You are on page 1of 10

The Effectiveness of Parental Discipline for Toddler Misbehavior at Different Levels of Child

Distress
Author(s): Robert E. Larzelere and Jack A. Merenda
Source: Family Relations, Vol. 43, No. 4, Family Processes and Child and Adolescent
Development (Oct., 1994), pp. 480-488
Published by: National Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/585381 .
Accessed: 16/01/2011 01:47

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at .
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfr. .

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Family Relations.

http://www.jstor.org
THE EFFECTIVENESSOF PARENTALDISCIPLINEFOR TODDLER
MISBEHAVIORAT DIFFERENTLEVELSOF CHILD DISTRESS*

Robert E. Larzelere andJack A. Merenda**

Behavioral theories and Hoffman's information-processing theory have differing implications for how the effectiveness of
parental discipline varies according to the level of distress experienced by the child. Consistent with behavioral theories, pun-
ishment was more effective in delaying the next recurrence of disobedience when toddlers' distress was high than when it was
low or moderate. Consistent with Hoffman, reasoning and a punishment-reasoning combination were most effective at a mod-
erate level of toddler distress. Thus, firm reasoning and a combination of reasoning with mild punishment are recommended.

D espite decades of research on A child's emotional reaction to disci- get a child to pay attention to the induc-
parental discipline, major contro- pline is considered to be relevant for tion component. If parents use too little
versies still remain. Leading re- moral internalization according to be- power assertion and love withdrawal,
search investigators have acknowledged havioral and cognitive theories of social- the child may ignore the induction com-
that evidence is inconclusive about the ization. First, behavioral theories hold ponent. Too much power assertion,
effectiveness of alternative parental dis- that moral inhibition occurs because of however, may interfere with the child's
cipline responses to child misbehavior conditioned anxiety. Conditioned anxi- cognitive processing of the induction
(e.g., Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Patter- ety, in turn, develops when misbehav- component.
son, 1982). Consider, for example, iors are paired with negative conse- Thus, according to Hoffman, some
whether punishment (i.e., negative con- quences (Aronfreed, 1968). Behavioral mild, intermediate level of power asser-
sequences) or reasoning should be pre- studies of negative consequences have tion is optimal for attention to and reten-
ferred as a discipline response to misbe- found that the greater the intensity of tion of the induction component. Al-
havior. Behavioral parental training pro- the consequences, the greater their ef- though the attentional processing aspect
grams feature time out as a consequence fectiveness (Azrin & Holz, 1966; Matson of Hoffman's theory is appealing, there
for child misbehavior (e.g., Forehand & & DiLorenzo, 1984; Van Houten, 1983). have been few attempts to verify it em-
McMahon, 1981). The only form of rea- This suggests that the higher the level of pirically. Hoffman's evidence consists
soning in such training programs is a a child's anxiety following negative con- primarily of child correlates of parental
specification of the conditions for time sequences, the greater the resulting reasoning, on the one hand, and of
out. On the other hand, cognitive social- moral inhibition. Some laboratory ana- power assertion, on the other (Hoffman,
ization theorists recommend reasoning logue studies have found that the severi- 1970, 1977). Parental use of reasoning is
rather than punishments such as time ty of negative consequences increased positively associated with moral behav-
out or spanking (e.g., Hoffman, 1977; subsequent moral inhibition when no ior in their children, whereas power as-
Lepper, 1983). verbal component was included in the sertion tends to be negatively associated
This study is part of a research pro- discipline response. However, the inclu- with moral behavior (Hoffman, 1970;
gram designed to synthesize behavioral sion of reasoning reduced or eliminated Rollins & Thomas, 1979). Hoffman ac-
and cognitive views of socialization, and the association between consequence counts for these results by arguing that
is one of the few attempts to do so. The severity and subsequent moral inhibition most induction elicits sufficient affect in
program investigates the effectiveness of (Cheyne, Goyeche, & Walters, 1969; children to get their attention, whereas
alternative parental discipline responses Cheyne & Walters, 1969; Parke, 1969). most power assertion elicits too much
in delaying the next recurrence of tod- Second, Hoffman's (1977, 1983) in- fear and anxiety for optimal cognitive
dler misbehavior. Of central interest in formation-processing theory views the processing.
this particular study is how the effects of cognitive and affective aspects of disci-
alternative discipline responses vary ac- pline incidents as crucial for moral inter-
cording to the level of child distress ex- nalization. He divides discipline respons-
perienced following the parental disci- *The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of a
es into three types: power assertion, Biola University Faculty Research Grant; a Biola University
pline response. love withdrawal, and induction. Power Sabbatical Grant; Grant T32 MH 17126 from the Center for
Studies of Antisocial and Violent Behavior, National Institute
Before summarizing the implica- assertion depends on parents' power ad- of Mental Health, to Oregon Social Learning Center; and Fa-
tions of behavioral versus cognitive the- vantage relative to that of the child, that ther Flanagan's Boys' Home. Students who provided indis-
is, the use of force, deprivation of privi- pensable help to this project included William N. Schneider,
ories for the role of child distress in dis- Joyce Handler, Angela Rose Huntsman, Diana Elliott, Richard
cipline effectiveness, some definitions leges, or threats. Love withdrawal tech- Erenst, Jack Young, Shari Bridgman, Sharmon Skill, Celia Ka-
are in order. In this article, a discipline niques implicitly remove parental love vanagh, Thomas Young, Roberta Miller, and Theodore Myer.
The authors are grateful to Jamie Sinclair and two anony-
technique is a specific tactic used by a toward the child, such as nonphysical mous reviewers for their very helpful comments on previous
parent in response to an incident of expressions of parental anger or disap- versions. This article is based on the second author's disser-
tation and was presented at the Convention of the American
child misbehavior. A discipline response proval. Induction communicates reasons Psychological Association, Toronto, August 1993.
is a set of one or more discipline tech- for the desired behavior, including con- **Robert E. Larzelere is Director of Residential Re-
necting appropriate behavior to the search, Father Flanagan's Boys' Home, Youth Care Bldg.,
niques that constitute the entire parental Boys Town, NE 68010. Jack A. Merenda is now a clinical
response to a particular misbehavior in- child's desires or to its effect on other psychologist at Associated Psychological Services, 2 North
cident. Negative consequences or sim- people. Most discipline responses have a Lake Ave., Suite 610, Pasadena, CA 91101.

ply consequences refer to punitive disci- power-assertive component, a love-with- Key Words: aggression, noncompliance, parental disci-
pline techniques or responses, including drawal component, and an induction pline, punishment, toddlers.
time out, withdrawal of privileges, and component. Mild forms of the first two
nonabusive spanking. components are considered necessary to (Family Relations, 1994, 43, 480-488.)

