D
497S7D
2YPN
CHRM2
Candidate
no xe
no xe
no xe
no xe
no xe
gene
1 2 3 81.7 kb 4 41.1 kb 5 22.6 kb 6
Coding 3’ -UTR
5’ -UTR
Sequence
SNPs
06274231 sr
8554241 sr
4754241 sr
9654241 sr
7834241 sr
0870532 sr
734879 sr
8121 c c
5692877 sr
0710087 sr
8585541 sr
6468731 sr
4204281 sr
4711602 sr
7409977 sr
6870532 sr
4508496 sr
156423 sr
0568731 sr
8454241 sr
656423 sr
587 c c
5 xe2mrh c
046423 sr
056423 sr
2991918 sr
3991918 sr
Genetic
8554241 sr
association
maetsnwod
4-3nortn i
5-4nortn i
6-5nortn i
maertspu
RTU ’3
1no xe
5no xe
6no xe
Plate 10.4 Brain EEG oscillations may be useful endophenotypes for alcohol use disorders.