You are on page 1of 28

Talent Detection Programs in Sport: The Questionable Use of Psychological Measures

MarkRAnshel Middle Tennessee State University Ronnie Lidor University of Haifa

Recent empirical studies have questioned the use of tests assessing physical ability and skill level in talent detection (TD), also called talent identification (TID), and in early phases of sport development, particularly in terms ofpredicting future ability/success ofyoung prospects. The main purpose of this article is to question the efficacy of predicting future talent in sport using psychological measures. It is contended that predicting future high quality performance in sport is based on psychological measures that have low validity, are based on flawed research procedures, lack consistency in defining elite versus non-elite athletes, have inherent bias in the athlete selection process, and contain inaccuracies in measuring sport personality, among other concerns. From a philosophical perspective, issues are addressed that reflect TD as an unethical procedure. Concerns about TD/TID programs include philosophical issues that question the integrity and appropriateness of talent prediction programs are also discussed. Plausible options for replacing TD/TID programs with other approaches (e.g., talent development, performance profiling) are provided Recommendations include improved athlete selection for further skill development, availability of high quality coaching to athletes of all skill levels (particularly in youth sports), and more efficient use of limited financial resources for non-elite community sports programs.

Address correspondence to: Mark H. Anshel, Ph.D., Middle Tennessee State University Department of Health and Human Performance, Box 96, Murfreeshoro, Tennessee 37132 E-mail: Mark.Anshel@mtsu.edu, Fax: 1-615-898-5020 239

240/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

The pressure exerted by sports organizations, sponsors, and, in some countries, by governmental bodies (e.g.. Sport Authorities, Olympic Training Centers) on young athletes to be successful sports competitors is greater than ever. Perhaps not surprisingly, there is also considerable pressure to predict future high quality sport performance in competitive settings by using various physiological, anthropometrical, and psychological tests on young athletes (see Abbott & Collins, 2002,2004; Martindale, Collins, & Daubney, 2005; Tranckle & Cushion, 2006). Vaeyens, Gullich, Warr, and Philippaerts (2009) contend that programs whose aim is to predict future sport success, called talent detection (TD) or talent identification (TID) programs, "are designed to identify young athletes who possess extraordinary potential for success in senior elite sport, and to select and recruit them into talent promotion programs" (p. 1367). The purpose of these programs is, ostensibly, to "increase athletes' potential by means of a variety of institutional measures designed to accelerate talent development" (p. 1367). Another purpose is to provide tests - motor, physiological, anthropological, biomechanical, and psychological - during an athlete's "early" years in order to predict long-term success in competitive sport. The intention of these programs is to allocate scarce resources toward individual athletes whose test scores show "promising" talent, at least in the long-term. Are these objectives met? Are the tests valid? Is this a good idea, at least from a philosophical perspective? The main purposes of this article are: ( 1 ) to critique, both empirically and philosophically, the value of predicting future talent in sport using psychological measures, and (2) to offer suggested directions for enhancing the processes of TD/TID, and talent development (TDV). The existing evidence suggests that the use of psychological inventories to predict future success and achievement in elite-level competitive sport lacks validity and proper ethics. The review will include the following: (1) defining important concepts, such as talent detection, also called talent identification, (2) providing empirical arguments in favor of TD programs, (3) making a case against the use of psychological measures in TD programs, (4) examining the philosophical arguments against the use of psychological measures in TD programs, and finally, (5) providing recommendations and guidelines for initiating talent development programs in sport. Defining Terms and Concepts Brown (2001) and St-Aubin and Sidney (1996) have defined TD as the methodological process of predicting sport performance over various periods of time by obtaining information on the prospects' physical, physiological, and technical abilities, either alone or in combination, with measures of psychological aptitudes. TD has also been described as a process by which children are encouraged to participate in the sports in which they are most likely to

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT. ../241 succeed, based on the results of testing selected parameters (Bompa, 1999). Woodman (1985) defined TD programs (in Australia) as "the screening of young athletes to determine those most likely to succeed in sport and directing them towards the sports to which they are most suited" (p. 49). To Hahn and Tumilty (1989), TD programs consist of the selection of individuals who have shown to have the characteristics important for success at the highest levels of a particular sport. Durand-Bush and Salmela (2001) contend that TD programs reflect the attempt to match various performer characteristics - innate, learned, or due to training - with task demands of a given sport, to ensure the highest probability of maximum performance outcome. Williams and Reilly (2000) define TD as the discovery of potential performers who are currently not involved in any sport program. Finally, Lidor, Ct, and Hackfort (2009) use the term talent identification as "the process of recognizing individuals currently involved in sport with the potential to become elite athletes/players" (p. 134). All of these definitions consider TD as any conscious effort that recognizes individuals who have the potential to become elite athletes. Two concepts that have been used interchangeably, but erroneously, with TD are talent selection (TS) and talent development (TDV). TS consists of the ongoing process of identifying athletes/players at various stages of the training program. To Lidor et al. (2009), TS programs refer to specific tasks or tests that target an athlete's capability to demonstrate competence in a particular sport or position within that sport. TDV, on the other hand, "implies that the athletes/players are being provided with the appropriate learning/practice conditions to promote and realize their potential in a specific sport" (Lidor et al, p. 134). TS does not include attempts to predict future success based on identifying the athlete's psychological characteristics. Ostensible Advantages of TD/TID Programs TID programs are often of great importance to select sporting bodies of governments that seek national and international status in competitive sport and apply scarce financial resources toward developing potential champion athletes, so that they may achieve national and international recognition (Vaeyens et al., 2009). Effective use of these financial resources are compromised, however, if these efforts fail to accurately predict future success in competitive sport, particularly among younger competitors (Lidor et al., 2009). Ideally, therefore, the early detection of talent provides the opportunity to obtain the best "retum," or "investment," in giving potential elite level competitors the required resources in coaching expertise, equipment, facilities, practice time, and opportunities to reach their full sport potential. TID programs have been attempted with respect to the testing of sport skills, as well as of physiological and anthropological parameters.

242/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

Proponents of TD programs claim that showing that sport skill tests are, in fact, efficacious in predicting future sport skill performance adds credence to the examination of the psychological dimension (Vaeyens et al., 2009). In fact, the results of selected studies have indicated that measures of motor ability and motor skill proficiency predict future sport performance to a relatively high degree, at least at the elite level (e.g., Falk, Lidor, Lander, & Lang, 2004; Kerr, Booth, Dainty, & Gaborault, 1980). Tennis Canada's First Serve TD program includes measurements of skin-fold, bone diameter, body girth, and reaction times that, ostensibly, accurately predict an athlete's future sport skill level and the athlete's compatibility with the demands of a particular sport (Leone, 1993). The program does not, however, include psychological measures. Another apparent advantage of TD programs is that they maximize the number of gifted individuals participating in a given sport, resulting in stronger domestic competition and likely increasing the number of internationally competitive athletes (Durand-Bush & Salmela, 2001 ; Hahn, 1990). This is because TD programs ostensibly promote competitiveness and direct athletes toward sports in which they are more likely to succeed, increasing the number of athletes aiming for elite levels of sport (Abbott & Collins, 2002,2004; Bompa, 1999). Meeting psychological needs reduces sport attrition, that is, the likelihood of dropping out of a sport at which the athlete is expected to succeed (Petlichkoff, 1993, 1996). As Petlichkoff noted, attempts to determine the sport that best represents a young athlete's skills might provide a more efficient way than traditional trial-and-error approaches. Along these lines, Bompa (1999), Hahn (1990), and Haskell (1983) contend that TD programs profile the athletes' strengths and weaknesses, and provide them with relevant feedback so that they can effectively monitor their progress throughout the entire training program. For example, Pethchkoff ( 1993,1996) contends that children who driftfi-omsport to sport in an attempt to fmd a satisfying and rewarding experience waste an enormous amount of time and resources. As a result, many children with high quality sport talent do not find their niche in sport, consuming considerable time in their search for a sport that is compatible with their skills and goals (Feldman, 1986). TD programs, therefore, can maximize the number of children who have positive sport experiences and a greater likelihood of success, thereby reducing the rate of sport dropout (St-Aubin & Sidney, 1996). The productivity of elite coaches is also enhanced by ensuring that their time, energy, and resources are directed toward the development of younger athletes who have the potential to succeed in elite sport (Bloom, 2002; MacNamara, Button, & Collins, 2010). One additional factor in favor of TD programs is the limited statistical evidence from numerous studies, reviewed by Deaner and Silva (2002), in which discriminant function analysis has detected unique psychological characteristics that predict long-term sport success

