You are on page 1of 10

Scand J Med Sci Sports 2008: 18: 664–673 Copyright & 2007 The Author

Printed in Singapore . All rights reserved Journal compilation & 2007 Blackwell Munksgaard
DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x

What makes coaches tick? The impact of coaches’ intrinsic and


extrinsic motives on their own satisfaction and that of their athletes
S. Jowett
School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK
Corresponding author: Sophia Jowett, PhD, School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough
LE11 3TU, UK. Tel: 144 1509 226331, Fax: 144 1509 226301, Email: S.Jowett@lboro.ac.uk
Accepted for publication 12 April 2007

This study aims to investigate the influence of two types of regression analyses indicated that while intrinsic motivation
motivational forces on coach and athlete satisfaction. The was moderately and positively related to all facets of coach
focus is on intrinsic and extrinsic motives that initiate coach- satisfaction, extrinsic motivation was only related to coach
related behavior. A questionnaire that measures both types satisfaction with the coach–athlete relationship. Athletes’
of motivation and three facets of satisfaction (i.e., satisfac- satisfaction with the coach–athlete relationship was only
tion with performance, with instruction, and with the coach– associated with the coach’s intrinsic motivation. Interaction
athlete relationship) was completed by 138 coaches. One effects among the two types of motivation were significant
athlete from each of the coaches who participated in the suggesting that extrinsic motivation can potentially under-
study was also asked to complete a questionnaire that mine intrinsic motivation when intrinsic motivation is low.
measures their satisfaction with performance, instruction, The findings are discussed based on assumptions put for-
and the coach–athlete relationship. Results from a series of ward by self-determination theory.

In the context of sport and exercise, motivation as a vironmental forces) on both coaches’ and athletes’
general psychological concept has been researched satisfaction.
extensively and systematically for over two decades. Sports coaching is slowly but steadily becoming a
Most of this research has applied theoretical frame- profession in many parts of the world. In the United
works such as achievement goal theory (e.g., Duda, Kingdom, the profession, professionalism, and pro-
2001), self-determination theory (e.g., Frederick & fessionalization of coaching have been widely dis-
Ryan, 1995), and the hierarchical model of intrinsic cussed in various policy documents (see, e.g.,
and extrinsic motivation (e.g., Vallerand, 1997) Department for Culture, Media and Sport, 2002).
to investigate motivational issues concerning In such documents, it is envisaged that coaching will
mainly young sport participants. Research that be elevated to a profession by 2012 and that it will
involves motivational issues concerning adult and/ gain a professional structure and career pathway. In
or professional coaches is limited. Nonetheless, the United States of America, coaching is already
coach motivation is an important psychological viewed as a profession in which employment is
characteristic because it can determine the form, expected to increase faster than the average of all
direction, intensity, and duration of coach behavior. professions through the year 2014 (see Occupational
It has been suggested that enthusiastic and satisfied Outlook Handbook, 2006–2007). Coaching is a pro-
coaches are these coaches who are highly motivated fession in Australia, Canada, and Greece requiring
(e.g., Martens, 1997). Thus, the study of coach formal training, exams, certificates, and licensing.
motivation is important because the generated Within a professionalized coaching context, intrinsic
knowledge could provide valuable information motives (e.g., personal enjoyment and achievement,
about what factors stimulate, maintain, and channel interest in the job, and feelings of challenge) and
coach behavior toward certain goals (e.g., desire to extrinsic motives (e.g., pay, promotions, benefits, job
impart information and achieve excellence). It could security, work conditions, policies, and supervision)
also supply practical information in terms of recruit- will often be available and form part of the opera-
ing, educating, and retaining coaches. This study tional coaching structure. Consequently, profes-
aims to investigate the influence of intrinsic sional coaches may be involved in coaching for its
motives (personal needs) and extrinsic motives (en- intrinsic and extrinsic rewards to support and protect

