You are on page 1of 14

Using Multilevel Modeling to Investigate Predictors of Literacy in the

National Adult Literacy Survey.

Janet K. Sheehan-Holt
M Cecil Smith

Northern Illinois University

March 17, 2000


Multilevel Modeling 2

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to demonstrate how multilevel analyses can be used with
large-scale data sets such as the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS). The results of
ordinary least squares (OLS) regression and hierarchical linear modeling (HLM)
analyses were compared for modeling predictors of literacy from the NALS. Our results
indicate that contextual factors, such as mean income of the neighborhood, are important
to take into account when predicting adult literacy proficiencies when studying racial
and ethnic differences. Also, contextual effects estimates and their standard errors were
found to differ between HLM and OLS. Finally, contextual-effects studies of adult
literacy using OLS produced a different model of the predictors of adult literacy than did
HLM. Statistical justification is given for the discrepant results between the two methods
and HLM is recommended as the appropriate statistical tool for studying predictors of
literacy when using the NALS.
Multilevel Modeling 3

Using Multilevel Modeling to Investigate Predictors of Literacy in the


National Adult Literacy Survey.

Recent advances in statistical methodology and computing power have made


more sophisticated data analytic tools such as hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) readily
available. This methodological tool is well suited for research using national databases
since (a) they often involve very large sample sizes and (b) complex sampling designs are
often used. The intent of this study was to investigate the use of these tools, with an
emphasis on HLM, for use in investigating substantive problems of interest in the
National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS).
The NALS (Kirsch et al., 1993) is the most recent and comprehensive survey
conducted of American adults’ literacy skills and practices. The NALS data were
gathered on a nationally representative sample of 26,091 adults, ages 16 and older
between January and August, 1992. The sampling design for the NALS survey is a
multistage cluster sample in which counties or groups of counties, i.e., probability
sampling units (PSUs), are first randomly selected. From the PSUs census blocks or
groups of census blocks, i.e., segments, are randomly selected. At this stage, segments
that were identified as high minority were over-sampled in order to ensure reliable
estimates of Blacks’ and Hispanics’ literacy proficiencies. Households were then
randomly selected from the segments, and one or two adults from each household were
selected for the survey. Further details regarding data collection are found in Kirsch et al.
(1993).
Multilevel Modeling 4

The cluster-sampling design of the NALS makes the data set a prime candidate for
multilevel modeling techniques such as hierarchical linear modeling (HLM; Bryk &
Raudenbush, 1992; de Leeuw & Kreft, 1995; Draper, 1995; Morris, 1995;
Raudenbush, 1995; Rogosa & Saner, 1995). Like regression analysis, HLM
techniques can condition on many background variables at the individual-level.
However, HLM has the advantage of yielding appropriate estimates of standard
errors when there are moderate to high intraclass correlations (ICC), resulting from
similarity among individuals within a higher-level unit. HLM is a technique
evolved from the random coefficient tradition (Dempster, Rubin, & Tsutakawa,
1981; Swamy,1973) in which individual-level parameters are assumed to vary
randomly across higher-level units, such as neighborhoods. Therefore, variation in
student-level intercepts or slopes can be modeled with neighborhood-level
variables. This eliminates the debate over whether dis-aggregated or aggregated
analyses should be used with multilevel data, because both levels can be used to
model individual-level variability.
Even though data from the NALS are cluster-sampled at four levels, data are only
collected at the individual level. Hence, using HLM would seemingly have only one
advantage over OLS (ordinary least squares) regression analyses; yielding more
appropriate estimates of standard errors when intraclass correlations are high. Because no
data are reported at the higher levels, e.g., household, segment, and PSU, the ability of
HLM to model cross-level effects would not seem to be as relevant to the NALS data. It
is the purpose, however, of this study to demonstrate how both benefits of multilevel
analyses can be ascertained with data such as the NALS. Further, comparing the results
of OLS to HLM analyses when modeling predictors of literacy from the NALS allows us
to … ?
Contextual Analyses
We chose to model segment-level, as well as individual-level, variation in the
NALS because segments represent groups of census blocks or small geographical regions,
such as neighborhoods, which could serve as a proxy for these neighborhoods. Therefore,
by modeling both individual-level variation and segment-level variation we could better
Multilevel Modeling 5

