You are on page 1of 13

Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Hippocampal lesioned rats are able to learn a spatial position using


non-spatial strategies
Bruno Pouzet a,1, Wei-Ning Zhang a, Joram Feldon a,*, J. Nicholas P. Rawlins b
a
Behavioral Neurobiology Laboratory, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology, Schorenstrasse 16, CH 8603 Schwerzenbach, Switzerland
b
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK

Received 10 August 2001; received in revised form 2 January 2002; accepted 2 January 2002

Abstract

In the last two decades, many experiments have demonstrated that the hippocampus plays a role in the learning and processing of
spatial and contextual information. Despite these demonstrations, some recent publications have indicated that the hippocampus is
not the only structure involved in spatial learning and that even after hippocampal lesions, rats can perform spatial tasks. However,
it is not well established whether animals with hippocampal dysfunction still have some spatial learning capacities or develop non-
spatial solutions; these may require lengthier acquisition training. We now report the effects of conventional, dorsal hippocampal
ablation on rats’ performance on the water maze. We tested rats using a short (4 days) versus a long (16 days) acquisition period. We
demonstrated that animals with dorsal hippocampal lesions have some residual capacity for learning the localization of a hidden
escape platform in a pool during both a reference memory task and a working memory task. The animals with dorsal hippocampal
lesions learned to escape at a fixed location, but only with extended training. It is suggested that these animals used non-spatial
strategies to compensate for a spatial memory impairment. The results are discussed with respect to the experimental procedure and
the strategy applied by the lesioned rats. # 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Hippocampus; Spatial learning; Morris water maze; Lesion

1. Introduction handling (even complex) non-spatial information


[1,8,14], although this spared capacity is subject to
Since the studies of Scoville and Milner in 1957 [44], it discussion [41,66]. Other structures anatomically related
has been demonstrated by several investigators in to the hippocampus, such as the dorsal striatum [21], the
humans that, under some task conditions, damage to subiculum [26], subcortical structures [51,64] or the
the hippocampal formation results in severe memory parietal [42] and entorhinal cortex [43] have also been
impairment, affecting non-spatial learning processes implicated in the spatial memory capacities of animals
[11], or spatial learning processes [22,45,46,57] (but see or humans [36,47]. Consequently, there have been many
[40,56]). In parallel, two decades of animal research have attempts to establish models of memory impairments in
aimed at creating experimental models of the memory hippocampectomized animals, but the results have been
dysfunctions observed in humans. The general finding variable, depending on both the experimental paradigms
that rats without a hippocampus are impaired on tasks used and the precise location of the lesion, i.e. in the
that require the utilization of spatial [4] and contextual hippocampus, or more generally in the hippocampal
information [17,20,35], stands in contrast to the spared formation, the definition of which is also variable [2]. In
performance that has been found in learning about and the 1980s, Morris [23,25] designed the water maze task
that has been used in hundreds of studies with rodents,
many of which led to the claim that the hippocampus,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 41-1-655-7448; fax: 41-1-655- perhaps disproportionately, is the substrate of learning
7203
E-mail address: feldon@toxi.biol.ethz.ch (J. Feldon).
and more precisely of spatial learning [29,31,55]. This
1
Present address: H. Lundbeck A/S, Ottilia vej 7/9, conclusion was based on the observation that in the
Psychopharmacology Psychosis, DK 2500 Valby, Denmark. water maze, normal rats quickly acquire a place naviga-
0166-4328/02/$ - see front matter # 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 1 6 6 - 4 3 2 8 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 7 - 4
280 B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

tion response when required to find and to learn about reference or working memory task. In the present
the location of a hidden escape platform in a swimming experiment, we used variants of water-maze procedures
pool [23], whereas rats with hippocampal dysfunction to extend Whishaw et al.’s analysis of the role of
induced by hippocampal lesions [25,54] or fimbria / allocentric and idiocentric strategies in spatial memory
fornix lesions [51] are severely impaired on this task. in rats with hippocampal lesions. As the nature of the
Current positions regarding hippocampal involvement spatial memory impairment appears to vary depending
in learning and memory differ primarily in proposing on the localization of the lesion in the hippocampal
either that the hippocampus is involved in the processing formation [13] or afferent and efferent pathways [6], we
of and memory for one type of information, specifically, chose to operate rats at the level of the dorsal
spatial information [29,31,32,65] or that the structure is hippocampus, which is recognized as a crucial area of
more involved in particular learning and/or memory the hippocampus for spatial memory impairment [3,28].
processes that are non-specific for information type, e.g. Our hypothesis was that hippocampal lesioned rats
declarative memory [7,10,48,49], working memory [5], would have difficulty in learning or recalling informa-
temporary memory [39], and configural learning [52]. tion about spatial locations and how to reach them; that
Despite attempts to integrate these theories [15], the this would result in a clear navigational deficit; but that
precise involvement of the hippocampus, and the stage the rats would be able to compensate for this deficit by
of memory processing at which the hippocampus plays a using an appropriate strategy, although they would
role, remain unclear. In addition, it has been suggested therefore require more time than normal rats to solve
that rats use two different strategies to handle spatial the task. We therefore compared controls with dorsal
information, i.e. an idiocentric strategy, based on the hippocampal lesioned rats that were given either a
subject’s body position in space, and an allocentric normal acquisition task (16 trials) or an extended
strategy, which is independent of the subject’s body training (64 trials), to determine their ultimate perfor-
position in space [19,58]. Recently, Whishaw and cow- mance capabilities. We carried out probe tests as
orkers demonstrated that rats with hippocampal lesions additional assessments of spatial learning; and we
have more difficulty in deriving and using an appro- subsequently retrained the animals on a spatial working
priate strategy to reach a defined location in space than memory task. This provided a further test of the
in learning about a spatial location [63]. Moreover, the lesioned rats’ use of allocentric and idiocentric strate-
same author using a non-water maze task suggested that gies.
the hippocampal formation of rats might be involved
not in spatial mapping itself, but instead in the move-
ments required for exploration and in the integration of 2. Materials and methods
these movements to permit mapping [59,61]. Thus,
determining the involvement and the precise role of 2.1. Subjects
the hippocampus in spatial memory processing appears
to be difficult and requires specific lesions to be Twenty-four male Wistar rats (Zur:WIST[HanIbm],
combined with careful behavioral analyses in precise Institute of Toxicology, Schwerzenbach, Switzerland),
paradigms. The learning of spatial cues during a test is approximately 2.5 months old and weighing approxi-
expected to occur much faster than the development of mately 300 g, were used in this experiment. They were
non-spatial strategies [60]. Thus, spatial learning should housed four to a cage under a reversed light /dark cycle
permit subjects attempting to solve a spatial memory (lights on: 19:00 /07:00) with free access to food and
test to perform more quickly than subjects using non- water. After surgery, rats were individually caged.
spatial strategies.
The aim of the present study was to extend our 2.2. Surgery
previous studies on the role of the hippocampal forma-
tion in spatial learning processes [37] and to clarify 2.2.1. Dorsal hippocampal electrolytic lesion
whether any spared abilities in hippocampal lesioned The rats were allocated to two groups: 14 dorsal
animals are due to the sparing of spatial memory hippocampal operated (DHE) and 10 sham operated
capacities, or are consequences of improved non-spatial (Sham). Rats were anaesthetized with 1 ml/kg body
strategies. Based on a detailed time study showing that weight of pentobarbital (50 mg/ml) and placed in a Kopf
lesioned animals performed much slower than normal stereotaxic frame. An incision was made in the scalp to
subjects in an identical task, we expected to be able to expose the skull. The vertical coordinates of bregma and
differentiate spatial and non-spatial strategies in solving lambda were measured in order to align them in the
a reference or working memory task in control versus same (level head) plane, and a square hole approxi-
lesioned subjects. This would enable us to demonstrate mately 3 /4 mm was drilled on each side of the skull
whether dorsal hippocampal lesions spared either spatial overlying the dorsal hippocampus. Bilateral electrolytic
or non-spatial strategies in rats permitting to solve a lesions were made using a 0.3 mm stainless steel
B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291 281