480 FAMILY October 1994 [tl


A few studies have provided indirect 2. Consistent with Hoffman's theo- all but one of the others had completed
evidence for the attentional aspect of ry, when reasoning is incorporated into high school. Eighty-five percent were
Hoffman's theory. Research has shown the discipline response, intermediate Caucasian, 13% were Hispanic, and 3%
that the verbal component of a discipline levels of child distress are expected to were Asian-American. All were two-par-
response is more effective under condi- correspond with optimal effectiveness. ent families.
tions of mild consequences than under 3. Consistent with the laboratory
conditions of severe consequences analogue studies, when negative conse- Procedure
(Cheyne et al., 1969; Cheyne & Walters, quences and reasoning are used togeth- Mothers provided data using a struc-
1969; Hoffman, 1963). Cheyne's studies er, effectiveness will not be positively tured diary format similar to methods
found physiological indicators of atten- related to the level of child distress used by Goodenough (1931) and by
tional processes (e.g., heart-rate decelera- (Cheyne et al., 1969; Parke, 1969). Zahn-Waxler and Radke-Yarrow (1982).
tion) only under conditions that com-
Effectiveness will be measured by Each mother received a 90-minute train-
bined a verbal component with mild
the mean delay until the next recur- ing session in her own home to familiar-
consequences (as opposed to severe
rence of that type of misbehavior. The ize her with the procedures for record-
consequences). Kochanska (1991) mea-
targeted discipline problems represent ing the relevant data in the Discipline
sured the extent to which parents used
the two most common kinds of toddler Record booklet (see Figure 1). She re-
mostly power assertion methods or in-
misbehavior: disobedience and fighting viewed written definitions of the 21
duction methods when their child was 1-
(Larzelere, Amberson, & Martin, 1992). parental discipline techniques on the
1/2 and 3-1/2 years of age. Greater use of
The age range of 25 to 38 months was Discipline Record and practiced them
induction predicted several measures of
chosen because perceived discipline using written and video-taped vignettes
conscience 5 to 6 years later, but only in
problems reach their maximum frequen- of discipline incidents. The video train-
temperamentally anxious children. Rela-
cy during those months (Larzelere et al., ing tape depicted the 21 parental disci-
tive usage of power assertion versus in-
1992). pline techniques and then illustrated
duction was generally unrelated to subse-
role play vignettes of complex discipline
quent measures of conscience in less One important question about rea- sequences for the mothers to code.
anxious children. These findings may be soning with toddlers is whether they are
consistent with Hoffman's posited atten- developmentally ready to process that The experimenters telephoned the
tional processes in that greater power as- information. Hoffman (1983) has argued subjects twice weekly throughout the
sertion may be necessary to get the atten- that most children have sufficient cogni- duration of the study to maintain com-
tion of less anxious preschoolers than is tive processing skills by ages 2 or 3. pliance with the data collection and to
the case for more anxious preschoolers. Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, Wagner, discuss any practical problems. To deter-
Thus, a balance between induction and and Chapman (1992) concluded, "chil- mine the consistency of record keeping,
power assertion may be necessary to dren as young as 2 years old have the each mother was asked on three occa-
elicit an optimal degree of attention from cognitive capacity to interpret the physi- sions how soon a discipline incident was
less anxious toddlers. But no study has cal and psychological states of others" typically recorded in the Discipline
confirmed the major implication of the (p. 127). Therefore, one aspect of this Record and what percentage of relevant
attentional aspect of Hoffman's theory, study is to determine whether reasoning incidents they recorded in the previous
that is, that an intermediate level of af- is useful as a discipline response for week. The mean answers to these ques-
fect (or power assertion) during a disci- tions were 9.6 minutes and 88.0%, re-
young preschoolers.
pline episode is associated with higher spectively.
moral internalization than are either
lower or higher levels of affect. Measures
Although behavioral therapists have The Discipline Record. The Disci-
emphasized time out and cognitive-so- Subjects pline Record (see Figure 1) is a struc-
cialization theorists have emphasized Forty volunteer mothers of children tured parental diary in which parents
reasoning, few studies have investigated from 25 to 38 months of age participat- record (a) each occurrence of their
the effects of combining negative conse- ed in the study. The toddlers included child's fighting or disobedience and (b)
quences and reasoning in a discipline re- 21 boys and 19 girls. Most of the moth- each discipline technique they used in
sponse. The studies that have consid- ers responded to a local newspaper response to that occurrence, selected
ered a punishment-reasoning combina- story offering a $50 United States Sav- from a list of 21 techniques. In addition,
tion as a distinct category have generally ings Bond to participants in the study. A the Discipline Record contains spaces
found it to be a relatively effective disci- few mothers were referred by other for designating which type of misbehav-
pline response (Chapman & Zahn- study participants. Sixty percent of the ior occurred (fighting or disobedience)
Waxler, 1982; Cheyne & Walters, 1969; mothers were full-time homemakers; and the date and time of each incident.
Crockenberg & Litman, 1990; Davies, this was preferred to maximize the con- Fighting was defined for the mothers as
McMahon, Flessati, & Tiedemann, 1984; sistency of the data collection. Mothers "physical fighting with siblings or other
Dix & Grusec, 1983; LaVoie, 1974; working outside the home for more than children," and disobedience was defined
Parke, 1969). 24 hours per week were not eligible for for them as "disobedience to spoken
The major hypotheses of this study the study. parental commands." Mothers also
are derived from behavioral theories and recorded the time periods of toddler
Fifty percent of the working moth-
from Hoffman's theory: ers were in working-class occupations, sleep or separation from the mother.
1. Consistent with behavioral theo- as were 45% of the fathers. The other The effectiveness of a discipline re-
ries, when negative consequences are employed parents were in middle-class sponse was measured by the length of
used without reasoning, the effective- occupations. The median family income time until the next recurrence of the tar-
ness of the discipline response is expect- was $34,500 in 1986. One third of the get misbehavior, either disobedience or
ed to increase with the level of child dis- mothers had college degrees, another fighting. Separation time and sleep time
tress. third had some college education, and were not counted in the time until re-