represent the major influences on adult literacy skills and practices, individual attributes
and characteristics, as well as the respondents’ neighborhood characteristics.
Since segment-level variables were not collected, however, it was necessary to
take a contextual approach to model neighborhood variables, that is, to use the mean of
individual-level variables as a measure of neighborhood-level characteristics. This type
of contextual analysis has been researched extensively in the literature (e.g., see Willms’
review, 1986). [Jan: probably need to summarize this work briefly—in a half paragraph
or so.]
Cronbach and Webb (1975) addressed [this problem of confounding contextual
effects] > unclear as to what you are referring!
by partitioning the variation into between-context and within-context components. The
within-context regression is formed by first deviating the scores of the individual-level
predictor from the segment-level mean ( X ij −X j ) , and regressing the outcome on the
deviated scores. The between-context component is determined by aggregating the data
to the segment level for the regression analysis. Hence the mean of Y is regressed on the
mean of X.
Cronbach and Webb (date) demonstrated that very different conclusions can be
reached when the conventional OLS analysis is replaced with such a partitioned analysis.
Yet, the separate analyses approach used by Cronbach and Webb (1975) and detailed by
Cronbach and Snow (1977) can be effectively employed to interpret contextual effects
from hierarchically nested samples, particularly when intraclass correlations are low.
The separate analysis approach can also be obtained from a singular regression analysis
in which both X and X are regressed on the outcome. The within-groups
regression
Yij =β0 +βw X ij +βc X j +εij

(1)

coefficient is represented by β w while β c represents β b - β w, the difference between


the within-groups regression coefficient and the between-groups regression coefficient.
This difference is the contextual effect of X on Y.
Multilevel Modeling 6

Multilevel Modeling
Another technique which has been used to make cross-level inferences within
school-effects studies is multilevel modeling. Multilevel modeling has particular merit
when analyzing data which have high intraclass correlations due to the hierarchical
structure of the data. When analyzing data with intraclass correlations using
conventional regression analysis, the data are forced to fit a model that does not reflect
how they were collected. Conversely, multilevel techniques draw strength from
appropriately modeling the data at each level of the sampling design. In multilevel
modeling a separate micro-level model is defined for each macro unit. In a
neighborhood-effects study this would mean that individual level regression coefficients
are modeled by neighborhood-level variables (de Leeuw & Kreft, 1986).
Random-Intercepts Models
Random-intercept models are a particular type of multilevel model that are often
used to make cross-level inferences in which the intercepts are not assumed to be
constant for all contexts. These multilevel models circumvent a limitation of the OLS
separate analyses approach, the assumption that the intercepts across all second-level
units, e.g., neighborhoods, are homogeneous. The extent to which mean literacy
proficiencies and practices vary across neighborhoods determines the extent to which a
multilevel model will better fit the data than an OLS model. This variation can be
measured with an intraclass correlation. When the intraclass correlation is high the
average outcome varies considerably across the level-two units. In addition to providing a
more realistic model of the data, the random-intercepts model is also an improvement
over the conventional multiple regression model because it calculates the correct standard
errors. Moreover, the random-intercepts model improves the estimation of the
parameters for the separate neighborhoods. An empirical Bayes estimation procedure is
used to weight the regression coefficient estimates of each neighborhood by a reliability
coefficient calculated for each neighborhood. This process is known as shrinkage
because the estimates are “shrunk” toward the estimated group mean coefficients. Those
neighborhoods providing less reliable estimates experience the most shrinkage (Cheung,
Keeves, Sellin & Tsoi, 1990; Raudenbush, 1988). The resulting shrinkage estimates are
Multilevel Modeling 7

more precise parameter estimates than those generated through ordinary least square
methods.
The contextual model is one in which there is an individual-level predictor, X, and
one group-level predictor, X (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992).