electrode used as cathode, insulated except for the tip the water surface. When in place, the white disk served
(0.5 mm). The anode was a clip fixed on the left subject’s as a distinct visual cue for the location of the otherwise
ear. A constant current DC source was used. Rats hidden platform. Four points, equally spaced along the
received three bilateral lesions using a 2 mA, 15 s current circumference of the pool, were arbitrarily assigned as:
at the following coordinates, in mm, posterior (P) to N, E, S, W. These points served as the starting positions
bregma, lateral (L) to bregma and ventral (V) to dura: P at which the rat was lowered gently into the water, with
2.5, L 1.4, V 3.8; P 3.6, L 1.8, V 3.4; P 4.7, L 2.8, V 3.2 its head facing the wall of the water maze. The area of
[33]. The electrodes were left in place for 30 s before the pool was also conceptually divided into four
being retracted from the brain. Sham operated rats quadrants (NE, SE, SW and NW) of equal size. A
underwent the same surgical procedure, but the electro- video camera, connected to an image analysis system
des were inserted 1.2 mm ventral to dura and no current (HVS Image, UK) which in turn was connected to a
was passed. Rats were allowed to recover for 2 weeks in microcomputer running the HVS maze software, was
their animal room. One animal did not survive the mounted above the center of the water maze. The swim
recovery period. path of the animal was tracked, digitized and stored for
subsequent behavioral analysis using the same software.
2.3. Histology Each trial was started and ended manually by the
experimenter, who operated a remote switch connected
2.3.1. Gallyas fiber and cresyl violet stains to the microcomputer.
After the completion of behavioral testing, all hippo-
campal lesioned and five sham operated rats were 2.4.2. Reference memory task
anaesthetized with an overdose of equithesin and The reference memory water maze task consisted of 5
exsanguinated transcardially with 50 ml of 0.9% NaCl, stages organized as follows: 4 (Group A) or 16 days
followed by a perfusion with 200 ml of 4% formalin. The (Group B) of acquisition with a hidden platform, 1 day
part of the brain at the level of the hippocampus was of probe test, 4 days of reversal, 1 additional day of
post fixed in 30% sucrose in phosphate buffered saline probe test and finally 3 days of visible platform.
for 48 h and cut in 40 mm coronal sections on a freezing
microtome. For the examination of the hippocampal 2.4.2.1. Acquisition. Rats were given four trials per day
lesions, every fourth section through the hippocampus with a submerged platform that the rats could climb
was mounted on gelatin coated slides and stained with onto, to escape from the water. On each of the four
cresyl violet. After staining, the sections were dehy- trials, a different starting point (varied among N, E, S
drated through an alcohol series, cleared with xylene, and W) was used and the location of the escape platform
and coverslipped with Eukitt (Kindler, Freiburg, Ger- was fixed (in the middle of quadrant NW, 38 cm from
many). Subsequently, the sections were studied with a the wall). The starting points were different for con-
Zeiss Axiophot light microscope to verify the location secutive trials. At the beginning of a trial, the rat was
and extent of the lesions (according to the atlas of placed into the water at the start location. A trial ended
Paxinos and Watson [33]). when the rat escaped onto the platform and the escape
latency for each trial was recorded. If a rat failed to
2.4. Apparatus and procedure escape within 90 s, it was either guided to the platform,
or placed onto it, by the experimenter. On such trials, an
2.4.1. Morris water maze escape latency of 90 s was recorded. The rat was allowed
The water maze was a circular tank 2 m in diameter to spend 15 s on the escape platform and then placed for
and 0.6 m in height, made of Fiberglas and painted 20 s in a holding cage before the next trial began,
black. It was positioned in the middle of a well-lit testing resulting in an inter-trial interval of 35 s. At the end of
room (3 /4.9 /2.9 m) enriched with distal visual the session, the rat was dried with a towel before being
stimuli. The bottom of the maze was raised 0.45 m returned to its home cage.
above the room floor. At the beginning of each day, the
tank was filled with a mixture of cold and hot tap water 2.4.2.2. First probe test. In this 1-day test, the escape
to a depth of 0.3 m, and the water temperature was platform was removed from the water maze. The animal
maintained at 219/2 8C. A stable circular platform, was allowed to swim for 60 s before being removed from
measuring 11 cm in diameter and painted black, was the maze.
submerged 2 cm below the surface of the water hidden
from the rat’s view. It had a rough surface, which 2.4.2.3. Reversal. Rats were given four trials per day
allowed the rat to climb onto it easily once its presence similar to those described for the acquisition, but the
was detected. A separate white circular disk, also escape platform was located in a novel position,
measuring 11 cm in diameter, could be mounted on opposite the location used for the acquisition trials (in
top of the hidden platform so that it was 10 cm above the middle of quadrant SE, 38 cm from the wall).
282 B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