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /243 among young athletes. For example, athletes are categorized as "elite" and "non-elite" on measures of self-confidence (Andersen, 1976;Vealey, 1985,2002), ambition (Mahoney, 1989), self-motivation (Mahoney, 1989), emotional stability (Missoum & Laforestrie, 1981), and enthusiasm (Missoum & Laforestrie, 1981). When these advantages are considered together, an effective TD program will not only identify (younger) athletes who already possess desirable psychological characteristics that are commensurate with successful sport performance, but will also create a template against which other athletes (and their coaches and parents) can aspire and leam over time (Renger, 1993). Table 1 lists selected studies that discriminate between successful and less-successful athletes. The Case Against the Use of Psychological Measures in TD Programs The case against the use of psychological measures in TD sports programs rests primarily on three factors, failure to take into consideration the performers' physical maturation, the coach's role in the athlete's skill development, and flaws in the scientific process. The Performers ' Physical Maturation TD programs may assist coaches, athletes, and the athletes' parents in identifying the type of sport that is most compatible based on the performer's physical attributes. These programs, however, may not accurately predict future skill development and sport performance. Predicting future successful sport performance using physiological and anthropological measures has received uneven support in the exercise science literature (Lidor et al., 2009). For example, based on their review of 13 studies that were aimed at distinguishing between highly-talented and less-talented athletes, Lidor et al. concluded that "no clear-cut evidence has been found to support the predictive value of physical tests in talent detection and early development in sport" (p. 140). They cite numerous studies indicating "no correlation of physical tests with final selection and ranking of athletes" (p. 140). Along these lines. Till, Cobley, O'Hara, Chapman, and Cooke (2010) found low relationships between anthropomtrie, physiological, and selected characteristics in high performance junior rugby league players in the United Kingdom. The authors concluded that these results raise concems about the ability of motor skill testing to identify characteristics of immediate and long-term player selection and development. Similar concems have surfaced conceming the use of psychological testing for prediction purposes.

244/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

Table 1.
Psychological characteristics discriminating between successful and less successfid athletes Constructs discriminating Instruments Used Sport Study N Sex Age skill level Andersen (1976)

152 Male

18-22

Swimming

Cattell 16 PF Social skills

Self-confidence

Hahn (1990)

N/A N/A

N/A

N/A

Review of Literature

Stubbomness Self-confidence Goal-orientation Self-motivation Anxiety*

Haskell (1983)

N/A N/A

N/A

N/A

Review of Literature

Self-confidence Goal-orientation . Self-control Enthusiasm Self-motivation

Ho (1987)

N/A N/A

N/A

N/A

Review of Literature

Stubbomness Ambition Self-control Intelligence

Hogg (1986)

N/A N/A

N/A

N/A

Review of Literature

Stubbomness Self-confidence Goal-orientation Emotional stability Ambition Social skills Self-motivation

* denotes a decrease in the designated trait

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /245 Table 1 Continued.


Psychological characteristics discriminating between successful and less successfid athletes Constructs Study N Sex Age Sport Instruments Used discriminating skiU level Jerome (1993) 273 Female 11-25 Synchronized Cattell HSPQ 16 PF Happy-go-lucky Swimming SCAT Anxiety* Rotto-I-E Exta-nalLOC Buss-Durkee Hostility Self-Analysis Test Motivation Analysis Test (MAT) Swimming Intaviews Ambition Self-motivation Emotional stability Intelligence Self-motivation Anxiety* Neuroticism* Self-motivation Neuroticism* Anxiety*

Kalinowski (1985) Komadel (1988)

24

Male & N/A Female N/A

N/A N/A

N/A

Review of Literature

Mahoney (1989")

67

Male

14-20+ Weightlifting SCL-90R Mood (POMS)

Mahoney, Gabriel, &


PeiHfin

713 Male& 17-25


Female

Various

Psychological Skills in Sport (PSIS-P5)

(1987) Missoum & 220 Male Laforestrie (1981) 16-21 Various Eysenck Personality Inventory Emotional stability Ambition Enthusiasm

* denotes a decrease in the designated trait

246 / Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

Along these lines, researchers and practitioners have examined the relationship between TD and TDV in sport. For example, Gulbin, Oldenziel, Weissensteiner, and Gagne (2010) reviewed "key developmental experiences and insights" of 673 high performance Australian athletes (p. 149). They determined that elite athletes possess several selected characteristics that are not found in their non-elite counterparts. All of the identified characteristics, however, were behavioral (e.g., commitment to practice, access to high quality coaching) and not psychological in nature. In addition, no personalify traits were listed. In their review of related literature, Lidor et al. (2009) concluded that assessing physical ability and skill level in order to determine future talent of athletes offers no clear support of the predictive value of these tests, either for individual or for team sports. Thus, while TD programs may help a young athlete decide to which sport he or she is best suited, the capability of these programs to predict future sport success may not be as promising. Coach Expertise and Influence The athlete's coach is almost always the most important external source that influences the development of physical and mental skills (Bloom, 2002). Two issues must be addressed with respect to the coach's role in the use of psychological measures in detecting and predicting an athlete's talent. First, reliance on the use of psychological inventories in TID programs imdermines the coach's role in developing the athlete's talent. Predicting future performance fi-om inventories does not take into account a coach's expertise. Coaches are primarily responsible for each athlete's development and maturation, particularly at the elite level (Salmela & Rgnier, 1985). The coach's expertise is far more likely to influence an athlete's performance potential than psychological testing, especially over the long-term (Bloom). The second point related to coaches is that athletes who are designated as having "high," or "good," potential to achieve in sport are likely to receive far superior coaching than their less-skilled peers. This phenomenon, called an "expectancy effect," consists of a person in a subordinate position (e.g., child, student, athlete, experimental participant) responding to an authority figure (e.g., parent, teacher, parent, coach, experimenter) in a manner that is consistent with the authority figure's expectations (Thomas, Nelson, & Silverman, 2011 ). Three fypes of expectancy effects include halo effect, Rosenthal effect, and Hawthorne effect (see Thomas et al., for descriptions). In the current context, this phenomenon might refer to as a coaching bias built into TID programs. Two studies lend credence to this view. Christensen (2009) conducted in-depth interviews with eight elite soccer coaches, who identified the characteristics of highly-skilled soccer players. Christensen found that coaches predicted future success among highly-rated players who "were assumed to be willing to leam" and were "perceived to be hard working and dedicated" by their coaches (p. 379). These