Yij =β0 j +β1 j ( X ij −X .. ) +rij

β0 j =γ00 +γ01 X . j +u 0 j

β1 j =γ10
(2)
In this model, in which the level-one predictor is centered around its grand mean, γ 10

represents the within-groups regression coefficient and γ 01 represents the contextual


effect of the predictor on Y. Therefore, both OLS and HLM can be used to study
contextual effects. The OLS estimates should be unbiased, yet their standard errors may
be negatively biased (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992).
Three questions were addressed in this study. First, is a contextual analysis
preferred when investigating predictors of adult literacy from the NALS? Second, how do
the estimates of within-groups effects and contextual effects differ between OLS and
HLM methods? Third, do these differences result in different conclusions about the
predictors of adult literacy?
Method
Measures of Literacy
Measures of adult literacy used in this study included literacy proficiencies and
literacy practices. NALS literacy proficiencies were reported using three scales, prose,
document, and quantitative (PDQ) literacy--each ranging from 0 to 500. For efficiency,
NALS used a matrix sampling approach in which each test-taker answered only a portion
of the items for each literacy proficiency scale. Therefore, by design, there is much
missing data. Multiple imputation procedures are then used to generate a set of plausible
values for the respondent’s scores within each of the three literacy scales, using the
respondent’s raw scores, information about the respondent’s background, and test item
Multilevel Modeling 8

data (e.g., item difficulty). Our analyses used the average of the plausible values for each
scale.
The nature and variety of adults’ literacy practices were determined through the
NALS background survey. Respondents were asked about their reading of newspapers,
books, magazines, documents, and use of writing and quantitative skills for both work-
related and personal reasons. The data pertaining to reading practices, that is, reading
periodicals (newspapers and magazines), books, and documents for work and personal
reasons are examined in this study. [Jan: do we need to explain how we arrived at the
reading practice scores?]
Predictors of Literacy
Prediction of adult literacy skills and practices were made from the following
individual-level predictors: educational attainment, age, race, labor force participation,
basic skills training, (i.e., whether the respondent had ever participated in basic skills
education), parents’ educational attainment, English as the primary home language,
disability or long-term illness, newspaper reading practice (i.e., the number of newspaper
sections read per week), and total family income. The variable race1 represented the
mean difference between Blacks and Whites, race2 represented the mean difference
between Hispanics and Whites, and race3 represented the mean difference between other
minority groups and Whites. Race was then coded as three dummy-coded variables:
race1, race 2, and race 3. Whites were coded as 0 for all three variables. Mean income
of the neighborhood was used as a contextual variable since, in previous research, it has
been shown to be a statistically significant predictor of literacy proficiencies. Other
contextual variables, however, are not statistically significant predictors (e.g., mean
educational attainment of the neighborhood; Sheehan, Smith, & E, 1997) and so were not
used in our analyses.
Procedures
Initially, regression analyses were performed predicting each of the measures of
adult literacy from the full set of predictors, excluding the neighborhood-level variable.
These results were compared to HLM contextual analyses using the mean income of the
neighborhood as a control variable. Differences in the conclusions one could make from
the two analyses are discussed. Second, an abbreviated analysis of adult literacy was
Multilevel Modeling 9

conducted on both HLM and OLS to compare the contextual effects between the two
methods. In these analyses only X and X are used as predictors, to partition the
regression effect into within-groups and between-groups. The contextual effect (i.e.,
mean income of the neighborhood on literacy proficiencies and practices) is then
calculated and compared between the two analysis methods. Third, full analyses of adult
literacy using all the predictors, including the contextual effects, were conducted to
determine if differences in the contextual effects between the OLS and the HLM methods
resulted in any important differences in modeling adult literacy. In the HLM analyses, the
intercepts were allowed to vary across the level-two units, but the slopes were fixed.
Results
Intraclass correlations for the segment level were low to moderate for the
measures of adult literacy. Literacy practices had low intraclass correlations, ranging
from .016 for personal document reading to .026 for newspaper reading, while literacy
proficiencies had moderate intraclass correlations, ranging from .134 for document
literacy to .164 for prose literacy.
In the first phase of the analysis the statistically significant predictors from the
OLS analyses and HLM contextual analyses were compared. The only coefficients in
which the conclusions between the two analyses differed were the relationships between
race and newspaper, personal document, and work document reading. Although no
statistically significant race effects were detected by HLM for personal document and
work document reading, a statistically significant effect for race1 was detected using
OLS regression for work document reading (b=-1.023, t(14,270)=-3.056, p<.01) and a
statistically significant effect for race2 was detected using OLS regression for personal
document reading (b=-.571, t(17,296)=-2.077, p<.05). For newspaper reading, a statistically
significant effect for race1 was detected with HLM (γ =1.61, t=2.596, p<.05), but not
with OLS regression.
For the second phase of the study, contextual analyses were conducted using both
HLM and OLS regression for purposes of comparing the estimates of the contextual
effects, bc. Simplified models of literacy were used to facilitate interpretation, in which
only the predictors family income, X1, and X 1, mean income of the neighborhood, were
used as described in equations 1 and 2. Results of both sets of analyses are presented in
Multilevel Modeling 10