October 1994 FAMILY 481


FEAMILYN48
Figure 1. The discipline record. and Forced Compliance, and Other. By
definition, an Other response included
neither kind of consequences nor reason-
THE DISCIPLINERECORD ing. Any response that included "other
PARENTAL ATITUDE Not Angry (NA) - Slightly ACTING corporal punishment," which was con-
*KEYAngry (SA) - Mod. Angry (MA) - Very Angry (VA) sidered the most abusive discipline tech-
CHILD'S EMOTION (Intensity) Not Distressed (ND)- 1 Divert Child to Other Beh.
Slightly Distressed (SD) - Mod. Distressed (MD) - Fo-d-r-m-eo
nique, was also included in the Other re-
Very Distressed (VD) J 2 Appropriate Beh. sponse category even if Consequences or
Behavior FD FD FD FD FD Reasoning was used. Three fighting inci-
Date 3 Planned Ignoring
Time
dents and 14 disobedience incidents with
Room 4 Model the Behavior a Consequences or Reasoning compo-
TALKING _ Putting child in boring nent were placed in the Other category
_________________ 5 place, timeout
1 Command to Start 6 Remove Source of Diff.
due to this rule. The most common form
of "other corporal punishment" was slap-
2 Command to Stop 7 Slap child's hand ping a child's face. Forced Compliance
3 Desc. of Consequence 8 Spank without Reasoning was included in the
Other category after determining that the
4 Explanation 9 Other corporal punishment
mean Child Distress associated with it did
5 Label Behavior Bad 10 Withdraw privileges not differ significantly from the rest of
the Other category.
6 Offer a Reward Other techniques
Test-retest reliabilities (i.e., stability
7 Scold/Shame PARENT'S ATTITUDE'
correlations) were calculated for each of
8 Seek Information W the seven discipline responses separate-
y
ly by misbehavior type, by correlating
9 Threaten
0 frequency of reported use during the
10 Verbal withdrawl of Aff. CHILD'S Intensity* first 2 weeks with frequency of reported
Duration (mins)
11 Yell
=_=_=_EMOTION
use during the last 2 weeks of data col-
lection. For disobedience incidents, the
test-retest reliabilities of the seven disci-
pline responses were .87 (Noncorporal
currence. Because the distribution of the cantly longer recurrence delays than Consequences alone), .45 (Corporal
recurrence-delay times was skewed, a were typicalfor that child. Consequences alone), .67 (Reasoning
log transformation of the time was used alone), .77 (Reasoning plus Forced Com-
To isolate within-subject differ- pliance), .34 (Reasoning plus Noncorpo-
for most analyses, log1o (time + 1). ences, some analyses used family-specif- ral Consequences), .48 (Reasoning plus
Using recurrence delays to measure ic z scores. This z score was a further Corporal Consequences), and .71
effectiveness of discipline responses is transformation of the log delay, based (Other). For fighting incidents, the relia-
an improvement from correlational anal- on each family's own mean and standard bilities were .74, .78, .93, .64, .27, .73,
yses in that the antecedent variables (dis- deviation of its log-delay scores. Each re- and .71, respectively. These are minimal-
cipline response and associated child sulting z score indicated the extent to ly acceptable, except for the least fre-
distress) always precede the consequent which the recurrence delay was longer quent categories involving Corporal or
variable (recurrence delay). The (positive scores) or shorter (negative Noncorporal Consequences. Neverthe-
strength of the evidence for causality in scores) than the mean for that particular less, the distinction between those two
such analyses is diminished, however, child. Analyses of family-specific z types of consequences was maintained
by the extent to which the recurrence scores attempted to provide more because of the importance of the dis-
delays are due to between-subject differ- causally relevant information than in the tinction between them.
ences instead of within-subject differ- main analyses.
ences. The problem is that part of the Child Distress was measured by an
The 21 discipline techniques report- equally weighted composite of the two
differences in mean delays until a misbe- ed on the Discipline Record were used to
havior recurs is due to subject differ- distress items from the bottom of the
define seven discipline responses, de- Discipline Record (Intensity and Dura-
ences in the total frequency of reported rived from the following four discipline
misbehaviors over the 4-week data col- tion), divided into four levels ranging
categories: Corporal Consequences con- from lowest to highest distress. Mothers
lection period. The mother who report- sisted of any reported use of "slap child's
ed 140 fighting incidents necessarily av- reported the intensity and duration of
hand" or "spank." Noncorporal Conse- the child's distress following each disci-
eraged shorter times between fighting quences included any use of "time out"
incidents than did the mother who re- pline incident. Discipline incidents that
or "withdraw privileges." Forced Compli- were reported as having no child dis-
ported only three fighting incidents. ance consisted of any use of "forced per-
Thus, any mean differences in recur- tress (i.e., the lowest intensity level and
formance of appropriate behavior" or "re- zero minutes duration) were counted as
rence delays in the main analyses could move source of difficulty." Reasoning in-
have been due to mothers of frequent the lowest level of distress. The remain-
cluded any use of "description of conse- ing incidents were divided as equally as
fighters being less likely to use an appar- quences," "explanation," or "seek infor-
ently optimal discipline response than possible into three additional levels of
mation." The seven resulting discipline distress, ranging from low to high dis-
were the mothers of infrequent fighters, responses for this article were Corporal
which would reflect between-subject tress. The maximum low distress score
Consequences alone (i.e., without Rea- represented being slightly distressed for
differences. Stronger causal evidence soning), Noncorporal Consequences
would be shown by within-subject dif- 1 minute. The maximum medium dis-
alone, Reasoning alone, Reasoning and tress score consisted of being moderate-
ferences, that is, evidence that an opti- Corporal Consequences, Reasoning and
mal discipline response led to signifi- ly distressed for 2 minutes. The high dis-
Noncorporal Consequences, Reasoning
482 FAMILY October 1994 Ii
RELAllONS
Table 1 The effects of Child Distress by Dis-
Mean Child Distress Scores by Discipline Response cipline Response on recurrence delays
Type of Discipline Incident were tested with a 4 X 7 (Child Distress
X Discipline Response) ANOVA, using
Discipline Response Disobedience Fighting
the log transformation of recurrence de-
Noncorporal consequences 7.88a 13.lOa lays. The effect of Child Distress on re-
Corporal consequences 9.57b 8.29b currence delays differed for different
Reasoning 4.82 5.77c Discipline Responses, as indicated by a
Reasoning and forced compliance 8.15a 8.80b significant Distress X Discipline interac-
Reasoning and noncorporal consequences 11.04bc 12.13a tion, F(18, 2,894) = 1.65,p < .05. There
Reasoning and corporal consequences ll.3OC 13-93a were also significant main effects due to
Other 3.79 5.05c Discipline, F(6, 2,894) = 2.12, p < .05,
Note. The means within a column that share the same subscript are not significantly different and Distress, F(3, 2,894) = 12.76, p <
from each other, using Fisher's Least Significant Difference test, p < .05. All other pairwise com- .001. Table 2 gives the mean hours until
parisons within the same column are significantly different. a Disobedience recurrence for each Dis-
cipline response by the four levels of
Child Distress.
tress score represented greater intensity with significantly higher Child Distress
or durations longer than 2 minutes than when Consequences were used Following Keppel (1982), we fol-
alone. lowed up the significant overall interac-
tion with an interaction contrast to test
Effects on Delays Until the differing predictions of behavioral
theories versus Hoffman's theory on the
Effects of Discipline Disobedience Recurrences association between Child Distress and
When Consequences were used recurrence delays. An interaction con-
Responses on Child Distress trast combines specific contrasts on the
alone, the delays until the next Disobedi-
Child Distress varied by the seven ence recurrence were longer at higher two ANOVA factors involved in the in-
discipline responses in a way generally levels of Child Distress than at lower or teraction. On the Discipline variable, the
consistent with expectations. One-way moderate levels of Child Distress. This is relevant contrast compared the two
analyses of variance (ANOVAs) resulted consistent with behavioral research on Consequences-alone discipline respons-
in significant effects of Discipline re- punishment. However, when Reasoning es versus the four responses incorporat-
sponses on Child Distress, F(6, 2,914) = was used in a discipline response, the re- ing Reasoning. On the Distress variable,
81.27 for Disobedience incidents, F(6, currence delays were longer at moder- the contrast was [-1, -3, -1, +5], in
758) = 29.68 for Fighting incidents, ps < ate levels of Child Distress than at either which the four coefficients represented
.001 (see Table 1). Discipline responses lower or higher levels of distress. Fur- no, low, medium, and high levels of Dis-
that included Consequences elicited ther, this was true whether Reasoning tress, respectively. This contrast repre-
more distress than did responses with- was combined with Consequences or sents the difference between a linear
out Consequences. The addition of Rea- not. This is consistent with Hoffman's contrast as predicted by behavioral theo-
soning to Consequences was associated (1977) theory. ries for Consequences alone (i.e.,