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT. ../247 qualities are derived from good coaching rather than being generated from an inventory that ostensibly predicts the level of future sport performance. In another study, Davids and Baker (2007) found that highly-skilled coaches are more likely to be associated with elite athletes due to their excellent teaching and leadership skills. Specifically, better coaches (of elite athletes) offer superior structure and content of practice, maximize training time, and engage in meticulous planning. Thus, the degree of coach expertise is a mediating, but rarely controlled, variable in the attempt to validate the efficacy of TD/TID programs (Reilly, Williams Nevill & Franks, 2000). Flaws in the Scientific Process Flaws in the scientific process, which represent particularly powerful issues in questioning the role of psychological factors in talent TD, include these 12 components: (1) vague definitions of selected constructs, (2) inconsistency in defining an "elite" athlete, (3) invalid inventories/poor predictive validity, (4) poor research methodology and statistical procedures, (5) sample bias, (6) failure to use baseline measures, (7) extensive use of cross-sectional comparisons, (8) paucity of skill level comparisons, (9) poor inventory construction, (10) lhnitations in personality research, (11) inherent problems with self-report, and (12) overreliance on anecdotal evidence. Vague Definitions of Selected Constructs The terms "mental toughness," "competitiveness," and "psychological readiness" are often used when attempting to determine an athlete's potential for future success. Ahnost unknown, however, are their operational defmitions, and the extent to which these characteristics identify or predict sport skill level (Singer & Janelle, 1999). In addition, interpreting and applying such arguments would challenge most sport psychology consultants and coaches. It is not known, for example, whether these measures are stable (i.e., trait) or situational (i.e., state) constructs, or whether they refiect relatively stable, cross-situational dispositions (i.e., traits) and thus are open to change through intervention and experience (i.e., state constructs), as proposed by Anshel (2012). Similar limitations are inherent in examining specific psychological characteristics in TD research (Durand-Bush & Salmela, 2001). Inconsistency in Defining an "Elite" Athlete Examining psychological characteristics of athletes in predicting future success has usually consisted of comparing "elite" and "non-elite" athletes. Operationally defining an "elite" sports competitor, however, has been markedly inconsistent in the literature (Anshel, 2012). Often, researchers have used statistical procedures to discriminate skill level as a fimc-

248/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

tion of his or her current success or achievement (Matsudo, 1996). Traditionally, elite athletes have been defined as individuals "who are eligible for competition at the national, intemational, or Olympic level, or who are professional sports persons" (Van den Auweele, Cuyper, Van Mele, & Rzewnicki, 1993, p. 257). An additional definition of the elite athlete includes individuals who are eligible for such competition, but may not actually compete (e.g., Spamer & Coetzee, 2002), while another definition refers to athletes who are currently involved in sport competition at a particular level (Falk et al., 2004). An additional concem is that the term "elite" is often culturally specific (Gan, Anshel, & Kim, 2009). For example, an elite athlete may be defined as a sports competitor at the national level in some studies, while in other studies the term "elite" is used for college students who played on their high school sports teams. It is unlikely, therefore, that elite athletes, as identified in various studies from different cultures will display the same characteristics in, for example, Africa (Spamer & Coetzee, 2002), Asia (Gan et al., 2009), Europe (Williams & Reilly, 2000), and North America (Brown, 2001). This inconsistency compromises the primary objective of TD programs - to predict the future quality of sport performance. Invalid Inventories: Poor Predictive Validity Perhaps one of the most compelling cases against the use of psychological measures in TD programs is poor predictive validity. Predictive validity reflects "the degree to which a measuring instrument or test yields information allowing prediction of actual behavior or performance" (Myers & Hansen, 2012, p. 592). A plethora of published studies comparing elite and non-elite or high and low-skilled sports competitors on selected psychological variables did not have promising results. For example, Prescott (1996) attempted to identify motivation, goal orientation, attribution, and locus of control as predictors of talent among British gymnasts aged 7-10 yrs, and found a very low prediction rate. In their extensive review, Deaner and Silva (2002) concluded that "while some of these studies do show personality differences based on sport type and gender...many of these studies are old and focus only on a few select sports or a few select characteristics" (p. 61). Poor Research Methodology and Statistical Procedures Researchers and theorists have noted inherent limitations of many studies concemed with identifying current psychological characteristics of athletes, comparing athletes categorized as elite and non-elite or making cross cultural comparisons (Gauvin & Russell, 1993), and predicting athletes' future achievement level in sport. Some of these issues have concemed the use of inventories that were not intended for the current sample (Morgan, 1997), and improper psychometric validation and statistical procedures that render the instrument invalid

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT. ../249 (Schutz & Gessaroli, 1993). In his review of related literature concerning methodological research problems, Morgan lists "the absence of randomization, small sample size, inadequate psychological measures, and experimenter expectancy effects, among other flaws" (p. 4). Also problematic in this area is that researchers have labeled constructs interchangeably, such as juxtaposing the athltts's personality traits with his or her orientations, styles, dispositions, and behavioral tendencies (Anshel, 2012). Each of these constructs differ; some are more amenable to change through counseling and treatment (e.g., orientations of mental toughness or competitiveness; behavioral tendencies such as pre-performance routines) than others (e.g., trait anxiefy, neuroticism, trait anger, stimulus-seeking). Failure to control for moderator variables such as gender and culture provides an additional concern. Gauvin and Russell contend, for instance, that "it is widely acknowledged that such cultural factors can potentially produce major distortions and inaccuracies in test interpretation" (p. 892). Based on their thorough review of related literature, Gauvin and Russell concluded that "the selection of sport/exercise-specific tests and scales., .requires a careful conceptual analysis of the constructs under investigation, an examination of the measurement assumptions of the theoretical framework employed, and in some cases, a consideration of the amount of variance explained in the target variables" (p. 899). Clearly, future study is needed toward the continued development and validation of psychological measures that attempt to predict and identify the potential for future talent in sport. Taken together, a common threat to internal and external validity is the use of a selfreport instrument that was neither constructed nor validated for the intended sample (Thomas et al., 2011). For instance, sample characteristics, or the psychological demands of specific sports in which "desirable" traits are being identified, are not taken into account when developing inventory items (Andersen, 1976; Gauvin & Russell, 2003; Hahn, 1990). Consequently, one inherent limitation in the existing literature is the lack of consistency in determining for whom the inventory was intended and to whom it may be applied (e.g., a universify athlete, a highly talented competitor at the community level, a national or an international level competitor, or an Olympic or professional performer). The use of improper statistical analyses in TD research has been ubiquitous (see Renger, 1993; Schutz, 1998; Schutz & Gessaroli, 1993; and Vealey, 1985, for reviews). While an exhaustive review of these limitations goes beyond the scope of this paper, specific examples abound. For example, one statistical approach by researchers has been to attempt to statistically separate elite from non-elite athletes using multiple regression models and discriminant analyses. However, the use of a regression equation on a different population from the one for which it was developed is inappropriate (Nesselroade & Baltes, 1979). While it is important to test for predictive validity by cross-validating results (Renger, 1993), most studies have not included