2.4.2.4. Second probe test. Identical to the first probe swim speeds, but are not presented here, because, results
test. for path lengths were always consistent with those from
the latency analyses, and speed was not affected.
2.4.2.5. Visible platform. Rats were given four trials per
day similar to those described above for the acquisition,
but the escape platform was made ‘visible’ by mounting 2.6.1. Reference memory task visible platform
a visual cue on top of it. The starting point was constant Data were analyzed by 2 /3 /4 (Lesion /Day /
across trials, but the location of the escape platform Trial) ANOVAs of escape latency for the visible plat-
varied among three possible positions (in the middle of form session.
the quadrant NW, NE or SW, 38 cm from the wall). The
positions were different for consecutive trials, and a new 2.6.1.1. Acquisition and reversal. Data of the four first
starting point was fixed every day. days of the acquisition sessions were analyzed by 2/
2 /4 /4 (Lesion /Training /Day /Trial) ANOVAs
2.4.3. Working memory task of escape latency. Training stands for the length of
The working memory water maze task consisted of 9 acquisition training: 4 or 16 days. For Group B, the 16
days of acquisition. days of the acquisition sessions were analyzed by a 2/
16 /4 (Lesion /Day /Trial) ANOVAs. Asymptotic
2.4.3.1. Acquisition. Rats were given four trials per day performance at the end of this extended training was
similar to those described for the acquisition of the assessed using a 2 /3 /4 (Lesion /Day /Trial) ANO-
reference memory task, but a new platform location was VAs that considered only the last 3 days. Data from the
fixed every day and a different starting point (varied reversal sessions were analyzed by a 2 /2 /4/4
among N, E, S and W) was used for each trial. Nine (Lesion /Training/Day /Trial) ANOVAs of escape
platform positions were possible, either in the middle of latency for both Group A and B.
quadrant NW, NE, SW or SE, 38 cm from the wall, or
adjacent to the wall, midway between each pair of
starting points. The last platform location was located at 2.6.1.2. Probe tests. Data were analyzed by 2 /2/4/
the center of the pool. 6 (Lesion /Training /Quadrant /Block) ANOVAs of
the absolute time spent in each quadrant of the pool
2.5. Experimental design defined as target, adjacent-left, adjacent-right and op-
posite. Block is a repeated measurements factor of six
Twenty-three rats (thirteen lesioned, ten sham) were bins (10 s) that permitted the description of the rat’s
run in the reference memory task. Eleven rats (six travel over time within a trial. The target quadrant was
lesioned, five sham) previously run in the reference the one in which the platform had been placed during
memory task were run in the working memory task. For the acquisition or reversal sessions. Since the fourth
the reference memory task, the total batch of rats was quadrant data point is never independent of the other
subdivided on day 4 after a common acquisition training three, the P values were adjusted to reflect a reduction in
into two groups: Group A (Group A; six DHE / five the degrees of freedom for the effect of quadrant and for
Sham) received no further acquisition training (4 days in the interactions involving the quadrant effect. Finally,
total), and group B (Group B: seven DHE / five Sham) 2 /2 /6 (Lesion /Training/Block) ANOVAs com-
received a further 12 days of acquisition, making 16 days pared the absolute times spent in the target quadrant
in total. Rats were matched for their mean latencies to itself.
reach the escape platform during the four acquisition All results presented as a factor of day, refer to the
days prior to their allocation to Group A and B. Group mean latency of the four daily trials.
A but not Group B was subsequently tested in the
working memory task. 2.6.2. Working memory task
After the completion of behavioral testing, all of the
operated animals and five randomly chosen sham
operated rats were analyzed histologically. 2.6.2.1. Acquisition. Data of the acquisition sessions
were analyzed by a 2 /9 /4 (Lesion /Day /Trial)
2.6. Data analysis ANOVAs of escape latency. Two similar but separate
ANOVAs were performed using the days with the
The data were analyzed using an analysis of variance platform located at the edge of the wall (4 days), and
(ANOVA), calculated with the StatView and Super- the days with the platform located in the center of the
ANOVA software system (Abacus Concepts, Inc., Berke- quadrants or the center of the pool (5 days). As for the
ley, CA, 1992). We present escape latency data. Parallel reference memory task, all results presented as a factor
analyses were conducted to compare path lengths and of day, refer to the mean latency of the four daily trials.
B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291 283

3. Results minor damage was induced at the level of the fimbria.


The presence of neocortical damage was not taken as
ground for exclusion from the experimental lesion group
3.1. Histological [16]. No damage could be discerned throughout the
hippocampus of the sham operated rats. The final
As can be seen in Fig. 1, which shows both drawings number of rats used in the behavioral analysis was 12
and a photomicrograph of the brains of lesioned rats, in DHE and ten Sham. Six DHE and five Sham rats were
rats with electrolytic lesions, the dorsal hippocampus allocated to Group A, and six DHE and five Sham to
was always completely lesioned. In one lesioned rat, the Group B.
lesion damaged only the dentate gyrus and partially the
CA1 and CA3. This rat was rejected from the analysis.
3.2. Behavioral
Some damage at the level of the alveus and the external
capsula was observed in most of the animals, but only
3.2.1. Reference memory task

3.2.1.1. Acquisition. Fig. 2 presents the mean latency to


reach the hidden platform for the Sham and DHE rats
over 4 days of training after the matching of animals in
Group A and B. As can be seen, whereas the Sham rats
reduced their latency to find the platform as training
progressed, DHE rats showed no improvement through-
out. This was supported by the 2/2 /4 /4 ANOVA
(Lesion /Training/Day /Trial) which yielded a main
effect of Lesion (F1,18 /13.00, P B/0.005), and a
significant interaction of Lesion /Day (F3,54 /8.01,
P B/0.001). There was no significant main effect of
Training, and no interaction with Lesion (all F ’s B/1).
The overall mean latency collapsed over 4 days of
training, after matching the rats in Group A and B, was
30.819/3.94 and 27.449/6.87 s for the Sham, and
56.629/7.28 and 51.189/7.72 s for the DHE rats,
respectively.
Fig. 3 presents the mean latency to reach the hidden
platform of the Sham and DHE rats of Group B over 16
days of training. As can be seen, whereas the Sham rats
reached a stable latency to find the platform, DHE rats
showed a regular improvement as training progressed. A
2 /16/4 ANOVA (Lesion/Day /Trial) yielded a
main effect of Lesion (F 1,9 /31.02, P B/0.001) and an
interaction of Lesion/Day (F 15,135 /2.38, P B/

Fig. 1. (A) Photomicrograph of the extent of the dorsal hippocampal


lesion (DHE) and a sham control rat (Sham). Cresyl violet stain. (B)
Schematic reconstruction of the smallest (solid black areas) and the Fig. 2. Mean escape latency (in s) to reach a hidden platform during 4
largest (solid grey areas) extents of damage to the dorsal hippocampal days of acquisition training of the reference memory task for dorsal
region and the upper cortex. Coordinates of the horizontal sections are hippocampal lesioned rats (DHE) and sham operated rats (Sham),
indicated with reference to bregma according to the atlas of Paxinos allocated to Group A and B (after matching based on latency to reach
and Watson (1986). the hidden platform on day 4).
284 B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