Table 2
Mean Hours Until Recurrence of Disobedience by Discipline Response and Degree of Child Distress
Degree of Child Distress Significancea
z-scored
Discipline Response 1. None 2. Low 3. Medium 4. High Log(hrs.) log (hrs.)
Negative Consequences Without Reasoning
Noncorporal consequences 1.14 2.30 2.28 3.98 L L
(43) (43) (36) (50) 4>2,3> 1 4,2>1
Corporal consequences 1.74 2.66 2.88 3.30 L n.s.
(23) (61) (90) (103) 4>
Reasoning
Reasoning alone 2.06 2.31 2.61 2.24 n.s. n.s.
(199) (161) (130) (97)
Reasoning and forced compliance 2.28 2.30 2.47 2.43 n.s. n.s.
(22) (40) (51) (64)
Reasoning and noncorporal consequences 1.94 3.98 2.61 3.04 n.s. n.s.
(8) (12) (14) (35)
Reasoning and corporal consequences 2.51 3.17 4.28 2.56 n.s. C
(7) (9) (25) (54) 3>1
Neither Negative Consequences Nor Reasoning
Other 1.81 2.11 2.47 2.50 L n.s.
(682) (411) (276) (176) 4,3,2 >
Note. The number of discipline incidents within each cell is in parentheses.
aTests of statistical significance of Child Distress within each type of discipline response: L = significant linear effect, p < .05. C = significant curvilin-
ear effect, p < .05. The numbers summarize pairwise comparisons, using Fisher's Least Significant Difference Test, p < .05. For example, 4 > 2,3 > 1
means that "4. High" Distress had a significantly higher mean log delay than did either "2. Low" or "3. Medium," both of which had significantly
higher mean log delays than did "1. None." The left-hand column summarizes statistical tests using a log transformation of hours to reduce skew-
ness, Logl0 (hours + 1). The right-hand column summarizes statistical tests of family-specific z-score transformations of the logl0 (hours + 1), to iso-
late within-family effects. n.s. = not significant.