250/Journal of Sport Behavior. Vol. 35, No. 3

attempts at cross-validation. Another statistical limitation in TD assessment is the frequent use of univariate, not multivariate, statistics resulting in low predictive power, the virtual absence of statistical interactions, and the failure to consider the complex network of factors underlying sport performance (Schutz, 1998). One misuse of multivariate statistics is the violation of acceptable caseto-predictor ratios, resulting in a loss of statistical power. An acceptable ratio is 5:1, and preferably 6:1 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Instead, Salmela and Rgnier (1985) propose using discriminant function analyses (DFA) to determine if the selected variables discriminate among the members of each group, and specifically, to find variables that are appropriate for testing the targeted population. DFA may identify athletes who are highly skilled, however, it does not predict future performance (Rgnier, Salmela, & Russell, 1993 ; Schutz & Gessaroli, 1993). Regression analyses more accurately predict outcomes within a targeted population. Yet another limitation of TD research is the incorrect interpretation of correlational data as cause and effect (Schutz & Gessaroli, 1993). Studies of the TD literature report a lack of proper research methods and statistical procedures (see Anshel, 2012; Durand-Bush & Salmela, 2001; Lidor et al., 2009; Schutz & Gessaroli, 1993; St-Aubin & Sidney, 1996; Van den Auweele et al., 1993;Vealey, 1985,2002). According to these authors, the primary issues that have compromised the integrity of attempts to predict high quality sport performance (i.e., TID) among child or adolescent age groups include sample bias, failure to use baseline measures, improper statistical procedures, extensive use of cross-sectional comparisons, a paucity of skill level comparisons, failure to control for coach expertise, poor inventory construction, inherent limitations of self-report, and over-reliance on anecdotal evidence. Sample Bias Selection bias for research purposes occurs in cases where the participants in studies are recruited based on their availability, their personal motivation to engage in the study, investigator coercion (i.e., participation not fully voluntary), or the athletes' current skill level and pre-existing personal characteristics (Thomas et al, 2011). Selection bias may result in statistical regression or spontaneous remission, which may innate positive results. Collectively, these biases may contribute to an expectancy effect (Martinek, & Karper, 1984), also called a self-fulfilling prophecy (Hom, Lox, & Labrador, 1998). This is because these athletes are usually labeled "elite" or "highly skilled," thereby influencing the coaches' (or researchers') attitudes, expectations, and behaviors toward these pre-labeled players. Ostensibly, then, athletes with "superior" scores on selected psychological characteristics may excel because of the high expectations of their coaches or researchers. Coaches with high expectations of

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /251 athletes tend to provide more positive and instructional feedback than do coaches with relatively lower expectations (Hom et al., 1998; Martinek, Crowe, & Rejeski, 1982). Failure to Use Baseline Measures TD studies have often failed to establish a baseline measure for dependent variables (Spamer & Coetzee, 2002), which is important when accounting for the athlete's previous experience and current skill level, initial differences in group comparisons, and the use of cognitive and behavioral strategies that influence the athletes' cognition, affect, and performance. Developmental research is a particular research method that requires the comparison of initial and subsequent performance over time as a function of interventions (e.g., fitness training, skill development, coaching, and the use of mental skills). Consequently, researchers and practitioners in talent research are often unable to detect changes in performance in relation to the inventory's initial (baseline) scores. Extensive Use of Cross-Sectional Comparisons Most TD studies have been cross-sectional, as opposed to longitudinal (Lidor et al., 2009). Cross-sectional designs limit the power to predict which traits, if any, are associated with the athlete's long-term commitment to a given sport (Kantowitz, Roediger, & Elmes, 2005). Cross-sectional designs are also limited due to the pyramid effect, that is, as the athletic pyramid narrows, athletes who do not exhibit the traits required for continued participation are often eliminated (Jerome, 1993). Longitudinal studies are more sensitive to changes in psychological characteristics than cross-sectional studies, thereby eliminating the pyramid effect (Bloom, 1985; Thomas et al., 2011). While cross-sectional studies should be viewed as merely a first step in TD programs (Poppleton & Salmoni, 1991), longitudinal research is needed to improve prediction rates and accuracy (Matsudo, 1996). In a rare longitudinal study in this area, Vaeyens et al. (2009) compared the performance characteristics of youth sports' athletes between world class and national level senior athletes, and found no significant differences between the two groups. One key finding of this review was that sporting success and intense discipline-specific training and competition among adolescents did not contribute to explaining or predicting long-term success as an adult. They concluded that "early sport specialization as a child does not appear to be a prerequisite for attaining expertise as an adult" (p. 13 74). Paucity of Skill Level Comparisons Comparing elite and non-elite athletes allows the use of multivariate statistics to identify traits that discriminate between these groups. Attempts to determine differences between elite and non-elite athletes, necessary for examining the unique attributes of higher skilled competi-

252 /Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

tors, are rarely compared in the same study (see Elliott, Ackland, Blanksby, & Bloomfield, 1990; Roetert, Brown, Piorkowski, & Woods, 1996). The authors contend that athletes are not typically designated as "successful" or "unsuccessful." The combined result has low discriminatory power. Poor Inventory Construction This section warrants a brief review based on the extent to which poor inventory construction exists throughout the sport science literature. According to Schutz and Gessaroli (1993), inventories used in studies to identify psychological characteristics of athletes for descriptive or predictive purposes have suffered from poor item construction and a paucify of proper psychometric properties. The authors cite several inventories that did not include the necessary psychometric data. Failing to control for sport type was apparent in most of the studies. Schutz and Gessaroli contend that the pervasive absence of a conceptual basis for item content, as well as low content and predictive validify, have been symptomatic of these problems. Limitations in Personality Research Attempts to determine and predict the qualify of sports performance is rooted in sport personalify research (see Anshel, 2012, and Vealey, 2002, for reviews). Perhaps nowhere throughout the sport and performance psychology literature is the case against TD programs stronger than in the sport personalify research. TD studies are designed to predict the qualify of future sport performance based on selected personalify traits or dispositions (Van den Auweele, Nys, Rzewnicki, & Van Mele, 2001). Contrary to this assumption, however, the sport personalify literature reflects a paucify of research supportive of trait personalify theory (Van den Auweele et al., 2001). Personalify tests in general have a very low prediction rate - only 8-10% of explained variance - in determining future sport performance qualify (Anshel, 2012). In addition, there has been an overall failure on most measures to associate "high" and "low" scores with athletic success in the majorify of psychological inventories (Deaner & Silva, 2002). This has direct implications for TD programs, which are predicated on the validify of psychological meastires to predict future behavior, specifically, the qualify of sport skill performance. Personalify testing as a predictor of performance success has been heavily criticized in the extant literature as inherently flawed, due in part to the lack of psychometric support (Schutz, 1998). Common limitations include response bias, failure to take into account situational factors, sport-specific demands (see Van den Auweele et al., 2001 ; Vealey, 1985,2002), and not controlling for cultural differences (Duda & Hayashi, 1998). Testing elite athletes with various psychological inventories, such as the Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr, &