664
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Coaching motives
the work they do. The discussion that follows pre- factors are largely intrinsic in nature. Within Self-
sents research studies that have been conducted to Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985),
investigate specific motives that coaches hold for intrinsic motivation, defined as being motivated into
coaching. doing something that one finds innately interesting,
An international study in which coaching motives has been viewed as important because it can posi-
across four countries (Australia, Canada, United tively affect behavior, performance, and well-being.
Kingdom, and Japan) were explored, revealed that According to the theory, a coach who is intrinsically
the main motives of coaches practising in the United motivated will experience satisfaction, happiness,
Kingdom included, fun and enjoyment, a means to and a sense of achievement. On the other hand,
continue sport involvement, a sense of pride, achieve- extrinsic motivation, defined as being motivated
ment and success, as well as fitness and health into doing something because it leads to a specific
(Tamura et al., 1993). Moreover, a national study goal, has been characterized as damaging because it
conducted for the English Sports Council (1997) can control people’s behavior. Accordingly, a coach
reported that the most popular reasons for getting who is extrinsically motivated to coach will approach
involved in coaching included natural progression various coaching tasks with despise and resistance in
from competitive sport and general interest in sport; order to gain status, promotion, or positive evalua-
whereas the most popular reasons for maintaining tions from others.
involvement in coaching included helping others to Deci’s (1980) Cognitive Evaluation Theory (a sub-
improve, enjoyment from teaching, and making a theory contained within SDT) claims that perceptions
contribution to sport. that underline an individual’s internal control repre-
Within sport and exercise psychology, a handful of sent the basic feature that distinguishes intrinsically
empirical studies have investigated what drives coa- motivated behavior from extrinsically motivated be-
ches into coaching. For example, Stevens and Weiss havior (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000a, b;
(1991) examined female coaching motivation and Gagnè & Deci, 2005). For example, coaches who are
found that the major factors were the enjoyment intrinsically motivated perceive themselves to be
associated with working with athletes as well as the more in control of their actions relative to coaches
pleasure gained from coaching and teaching. In who are extrinsically motivated. Early experimental
another study, Weiss et al. (1991) interviewed 28 work has supported that extrinsic motivation, in the
female coaches following a coaching internship. form of extrinsic reinforcement or rewards, is not
Major themes emerged following an inductive con- necessarily a motivating factor, and it has been
tent analysis and revealed that female coaches ex- argued that tangible rewards in fact undermine
pressed both positive motives and negative coaching intrinsic motivation (Deci, 1971). A meta-analytic
motives (i.e., disincentives). Positive themes included review of experimental research has supported that
the satisfaction of working with young sport perfor- ‘‘tangible rewards tend to have a substantially nega-
mers, the development of coaching skills and asso- tive effect on intrinsic motivation’’ (Deci et al., 1999,
ciated fun, whereas negative themes included pp. 658–659). Overall, theoretical assumptions and
negative interactions with mentor coach, low percep- empirical findings, generated from empirical studies
tions of competence, negative relationships with in coach motivation (e.g., Stevens & Weiss, 1991;
athletes, and a lack of administrative support. Frederick & Morrison, 1999) as well as athlete
In a more recent study, Frederick and Morrison motivation (e.g., Whitehead & Corbin, 1991; Man-
(1999) found that collegiate coaches had five broad digo & Holt, 2000), highlight the importance of
coaching motives: intrinsic motivation, extrinsic intrinsic sources of motivation.
motivation, social motivation, educational growth Although intrinsic motives are seemingly impor-
motivation, and professional relations motivation. tant, extrinsic motives may not necessarily under-
They found that coaches who endorsed a high mine coach motivation but may have an important
extrinsic-low intrinsic motivational attitude were role to play particularly as the nature of sport
distant, tense, and private in their approach with coaching gains professional status. A study by Potrac
their athletes, while coaches who endorsed a low et al. (2002) investigated the coaching behaviors of a
extrinsic–high intrinsic motivational attitude were professional English soccer coach. Inductive analysis
more autonomous, yet more closely connected with of the interview data revealed that the coach’s
their athletes. It was suggested that coaches’ intrinsic primary motivation was to develop a winning team
motivation contained affiliation and that coaches’ (an external source of motivation) while improve-
extrinsic motivation contained domination. It was ment of individual players’ technique and decision-
finally concluded that coaches’ motives impact on making (internal sources of motivation) were sec-
athletes’ satisfaction and performance. ondary concerns. Moreover, Cushion and Jones
Overall, these studies suggest that there are a range (2006) showed that in the context of professional
of coach-related motivating factors and that these youth soccer, players who accepted their coaches’