Iiliii October 1994 FAMILY 483


~~~~~~~~~~~RELATIONS
Figure 2. Meanhours until the next disobedience recurrenceby child distressand discipline number of incidents within each disci-
response. pline response. Figure 2 shows the ef-
4 fects of Child Distress on recurrence de-
lays after collapsing the seven discipline
responses in Table 2 into four larger dis-
cipline categories. Only for Reasoning
plus Consequences did the curvilinear
trend for the effect of Child Distress on
recurrence delay approach significance,
t(2,894) = 1.95,p < .10.
The above use of ANOVA differs
from standard usage. The unit of analysis
consisted of the 2,922 disobedience inci-
dents, not the 38 families. The number
of incidents per family ranged from 6 to
210, with a mean of 77. By using disobe-
dience incidents as the unit of analysis,
the analyses reflected the specific disci-
pline response and distress level associ-
ated with each incident. However, the
independence assumption of ANOVA
was violated.
In an initial attempt to correct for
the lack of independence, a 7 X 4 (Disci-
pline X Distress) ANCOVA was run, con-
trolling for the log of the immediately
prior interval between Disobedience in-
cidents. The log of the prior interval cor-
None Low Medium High related .27 with the log of the recur-
rence delay, p < .05. When controlling
Child Distress for the prior interval, the Discipline X
Distress interaction showed only a trend
Consequences --Other -V Reason, not Conseq. -WReason+ Conseq.
toward significance, F(18, 2,857) = 1.46,
p < .10. The main effect due to Distress
remained significant, F(3, 2,857) = 4.78,
[-3, -1, +1, +3]) and a curvilinear con- currence after Reasoning responses in- p < .001, but the Discipline main effect
trast predicted by Hoffman when Rea- creased from 2.09 hours through 2.41 was no longer significant, F(6, 2,857) =
soning was part of the discipline re- hours to 2.73 hours, for Distress levels of 1.41, n.s. The theory-derived interaction
sponse (i.e., [-2, +2, +2, -2]). The differ- none, low, and medium, respectively. contrast remained significant, t(2,857) =
ence between the linear and curvilinear The mean hours then dropped to 2.46 -2.83,p < .01.
contrasts specified precisely the differ- hours at the high Distress level. Thus the The correlation between the log of
ence in expected effects of Child Dis- medium (third) level of Distress seems the prior interval with the log of the re-
tress on recurrence delays, comparing optimal for discipline effectiveness when currence delay was due primarily to sub-
Consequences alone against all four Rea- Reasoning is part of the discipline re- ject differences in average log recur-
soning responses. sponse. Consistent with that, medium rence delays. After controlling for sub-
Multiplying the Discipline contrast Distress was associated with a signifi- jects' mean log recurrence delay, the
by the Distress contrast yielded the inter- cantly longer recurrence delay, whether semipartial correlation between the log
action contrast's coefficients for each of compared to no Distress, t(2,894) = 2.65, of the prior interval and the log of recur-
the 28 cells of the 4 X 7 ANOVA. This in- p < .01, or compared to the mean of the rence delay was no longer significant, r
teraction contrast was significant, indi- other three Distress levels, t(2,894) = = .02. Thus, autocorrelation, the most
2.01,p < .05. likely source of a violation of the inde-
cating that the relationship between
Child Distress and recurrence delay dif- In Table 2, the simple effects of Dis- pendence assumption of ANOVA, is in-
fered for Consequences and for Reason- tress on recurrence delays for the two consequential except for between-sub-
ing responses in the precise way predict- Consequences responses both showed a ject differences in mean recurrence de-
ed by behavioral versus Hoffman's theo- significant linear relationship between lays, which do not violate ANOVA's in-
ries, respectively, t(2,894) = -3.16, p < Child Distress and recurrence delays. dependence assumption.
.01 (see Figure 2). In the case of Conse- The pattern of effects of Distress on re- However, the subject differences in
quences alone, Child Distress was linear- currence delays fit a curvilinear pattern mean log recurrence delays imply that
ly related to recurrence delays, consis- for each of the four discipline responses the above results reflect both between-
tent with behavioral theories, F(1, 2,894) that included Reasoning. That is, the subject effects and within-subject effects.
= 33.46, p < .001. For all four Reasoning longest average recurrence delay was as- To isolate within-subject effects, the 4 X
responses combined, the association of sociated with either the low or the 7 (Distress X Discipline) ANOVA was re-
Child Distress with recurrence delay had medium Distress level for each of the peated with the z score of the log delay
a significant linear effect, F(1, 2,894) = Reasoning responses. However, none of as the dependent variable. Neither main
3.86, p < .05, and a near-significant curvi- the effects of Child Distress on recur- effect nor the interaction showed a sig-
linear effect, F(1, 2,894) = 3.03, p < .10. rence delay showed a significant curvi- nificant effect on the z-scored measure.
The mean hours until a Disobedience re- linear effect, due partly to the decreased
484 FAMILY October 1994 __
RELAnONS11
Table 3
Mean Hours Until Recurrence of Fighting by Discipline Response and Degree of Child Distress
Degree of Child Distress Significancea
z-scored
Discipline Response 1. None 2. Low 3. Medium 4. High Log (hrs.) log (hrs.)
Negative Consequences Without Reasoning
Noncorporal consequences 8.45 4.15 7.11 5.88 n.s. n.s.
(2) (6) (6) (29)
Corporal consequences 3.36 3.01 5.94 6.97 n.s. L
(5) (9) (23) (14) 3> 1, 2
Reasoning
Reasoning alone 4.58 3.42 3.77 5.92 cbc n.s.
(37) (48) (47) (33) 4 >2
Reasoning and forced compliance 10.22 4.04 5.39 6.58 n.s. n.s.
(9) (8) (17) (23)
Reasoning and noncorporal consequences 15.92 0.76 6.46 11.74 cc cc
(2) (2) (7) (11) 4, 1 >2
Reasoning and corporal consequences 7.80 - 7.18 5.41 n.s. Lb
(2) (0) (3) (22)
Neither Negative Consequences Nor Reasoning
Other 5.42 3.74 5.14 6.02 cc n.s.
(127) (107) (87) (79) 4, 1 > 2
Note. The number of discipline incidents within each cell is in parentheses.
aTests of statistical significance of Child Distress within each type of discipline response: L = significant linear effect, p < .05. C = significant curvilin-
ear effect, p < .05. The numbers summarize pairwise comparisons, using Fisher's Least Significant Difference Test, p < .05. For example, 4, 1 > 2
means that "4. High" Distress and "1. None" each had significantly higher mean log delays than did "2. Low" Distress. The left-hand column summa-
rizes statistical tests using a log transformation of hours to reduce skewness, Logl0 (hours + 1). The right-hand column summarizes statistical tests of
family-specific z-score transformations of the log10 (hours + 1), to isolate within-family effects. n.s. = not significant.
bp < .10, two-tailed test (p < .05, one-tailed test).
cCurvilinear effect in direction opposite of that predicted by Hoffman's theory (U-shaped instead of inverted U).

In addition, the theory-derived interac- with a 4 X 7 (Child Distress X Discipline consistent with Hoffman's (1977) highly
tion contrast did not remain significant, Response) ANOVA, using the log trans- regarded theory of moral internalization.
t(2,894) = -1.38, p = .17. However, two formation of recurrence delay. There The results tell a different story
simple effects showed significant results was a main effect for Distress level, F(3, when negative consequences are used
that were consistent with the above re- 738) = 5.58, but the Discipline effect alone. The findings suggest that negative
sults (see Table 2): Distress had a posi- and the Distress X Discipline interaction consequences without reasoning are
tive linear effect on the z-scored delay were not significant. more effective when the child reacts to
measure for Noncorporal Consequences. Follow-up analyses of the Distress ef- those consequences with high emotion-
Distress had a significant curvilinear ef- fect found a significant curvilinear effect, al distress than when the child reacts
fect on the z-scored delay measure for F(1, 738) = 13.00, p < .001, and a near- with low or moderate emotional dis-
Reasoning plus Corporal Consequences. significant linear effect, F(1, 738) = 3.53, tress. This pattern is consistent with be-
In simple effects of combined cate- p < .10. Fisher's Least Significant Differ- havioral research on punishment (e.g.,
gories of discipline responses, the linear ence Test indicated that low Distress had Azrin & Holz, 1966), but it implies an
effect of Child Distress on the z score a significantly shorter mean delay than ethical dilemma between the values of
log delay remained significant for all did any of the other Distress levels, ps < minimizing a child's emotional distress
Consequences-alone discipline respons- .05. These results held up when control- and using effective discipline responses.
es, F(1, 2,894) = 5.13,p < .05. However, ling for the immediately prior interval be-
One possible solution to this ethical
the curvilinear effect of Child Distress tween fighting incidents. However, no
dilemma is to include reasoning when-
on the z-scored log delay for Reasoning results approached significance when
ever negative consequences are used. By
responses only approached significance, controlling for subject mean delays by
for example, for Reasoning plus Conse- adding reasoning to negative conse-
using z-scored log delays. quences, the advantage of increasing the
quences, F(1, 2,894) = 3.22,p < .10.
child's distress is eliminated. Laboratory
Effects on Delays Until analogue studies have demonstrated this
previously (e.g., Parke, 1969). However,
Fighting Recurrences The results show that when a dis- this is the first naturalistic evidence that
obedient child reacts to parental disci- the advantage of high child distress for
A different pattern of results
pline with moderate emotional distress, the effectiveness of negative conse-
emerged for Fighting incidents (see
parental reasoning is more effective than quences disappears when reasoning is
Table 3 and Figure 3). In general, disci-
when the child reacts with low or high added.
pline responses of any kind delayed the
levels of emotional distress. Thus, par-
next fighting recurrence longer at both It should be noted that the most
ents of toddlers can use reasoning with
the no-Distress level and at the high-Dis- common levels of child distress for disci-
greater success when discipline strate-
tress level than they did at the low-Dis- pline responses were often either too
gies elicit a moderate level of emotional
tress level. high or too low for optimal effectiveness
distress in their children. If the actions
The effects of Child Distress by Dis- of the parents produce a low or high (see Table 2). Taking Disobedience inci-
cipline Response on the delay until the level of child distress, inductive reason- dents as an example, 57% of the occur-
next recurrence of Fighting was tested ing will be less effective. This pattern is rences of a combination of Reasoning

ri_October 1994 FAMILY