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /253 Droppleman, 1992), the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI; Hathaway & McKinley, 1970), or the State-Trait Anxiefy Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vaag, & Jacobs, 1983), shows that no single measure or set of psychological characteristics is sufiRcient for predicting the qualify of future athletic performance (Cox, 2012; Van den Auweele et al., 1993, 2001). The lack of ecological validify of constructs and dependent measures represents another inherent limitation of personalify research with respect to TD programs. Schutz (1998), and more recently Deaner and Silva (2002), contend that personalify research in sport psychology has failed to live up to its early promise of predictive abilify, particularly with respect to long-term predictions of future skill level in sport. The use of personalify scales as relevant components of TD programs has generally not been supported (Schutz, 1998). While selected studies have shown the uniqueness of personalify traits shared by most elite athletes, Deaner and Silva (2002) concluded that "studies showing a significant relationship between personalify and sport performance carmot be considered reliable" due to numerous methodological and theoretical limitations (p. 51). Finally, Davids and Baker (2007) contend that "the links between psychological traits and performance are not as clear (as physiological traits)" (p. 968). Taken together, it is apparent that attempts to use psychological tests for predicting future success have been fraught with disappointing results. Inherent Limitations of Self-Report Self-report has inherent limitations in psychological research, including the use of psychological inventories for TID programs. For example, respondents, including athletes, can easily fake their answers in accordance with the expectations or preferences of others (Miller & Edgington, 1984). This practice, referred to as social desirability response bias, is defined as "the tendency for a person to respond in a way that seems socially appealing, regardless of his or her true characteristics" (Furr & Bacharach, 2008, p. 246). As Morgan ( 1978) contends, in the sport psychology literature "psychometricians are well aware of the problems associated with response distortion, and it is widely recognized that most self-report inventories are easily faked" (p. 223). The use of corroborative measures related to the athlete's skill assessments by coaches and other experts might at least partially circumvent the limitations of selfreport. Overreliance on Anecdotal Evidence The empirical perspective of TD has traditionally relied primarily on anecdotal evidence for providing standards of desirable psychological and behavioral characteristics. This is called the "bottom up solution," in which knowledge is gained from the collective wisdom obtained from individual interviews of sports competitors. Anecdotal evidence has been shown

254/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

to have poor predictive power because it reflects an N of 1, that is, reporting the experiences of one individual (see Anshel, 1993, for a description of the limitations of anecdotal evidence on drugs in sport). Anecdotal reports limit the generalizabilify of information and fail to account for individual differences (e.g., heritabilify, socialization) and task and situational factors inherent in competitive sport (Anshel, 2012); it is not a research method (Thomas et al, 2011). While a compilation of anecdotal reports may provide justification for conducting further research, far more problematic is the use of anecdotal reports to justify the efficacy of TD programs. Philosophical Issues Philosophical issues address ethical considerations, such as the "appropriateness" of TD programs or the allocation of communify financial and physical resources. Addressing, these issues requires recognizing that the main objectives of TD/TED programs are associated with discriminating between athletes who appear to have, as opposed to who do not have, the potential to reach the status of "elite" sport. These objectives include: (a) attempting to match individuals to sport activities to which they are best suited based on physiological and psychological measures; (b) selecting or eliminating certain athletes for future participation at elite levels of sport, and providing those "selected" competitors with optimal training and coaching conditions, and (c) allowing sports organizations, coaches, parents, and the athletes themselves to determine the extent to which they are committed to the necessary time and fmancial resources needed to reach the challenging goals of elite level sport (Abbott & Collins, 2002; Brown, 2001 ; Williams & Reilly, 2000). Questions remain, however, about the ethical and moral considerations of using psychological instruments to aid in making these decisions and to draw these conclusions. Four issues are discussed in this section: (1) the Gatekeeper Syndrome, (2) the varied expertise of sport psychologist and consultants, (3) limited fmancial resources, and (4) questioning the validify of identifying pre-requisite characteristics. The Gatekeeper Syndrome Individuals or groups who claim to have the knowledge and power to make final decisions that have long-term implications on the lives of others, such as determining an athlete's future involvement in sport, exhibit the Gatekeeper Syndrome (MacNamara et al., 2010). "Gatekeepers" are individuals who regulate and monitor accepted knowledge in a field of study and practice (Christensen, 2009; St-Aubin & Sidney, 1996). In the present context, the "gatekeeper" determines the future status of an athlete based on the athlete's score on a psychological inventory. The philosophical question is whether a coach (or anyone else)

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /255 should act as gatekeepers in using psychological inventories to predict an individual's future sport success. Who are these "gatekeepers?" St-Aubin and Sidney (1996) described a TD gatekeeper system in which "the expertise of... sport scientists is relied upon in the decision-making process... particularly sport psychologists" (p. 10). Martindale et al. (2005) contend that recent advances in sport psychology measurement techniques, advanced research, and more experience in practice settings should be accompanied by less premature judging and predicting of sport talent. From a philosophical perspective, the issue is whether researchers and coaches, using psychological inventories as part of TD programs, should be the gatekeepers of an athlete's future - particularly in the absence of additional high quality coaching, training, mental skills, and sport experiences. The question is whether a gatekeeper can accurately determine the desirable, or even the requisite, psychological characteristics of elite sports participants, and then claim to accurately measure them in predicting future sport success. Varied Expertise of Sport Psychologists and Consultants Should TD programs that are based on the administration of psychological inventories be controlled by sport psychologists (individuals who are licensed psychologists, hence the title "psychologist") and sport psychology consultants (individuals who have expertise administering mental skills training but are not licensed psychologists and, therefore, may not use the title "psychologist")? Several researchers over the years (e.g., Anshel, 1992) have argued against the use of sport psychology practitioners in the use of inventories for prediction and even for diagnostic purposes. What would researchers and practitioners do with these inventory scores? Do these professionals have sufficient training in generating, administering, scoring, and interpreting the inventories? Is there training that instructs practitioners on proper ways to apply inventory data? While in recent years an increase has been seen in the proper training and greater monitoring of certification and training procedures in the practice of applied sport psychology (Lidor, Morris, Bardaxoglou, & Becker, 2001 ; Singer & Anshel, 2006), there has also been general disagreement - and even confusion - in detemiining the proper educational background, requisite skills, and training for effective practitioners in this field. It is important to acknowledge two factors conceming the influence of sport psychology on talent development. First, a thorough review ofthe related literature clearly indicates that sport psychology practitioners in many counfries have been highly effective in enhancing mental skills and sport performance of male and female athletes in virtually all sports and skill levels (Alfermann & Lidor, 2005). Second, however, is that the field's current level of development precludes any clear and consistent ability to identify and assess the requisite