665
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Jowett
values and beliefs perceived to feel a step closer to Thus, this study examined three variables of satis-
achieving performance success (e.g., professional faction in order to ascertain the degree to which
status as a soccer player). It is possible that the coaches’ and their athletes’ satisfaction is affected by
evolving nature of professionalized sports coaching coaches’ intrinsic and/or extrinsic motives for coach-
requires that both intrinsic motives (e.g., personal ing. In addition, athletes’ satisfaction is studied in an
pleasure and enjoyment) and extrinsic motives (e.g., effort to explore the notion that coaches’ motivation
success and pay) work in synergy to meet not only promotes satisfying sport participation in athletes
coaches but also their athletes’ needs for satisfaction (e.g., Frederick & Morrison, 1999; Janssen & Dale,
and performance accomplishments. 2002). Finally, this dyadic approach to the study
Cameron et al. (2001) recently conducted a meta- highlights the interdependence that exists between a
analysis involving empirical research carried out coach and his/her athlete (Jowett, 2007). Thus, the
within the psychology of extrinsic and intrinsic expectation is that the coach-athlete interdependence
motivation. The study aimed to clarify whether would transpire in the links between coaches’ moti-
extrinsic rewards have negative effects on such out- vation and athletes’ feelings of satisfaction. Based on
comes as enjoyment and performance. The findings the above discussion, the following hypotheses are
from their analysis indicated that extrinsic rewards formulated:
do not have pervasive negative effects on intrinsic
1. Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation
motivation. They further argued that rewards can be
will be positively related to coach and athlete
helpful to shape performance, to establish interest,
satisfaction-related variables.
and to maintain or enhance effort and persistence at
2. An interaction of intrinsic motivation and ex-
a task. Researchers in organizational psychology
trinsic motivation will produce an effect on
have supported the claim that extrinsic motivation
satisfaction-related variables.
does not have negative effects on working adults
(e.g., Bateman & Grant, 2006). Thus, research that Hypothesis 1 is consistent with prevailing assump-
examines the relative effectiveness of intrinsic moti- tions about the direct and positive association of
vation and extrinsic motivation of professional, adult intrinsic motives on positive outcomes such as satis-
coaches on their own feelings of satisfaction and that faction. This hypothesis further tests whether extrin-
of their athletes is warranted. sic rewards predict satisfaction that is similar to the
operation of intrinsic motivation. Hypothesis 2 is
based on whether intrinsic and extrinsic types of
The present study motivation interact in producing positive or negative
effects on satisfaction-related variables. Testing this
This study aims to explore two sets of coach motives, hypothesis would provide evidence of potential un-
namely intrinsic and extrinsic motives, and their dermining effects of extrinsic motivation.
impact on both coaches’ and athletes’ satisfaction.
Satisfaction has been viewed as an important correlate
of motivation (see e.g., Herzberg, 1959; Hackman & Method
Oldham, 1980; Baron, 1991; Gagnè & Deci, 2005). Participants
Within organizational psychology, one of the most
Participants represented a cross-section of 138 male (n 5 114)
influential and enduring theoretical frameworks and female (n 5 24) coaches who practiced coaching in sports
to link motivation and satisfaction is Frederick like athletics (n 5 16), badminton (n 5 15), cycling (n 5 17),
Herzberg’s (1959) two-factor theory of motivation equestrian (n 5 2), golf (n 5 19), gymnastics (n 5 4), rowing
and hygiene. Herzberg’s theory explains what job (n 5 10), sailing (n 5 1), squash (n 5 10), swimming (n 5 23),
factors motivate employees. He proposed that such tennis (n 5 4), trampoline (n 5 2), and triathlon (n 5 15). The
level to which coaching was delivered varied from university
factors as company policy, supervision, interpersonal (n 5 18), regional (n 5 19), club (n 5 56), national (n 5 28), and
relations, working conditions, and salary are hygiene international level (n 5 16). Coaches were asked to categorize
factors rather than motivators. According to the their age in one of four groups: five coaches belonged to the
theory, the absence of hygiene factors can create job 18- to 25-year-old group, 77 coaches belonged to the 26- to 39-
dissatisfaction, but their presence does not necessarily year-old group, 41 coaches belonged in the 40- to 55-year-old
group, and 15 coaches in the 561-year-old group. Coaches’
lead to satisfaction. Herzberg further proposed that experience in coaching varied across five categories: 19 coa-
five factors are strong determinants of job satisfaction: ches were relatively new to the profession (1–3 years), 18
achievement, recognition, the work itself, responsibil- coaches were coaching for a longer period (4–7 years), 56
ity, and advancement. These motivators (satisfiers) coaches recorded coaching experience that spanned from 8 to
were associated with long-term positive effects in job 15 years, 28 coaches were coaching for much longer (15–30
years), while 17 coaches had at least 31 years coaching
performance while the hygiene factors (dissatisfiers) experience. The participants reported their status as either
consistently produced only short-term changes in job full-time coaches (39% salaried coaches), part-time coaches
performance. (45% coached for a fee), or volunteer coaches (16% coached