~~~~~~~~~RELATIO 485
Figure 3. Meanhours until the next fightingrecurrenceby child distressand disciplineresponse. during Fighting incidents. Given some
level of child distress, the effectiveness
12 of discipline increases with greater child
distress, consistent with the results for
Consequences alone following Disobedi-
ence incidents. The practical implication
10 of this explanation is that parents should
wait until after their children have
cooled off before reasoning with them
about alternatives to fighting, except in
the case of minor incidents of physical
8 aggression. The purpose of a discipline
response to most fighting would be to
put a stop to the fighting now and ex-
plain alternatives and reasons later.
The methodologies used in this
study have strengths and weaknesses
that need to be addressed. Strengths in-
clude measures and analyses specific to
each discipline incident, including mea-
suring the effectiveness of a discipline
response with the delay until the next
misbehavior recurrence. We also at-
2! tempted to go beyond correlational, be-
tween-subject analyses. However, only a
few relevant statistical tests were signifi-
cant when within-subject effects were
0 I isolated by using family-specific z
Medium scores. It should be noted that z scores
None Low High
may overcorrect, by equating each fami-
Child Distress ly's average recurrence delay regardless
of the child's temperament and the par-
Consequences A-Other - Reason, not Conseq. * Reason + Conseq. enting quality.
The strengths of these research
strategies have corresponding weakness-
and Corporal Consequences resulted in child's attention away from the frustrat- es. One weakness of using discipline in-
high Child Distress, which exceeded the ing situation, the total child distress cidents as the unit of analysis is that the
maximally effective, intermediate levels might be too much for optimal cognitive ANOVA assumption of independent ob-
of Child Distress. Further,34%of the oc- processing. Third, the motivating effect servations was violated. Successive re-
currences of Reasoning alone were asso- of the child's distress might be split be- currence delays were independent of
ciated with no Child Distress, which tween satisfying the parent and resolv- each other, after controlling for families'
was associated with the least effective ing the interpersonal conflict. mean recurrence delays. Nevertheless,
use of Reasoningalone. These explanations may account for the central results were due to between-
The enhanced effectiveness of rea- why discipline responses were least ef- family differences as well as within-fami-
soning at intermediatelevels of child dis- fective at the second level of child dis- ly differences in the effectiveness of dif-
tress was found only for disobedience tress ("Low"), which was predicted to ferent discipline responses. This raises
incidents, not for fighting incidents. The be one of the more effective distress lev- the possibility that the results could be
differential results for disobedience ver- els, at least for Reasoning responses. If due to idiosyncracies of this sample of
sus fighting incidents may be due to the intermediate and high levels of child dis- 40 families. Further, a small number of
source of the child's distress, which tress involve peer-elicited distress to a those families may have influenced Dis-
comes primarilyfrom the parent in dis- substantial degree, then the child may cipline-Distress combinations that were
obedience incidents, but primarilyfrom pay adequate attention to the parental crucial for the results obtained in this
the peer or sibling in fighting incidents. discipline only at the level of no child study. Thus, replications of these results
distress and at the higher distress levels. are needed before we can have full con-
Thus, unlike disobedience incidents, in-
When there is no child distress, the par- fidence in them.
termediate distress after fighting inci-
dents may be associated with continuing ent is not competing with the peer or Future research could improve on
attention to the peer or sibling, not at- sibling for attention. When the child is distinguishing different kinds of emotion-
tention to the reasoning component of already upset with a play partner, a side al reactions of children to discipline in-
the parent's discipline response. effect of getting the child to pay atten- terventions. Hoffman (1977) has argued
tion to the parent may be high levels of that fear leads to an external moral orien-
Distress caused by fighting could in- child distress. tation (e.g., fear of getting caught),
terfere with cognitive processing of the whereas empathy facilitates an internal
parental reasoning component in three In contrast to the case with Disobe-
dience incidents, Reasoning makes no moral orientation. Using negative conse-
ways. First, the frustratingaspect of the quences in a way that elicits high child
fighting situation might hinder the difference in the relationship of child
distress to discipline effectiveness. This distress may lead to an external moral
child's attention to parental reasoning. orientation, whereas reasoning may be
Second, when the parent applies implies that the child is not cognitively
processing the reasoning component more conducive to an internal moral ori-
enough power assertion to force the entation.