Group B, who persevered in the target quadrant. This


was supported by the 2/2/4/6 ANOVA (Lesion/
Training /Quadrant/Block) of the absolute time
spent per quadrant, which yielded a main effect of
Quadrant (F 2,36 /18.29, P B/0.001), and an interaction
of Lesion /Quadrant (F 2,36 /8.04, P B/0.002),
Training /Quadrant (F 2,36 /4.02, P B/0.05), and fi-
nally an interaction of Lesion /Training/Quadrant/
Block (F 10,180 /2.72, P B/0.01). A 2 /2 /6 ANOVA
(Lesion /Training/Block) of the absolute time spent
in quadrant four (target) showed a main effect of Lesion
Fig. 3. Mean escape latency (in s) to reach a hidden platform during (F 1,18 /20.63, P B/0.001), of Training (F1,18 /9.56,
the 16 days of extended acquisition training of the reference memory P B/0.01), and of Block (F 5,90 /6.22, P B/0.001), an
task for dorsal hippocampal lesioned rats (DHE) and sham operated interaction of Lesion /Block (F 5,90 /2.33, P B/0.05),
rats (Sham) allocated to Group B.
and more interestingly, an interaction of Lesion /
Training /Block (F 5,90 /5.12, P B/0.001).
0.005). A main effect of Day (F 15,135 /8.26, P B/
0.001) showed an overall improvement between days.
For the last 3 days (days 14/16), a three way ANOVA 3.2.1.3. Reversal task. Fig. 5 presents the mean latency
(Lesion /Day /Trial) of the mean latency to reach the to reach the hidden platform, located in a different
hidden platform yielded a main effect of Lesion (F 1,9 / quadrant (SE) from the one used in acquisition (NW),
70.02, P B/0.001), but no significant interaction of for the Sham and DHE rats in Group A and B over 4
Lesion /Day (F B/1.0), which demonstrated a stable days of training. As had been observed in initial
latency for the DHE rats close to that of the Sham acquisition, the Sham rats reduced their latency to find
operated rats, although consistently higher, confirmed the platform as training progressed, but DHE rats
by the absence of significant main effect of Day (F B/ improved their latency only slightly over the 4 days.
1.0). This was supported by a 2 /2 /4 /4 ANOVA
(Lesion /Training/Day /Trial) which yielded a
main effect of Lesion (F 1,18 /85.15, P B/0.001), of
3.2.1.2. Probe test 1. Fig. 4 presents the absolute time
Day (F 3,54 /10.66, P B/0.001), and of Trial (F 3,54 /
spent in quadrant 4 of the pool, where the escape
6.00, P B/0.002), and an interaction of Lesion /Trial
platform was located during acquisition training, in bins
(F 3,54 /3.29, P B/0.03), and of Day /Trial (F 9,162 /
of 10 s for the Sham and DHE rats in Group A and B
2.38, P B/0.02). The overall mean latency collapsed over
over the 60 s of the session.
4 days of reversal training was 20.729/2.79 s for the
As can be seen in Fig. 4, the Sham operated rats
Sham and 61.379/6.26 s for the DHE rats in Group A,
quickly chose quadrant NW (target), especially subjects
and 16.639/2.21 s for the Sham and 54.179/3.34 s for the
in Group B, reflecting their extended acquisition train-
DHE rats in Group B.
ing. It can also be seen that the Sham animals from
Group B gave up after 20 s had elapsed. The DHE rats
from Group A never showed any preference for visiting 3.2.1.4. Probe test 2. Fig. 6 presents the absolute time
the target quadrant, in contrast to the DHE rats from spent in quadrant 2 of the pool, where the escape
platform was located during reversal training, in bins of

Fig. 4. Results of the first probe test (after acquisition) showing the Fig. 5. Mean escape latency (in s) to reach a hidden platform during
time spent in quadrant NW, where the escape platform was located the 4 days of reversal training of the reference memory task for dorsal
during acquisition training, i.e. the target quadrant, over six successive hippocampal lesioned rats (DHE) and sham operated rats (Sham)
10-s bins. allocated to Group A and B.
B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291 285

P B/0.02), and an interaction of Lesion /Trial


(F 3,27 /5.98, P B/0.003). The overall mean latency,
collapsed over 9 days of training, was 25.619/0.78 s for
the Sham and 43.279/2.65 s for the DHE rats.
Looking at the pattern of results of the overall
ANOVA day by day, it appeared that the lesioned rats
behaved differently depending on the day and, conse-
quently, on the platform location. Therefore, we ana-
lyzed separately the days on which the platform was
located either at the edge of the wall of the pool (days 2,
4, 6, 8) or in the center of the quadrants and pool (days
Fig. 6. Results of the second probe test (after reversal) showing the
1, 3, 5, 7, 9). With respect to the trials for which the
time spent in quadrant SE, where the escape platform was located platform was located at the edge of the wall, the between
during reversal training, i.e. the target quadrant, over six successive 10- days (Fig. 7A) and between trials (Fig. 7B) analyses
s bins. showed no differences between the lesioned group and
the Sham animals in their mean latency to reach the
10 s for the Sham and DHE rats in Group A and B over hidden platform. This was supported by the 2/4/4
the 60 s of the session. From the overall and block ANOVA (Lesion/Day /Trial), which yielded only a
analysis, it appears that the DHE group performed less
main effect of Day (F 3,27 /3.92, P B/0.02) and of Trial
accurately than Sham animals, but compared with the
(F 3,27 /17.69, P B/0.001), but no main effect of Lesion
first probe test all groups expressed a low preference for
(F /1) and no interactions (all F ’s B/2). The overall
the target quadrant. The 2/2/4 /6 ANOVA
mean latency, collapsed over 4 days of training, was
(Lesion /Training /Quadrant /Block) of the absolute
time spent per quadrant yielded a main effect of Block 22.869/2.76 s for the Sham and 27.959/3.97 s for the
(F 5,90 /38.87, P B/0.001), and a significant interaction DHE rats.
of Lesion /Quadrant (F 2,36 /4.86, P B/0.05). A sub- In contrast, when considering only the trials for which
sequent 2 /2 /6 ANOVA (Lesion /Training /Block) the platform was located in the central area of the pool,
of the absolute time spent in quadrant 2 (target) showed the between days (Fig. 8A) and between trials (Fig. 8B)
a main effect of Lesion (F 1,18 /9.89, P B/0.006), analyses of the mean latency to reach the hidden
reflecting the poor performance of the DHE rats. A platform demonstrated clear lesion effects. The 2/
further 2 /2 /6 ANOVA (Lesion /Training /Block) 5 /4 ANOVA (Lesion /Day /Trial) yielded a main
of the absolute time spent in quadrant 4 (opposite effect of Lesion (F 1,9 /24.18, P B/0.001), of Day
quadrant, target of the acquisition sessions) showed a (F 4,36 /5.16, P B/0.003), and of Trial (F 3,27 /17.63,
main effect of Training (F 1,18 /4.91, P B/0.04), result- P B/0.001) and an interaction of Lesion /Trial
ing from the rats of Group B returning preferentially to (F 3,27 /4.63, P B/0.01). The overall mean latency,
this quadrant. collapsed over 5 days of training, was 27.819/1.97 s
for the Sham and 55.539/4.84 s for the DHE rats.
3.2.1.5. Visible platform. All groups showed a consistent
reduction in escape latency across the 3 days and the
four trials within days. A 2 /3 /4 (Lesion /Day /
Trial) ANOVA on the escape latency measure yielded
no significant main effect of Lesion or interaction (all
P ’s /0.05). There was a significant effect of Day
(F 2,40 /11.81, P B/0.001) and Trial (F 3,60 /14.56,
P B/0.001).