666
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Coaching motives
without payment). The majority (n 5 134) of the coaches were also included three items (e.g., ‘‘I am satisfied with the training
British nationals and coached in England with the exception programme this season’’) and the internal consistency was 0.83
of eight coaches who coached in Scotland. for athletes and 0.73 for coaches. Two further items were also
Moreover, data were collected from 138 male (n 5 72) and developed to measure coaches’ and athletes’ perceptions of
female (n 5 66) athletes, each one was coached by the coaches their overall satisfaction with the coach–athlete relationship
recruited to participate in the study. The age of the athletes (i.e., ‘‘I am satisfied with the overall coach–athlete relation-
was categorized in four categories: 26 athletes were categor- ship’’ and ‘‘My athlete/coach is satisfied with our overall
ized in the o18-year-old group, 59 athletes were categorized in coach–athlete relationship’’). The internal consistency of these
the 19- to 23-year-old group, 26 athletes were categorized in items was 0.79 for athletes and 0.73 for coaches. A 7-point
the 24- to 30-year-old group, and 27 athletes were categorized response scale was used, where 1 5 ‘‘strongly disagree,’’
in the 431-year-old group. For both the athlete and the coach 4 5 ‘‘half-way,’’ and 7 5 ‘‘strongly agree,’’ (The satisfaction
samples, the gender distribution was as follows: female coa- items are presented in Appendix A.)
ches coached eight male athletes and 16 female athletes, while
male coaches coached 50 female athletes and 64 male athletes.
(This study is part of a larger research project.) Procedures
Two research assistants administered the questionnaires em-
ploying a variety of methods (telephone, email, and paper-
Instruments and-pencil). The participants’ informed consent was sought
once the objectives of the study were explained, ethical issues
Two subscales from the Coach Motivation Scale (CMS; of confidentiality were ensured, and the criteria for participa-
Frederick & Morrison, 1999) were utilized to examine coa- tion were listed (e.g., coaches’ and athletes’ age had to be at
ches’ intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. The CMS was devel-
least 18 and they had to be involved in the sport on a regular
oped based on the principles of Self-Determination Theory basis). The study received full ethical approval from the
(Deci & Ryan, 1985) and thus its emphasis on differentiating University Ethical Advisory Committee before data collection.
intrinsic and extrinsic coaching motives. The intrinsic motiva-
tion subscale includes six items and indicates to what extent
the sources of engagement are intrinsic such as fun and
enjoyment (e.g., ‘‘I coach because it’s fun’’; and ‘‘I coach Data analysis
because the work is interesting’’). The extrinsic motivation Means, Standard Deviations (SDs), response range, and
subscale includes five items and reflects motivational sources correlations of all study variables were calculated. A series
that are external such as high salary or good benefits (e.g., ‘‘I of regression analyses were then employed to investigate the
coach because the pay is good’’; and ‘‘I coach because the relationships between the dependent variables of coach and
‘‘perks’’ of the job are good’’). (The coach motivation items athlete satisfaction with the independent variables of intrinsic
are presented in Appendix A.) A 7-point response scale was and extrinsic motivation. Tests of Hypothesis 1 involved
used, where 1 5 ‘‘strongly disagree,’’ 4 5 ‘‘half-way,’’ and computing six separate multiple regression correlations. The
7 5 ‘‘strongly agree,’’ The internal consistency with the sample goal was to determine the direction and strength of the
of this study was acceptable with 0.85 for intrinsic motivation relationships between the independent (motivation types)
and 0.83 for extrinsic motivation. and dependent variables (satisfaction-related). Tests of Hy-
The scale was subjected to confirmatory factor analysis pothesis 2 involved computing moderated hierarchical regres-
(CFA) because evidence of its validity has not been extensive sion analyses in which the independent variables were centered
(see Frederick & Morrison, 1999). CFA was performed to (i.e., they were put in deviation score form so that their means
assess the two-dimensional factorial structure of intrinsic and are zero). These analyses aimed to determine whether intrinsic
extrinsic motives with this sample. Hu and Bentler (1998) and extrinsic motivation could predict interactively each one
recommended the usefulness of standardized root mean of the six satisfaction-related variables. The procedures fol-
square residual (SRMR) and the comparative fit index (CFI) lowed are in accordance with recommendations put forward
in model assessment. It has also been suggested that any model by Aiken and West (1991), Cohen et al. (2003), and Biddle et al.
reporting values above 0.90 for CFI (Bentler & Bonett, 1980) (2001).
and below 0.08 for SRMR (Hu & Bentler, 1998) should be
viewed as acceptable. The fit indexes for the two-dimensional
structure of intrinsic and extrinsic motives were 0.93 for CFI
and 0.07 for SRMR. These indexes suggest a good model fit
Results
with the data. Notwithstanding these results, two items (‘‘I Table 1 displays descriptive statistics, response range,
coach because I enjoy the thrill of the competitive situation’’
and ‘‘I coach because I like being in charge of others’’) reliability scores, and bivariate correlations among
recorded high measurement error, low standardized coeffi- all variables. The results of the regression analyses
cients, and low standardized r-squared indicating that these are displayed in Table 2 and include information
items may be less effective. Therefore, future research should about the standardized and unstandardized regres-
pay attention to how these items perform. sion coefficients for the independent variables con-
The Athlete Satisfaction Questionnaire (Riemer & Chella-
durai, 1998) was employed to measure both athlete and coach tained in Hypothesis 1. Table 3 includes information
satisfaction-related variables. For the purpose of this study related to Hypothesis 2 and presents the moderated
two satisfaction subscales were utilized to assess both athletes’ regressions concentrating on the interaction effects
and coaches’ perceptions of satisfaction: (a) satisfaction with between the independent variables.
performance, and (b) satisfaction with instruction. Satisfac- Hypothesis 1 concerned the nature of the relation-
tion with performance included three items (e.g., ‘‘I am
satisfied with my athlete’s skill improvement thus far’’). The ship between the six dependent variables of coach
subscales’ internal consistency with this sample was 0.80 for and athlete satisfaction (i.e., satisfaction with instruc-
athletes and 0.75 for coaches. Satisfaction with instruction tion, with performance, and with relationship) and

667
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Jowett
Table 1. Means, SD, range of responses, reliabilities (in brackets), and correlations for all variables

Mean SD Range 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Intrinsic motivation 6.10 0.81 2.33–7.00 (0.85)


Extrinsic motivation 3.44 1.50 1.00–6.60 0.26** (0.83)
Coach performance satisfaction 5.84 0.83 3.00–7.00 0.21* 0.05 (0.75)
Coach instruction satisfaction 3.87 1.35 2.67–7.00 0.23** 0.09 0.62** (0.73)
Coach relationship satisfaction 5.94 0.86 3.00–7.00 0.43** 0.25** 0.47** 0.36** (0.73)
Athlete performance satisfaction 5.82 0.93 2.33–7.00 0.02 0.04 0.31** 0.06 0.31** (0.80)
Athlete instruction satisfaction 3.62 1.26 1.00–7.00 0.17 0.02 0.11 0.07 0.25 0.36** (0.83)
Athlete relationship satisfaction 5.83 0.90 1.50–7.00 0.27** 0.15 0.17* 0.05 0.35** 0.31** 0.67** (0.79)

N 5 134 (listwise deletion of missing data).


*Po0.05; **Po0.01.