Table 1. All of the coefficients were statistically significant, with p< .001. However, the
magnitude of the coefficients and their standard errors differed between the two analyses.
The difference in the contextual effects estimates between the two methods increased
with larger intraclass correlations. For the reading practices, which had low intraclass
correlations, the standardized difference between the estimates ranged from 0.307 for
work documents to 1.414 for newspaper reading. However, the standardized differences
between the coefficients for prose, document, and quantitative literacy were 5.59, 5.36,
and 6.36, respectively. The OLS coefficients for the contextual effects were consistently
larger and had smaller standard errors than the HLM estimates.
In phase three of the data analysis, predictors of adult literacy were modeled with
OLS regression analyses by including the mean income of the neighborhood as a variable
in the model, as well as the other predictors. Similarly, HLM was used to predict adult
literacy using mean income of the neighborhood as a predictor at level two and the
remaining predictors at level one. The statistically significant findings were compared
between the two approaches to determine if important differences in the prediction of
adult literacy were apparent. In only two cases were predictors found to be statistically
significant in the HLM analyses but not in the OLS analyses. However, in many
instances, predictors were found to be statistically significant only in the OLS analyses.
These variables are listed in Table 2. Overall, there were numerous differences in the
conclusions that could be drawn about the predictors of adult literacy.
Discussion
A contextual-effects analysis of the NALS data allowed for the estimation of the
effect of the mean income of the neighborhood on adult literacy proficiencies and
practices, when controlling for family income. When doing so, a very different picture of
racial group differences in adult literacy emerges than is typically found (Kirsch,
Jungeblut, Jenkins, & Kolstad, 1993). By controlling for mean income of the
neighborhood, the HLM approach uncovered a mean difference in newspaper reading
favoring Blacks, which was not detected in the OLS analysis of individual predictors.
Further, where the HLM analysis did not detect any significant differences between the
different ethnic groups for work document and personal document reading, the OLS
analysis found Blacks to read fewer work documents than Whites and Hispanics and to
Multilevel Modeling 11

read fewer personal documents than Whites. It is apparently important to control for
mean income of the neighborhood when studying racial differences because poverty
levels are likely to be higher within some minority-populated neighborhoods. Otherwise,
analyses will result in a bias toward majority (White) groups (*** need citations***--
why?).
It was also discovered that a contextual-effects analysis using OLS regression
yields different estimates of the contextual effect and its standard error than HLM
analyses, particularly with moderate intraclass correlations (e.g., in the .15 range). It is
expected that the standard errors of the contextual effects would be negatively biased in
regression analyses, because of the violation of the independence assumption when there
is substantial variation across level-two units, as indicated by the intraclass correlation
(Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). Although estimates of the contextual effects should be
unbiased in OLS, HLM yields more efficient estimates when samples are unbalanced
(Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992, pp. 38, 122-123). It cannot be ascertained whether the OLS
estimates were unbiased in these comparisons without knowledge of the parameter
values. However, this consistent discrepancy between estimates of the two analysis
methods suggests that further investigation of this issue may be warranted.
As indicated by the third phase of the analysis of this study a contextual-effects
analysis using OLS differs markedly from an HLM contextual analysis of this data set.
There were several instances in which the predictors were statistically significant in the
OLS analyses but not in the HLM analyses. This result occurred even with the outcome
measures that had lower intraclass correlations across neighborhoods. The regression
estimates in both HLM and OLS are weighted estimates, which depend on nj. If sample
sizes are equivalent across the level-two units, estimates from both should be equivalent.
However, in the NALS data, as in many national databases, there are very unequal
sample sizes across the sampling units. In this case, the fixed-effects coefficients in an
HLM analysis will be weighted by ∆ -1
= (Vj + ι )-1, where Vj =σ 2/nj when variances
00

are equal across the j level-two units, and ι 00 represents the variance in mean outcomes
across the level-two units. On the contrary, OLS estimates are weighted solely by nj,
hence differences in estimates may occur between the two, particularly when there is
considerable variance in mean outcomes across the level-two units (Bryk & Raudenbush,
Multilevel Modeling 12