3.2.2. Working memory task


Over the trials and the 9 days, the Sham rats reduced
their latency to find the platform as training progressed,
but DHE rats did not show an equivalent overall
improvement. This was supported by the 2 /9 /4
ANOVA (Lesion /Day /Trial), which yielded a main
Fig. 7. Mean escape latency (in s) to reach a hidden platform shown
effect of Lesion (F 1,9 /34.45, P B/0.001), of Day for days (A) and trials (B) over the 4 days of the working memory task
(F 8,72 /7.54, P B/0.001), of Trial (F 3,27 /35.50, P B/ during which the escape platform was located at the edge of the
0.001), an interaction of Lesion /Day (F 8,72 /2.65, swimming pool (days 2, 4, 6, and 8).
286 B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

Fig. 8. Mean escape latency (in s) to reach a hidden platform shown


for days (A) and trials (B) over the 5 days of the working memory task
during which the escape platform was located in the center of the
virtual quadrant of the swimming pool (days 1, 3, 5, and 7) or in the
center of the pool (day 9).

Fig. 10. Representative search patterns of dorsal hippocampal le-


sioned rats (DHE) and sham operated rats (Sham) during days 7 and 8
of the acquisition training in the working memory task. The four
starting points are indicated by ‘N’, ‘E’, ‘S’ and ‘W’, respectively. The
location of the escape platform used during acquisition training is
represented by a solid circle. The top two rows show swim paths of rats
with the escape platform located in the center of one of the quadrants
of the pool. The bottom two rows show swim paths with the escape
platform at the edge of the wall.

location (see Fig. 10). When the platform was centrally


located in the pool, the lesioned animals’ large looping
swim courses prevented them from reaching the plat-
form efficiently, compared with the control animals who
improved very rapidly and swam almost straight toward
the platform after the second trial. When the platform
was located close to the edge of the pool, lesioned
animals positively benefited from their looping swim
courses, and were as accurate as the control animals in
Fig. 9. Representative search patterns of dorsal hippocampal lesioned finding the platform during and after the second trial.
rats (DHE) and sham operated rats (Sham) from group A (Group A)
and group B (Group B) during the last day of the acquisition training
(day 4 for Group A and day 16 for Group B) and the probe test day in
the reference memory task. The four starting points are indicated by
‘N’, ‘E’, ‘S’ and ‘W’, respectively. The location of the escape platform 4. Discussion
(removed for the probe test) used during acquisition training is
represented by a solid circle. The end of an unsuccessful swim path
Our main findings in these experiments are that the
is marked by an arrowhead.
dorsal hippocampal lesioned rats showed clear ability to
solve the water maze task, as demonstrated by their
3.2.3. Spatial strategy
gradual decreases in latency to find and climb onto the
As can be seen in Fig. 9, an extensive training of
hidden platform, even if they never reached the score of
lesioned animals (Group B), permitted them to locate
control animals. With extended training, lesioned ani-
the escape platform as effectively as control animals
mals reached an asymptotic level of performance, but
who received a short training (Group A). However, with a characteristic swim path different from control
unlike controls, the lesioned animals never swam swim paths, and were probably incapable of further
straight towards the platform, but instead performed improvement to control level of efficiency. We suggest
at least one large loop before targeting the platform. that this swim path indicates the reliance of the lesioned
During the working memory test, the lesioned animals rats on idiocentric navigation strategies. As can be seen
benefited when the platform was located close to the in Fig. 9, the search pattern of lesioned animals shows
wall of the pool rather than being in a more central that following extended training (Group B), they were
B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291 287

able to locate the escape platform in a large pool pal damages. On the other hand, we can not completely
similarly to control animals who received a short reject the possibility that the angular bundle was
training (Group A); moreover, lesioned animals with damaged. Could this have been responsible for the
extended training persevered on this place response task behavioral deficits in the lesioned animals? Damage at
during the first probe test (DHE-Group B) compared the level of the angular bundle would mean disruption
with controls (Sham-Group B) who covered a larger of the perforant path from the entorhinal cortex to the
area, presumably because they gave up. In the reversal hippocampus. However, we demonstrated in a previous
task, all the hippocampal lesioned rats were as severely study that cytotoxic lesions of the entorhinal cortex in
impaired as at the beginning of the acquisition regard- rats did not modify the ability of these animals to solve a
less of prior acquisition conditions. The working mem- spatial memory task, using the Morris water maze
ory task demonstrated that the tendency toward paradigm with an experimental design similar to the
impairment resulting from hippocampal dysfunction one use in this study [37]. Consequently, we do not
was still clear at the end of this long study, but is very believe that lesion of the angular bundle, if any, could
sensitive to the procedure and, more specifically, to the explain the memory dysfunction observed in this study
location of the platform in the pool. Like the impair- with dorso-hippocampal lesioned animals.
ment during the acquisition of the reference memory As has been shown by many groups, the hippocampal
task, this impairment reflected the different strategy lesioned rats were clearly impaired in comparison with
applied by the lesioned group of rats, which allowed controls [25,53] when they received only a short
them to benefit when the platform was located close to acquisition period (Group A), and to repeat Morris’s
the wall of the pool, but disadvantaged them if the comment, we did not need the histology study ‘to know
platform was located in the central area of the pool (see which of the rats have good lesions’ [24]. Our results are
Fig. 10). The longer latency to find the platform cannot completely consistent with the suggestion that the dorsal
be explained by a lower swimming speed, because, in no part of the hippocampus is crucial for spatial learning in
session was there a significant difference in the speed of rats. Not only was there a major difference in the latency
the two groups of rats. to reach the escape platform over the first 4 days
Histological analysis showed that the rats sustained between the sham operated and lesioned rats, but the
dorsal electrolytic lesions that covered at least 30% of former group quickly reduced its latency while the latter
the hippocampus, which according to Moser et al. [28] is did not improve at all. On the other hand, consistent
crucial to obtain a measurable impairment of the latency with other observations [26,60,62], in Group B, which
of animals to reach a hidden platform in a swimming received 16 days (64 trials) of acquisition, it appeared
pool. The electrolytic lesion completely destroyed the that the lesioned rats were able to improve their latency
dorsal Ammon’s horn (the dentate gyrus, and the to reach the escape platform up to an asymptotic level
hippocampal pyramid cell layers of both regio inferior after 14 days of training. Thus, dorsal hippocampal
and regio superior). Our lesions were therefore sufficient lesioned rats retained the capacity to progress on this
to produce the expected effect on water maze task task, but only in a situation comprising repetitions of
performance, while producing only very limited damage the learning event, as already claimed for both spatial
to the retrohippocampal connection with the striatum and olfactory paradigms [7,60]. The lesioned animals
[2], which has been shown to be correlated with spatial never reached the performance level of controls; this
information processing impairments [19,21], as the demonstrates that there was nonetheless a lasting spatial
fimbria was essentially undamaged. The lesions pro- impairment despite the extended training. Moreover, an
duced little subcortical extrahippocampal damage at the analysis restricted to the last 3 days of the acquisition
level of the track of the electrode, and the cortical area period showed a consistent lesion effect (P B/0.001) but
surrounding this track. The learning deficiency observed without any effect of days (all F ’s B/1), which demon-
in this study cannot be explained by reference to these strates that both groups of animals (sham operated and
minor areas of damage. Other previous studies have dorsal hippocampal lesioned) did not progress any
demonstrated that similar or larger lesions at this level further after 14 days of acquisition and that thereafter
of the cortex had no effect in a variety of spatial memory their behavioral difference remained stable. These
tasks [16,25,27]. Some of these studies used a paradigm results also provide evidence that over days, lesioned
identical to ours, but their lesions were produced with animals used a different strategy than the sham operated
more devastating techniques like aspiration, and even so rats to locate the escape platform. The swim path
the authors could not demonstrate impairment of recorded described a trajectory based on the well-
cortical lesioned animals [25]. Thus, there is no reason described large loops typical of hippocampal lesions
to consider that the learning deficiency observed in our [12,62]. These loops cost the rats much more time in
lesioned animals in comparison to sham animals in progressing over trials and within trials. These observa-
which electrodes were inserted in the cortex, is a tions are consistent with the suggestion that rats without
consequence of cortical damage rather than hippocam- hippocampus lack the ability to use a spatial strategy in
288 B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