256/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

mental skills and psychological characteristics that can accurately predict future skill level in sport (Martindale et al., 2005). Previous attempts over the years to overcome these problems have been met with serious fiaws. Blanksby's (1980) work provides an historical perspective on the controversy of adopting TID programs. He describes the Australian TD program for swimmers, and has proposed that identifying future elite level swimmers could be accomplished through the school system; tests of fundamental movement skill tests could be administered annually to all students. Blanksby also suggested that psychological tests of locus of control and sociograms that measure group dynamics be administered at the same time. He argues that psychological tests "might help the teacher to understand and counsel each child more adequately" (p. 18). Local sporting club representatives would visit the schools at Grade 5 to engage in "a combination of wise counseling, individual desire, and gravitation to areas of natural preference [that] would ensure greater participation and nurturing of talented youngsters..." (p. 18). The first author's visit to the People's Republic of China revealed that this selection strategy occurs today in China, as well as in many other countries. However, there are limitations to these suggestions. The appropriateness of using one personalify construct (e.g., locus of control, trait confidence) or sociometric data (e.g., team member interactions, attitudes toward teammates) is questionable. In addition, there are potential "costs" to the communify of categorizing children, as early as age 10 yrs, as having "poor" future potential to succeed in sport. Based on the extensive rate of dropping out of youth sport, it is plausible to surmise that many children will discontinue their participation in sport after receiving this negative information. Rather than fostering a culture-wide positive attitude of engaging in sport and other forms of physical activify, many children will conclude they do not have the proper skills to play sports and will feel unmotivated and lack confidence to leam and improve their sport skills. Also unknown is the number of children who will succumb to the self-fulfilling prophecy (Hom et al., 1998), in which the label "poor potential" nurtures low personal expectations and reduces the child's motivation to engage in regular exercise and other forms of physical activify. Limited Financial Resources Another very important philosophical issue that strongly affects the decision to sponsor TD programs is the use of limited communify and regional financial resources, both public (i.e., government) and private (i.e., corporate sponsorships). The community must make judicious decisions about the appropriate allocation of these resources, and the ethical consideration of devoting limited communify financial resources toward a program in which relatively

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT. ../257 few individuals (i.e., young athletes) will benefit (Green & Houlihan, 2005). Brown (2001) asserts that serving relatively few athletes at the cost of eliminating sport opportunities for a far greater number will likely eradicate the aspirations of many sports participants who have the capabilify of nurturing their talent through high qualify coaching, instruction, and practice. Existing frinds could be applied to recreational programs, improving fitness, learning new sport skills, and programs for high-risk individuals (i.e., adolescents involved in crime and drug abuse). As Ct and Hay (2002) concluded from their review of socialization processes in sport, these suggestions carry a significant influence in promoting involvement in children's sport. It is an issue of ethics, then, whether communify resources should serve "the few" at the expense of "the many" when it comes to the number of children and adolescents who would benefit from competitive sport. Questioning the Validity of Identifying Pre-Requisite Characteristics The TD paradigm falsely assumes that the psychological factors that accompany high qualify sports performance can be identified, or that these requisite characteristics even exist. For example. Brown (2001), Durand-Bush and Salmela (2001), and Tranckle and Cushion (2006) assert that coaches and researchers disagree about the most desirable psychological characteristics of elite-level competitors. A plethora of studies have attempted to ascertain the psychological characteristics of highly skilled competitors, and some characteristics have been consistently identified (e.g., confidence, risk-taking, competitiveness, optimism, mental toughness). However, whether these characteristics - or the inventories used to measure them predict an athlete's performance potential or discriminate between athletes who compete in elite and non-elite levels, remains questionable. As Abbott and Collins (2004) conclude, "...current talent identification and development processes are likely to exclude many 'talented' children from support programs while rare resources are 'misinvested' in others" (p. 395). Conclusions and Future Directions: Moving to Talent Development There is ample evidence that psychological measures do not discriminate between athletes of different skill levels. Various attempts at predicting future performance qualify, usually conducted as ex post facto research in which the personal histories and experiences of current elite athletes at various levels of competition are compared, show relatively few differences among the athletes (Davids & Baker, 2007; Vaeyens et al., 2009). Attempts at predicting an athlete's future success is meaningless without adequate resources to follow up this process and to develop the athlete's potential (Martindale et al, 2005). The limited fmancial resources of most communities, however, make this suggestion unrealistic and unlikely. Jarver (1982) contends that "even if it would be possible (to identify talent), how many 12 to 13-year-olds.

258/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

after being identified as hammer throwing talent, for example, would be interested to take up this activify to develop their talent" (p. 7). The use of psychological inventories for detecting athletic talent has been less than efficacious. There are altemative programs, however. Numerous authors have proposed that efforts toward the early detection and development of talent need to be re-conceptualized. Martindale et al. (2005), for instance, contend that sports programs should stress the "appropriate development" of sport skills rather than the early selection of young prospects, and then focus on meeting each athlete's individual needs through high qualify coaching and program opportunities. Instead of using psychological inventories to predict future sport success, Tranckle and Cushion (2006) suggest that it is more important to improve our understanding of an athlete's potential to perform sports skills based on the direct observations and assessments by skilled coaches, and to identify the physical and psychological characteristics of elite sport participants. Hoare (1996), for example, described an Australian program called "Talent Search," which consists of three phases - school screening, sport specific testing, and talent developing. Hoare argues that the process of TD may be inherently flawed hecause "the successful selection of athletic talent has relied upon experienced coaches, (a procedure that) is limited in that it only selects athletes from within that particular sport. If the athlete is better suited to another sport, this will not be determined" (p. 3). Consequently, psychological measures have been dropped from the current Australian TD program. Phillips, Davids, Renshaw, and Portus (2010) examined the factors that fostered skill development among fast bowlers in Australian cricket. They found that instead of the use of psychological profiles, it was of critical importance to provide younger athletes with the opportunify to compete with older cricket players, "forcing them to constantly adapt their behaviors and increase their level of performance" (p. 145). Again, behavioral strategies rather than psychological inventories appear to he more efficacious in developing sport talent. Another option, posited by Ct, Baker, and Abemethy (2007) and Ct, Lidor, and Hackfort (2009), is a system of talent support and guidance rather than prediction. RofFey and Gross (1991) contend that researchers and practitioners should attempt to help each athlete achieve his or her performance potential through the use of physical and mental skills training. They assert that "psychological skills appear to be leamed skills, and those who master skills such as mental blocking, intemalizing, goal setting, coping with pressure, and concentration are those who will make it" (p. 3 71 ). An additional, preferred, approach to identifying talent among younger athletes is a program called performance profiling (PP; Dale & Wrisberg, 1996; Jones, 1993). PP consists of cognitive-behavioral interventions that help coaches and consultants identify an appropriate

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT ../259 psychological intervention, enhance the competitor's self-motivation to conduct the intervention, and monitor performance changes during the intervention. Dale and Wrisberg (1996) used PP with a universify women's volleyball team for team goal-setting, resulting in "a more open atmosphere for communication" between the players and their coach (p. 261 ). Finally, perhaps the approach to predicting or developing sports talent should be a function of the old adage "practice makes perfect." Vaeyens et al. (2009) describe the "deliberate practice framework" in which "the level of attainment in any field is directly and monotonically related to the accumulated amount of deliberate practice in that field" (p. 1368). Based on their review of related literature, the authors describe a "time economic framework" in which athletic success is tied to accumulating "more hours of deliberate (high qualify) practice than your competitors" (p. 1369). The mechanism of this approach is to "accelerate talent development processes by extension and intensification of training time in the targeted sport discipline" (p. 1369). TDV remains a superior alternative to TD/TID because it is predicated on the coach's systematic observation and assessment of each athlete's skills, followed by a plan of action for proper training and skill reassessment (Ct et al., 2009; Ct, Salmela, Trudel, Baria, & Russell, 1995). It appears that rather than engage in attempts to predict future elite level performance of relatively few young athletes, limited fmancial and personal resources should be used: (a) to provide athletes of .all ages the opportunify to select sports that interest them and in which they demonsfrate competence, (b) to allow the normal processes of growth, development, and emotional maturify to form an integral part of the TD process, and (c) to teach athletes the sports skills and the cognitive and behavioral strategies that are necessary for improved sport performance (e.g., Abbott & Collins, 2004; Anshel, 2012). In conclusion, it is apparent that the field of sport psychology is not, and has never been, about the selection or elimination of younger performers whose responses to psychological inventories may greatfy determine their future level of sport competition and performance. Woodman (1985) contends that sports administrators, coaches, parents, and sport psychology consultants should focus on helping sports competitors of all ages reach their performance potential. To Woodman, "it is not enough just to identify talent, it must also be developed through the provision of appropriate training programs, throughout the development stages" (p. 49). As Salmela and Rgnier (1985) assert, "what the question of talent identification comes down to is whether we make our decisions (about an athlete's potential) using all of the available information or whether we wish to use partial hunches; whether we invest in the long-term solution or settle for the short-term fix; (and) whether we try to predict the somewhat predictable future or continue to predict the totally predictable past" (p. 93).