1992). When this occurs, analysts are advised to note that HLM estimators should be
more efficient, particularly when group sizes are very uneven. As noted by Glass and
Hopkins (1996, p. 246), since most estimators are consistent and unbiased when the n
size is large, the choice of an estimator is typically based on efficiency. However, the
major difference between HLM and OLS results is the estimates of the standard errors, in
which the OLS estimates are known to be negatively biased when there is variation
across the level-two units (Cheung LIST ALL, 1990). Therefore, in the NALS data where
sample sizes are unbalanced and the measures of literacy proficiencies have moderate
intraclass correlations, we endorse the use of the HLM method in estimating and testing
predictors of adult literacy.
The major disadvantage of HLM, however, is that it is a univariate approach in
which there can only be one outcome variable in a model. In complex national surveys in
which data are collected on many intervening or mediating variables, this is a non-trivial
issue because the relationships among several outcome variables cannot then be explored.
Hence a more multivariate technique would be useful which would take into account the
multilevel nature of the data. Recent developments in structural equation modeling
(SEM) make possible covariance structure modeling of nested data (Muthén, 1994;
Muthén & Satorra, 1995). Kaplan and Elliott (1997) recently demonstrated how such a
multilevel modeling approach could be used to study various educational quality
indicators (e.g., the organizational characteristics of schools). Therefore, this
methodology [WHICH METHOD?] might be particularly useful for investigating
complex models of adult literacy with the NALS.

[Jan—seems like this ends on kind of an ambiguous note, like we’re leaving
something unsaid; do we need to be more definitive in our conclusion?]
Multilevel Modeling 13

References

Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models:


Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cheung, K. C., Keeves, J. P., Sellin, N., & Tsoi, S. C. (1990). The analysis of
multilevel data in educational research: Studies of problems and their solutions.
International Journal of Educational Research, 14, 215-319.
Cronbach L. J and Webb, N. (1975). Between-class and within-class effects in a
reported aptitude x treatment interaction: Reanalysis of a study by
G. L. Anderson. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 717-724.
Cronbach L. J and Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods.
New York: Irvington.
de Leeuw, J., & Kreft, I. G. (1986). Random coefficients models for multilevel
analysis. Journal of Educational statistics, 11, 57-85.
de Leeuw, J., & Kreft, I. G. (1995). Questioning multilevel models. Journal of
Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 20, 171-189.
Dempster, A. P., Rubin, D. B., & Tsutakawa, R. K. (1981). Estimation in
covariance components models. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 76, 341-
353.
Draper, D. (1995). Inference and hierarchical modeling in the social sciences. .
Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 20, 115-147.
Glass, G., & Hopkins, K. (1996). Statistical methods in education and psychology
(3rd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Kaplan, D., & Elliott, P. R. (1997). A model-based approach to validating
education indicators using multilevel structural equation modeling. Journal of
Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 22, 323-347.
Kirsch, I.S., Jungeblut, A., Jenkins, L., & Kolstad, A. (1993). Adult literacy in
America: A first look at the results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Princeton, NJ:
Educational Testing Service.

Morris, C. N. (1995). Hierarchical models for educational data: An overview. .


Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 20, 190-200.
Multilevel Modeling 14

Muthén, B. O. (1994). Multilevel covariance structure analysis. Sociological


Methods & Research, 22, 376-398.
Muthén, B. O., & Satorra, A. (1995). Complex sample data in structural equation
modeling. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological Methodology (pp. 267-316).
Washington DC: American Sociological Association.
Raudenbush, S. W. (1988). Educational applications of hierarchical linear
models: A review. Journal of Educational Statistics, 13, 85-116.
Sheehan, J.K., Smith, MC., & E, N. (1997, March). Hierarchical modeling of
contextual effects on literacy proficiencies: An analysis of NALS. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL.
Swamy, P. A. (1973). Criteria, constraints, and multi-collinearity in random
coefficient regression models. Annals of Economic and Social Measurement, 2, 429-450.
Willms, J.D. (1986). Social class segregation and its relationship to pupils’
examination results in Scotland. American Sociological Review, 55, 224-241.

You might also like