learning the task and therefore, shift to a non-spatial as quickly as normal rats because, their hippocampal
strategy normally not used by controls. dysfunction obliged them to counterbalance a severe
The first probe test session confirmed that lesioned spatial cue learning or recall deficit by performing the
rats who received an extended training were able to task using a more restrictive strategy. The spatial
learn about a platform location. Following 64 acquisi- mapping theory proposed by O’Keefe and Nadel
tion trials (16 days), lesioned rats searched the correct describes the hippocampus as a cognitive map */a
quadrant of the pool when the platform was removed, memory system that computes and stores information
further confirming that they had learned at least some about allocentric space [31], but Nadel has argued that
form of a place response. We also observed that they ‘there should be many so-called spatial abilities that
progressed differently from sham operated rats. Sham would be unaffected by lesions in the hippocampal
operated rats went immediately to the target quadrant formation, and a rather select class of such abilities that
of the swimming pool but gave up looking for the are affected’ [29]. Our own results suggest that the rats’
platform after 20 s had elapsed. In contrast to the sham remaining abilities to locate the escape platform are
rats, dorsal hippocampal lesioned rats required more likely to have depended on non-spatial strategies, which
time to choose the target quadrant but continuously were sufficiently robust to permit the lesioned rats to
persevered on their task, and compared with the sham learn about the platform location under our training
rats, almost reached the same level of time per 10-s bins and testing conditions. It could be suggested that
spent in the target quadrant. As depicted in Fig. 9, lesioned rats used the pool wall as proximal cues to
dorsal hippocampal lesioned rats swam some large loops learn the platform location and to solve the spatial
in the pool and then quickly reduced the diameter of the memory task. Such a suggestion might account for the
loop to be closer to the escape platform, with the result obtained during the reference memory task with
platform always at the center of the loops. In the same lesioned animals following the edge of the wall. How-
task, sham operated rats went straight to the escape ever, this possibility could hardly explain the use of
platform and were consequently much faster. concentric loops to solve the working memory task.
In some of the subsequent stages of the experiment, On the second probe test, the behavior of the lesioned
dorsal hippocampal damage was still associated with rats in Group A was identical to that in the first probe
severe water maze impairments (thereby demonstrating test and they showed no preference for visiting any one
the lasting efficacy of the lesion effect). There were of the four quadrants of the pool. The sham operated
deficits in reversal learning, in the probe test that rats, in contrast, returned to the quadrant in which the
followed, and in working memory when the target escape platform was located during the reversal task
platform was near the center of the pool. Intermingled (target quadrant). In Group B, it seems that as a result
with these demonstrations of impairment there were of the extended training during the first acquisition
stages where performance was good: these were in the session inducing an inability to inhibit previous learning,
probe trial at the end of extended acquisition; during sham operated rats were impaired in learning the new
visually guided searching [25,38] (showing that motiva- task. Sham operated rats were able to improve their
tion and motor skills were essentially intact); and in the latency over the 4 days of the reversal session, but this
working memory task when the platform was located result could not be confirmed on the following probe
near the side walls. This overall pattern of results is test [18].
readily accounted for by the development of the Our results in the working memory task complement
characteristic swim paths that hippocampal lesioned those obtained by Whishaw [59]. He reported that, in a
rats demonstrated after extended training. This swim task in which the animals had to reach food cups located
pattern emerged both in Group B by the end of their at equal distances around the perimeter of a large table,
prolonged acquisition training, and in Group A in the comparable to the escape platform that the rats had to
working memory task, which made up the last stage of find in our experiment, rats with fimbria /fornix lesions
the experiment. The paths followed a looping trajectory, performed better than the control rats in terms of time
that would make it intrinsically more likely that the rats scores. If a task with the food cups located more
would contact platforms located near the side walls centrally to the table had been designed, one might
(accounting for the working memory task results), but expect results identical to ours; i.e. the lesioned rats
was also modifiable in the light of the animals’ would perform worse than the controls. On the con-
experiences of finding the platform. This provides an trary, in our study when the analysis was performed
account of the performance of the Group B rats in the considering only the days with the platform located in
probe trial at the end of their extended acquisition, in the center of the pool or the center of the quadrant,
which they swam in large loops early in the test, and there was a significant effect of trial, and a significant
then quickly reduced the diameter of the loops. We thus interaction of lesion with trial, suggesting that the dorsal
confirmed that rats with dorsal hippocampal lesions hippocampal lesioned rats were only able to progress
were perfectly able to localize a spatial location, but not when they had a chance of finding the platform; in other
B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291 289