260/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

References Abbott, A., & Collins, D. (2002). A theoretical and empirical analysis of a 'state of the art' talent identification model. High Abilities Studies, 13, 157-278. Abbott, A., & Collins, D. (2004). Eliminating the dichotomy between theory and practice in talent identification and development: considering the role of psychology. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 395-408. Alfermann, D., & Lidor, R. (2005). Educational programs for sport psychologists - an international perspective. In D. Hackfort, J. L. Duda, & R. Lidor (Eds.), Handbook of research in applied sport and exercise psychology: Intemational perspectives (pp. 377-384). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. Andersen, P. A. (1976). Personality differences in United States men's Olympics, national and non-national finalist swimmers and seven stroke classes. In F. Landry & W. A. Orban (Eds.), Motor learning, sport psychology, pedagogy and didactics of physical activity (pp. 327-336). Quebec: Symposia Specialists Inc. Anshel, M. H. (2012). Sport psychology: From theory to practice (5th ed.). San Francisco: Benjamin-Cummings. Anshel, M. H. (1992). The case against certification in sport psychology: In search of the phantom expert. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 265-286. Anshel, M. H. (1993). Drugs in sport. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphy, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 851-876). New York: Macmillan. Blanksby, B. B. (Spring, 1980). Measures of talent identification in competitive swimming. Sports Coach, 4, 13-19. Bloom, B. S. (Ed.). (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York: Ballantine Books. Bloom, G. (2002). Role of the elite coach in the development of talent. In J. M. Silva & E. E. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 466-483). San Francisco: Benjamin-Cummings. Bompa, T. (1999). Periodization: The theory and methodology of training (4th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Brown, J. (2001). Sport talent: How to identify and develop outstanding athletes. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Christensen, M. K. (2009). "An eye for talent": Talent identification and the "practical sense" of top-level soccer coaches. Sociology of Sport Journal, 26, 365-382. Ct, J., Baker, J., & Abemethy, B. (2007). Practice and play in the development of sport expertise. In G. Tenenbaum & R. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed., pp. 184-202). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /261 Cot, J., & Hay, J. (2002). Children's involvement in sport: A developmental perspective. In J. M. Silva & E. E. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 484-502). San Francisco: Benjamin-Cummings Ct, J., Lidor, R., & Hackfort, D. (2009). To sample or to specialize? - Seven postulates about youth sport activities that lead to continued participation and elite performance. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7, 7-17. Ct, J., Salmela, J., Trudel, P., Baria, A., & Russell, S. (1995). The coaching model: a grounded assessment of expert gymnastic coaches' knowledge. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 17, 1-17. Cox, R. H. (2012). Sport psychology: Concepts and applications (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Dale, G. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (1996). The use of a performance profiling technique in a team setting: Getting the athletes and coach on the "same page." The Sport Psychologist, 10, 261-277. Davids, K., & Baker, J. (2007). Genes, environment and sport performance: Why the nature-nurture dualism is no longer relevant. Sports Medicine, 37, 961-980. Deaner, H., & Silva, J. M. (2002). Personality and sport performance. In J. M. Silva & E. E. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 48-65). San Francisco: Benjamin-Cummings. Duda, J., & Hayashi, C. T. (1998). Measurement issues in cross-cultural research within sport and exercise psychology. In J. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 471-483). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. Durand-Bush, N., & Salmela, J. H. (2001). The development of talent in sport. In R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, & C. M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 269-289). New York: Wiley. Elliott, B. C , Ackland, T. R., Blanksby, B. A., & Bloomfield, J. (1990). A prospective study of physiological and kinanthropometric indicators of junior tennis performance. The Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 22, 87-92. Falk, B., Lidor, R., Lander, Y., & Lang, B. (2004). Talent identification and early development of elite water-polo players: a 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 347-355. Feldman, D. H. (1986). Nature's gambit: Child prodigies and the development of human potential. New York, NY: Basic Books. Fun-, R. M., & Bacharach, V. R. (2008). Psychometric s : An introduction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

262/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

Gan, Q., Anshel, M.H., & Kim, J. (2009). Sources and cognitive appraisals of acute stress as predictors of coping style among male and female Chinese athletes. Intemationai Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7, 68-88. Gauvin, L., & Russell, S. J. (2003). Sport-specific and culturally adaptive measures in sport and exercise psychology research: Issues and strategies. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 891-900). New York: Macmillan. Green, M., & Houlihan, B. (2005). Elite sport development - Policy learning and political priorities. London: Routledge. Gulbin, J. P., Oldenziel, K. E., Weissensteiner, J. R., & Gagne, F. (2010). A look through the rear view mirror: Developmental experiences and insights of high performance athletes. Talent Development & Excellence, 2, 149-164. Hahn, A. G. (1990). Identification and selection of talent in Austrahan rowing. Excel, 6, 511. Hahn, A. G., & Tumilty, D. (1989).The rowing talent identification program - an oudine. Excel, 5,12-14. Haskell, W. L. (1983). Assessing the athletic potential of young athletes. In N. J. Smith (Ed.), Sports medicine: health care for young athletes (pp. 33-58). Evanston, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics. Ho, R. (1987). Talent identification in China. In B. Petiot, J. H. Salmela, & T. B. Hoshizaki (Eds.), World identification systems for gymnastic talent (pp. 14-20). Montreal: Sport Psyche Editions. Hoare, D. (Spring, 1996). The Australian national talent search programme. Coaching Focus, 31, 3-4. Hogg, J. M. (1986). Developing talent among young athletes: Some psychological implications. In Proceedings of the Canadian Swim Coaches Association Conference (pp. 142-151). Esterel, Quebec. Horn, T. S., Lox, C , & Labrador, F. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy theory: When coaches' expectations become reality. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak performance (pp. 74-91). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield. Jarver, J. (1982). Do we need talent identification? Modem Athlete and Coach, 20, 7-8. Jerome, W. C. (1993). Using psychological variables to predict athletic success. In S. Serpa, J. Alves, V. Ferreira, & A. Paula-Brito (Eds.), Proceedings of the VIII World Congress of Sport Psychology (pp. 924-928). Lisbon, Portugal. Jones, G. (1993). The role of performance profiling in cognitive behavioral interventions in sport. The Sport Psychologist, 7, 160-172.