Table 2. Satisfaction-related variables regressed on intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation separately

Intrinsic motivation Extrinsic motivation

b (unstandardised coefficients) P b (unstandardised coefficients) P

Coach performance satisfaction 0.21 (0.25) 0.01 0.003 (0.002) 0.97


Coach instruction satisfaction 0.22 (0.22) 0.01 0.03 (0.02) 0.69
Coach relationship satisfaction 0.39 (0.41) 0.01 0.15 (0.09) 0.05
Athlete performance satisfaction 0.03 (0.04) 0.69 0.05 ( 0.03) 0.58
Athlete instruction satisfaction 0.12 (0.13) 0.18 0.008 ( 0.005) 0.93
Athlete relationship satisfaction 0.24 (0.27) 0.01 0.09 (0.05) 0.28

N 5 134 (listwise deletion of missing data).

Table 3. Satisfaction-related variables regressed on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

Variables R2 change F change b t

Coach performance satisfaction


Intrinsic 0.04 6.30* 0.21 2.41**
Extrinsic 0.05 3.13* 0.003 0.03
Intrinsic 0.06 2.91* 0.31 2.82**
Extrinsic 0.004 0.07
Intrinsic  extrinsic 0.10 1.55
Coach instruction satisfaction
Intrinsic 0.05 7.34** 0.22 2.51**
Extrinsic 0.02 3.73** 0.40 0.69
Intrinsic 0.07 3.53** 0.28 2.99**
Extrinsic 0.02 0.45
Intrinsic  extrinsic 0.10 1.74*
Coach relationship satisfaction
Intrinsic 0.19 31.0** 0.41 4.93**
Extrinsic 0.21 17.6** 0.09 1.91*
Intrinsic 0.23 13.0** 0.36 3.98**
Extrinsic 0.08 1.88*
Intrinsic  extrinsic 0.09 1.78*

N 5 136.
*Po.10; **Po.05 (two tailed).

the two motivation types of extrinsic and intrinsic with instruction (R2 5 0.05 and 0.05, Po0.05), and
motivation. The independent or predictor variables with performance (R2 5 0.04 and 0.04, Po0.05).
of coach intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motiva- Similarly, the independent variable of coach intrinsic
tion explained moderate amounts of variance in motivation and extrinsic motivation explained
coaches’ satisfaction with the coach–athlete relation- small amounts of variance in athlete satisfaction
ship (R2 5 0.19 and 0.21, Po0.01). Coach intrinsic with the coach–athlete relationship (R2 5 0.07 and
motivation and extrinsic motivation explained 0.08, Po0.01). The nature of the relationship be-
small amounts of variance in coach satisfaction tween the independent variables of coach intrinsic

668
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Coaching motives
motivation and extrinsic motivation and the depen- 0.5

Coach Relationship Satisfaction


0.4
dent variables of athlete satisfaction with instruction
0.3
(R2 5 0.01 and 0.01, P40.01), and with performance 0.2
(R2 5 0.001 and 0.003, P40.01) was trivial and not 0.1
significant. 0
− 0.1
Hypothesis 2 essentially tested the potential under-
− 0.2
mining (negative) effect of extrinsic motivation on − 0.3
the six dependent variables of satisfaction. An under- − 0.4
mining effect would occur if a significant interaction − 0.5
− 0.6
were noted. A significant interaction effect was noted
Extrinsic Motivation
because there was a significant increment in R2 when
the interaction term (intrinsic  extrinsic motivation) Fig. 2. Regression slopes for coach relationship satisfaction
was entered into the equation. Table 3 includes on extrinsic motivation under high ( & ) and low () condi-
satisfaction-related variables regressed on the inde- tions for intrinsic motivation.
pendent variables of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic
motivation, and their interaction. Specifically, two motivation is high, extrinsic motivation has no effect
interactions effects were significant using a two-tailed on coaches’ satisfaction with the relationship.
test and a significance level of 0.10. (These effects
would have been significant at 0.05 in a one-tailed
test.) The findings suggest that coach satisfaction- Discussion
related variables (satisfaction with instruction and
satisfaction with relationship) were predicted by the This study sampled a cross-section of adult and/or
interaction between the two types of motivation. professional coaches exposed to ongoing intrinsic
It has been explained that by plotting the regres- (coaching is fun, interesting, challenging) and/or
sion of the dependent variable (satisfaction-related) extrinsic (coaching pay, benefits, prestige) rewards
on one of the independent variables (extrinsic moti- and investigated the relationships between intrinsic
vation) the form of the interaction is determined (see and extrinsic motivation with coach and athlete
Aiken & West, 1991; Biddle et al., 2001). Corre- satisfaction. Two specific hypotheses were tested.
spondingly, high (1 SD above the mean) and low The first hypothesis aimed to test whether intrinsic
(1 SD below the mean) values were substituted for motivation and extrinsic motivation operate in simi-
the independent variables into the final regression lar ways. The findings from multiple regression
equation and plotted the slopes against the depen- analyses indicated that coaches’ both intrinsic and
dent variable. Figure 1 shows when intrinsic motiva- extrinsic motivation positively affected coaches’ sa-
tion is low, extrinsic motivation has a negative effect tisfaction with the coach–athlete relationship (i.e.,
on coaches’ satisfaction with instruction and when the degree to which the coach was satisfied with the
intrinsic motivation is high, extrinsic motivation has relationship developed with his/her athlete). Coa-
a positive effect on coaches satisfaction with instruc- ches’ intrinsic motivation, but not extrinsic motiva-
tion. Correspondingly, Fig. 2 shows that when in- tion, affected coaches’ satisfaction with performance
trinsic motivation is low, extrinsic motivation has a and instruction. Athletes’ satisfaction with perfor-
positive effect on coaches’ satisfaction with the re- mance and instruction appeared to be unaffected by
lationship with their athlete, and when intrinsic the type of coach motivation, with the exclusion of
satisfaction with the coach–athlete relationship.
While acknowledging that the variances explained
0.5 in these regressions were small, the findings partially
Coach instruction Satisfaction