words, they needed at least some successes to be able to quence of a changed motor response as such but an
progress through this working memory task. It is worth inability to use cues distributed around the pool.
noting that other groups using different spatial working
memory tasks [30] have shown that rats with hippo-
campal dysfunction also appeared to be able to improve Acknowledgements
their performance in a working memory test [13],
depending on the procedure applied. This work was supported by the Swiss Federal
In summary, we consider that hippocampal lesioned Institute of Technology Zurich and the Swiss National
rats are still able to discriminate their location in space, Science Foundation (Grant number: 3142009.94/2). The
and adjust their behavior accordingly, at least under authors thank the animal facility team for their care of
some conditions. However, there is no doubt that they the animals, and E. Weber for her histological prepara-
nonetheless have considerable difficulty in using envir- tions.
onmental cues or in integrating their spatial experiences
into efficient behavioral response repertoires. Our
results are consistent with the proposal that the rats References
with hippocampal lesions lost their capacity to use
[1] Aggleton JP, Hunt PR, Rawlins JNP. The effects of hippocampal
spatial cues efficiently, but in part compensated for lesions upon spatial and non-spatial tests of working memory.
this deficit by using idiocentric strategies. Our conclu- Behav Brain Res 1986;19:133 /46.
sions are also consistent with an interesting study by [2] Amaral DG, Witter MP. Hippocampal formation. In: Paxinos G,
Pearce et al. [34], who demonstrated that excitotoxic editor. The Rat Nervous System. San Diego: Academic Press,
1995:443 /93.
hippocampal lesioned rats were still able to appreciate
[3] Bannerman DM, Yee BK, Good MA, Heupel MJ, Iversen SD,
the direction in which a hidden platform could be found Rawlins JNP. Double dissociation of function within the hippo-
with respect to a landmark located in the swimming campus: a comparison of dorsal, ventral, and complete hippo-
pool. These authors specified that it is difficult to campal cytotoxic lesions. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:1170 /88.
describe the source of this directional information, but [4] Barnes CA. Spatial learning and memory processes: the search for
their neurobiological mechanisms in the rat. TINS 1988;11:163 /9.
they suggested that it could be due to an internal sense [5] Buzsáki G, Gage FH. Absence of long-term potentiation in the
of direction. This is a slightly different account from subcortically deafferented dentate gyrus. Brain Res 1989;484:94 /
that provided Whishaw [60], who reported that rats with 101.
ibotenic hippocampal lesions or electrolytic fimbria / [6] Cassel JC, Cassel S, Galani R, Kelche C, Will B, Jarrard L.
Fornix /Fimbria versus selective hippocampal lesions in rats:
fornix lesions could perform a probe test similarly to
effects on locomotor activity and spatial learning and memory.
normal rats after extensive training on a place task, Neurobiol Learn Mem 1998;69:22 /45.
using a stronger search behavior. We demonstrated that [7] Cohen NJ, Eichenbaum H. The theory that would not die: a
rats with dorsal hippocampal lesions performed a probe critical look at the spatial mapping theory of hippocampal
test similarly to sham operated rats only if the behavior function. Hippocampus 1991;1:265 /8.
[8] Davidson TL, Jarrard LE. Retention of concurrent conditional
of the rats is analyzed over the entire probe test session. discriminations in rats with ibotenate lesions of hippocampus.
The more detailed analysis of the first probe test session Psychobiology 1989;17:49 /60.
over time blocks highlighted the fact that dorsal [9] DiMattia BD, Kesner RP. Spatial cognitive maps: differential role
hippocampal lesioned rats had developed a different of parietal cortex and hippocampal formation. Behav Neurosci
1988;102:471 /80.
strategy [9] which was certainly idiocentric and which
[10] Eichenbaum H, Otto T, Cohen NJ. The hippocampus */what
cost them more time to solve the task than control rats does it do. Behav Neural Biol 1992;57:2 /36.
which are presumably able to use both allocentric and [11] Graf P, Squire LR, Mandler G. Amnesic patients perform
idiocentric strategies. A final question remains: are the normally on one kind of memory test for previously presented
distinctive response patterns we and others have ob- word. J Expt Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1984;10:164 /78.
[12] Hodges H. Maze procedures: the radial-arm and water maze
served the direct consequence of the lesion and the cause compared. Cogn Brain Res 1996;3:167 /81.
of the behavioral deficits that are reliably reported, or [13] Jarrard LE. On the neural bases of the spatial mapping system:
does the causal chain go in the opposite direction, such hippocampus versus hippocampal formation. Hippocampus
that the spatial deficits reliably reported are the direct 1991;1:236 /9.
[14] Jarrard LE. What does the hippocampus really do. Behav Brain
consequence of the lesions, and the residual abilities that
Res 1995;71:1 /10.
we see are the animals’ attempts to use other, less [15] Kesner R. The role of the hippocampus within an attribute model
appropriate, brain systems to compensate for the spatial of memory. Hippocampus 1991;1:279 /82.
impairments? The demonstration that hippocampal rats [16] Kesner RP, Novak JM. Serial position curve in rats: role of the
in the water maze behave like normal animals when a dorsal hipocampus. Science 1982;218:173 /5.
[17] Kim JJ, Fanselow MS. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of
curtain is fixed around the pool to hide extra-maze cues fear. Science 1992;256:675 /7.
[12,50] clearly implies that the strategy deployed by [18] Kimble DP. Possible inhibitory functions of the hippocampus.
animals with hippocampal dysfunction is not a conse- Neuropsychologia 1969;7:235 /44.
290 B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291