486 FAMILY October 1994 p-i


Aronfreed, J. (1968). Aversive control of socialization. In W. J.
Implications for Parent quences combination are better. Other Arnold (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol.
studies indicate that (a) any use of nega- 16, pp. 271-320). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Education tive consequences should have a speci- Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K.
Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and ap-
Regardless of their particular per- fied maximum intensity, such as a 3-to-5- plication (pp. 380-447). New York: Appleton-Century-
spective, all parent education programs minute maximum for time out or two Crofts.
Barkley, R. A. (1987). Defiant children: A clinician's manual
should consider the implications of Hoff- slaps with an open hand to the buttocks for parent training. New York: Guilford.
man's (1977, 1983) theory for their pro- that leave no marks (Forehand & McMa- Baumrind, D. (1973). The development of instrumental com-
hon, 1981); (b) negative consequences petence through socialization. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Min-
gram. Parent training programs that in- nesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 3-46).
corporate consequences such as time should be used primarily to back up Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
out should train parents to combine con- milder discipline responses, such as rea- Chapman, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1982). Young children's
compliance and noncompliance to parental discipline in a
sequences with reasoning in a way that soning, a single warning, or time out natural setting. InternationalJournal of Behavioural De-
minimizes child distress. This conclu- (Roberts & Powers, 1990); (c) responses velopment, 5, 81-94.
Cheyne, J. A., Goyeche, J. R. M., & Walters, R. H. (1969). At-
sion needs to be balanced, however, to misbehavior should focus on a small tention, anxiety, and rules in resistance-to-deviation in
with the tendency for children to be es- number of clearly targeted misbehaviors children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8,
while ignoring less serious misbehaviors 127-139.
pecially resistant when parents are first Cheyne, J. A., & Walters, R. H. (1969). Intensity of punish-
implementing such a training program (Roberts, McMahon, Forehand, & ment, timing of punishment, and cognitive structure as de-
(Barkley, 1987; Ollendick & Cerny, Humphreys, 1978); (d) parents should terminants of response inhibition. Journal of Experimen-
tal Child Psychology, 7, 231-244.
1981). For example, G. R. Patterson turn to alternative discipline responses Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as compe-
(personal communication, 1989) has or professional help rather than increas- tence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance,
compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psycholo-
found that preadolescents often misbe- ing the intensity of consequences if they gy, 26, 961-971.
have more persistently for about the first prove ineffective; and (e) negative con- Davies, G. R., McMahon, R. J., Flessati, E. W., & Tiedemann,
sequences should be appropriate to the G. L. (1984). Verbal rationales and modeling as adjuncts to
two weeks of his parenting program be- a parenting technique for child compliance. Child Devel-
fore improvements are seen. child's age. The effectiveness of a care- opment, 55, 1290-1298.
fully prescribed spanking as a backup Dix, T., & Grusec, J. E. (1983). Parental influence techniques:
Parenting programs that do not in- An attributional analysis. Child Development, 54, 645-652.
need to in- for time out has been demonstrated only Forehand, R. L., & McMahon, R. J. (1981). Helping the non-
corporate consequences
for children from 2 to 6 years of age compliant child: A clinician's guide to parent training.
struct parents to be particularly firm in New York: Guilford.
(Roberts & Powers, 1990). The effective-
using alternative types of discipline re- Friman, P. C. (1990). Nonaversive treatment of high-rate dis-
ness of time out has been demonstrated ruption: Child and provider effects. Exceptional Children,
sponses, such as reasoning. Otherwise 57, 64-69.
for preadolescents as well (Patterson,
trainers are in danger of fostering natter- Goodenough, F. L. (1931). Anger in young children. Min-
1982). These restrictions for effective neapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
ing, an increasing frequency of verbal
use of negative consequences obviously Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental dis-
complaints to the child, which in turn cipline methods on the child's internalization of values: A
Frequent require sober parents who are in control reconceptualization of current points of view. Develop-
are frequently ignored.
of their emotions. mental Psychology, 30, 4-19.
parental nattering is a predictor of delin- Hoffman, M. L. (1963). Parent discipline and the child's con-
quency (Patterson, 1982). The implications of this study need sideration for others. Child Development, 34, 573-588.
Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P. H. Mussen
All parent education programs need to be placed in their appropriate con- (Ed.), Carmichael's handbook of child psychology (3rd
text. The focus has been only on disci- ed., Vol. 2, pp. 261-354). New York: Wiley.
to consider how parents should adjust Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Moral internalization: Current theory
pline responses to misbehavior. Positive
their discipline responses according to and research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
each particular child and situation. If parenting in other contexts is probably mental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 85-133). New

verbal interventions are adequate to get at least as important. Other research has York: Academic Press.
Hoffman, M. L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in
a child's attention, power assertion may shown that sensitivity and responsive- moral internalization. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, &
ness to infant initiatives predict a secure W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social develop-
rarely be needed. A combination of mild ment (pp. 236-274). Cambridge: Cambridge University
attachment (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters,
power assertion and reasoning may Press.
work better with less anxious children. & Wall, 1978) and that parental affec- Holden, G. W. (1983). Avoiding conflict: Mothers as tacticians