0.4 support Hypothesis 1 and indicate that extrinsic


0.3 sources of motivation did not lead to neither coach
0.2 nor athlete satisfaction with performance and in-
0.1
struction. This finding is in line with Herzberg’s
0
−0.1
(1959) contention that the presence of some factors
−0.2 (essentially extrinsic sources of motivation) do not
−0.3 necessarily lead to satisfaction, yet their absence
−0.4 could lead to dissatisfaction. Future research inves-
−0.5 tigating the specific effects of the absence of extrinsic
Extrinsic Motivation sources of coach motivation on satisfaction is war-
Fig. 1. Regression slopes for coach instruction satisfaction ranted.
on extrinsic motivation under high ( & ) and low () condi- The finding that the extrinsic type of motivation
tions for intrinsic motivation. does not seem to have negative effects on coaches’

669
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Jowett
experiences of satisfaction may be explained using Porter & Lawler, 1968; Gagnè & Deci, 2005; Latham
the concept of internalization. Gagnè and Deci & Pinder, 2005). Within the coaching context, coa-
(2005; see also Ryan & Deci, 2000a) have explained ches are likely to receive both intrinsic rewards such
that extrinsically motivated behavior can vary in the as personal enjoyment, a sense of achievement, and
degree to which it is autonomous vs controlled. feelings of challenge, and extrinsic rewards such
Coaches’ autonomous extrinsic motivation refers to as pay, benefits, recognition, work conditions and
experiencing activities as interesting and sponta- policies. Consequently, monetary rewards as a moti-
neously satisfying. In contrast, coaches’ controlled vating factor in sports coaching, like in any other
extrinsic motivation is located at the other end of the work setting, should be considered as practically
spectrum and so it reflects feelings of external control important.
and pressure in behaving in certain ways. It is From an applied perspective, the findings suggest
suspected that coaches in this study had internalized that coaches’ enjoyment, happiness and satisfaction
(‘‘took in’’) extrinsic rewards as autonomous yet not with their coaching involvement is largely dependent
as fully internalized to positively and strongly relate on experiencing high levels of intrinsic motivation.
to satisfaction. Thus, by putting in place strategies that increase
Hypothesis 2 aimed to address the possible inter- coaches’ interest in coaching (e.g., time and freedom
play among intrinsic motivation and extrinsic moti- to make choices about when and where the training
vation on producing an effect on satisfaction-related will take place, what competitions to attend, how
variables. This hypothesis is important to test from a many athletes they manage, help coaches to develop
theoretical and practical perspective because it can effective relationships with athletes), coaching activ-
inform us of the potential undermining (or not) ities can be made more intrinsically rewarding. It is
effects of extrinsic motivation. The findings indicated also recommended that the coaching environment is
that intrinsic and extrinsic motivations interact and structured in such a way so that it offers both
when intrinsic motivation was low, extrinsic motiva- intrinsic and extrinsic sources of motivation – parti-
tion negatively affected coaches’ satisfaction with cularly because extrinsic rewards appear to enhance
instruction. Moreover, under conditions of high further intrinsic motivation.
intrinsic motivation, more extrinsically motivated The study has a number of theoretical and meth-
coaches experienced greater satisfaction with instruc- odological strengths. First, despite the empirical
tion. This finding suggests a synergistic effect of evidence that intrinsic motivation is important in
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation on coaches’ satis- both athletes and coaches, this is the first study to test
faction and supports Cameron et al.’s (2001) meta- that coach intrinsic and extrinsic motivation have
analytic findings that there is no statistical evidence additive and interactive effects on coach satisfaction.
for detrimental effects of extrinsic rewards on mea- This study is the first to have tested widely claimed
sures of intrinsic motivation. assumptions about the role of intrinsic and extrinsic
Moreover, the findings suggest an antagonistic motivation on behavior in the sport coaching setting.
interaction pattern between intrinsic and extrinsic Second, this study was a field study in which col-
motivation on coaches’ satisfaction with the coach– lected data came from coaches who coached on a
athlete relationship (see Table 3, the opposite sign of regular basis, at a good performance level, and
the interaction). Such a negative interaction (see received intrinsic and/or extrinsic rewards. Third,
Cohen et al., 2003) signifies that the importance of the study utilized a dyadic approach to investigate
coaches’ high intrinsic motivation was lessened by the impact of coaches’ motivation on their athletes’
high extrinsic motivation, and vice versa; a partial feelings of satisfaction. Finally, a number of direc-
pattern of influence of the independent variable of tions for future research and practical implications
motivation on coaches’ satisfaction with the coach– emerge from the findings of the study.
athlete relationship. Figure 2 further illustrates that The present study was focused on a single outcome
when intrinsic motivation was high, extrinsic motiva- variable (i.e., satisfaction); thus additional outcome
tion had no effect, while when intrinsic motivation variables such as performance, psychological well-
was low, extrinsic motivation positively influenced being, organizational trust, and commitment could
coaches’ satisfaction with the relationship they had be examined to provide a better picture of the work
established with the athlete. outcomes associated with coaches’ intrinsic and ex-
Overall, these findings highlight the importance of trinsic motivation. Moreover, future research should
intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation and also examine individual differences (e.g., personal
also points to the potential undermining effect of orientations, gender, coaching experience and quali-
extrinsic motivation for coaches who are less intrin- fications), and environmental factors associated with
sically motivated. Organizational psychology has sports coaching and/or factors that define the specific
long recognized the importance of intrinsic and context in which the coach operates (e.g., challenge,
extrinsic motivation in work (e.g., Herzberg, 1959; choice, feedback, performance level, sport type), as