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /263 Kalinowski, A. G. (1985). The development of Olympic swimmers. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 139-192). New York: Ballantine Books. Kantowitz, B. H., Roediger, H. L., & Elmes, D. G. (2005). Experimental psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth Leaming. Kerr, R., Booth, M., Dainty, D., & Gahorault, R. (1980). Talent identification from competitive diving. In P. Klavora & K. A. W. Wiper (Eds.), Psychological and sociological factors in sport - Proceedings of the Canadian Society for Psychomotor Leaming and Sport Psychology Annual Congress (pp. 270-276). Canada: Toronto. Komadel, L. (1988). The identification of performance potential. In A. Drix, H. G. Knuttgen, & K. Tittel (Eds.), Olympic book of sports medicine (pp. 275-285). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Science. Leone, M. (1993). First Serve: An introductory program far boys and girls ages 6 to 8. Toronto: Tennis Canada. Lidor, R., Ct, J., & Hackfort, D. (2009). To test or not to test? - The use of physical skill tests in talent detection and in early phases of sport development. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7, 131-146. Lidor, R., Morris, T., Bardaxoglou, N., & Becker, B. (2001). The world sport psychology sourcebook (3rd ed.). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. MacNamara, A., Button, A., & Collins, D. (2010). The role of psychological characteristics in facilitating the pathway to elite performance: Part 1: Identifying mental skills and behaviors. The Sport Psychologist, 24, 52-73. Mahoney, M. J. (1989). Psychological predictors of elite and non-elite performance in Olympic weightlifting. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 20, 1-12. Mahoney, M. J., Gabriel, T. J., & Perkins, T. S. (1987). Psychological skills and exceptional athletic performance. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 181-199. Martindale, R. J. J., Collins, D., & Dauhney, J. (2005). Talent development: A guide for practice and research within sport. Quest, 57, 353-375. Martinek, T. J., Crowe, P. B., & Rejeski, W. J. (1982). Pygmalion in the gym: Causes and effects of expectations in coaching and teaching. West Point, NY: Leisure Press. Martinek, T. J., & Karper, W. B. (1984). The effects of noncompetitive and competitive instructional climates on teacher expectancy effects in elementary physical education classes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 408-421. Matsudo, V. K. (1996). Prediction of future athletic excellence. In O. Bar-Or (Ed.), The child and adolescent athlete (pp. 92-109). New York: Blackwell Science. McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1992). Profile of mood state manual. San Diego: Education and Industrial Testing Service.

264 / Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

Miller, B. P., & Edgington, G. P. (1984). Psychological mood state distortion in a sporting context. Journal of Sport Behavior, 7, 91-94. Missoum, G., & Laforestrie, R. (1981). Psychologie du sport: approche des mcanismes psychologiques lis la dtection des jeunes sportifs et l'optimisation des performances. Bulletin de Psychologie, 37, 347-357. Morgan, W. P. (1997). Methodological considerations. In W. P. Morgan (Ed.), Physical activity & mental health (pp. 3-32). London: Taylor & Francis. Morgan, W. P. (1978). The credulous-skeptical argument in perspective. In W. F. Strub (Ed.), Sport psychology: An analysis of athlete behavior (pp. 218-227). Ithaca, NY: Mouvement. Myers, A., & Hansen, C. (2012). Experimental psychology (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Nesselroade, J. R., & Baltes, P. B. (1979). Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development. New York: Academic Press. Petlichkoff, L. M. (1993). Coaching children: understanding the motivational process. Sport Science Review, 5, 242-252. Petlichkoff, L. M. (1996). The dropout dilemma in youth sports. In O. Bar-Or (Ed.), The child and adolescent athlete (pp. 418-430). New York: Blackwell Science. Phillips, E., Davids, K., Renshaw, I., & Portus, M. (2010). The development of fast bowling experts in Australian cricket. Talent Development & Excellence, 2, 137-148. Poppleton, W. L., & Salmoni, A. W. (1991). Talent identification in swimming. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 20, 85-95. Prescott, J. (1996). Talent identification and development in female gymnasts. Coaching Focus, 31, 12-13. Rgnier, G., Salmela, J. H., & Russell, S. L. (1993). Talent detection and development in sport. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphy, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 290-313). New York: MacMillan. Reilly, T., Williams, A. M., Nevill, A., & Franks, A. (2000). A multidisciplinary approach to talent detection in soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 695-702. Renger, R. (1993). Predicting athletic success: issues related to analysis and interpretation of study findings. The Sport Psychologist, 7, 262-21 A. Roetert, E. P., Brown, S. W., Piorkowski, P. A., & Woods, R. B. (1996). Fitness comparisons among three different levels of elite tennis players. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 10, 139-143. Roffey, K., & Gross, J. B. (1991). A behavioural observation checklist for basketball talent identification. The 18th ACHPER National/International Conference Proceedings (Vol. 2, pp. 368-373). Perth, Western Australia: Edith Cowan Universify Press.

TALENT DETECTION IN SPORT... /265 Salmela, J. H., & Rgnier, G. (1985). Justification for sport talent identification programs. In L. E. Unestahl (Ed.), Sport psychology in theory and practice - Proceedings ofthe 6th World Congress in Sport Psychology (pp. 90-98). Copenhagen, Denmark. Schutz, R. W. (1998). Assessing the stability of psychological traits and measures. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 393-408). Morgantown, WV: Eitness Information Technology. Schutz, R. W., & Gessaroli, M. E. (1993). Use, misuse, and disuse of psychometrics in sport psychology research. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (j)p. 901-917). New York: MacMillan. Singer, R. N., & Anshel, M. H. (2006). An overview of interventions in sport. In J. Dosil (Ed.), The sport psychologist's handbook (pp. 63-88). West Sussex, UK: Wiley. Singer, R. N., & Janelle, C. M. (1999). Determining sport expertise: From genes to suprmes. Intemational Journal of Sport Psychology, 30, 117-150. Spamer, E. J., & Coetzee, M. (2002). Variables which distinguish between talented and less talented participants in youth sport - A comparative study. Kinesiology, 2, 141-152. Spielberger, C D . , Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vaag, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. St-Aubin, M. A., & Sidney, K. (1996). A rationale for talent detection in youth sports. Canadian Association for Health, Physical Education, Recreation & Dance Journal, 62,9-12. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed). New York: HarperCollins. Thomas, J. R., Nelson, J. K., & Silverman, S. J. (2011). Research methods in physical activity (6th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Till, K., Cobley, S., O'Hara, J., Chapman, C , & Cooke, C. (2010). Anthropomtrie, physiological and selection characteristics In high performance UK junior rugby league players. Talent Development & Excellence, 2, 193-207. Tranckle, P., & Cushion, C. J. (2006). Rethinking giftedness and talent in sport. Quest, 58, 265-282. Vaeyens, R., Gullich, A., Warr, C. R., Philippaerts, R. (2009). Talent identification and promotion programmes of Olympic athletes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 13671380. Van den Auweele, Y., Cuyper, B. D., Van Mele, V., & Rzewnicki, R. (1993). Talent detection and development in sport. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphy, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 257-289). New York: Macmillan.

266/Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3

Van den Auweele, Y., Nys, K., Rzewnicki, R., & Van Mele, V. (2001). Personality and the athlete. In R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, & C. M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 239-268). New York: Wiley. Vealey, R. S. (1985). Sport personology: A paradigmatic and methodological analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 216-235. Vealey, R. S. (2002). Personality and sport behavior. In T. Hom (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (pp. 43-82). Champaign, DL: Human Kinetics. Williams, A. M., & Reilly, T. (2000). Talent identification and development in soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 657-667. Woodman, L. (1985). Talent identification - Is competition enough? Sports Coach, 9, 4957.

You might also like