[19] Long JM, Kesner RP. The effects of dorsal versus ventral [40] Rosenbaum RS, Priselac S, Köhler S, Black SE, Gan F, Nadel L,
hippocampal, total hippocampal, parietal cortex lesions on Moscovitch M. Remote spatial memory in an amnesic person
memory for allocentric distance in rats. Behav Neurosci with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions. Nat Neurosci
1996;110:922 /32. 2000;3:1044 /8.
[20] Maren S, Fanselow MS. Electrolytic lesions of the fimbria/fomix, [41] Rudy JW, Sutherland RJ. The hippocampal formation is neces-
dorsal hippocampus, or entorhinal cortex produce anterograde sary for rats to learn and remember configural discriminations.
deficits in contextual fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiol Learn Behav Brain Res 1989;34:97 /109.
Mem 1997;67:142 /9. [42] Save E, Poucet B, Foreman N, Buhot MC. Object exploration
[21] McDonald RJ, White NM. Parallel information processing in the and reactions to spatial and nonspatial changes in Hooded rats
water maze: evidence for independent memory systems involving following damage to parietal cortex or hippocampal formation.
dorsal striatum and hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol Behav Neurosci 1992;106:447 /56.
1994;61:260 /70. [43] Schenk F, Morris RGM. Dissociation between components of
[22] Milner B. Visually-guided maze learning in man: effects of spatial memory in rats after recovery from the effects of retro-
bilateral hippocampal, bilateral frontal, and unilateral cerebral hippocampal lesions. Exp Brain Res 1985;58:11 /28.
lesions. Neuropsychologia 1965;3:317 /38. [44] Scoville WB, Miller B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral
[23] Morris RGM. Spatial localization does not require the presence hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20:11 /
of local cues. Learn Mot 1981;12:239 /60. 2.
[24] Morris RGM. Distinctive computations and relevant associative [45] Smith ML. Recall of spatial location by amnesic patient H.M.
processes: hippocampal role in processing, retrieval, but not Brain Cogn 1988;7:178 /83.
storage of allocantric spatial memory. Hippocampus [46] Smith ML, Milner B. The role of the right hippocampus in the
1991;1:287 /90. recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia 1981;19:781 /93.
[25] Morris RGM, Garrud P, Rawlins JNP, O’Keefe J. Place [47] Smith ML, Milner B. Right temporal impairment in the recall of
navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature spatial location: encoding deficit or rapid forgetting. Neuropsy-
1982;297:681 /3. chologia 1989;27:71 /82.
[26] Morris RGM, Schenk F, Tweedie F, Jarrard LE. Ibotenate [48] Squire LR. Memory and brain, New York: Oxford University
lesions of the hippocampus and/or subiculum: dissociating Press, 1987.
components of allocentric spatial learning. Eur J Neurosci [49] Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The neurology of memory: the case
1990;2:1016 /28. for correspondence between findings for man and non-human
[27] Moser E, Moser MB, Andersen P. Spatial learning impairment primate. In: Deutsch JA, editor. The physiological basis of
parallels the magnitude of dorsal hippocampal lesions, but is memory. New York: Academic Press, 1983:200 /69.
hardly present following ventral lesions. J Neurosci [50] Sutherland RJ. Place navigation by rats in a swimming pool. Can
1993;13:3916 /25. J Psychobiol 1984;38:322 /47.
[28] Moser MB, Moser EI, Forrest E, Andersen P, Morris RGM. [51] Sutherland RJ, Rodriguez AJ. The role of the fornix/fimbria and
Spatial learning with a minislab in the dorsal hippocampus. Proc some related subcortical structures in place learning and memory.
Natl Acad Sci 1995;92:9697 /701. Behav Brain Res 1989;32:265 /77.
[29] Nadel L. The hippocampus and space revisited. Hippocampus [52] Sutherland RJ, Rudy JW. Configural association theory: the role
1991;1:211 /29. of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia.
[30] Nilson OG, Shapiro ML, Gage FH, Olton DS. Spatial learning Psychobiology 1989;17:129 /44.
and memory following fimbria /fornix transection and grafting of [53] Sutherland RJ, Whishaw IQ, Kolb B. Abnormalities in EEG and
foetal septal neurons to the hippocampus. Exp Brain Res spatial performance following intrahippocampal injections of
1987;67:195 /215. neurotoxins. Soc Neurosci 1982;6:565 (Abstract).
[31] O’Keefe J. The hippocampus as a cognitive map, Oxford: Oxford [54] Sutherland RJ, Whishaw IQ, Kolb B. Spatial mapping: definitive
University Press, 1978. disruption by hippocampal or medial frontal cortical damage in
[32] O’Keefe J. An allocentric spatial model for the hippocampal the rat. Neurosci Lett 1982;31:271 /6.
cognitive map. Hippocampus 1991;1:230 /5. [55] Taube JS. Space, the final hippocampal frontier. Hippocampus
[33] Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 1991;1:247 /9.
2nd edition, San Diego: Academic press, 1986. [56] Teng E, Squire LR. Memory for places learned long ago is intact
[34] Pearce JM, Roberts ADL, Good M. Hippocampal lesions disrupt after hippocampal damage. Nature 1999;400:675 /7.
navigation based on cognitive maps but not heading vectors. [57] Warrington EK, Baddeley AD. Amnesia and memory for visual
Nature 1998;396:75 /7. location. Neuropsychologia 1974;12:237 /63.
[35] Phillips RG, Ledoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala [58] Whishaw IQ. Cholinergic receptor blockade in the rat impairs
and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. locale but not taxon strategies for place navigation in a swimming
Behav Neurosci 1992;106:274 /85. pool. Behav Neurosci 1985;99:979 /1005.
[36] Piggott S, Milner B. Memory for different aspects of complex [59] Whishaw IQ. Place learning in hippocampal rats and the path
visual scenes after right anterior temporal lobectomy. Soc integration hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1998;22:209 /20.
Neurosci 1990;16:26 (Abstract). [60] Whishaw IQ. Spatial mapping takes time. Hippocampus
[37] Pouzet B, Welzl H, Gubler MK, Broersen L, Veenman CL, 1998;8:122 /30.
Feldon J, Rawlins JNP, Yee BK. The effects of the NMDA- [61] Whishaw IQ, Gorny B. Path integration absent in scent-tracking
induced retrohippocampal lesions on performance of four spatial fimbria /fornix rats: evidence for hippocampal involvement in
memory tasks known to be sensitive to hippocampal damage in ‘sense of direction’ and ‘sense of distance’ using self-movement
the rat. Eur J Neurosci 1999;11:123 /40. cues. J Neurosci 1999;19:4662 /73.
[38] Rasmussen M, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. A systematic test of [62] Whishaw IQ, Cassel JC, Jarrard LE. Rats with fimbria /fomix
cognitive mapping, working-memory, and temporal discontiguity lesions display a place response in a swimming pool: a dissociation
theories of hippocampal function. Psychobiology 1989;17:335 / between getting there and knowing where. J Neurosci
48. 1995;15:5779 /88.
[39] Rawlins JNP. Association across time: the hippocampus as a [63] Whishaw IQ, Jarrard LE. Similarities versus differences in place
temporary memory store. Behav Brain Sci 1985;8:479 /96. learning and circadian activity in rats after fimbria /fornix section
B. Pouzet et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 133 (2002) 279 /291 291

or ibotenate removal of hippocampus cells. Hippocampus [65] Worden RP. Navigation by fragment fitting: a theory of
1995;5:595 /604. hippocampal function. Hippocampus 1992;2:165 /87.
[64] Whishaw IQ, Mittleman G, Bunch ST, Dunnett SB. Impairments [66] Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG. Lesions of the amygdala
in the acquisition, retention and selection of navigation strategies that spare adjacent cortical regions do not impair memory or
after medial caudate /putamen lesions in rats. Behav Brain Res exacerbate the impairment following lesions of the hippocampal
1987;24:125 /38. formation. J Neurosci 1989;9:1922 /36.

You might also like