670
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Coaching motives
well as the interpersonal climate (e.g., coaching der such environmental conditions coaches would be
mentor/manager, coach–athlete–parent network). fully equipped to experience feelings of fulfilment
Longitudinal and qualitative research co-examine while they strive to actualize their athletes’ talents.
how, why, and when the promotion of intrinsic
and/or extrinsic motivation leads to the most effec-
tive performance and satisfaction. Such research Perspectives
would generate knowledge of change and stability
of motivational factors. Moreover, the opportunity In sport and exercise settings, motivation as a psy-
to incorporate the measure of the three basic psy- chological concept has been researched extensively
chological needs of autonomy, competence and re- and systematically; however, most of this research
latedness (see Ryan & Deci, 2000a, b; Gagnè & Deci, involves motivational issues concerning young sport
2005), would help understand the manner to which participants. From a psychological perspective, the
extrinsic rewards become internalized and hence coach’s motivation is as important as the athlete’s
promote autonomous (extrinsic) motivation. Finally, motivation, because it offers valuable information
the development of an instrument that assesses coach about what factors stimulate, maintain, and channel
intrinsic motivation and the types of autonomous the behaviors of two distinct yet interrelated indivi-
and controlled extrinsic motivation would help the duals toward certain goals. The findings of this study
generation of knowledge pertaining to the motiva- reveal the role of coach motivation in their own
tional bases for effective coaching behavior. experience of satisfaction, as well as the satisfaction
Research within the field of coach motivation is experienced by their athletes. This study makes an
significant because it would help provide knowledge important contribution to the literature by highlight-
relative to what work environments promote intrin- ing important aspects of coach motivation that have
sic motivation, satisfaction, and full internalization both conceptual and practical value. Finally, this
of extrinsic motivation. In the context of sports study paves the way for more research in the under-
coaching (be it professional or not), coaches need studied area of coach motivation.
to operate in an environment that maximizes persis-
tence, effective performance, job satisfaction, positive Key words: coach motivation, intrinsic motivation,
work attitudes, and psychological commitment. Un- extrinsic motivation.

References
Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: 3rd ed. London: Lawrence Elbaum, Final report. (Sport and Recreation
testing and interpreting interactions. 2003. Division).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1991. Cushion C, Jones RL. Power, discourse, Duda JL. Achievement goal research in
Baron RA. Motivation in work settings: and symbolic violence in professional sport: pushing the boundaries and
reflections on the core of youth soccer: the case of Albion clarifying some misunderstandings. In:
organizational research. Motivat Emot football club. Sociol Sport J 2006: 23: Roberts GC, ed. Advances in
1991: 15: 1–8. 142–161. motivation in sport and exercise.
Bateman TS, Grant JM. 2006 Revisiting Deci EL. Effects of externally mediated Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics,
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. rewards on intrinsic motivation. J 2001: 129–182.
Unpublished manuscript. Personal Soc Psychol 1971: 18: 105– English Sports Council 1997. Survey of
Bentler PM, Bonett DG. Significance tests 115. English Coaches 1993.
and goodness of fit in the analysis of Deci EL. The psychology of self- Frederick CM, Morrison CS. Collegiate
covariance structures. Psychol Bull determination. Lexington, MA: D.C. coaches: an examination of motivation
1980: 88: 588–606. Health and Company, 1980. style and its relationship to decision
Biddle SJH, Markland D, Gilbourne D, Deci EL, Koestner R, Ryan RM. The making and personality. J Sport Behav
Chatzisarantis NLD, Sparkes AC. undermining effect is reality after all: 1999: 22: 221–233.
Research methods in sport and exercise extrinsic rewards, task interest, and Frederick CM, Ryan RM. Self-
psychology: quantitative and self-determination: Reply to determination in sport: a review using
qualitative issues. J Sports Sci 2001: 19: Eisenberger, Pierve, and Cameron cognitive evaluation theory. Int J Sport
777–809. (1999) and Lepper, Henderlong, and Psychol 1995: 26: 5–23.
Cameron J, Banko KM, Pierce WD. Gingras (1999). Psychol Bull 1999: 125: Gagnè M, Deci EL. Self-determination
Pervasive negative effects of rewards on 627–668. theory and work motivation. J Org
intrinsic motivation: the myth Deci EL, Ryan RM. Intrinsic motivation Behav 2005: 26: 331–362.
continues. The Behav Anal 2001: 24: 1– and self-determination in human Hackman JR, Oldham GR. Work
44. behaviour. New York: Plenum Press, redesign. Reading, MA: Addison-
Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. 1985. Wesley, 1980.
Applied multiple regression/correlation Department for Culture, Media and Herzberg F. The motivation to work.
analysis for the behavioural sciences, Sport 2002. The coaching task force: New York: Wiley, 1959.