Adjusting discipline responses to the tion, clear expectations, reinforcement in the supermarket. Child Development, 54, 233-244.
Keppel, G. (1982). Design and analysis: A researcher's
of appropriate behavior, granting of age- handbook (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
temperament and mood of the child
of appropriate autonomy, and skill devel- Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the
may be an important component development of guilt and conscience. Child Development,
parental flexibility (Grusec & Goodnow, opment all predict positive child out- 62,1379-1382.
comes (Baumrind, 1973; Friman, 1990; Larzelere, R. E., Amberson, T. G., & Martin, J. A. (1992).
1994). Changes in perceived discipline problems from 9 to 48
Larzelere, Klein, Schumm, & Alibrando, months of age. Family Relations, 41, 192-199.
What does this study contribute to 1989; Larzelere & Schneider, 1991; Larzelere, R. E., Klein, M., Schumm, W. R., & Alibrando, S. A.,
the debate about the role of negative Roberts et al., 1978; VanAken & Riksen- Jr. (1989). The effects of spanking and other parenting
characteristics on self-esteem and perceived fairness of
consequences or punishment in parental Walraven, 1992). Positive parent-child parental discipline. Psychological Reports, 64, 1140-1142.
discipline? The controversy may be due relations enhance the effectiveness of Larzelere, R. E., & Schneider, W. N. (1991, April). The associ-
to the fact that the effects of negative discipline responses (Maccoby & Martin, ation of parental discipline responses with delays until
recurrences offighting or disobedience in toddlers. Paper
consequences vary under different con- 1983). Further, parents can use proac- presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in
ditions. This study is useful in identify- tive discipline strategies to prevent Child Development, Seattle.
LaVoie, J. C. (1974). Aversive, cognitive, and parental deter-
ing some conditions that maximize the many discipline incidents (Holden, minants of punishment generalization in adolescent males.
effectiveness of negative consequences. 1983). The effectiveness of discipline re- Journal of Genetic Psychology, 124, 29-39.
Lepper, M. R. (1983). Social control processes and the inter-
First, negative consequences are sponses, the focus of this study, should nalization of social values: An attributional perspective. In
most effective in delaying misbehavior not be separated from the need for posi- E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social
cognition and social development: A sociocultural per-
recurrences when combined with rea- tive parenting in nondisciplinary parent- spective (pp. 294-330). Cambridge: Cambridge University
soning (Larzelere & Schneider, 1991). child situations. Press.
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the
The reasoning-consequences combina-
- goa t O
context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M.
tion is most effective when child distress Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4.
is kept lower than is typically the case Socialization, personality, and social development (4th
ed., pp. 1-101). New York: Wiley.
for that discipline response. This implies Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Matson, J. L., & DiLorenzo, T. M. (1984). Punishment and its
(1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of
that mild versions of a reasoning-conse- the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
alternatives: A new perspective for behavior modifica-
tion. New York: Springer.

October 1994 FAMILY 487


REUAlS
Ollendick, T. H., & Cerny, J. A. (1981). Clinical behavior Rollins, B. C., & Thomas, D. L. (1979). Parental support, Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The develop-
therapy with children. New York: Plenum. power, and control techniques in the socialization of chil- ment of prosocial behaviors: Alternative research strate-
Parke, R. D. (1969). Effectiveness of punishment as an inter- dren. In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds.), gies. In N. Eisenberg-Berg (Ed.), The development of
action of intensity, timing, agent nurturance, and cogni- Contemporary theories about the family (Vol. 1, pp. 317- prosocial behavior (pp. 109-137). New York: Academic
tive structuring. Child Development, 40, 213-235. 364). New York: Free Press. Press.
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercivefamilyprocess. Eugene, OR: VanAken, M. A. G., & Riksen-Walraven, I. M. (1992). Parental Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman,
Castalia Press. support and the development of competence in children. M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Develop-
Roberts, M. W., McMahon, R. J., Forehand, R., & Humphreys, International Journal of Behavioural Development, 15, mental Psychology, 28, 126-136.
L. (1978). The effect of parental instruction-giving on child 101-123.
compliance. Behavior Therapy, 9, 793-798. Van Houten, R. (1983). Punishment: From the animal labora-
Roberts, M. W., & Powers, S. W. (1990). Adjusting chair time tory to the applied setting. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche
out enforcement procedures for oppositional children. Be- (Eds.), The effects of punishment on human behavior
havior Therapy, 21, 257-271. (pp. 13-44). New York: Academic Press.

Family Life Education


l t 1994 Telacher's Kit Supplement
UFEEDUCATMON
FAMLY
The Family Life Education 1994 Teacher's Kit Supplement is now available. It
' ACHERSU"
EW consists of 30 lesson plans based upon the ten family life substance areas used as
N a a criteria for the Certified Family Life Educator program. The majority of the lesson
plans are geared toward high school and undergraduate level students with many
.;.. graduate level lesson plans as well. The Supplement is meant to be added to the
original Family Life Education Teacher's Kit; however, it can also stand alone. The Kit
and the Supplement provide family life educators with tested lesson plans that are, for
the most part, ready to use. Includes lesson plans, overhead masters, and handouts.

List of lessonplansin the 1994 Teacher'sKit Supplement


FAMILIES AND SOCIETY Using Problem Solving as a Method for Enhancing Positive
Changing Family Structure Development in Young Children
Benefits & Burdens of Paid & Unpaid Work Patterns
Role of Power in Understanding Racial Discrimination & FAMILY LAW & PUBLIC POLICY
Prejudice Sexuality & the Law
Legal Issues in Aging Families
INTERNAL DYNAMICS OF FAMILIES Culture of the Cults/Occults
Adjustment to Divorce
What's a Family? ETHICS
Rules of the Game Are We Teaching Values or Valuing?
Development of Ethical Thinking & Practice
HUMAN GROWTH & DEVELOPMENT Self-Formation: Values & Valuing in Life Skills Development
The Business of Dying
Food for Thought FAMILYLIFE EDUCATION METHODOLOGY
Role of Stress & Consequences Developing a Philosophical Rationale for Program
Development
HUMAN SEXUALITY Gender Equitable Family Life Education
Make Mine With Onions (using as a text, the book My Instructional Strategies in Family Life Education
Darling, My Hamburger by Paul Zindel)
Spread of Opinion: Adolescents & Contraceptives CFLE/NCFR member price $21.95
Sexual Scripts: Compatibility, Contexts & Changes Across the plus $3.00 shipping & handling.*
Lifespan
Nonmember price $24.95 plus $3.00 shipping &
INTERPERSONALRELATIONSHIPS handling.*
Intimacy - Analysis of Personal Account
Helping Couples Resolve Conflict *Orders must include $3.00 per kit shipping & handling U.S. & $6.00
Expressing Love to Older People per kit foreign & Canadian. U.S. funds drawn on U.S. banks only.
10% discount on 10 or more copies. Please make checks and money
FAMILY RESOURCE MANAGEMENT orders payable to NCFR. Visa and MasterCard accepted. Canadian
residents add 7% GST (123-830-465). MN residents add 6.5% tax.
Evaluating the Cost of Consumer Information FEI41-0762436
Families in Poverty l
Managing Time Resources r 9 National Council on Family Relations

PARENT EDUCATION & GUIDANCE


Ia*I Mr. E3989 CentralAvenue N.E., Suite 550
MN 55421
Minneapolis,
Parenting Styles & Possible Outcomes (612) 781-9331 * FAX (612) 781-9348
ParentingPositively NCFR E-mail: ncfr3989@aol.com

488 FAMILY October 1994 [M


RELA11NS Il

You might also like