671
16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Jowett
Hu L, Bentler PM. Fit indices in Martens R. Successful coaching, 2nd edn. Stevens C, Weiss MR. Motivation and
covariance structure modeling: Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, attrition of female coaches. Sport
sensitivity to underparameterized 1997. Psychologist 1991: 7: 244–261.
model misspecification. Psychol Occupational outlook handbook (2006– Tamura K, Davey C, Haslam L. A four
Methods 1998: 3: 424–453. 07). U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau country study of the training for sports
Janssen J, Dale G. The seven secrets of of Labor Statistics, Bulletin 2600. coaching in Australia, Canada, the
successful coaches: how to unlock and Porter LW, Lawler EE. III. Managerial United Kingdom and Japan. Osaka
unleash your team’s full potential. attitudes and performance. University of Health and Sports
Tuscon, AZ: Winning The Mental Homewood, IL: Irwin-Dorsey, 1968. Science, 1993.
Game, 2002. Potrac P, Jones R, Armour K. It’s all Vallerand RJ. Toward a hierarchical
Jowett S. Interdependence analysis and about getting respect: the coaching model of intrinsic and extrinsic
the 311 Cs in the coach–athlete behaviours of an expert English soccer motivation. In: Zanna MP, ed.
relationship. In: Jowett S, Lavallee D, coach. Sport Educ Soc 2002: 7: 183– Advances in experimental social
eds. Social psychology in sport. 202. psychology. New York: Academic
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, Riemer HA, Chelladurai P. Development Press, 1997: 29, 271–360.
2007: 15–27. of the athlete satisfaction questionnaire Weiss MR., Barber H, Sisley BL, Ebbeck V.
Latham GP, Pinder CC. Work (ASQ). J Sport Exerc Psychol 1998: 20: Developing competence and confidence
motivation theory and research at 127–156. in novice female coaches: perceptions of
the dawn of the twenty-first Ryan RM, Deci EL. Intrinsic and ability and affective experiences
century. Ann Rev Psychol 2005: 56: extrinsic motivation: classic definitions following a season-long coaching
485–516. and new directions. Contemp Educ internship. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1991:
Mandigo JL, Holt NL. Putting theory Psychol 2000a: 25: 54–67. 13: 336–363.
into practice: how cognitive Ryan RM, Deci EL. The ‘‘what’’ and Whitehead JR, Corbin CB. Youth fitness
evaluation theory can help us motivate ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits: human needs testing: the effective of percentile-based
children in physical activity and the self-determination of evaluative feedback on intrinsic
environments. J Phys Educ Recreat behaviour. Psychol Inquiry 2000b: 11: motivation. Res Quart Exerc Sport
Dance 2000: 71: 44–49. 227–268. 1991: 62: 225–231.

672
Appendix A

Table A1Questionnaire items used to assess coaching motivation and coaches’ and athletes’ satisfaction with performance, instruction and relationship

Coach items Athlete items

Coaching motivation items


Intrinsic motives
I like to coach sports
Because it is fun
Because the work is interesting
Because I find coaching to be exciting
Because I like the challenge of coaching
Because I enjoy the thrill of the competitive situation
Because it makes me happy
Extrinsic motives
Because the pay is good
Because the benefits associated with my coaching job are good
Because I like the prestige associated with coaching
Because I like being in charge of others
Because the ‘‘perks’’ of the job are good
Satisfaction items
Satisfaction with instruction
I am satisfied with the training program this season I am satisfied with the training program this season
I am satisfied with the instruction I have provided my athlete I am satisfied with the instruction I have received from my coach
I am satisfied with the way I have instructed the various tactics I am satisfied with the way my coach has instructed me in
and techniques relevant tactics and techniques
Satisfaction with performance
I am satisfied with the degree to which my athlete has reached his/her I am satisfied with the degree to which I have reached my
performance goals during the season performance goals during this season
I am satisfied with my athlete’s improvement over the previous season I am satisfied with the improvement in my performance over the
previous season
I am satisfied with my athlete’s skill improvement thus far I am satisfied with my skill improvement thus far
Satisfaction with media
I am satisfied with the media’s support of our sport I am satisfied with the media’s support of our sport
I am satisfied with the support from the club/sport association. I am satisfied with the support from the club/sport association.
I am satisfied with the supportiveness of the fans I am satisfied with the supportiveness of the fans
Relation satisfaction
I am satisfied with my overall coach-athlete relationship I am satisfied with my overall coach-athlete relationship
My athlete is satisfied with our overall coach-athlete relationship My coach is satisfied with our overall coach-athlete relationship

673
Coaching motives

16000838, 2008, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00705.x by Cochrane Croatia, Wiley Online Library on [12/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License

You might also like