You are on page 1of 5

Spatial Knowledge and Geometric Representation in a Child Blind from Birth

Barbara Landau; Henry Gleitman; Elizabeth Spelke

Science, New Series, Vol. 213, No. 4513. (Sep. 11, 1981), pp. 1275-1278.

Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819810911%293%3A213%3A4513%3C1275%3ASKAGRI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-9

Science is currently published by American Association for the Advancement of Science.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained
prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in
the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/journals/aaas.html.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is an independent not-for-profit organization dedicated to and preserving a digital archive of scholarly journals. For
more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

http://www.jstor.org
Fri May 18 03:36:43 2007
preferred the twigs that were lower in Littlefield, R. M. Potter, J . Mummul. 61, 734 7. R. H. Smith, U.S. For. Serv. Gen. Tech. Bull.
(1980). 1532 (1977).
monoterpene concentration (4). 5. K. B. Sturgeon, Evolution 33, 803 (1979); M. A. 8. For the December data, a stepwise regression
The extent to which a-pinene influ- Radwan and G. L. Crouch, J . Wildl. Munuge. analysis, with the MAXR option of the SAS
38, 32 (1974). statistical package [J. H. Goodnight, in SAS
ences the choice o f food by tassel-eared 6. A 2.0-g sample of cortical tissue was frozen User's Guide, J. T. Helwig and K. A.Council,
squirrels was investigated in another se- under liquid nitrogen, ground to a powder, Eds. (SAS Institute, Raleigh, N.C., 1979)], indi-
transferred with 50 ml of distilled water, and cated that a-pinene accounted for 29.5 percent
ries o f tests ia which subjects were al- steam-distilled with n-pentane for 1 hour. The of the variance ( P = ,013); the semipartials
pentane extract was dried over anhydrous (SR2)for 3-carene, P-pinene, and limonene were
lovved to choose between plain mash Na,SO,, and the pentane was removed (micro not significant ( P > .25). Similar results were
(made from ground sunflower seeds and Vigreaux column). When the volume of the obtained for the seasonally combined data with
extract was less than 0.1 ml, an internal standard a-pinene accounting for 34.5 percent of the
laboratory food) and mash laced with of 10.0 p1 of n-tetradecane in n-heptane (200 p1 variance ( P = ,006); again the semipartials for 3-
varying amounts o f a-pinene (9).Repeat- of C,,H,, in 10 ml of C7H,,) wa added, and the -
carede. B-oinene. and limonene were not sianifi-
volume was then increased to 0.1 ml with n- cant (P i ' . z s ) .
ed-measures analysis o f variance indicat- heptane. The concentrated extract was analyzed 9. The mash consisted of 50 percent ground sun-
ed that the a-pinene content affected by gas chromatography (F and M model 810), flower seeds and 50 percent mouse food
with dual-flame ionization detection and a 104-m (Charles River); 5 ml of safflower oil was added
food consumption FF(2, 14) = 3.72; glass capillary column coated with Carbowax to each 100 g of mash. The mash containing
20M). A 5.0-pl sample of the concentrated monoterpene was made up in concentrations of
P = .025, one-tailed test];as the amount extract with added internal standard was inject- 0.1, 0.3, and 0.5 ml of a-pinene per 100 g of
o f a-pinene increased from 0.1 to 0.3 to ed into a glass-lined injection port at a split ratio mash. On each testina dav. after 23 hours of
of approximately I00 : 1. The oven conditions food deprivation, e a c 6 squirrel was given two
0.5 ml per 100 g o f mash, the proportion were 5 minutes at 65"C, then increased 1" per food dishes; one contained 20 g of plain mash,
o f food consumed decreased from 0.45 to minute to 16S°C, and isothermal thereafter. He- the other 20 g of mash laced with a-pinene. Once
lium make-up gas was supplied at 30 mllmin at a squirrel began to feed from either dish, it was
0.41 to 0.24, respectively. 'The drop in the exit end of the glass capillary column. The given 20 minutes to eat; then both dishes were
co~~sumption at 0.5 ml per 100 g was data were recorded on a Varian CDS-I I I chro- removed and the amount of each type of food
matography data system, which gave the area eaten was recorded. Each squirrel received five
significant (Scheffe test; P < .05, one- percent composition for the various components trials at each of the three concentration levels.
and the area of each component relative to 10 The monoterpene was obtained from Glidden,
tailed test) and showed that higher con- percent of the area of the internal standard. Organic Chemicals Division, SCM Corporation,
cer~trations o f a-pinene caused tassel- Presentation of data in this manner permitted Jacksonville, Fla.
the evaluation of amounts of each component in We thank J. R. McKnight, J. C. Pederson, R. M.
eased squirrels to reduce their intake o f a the samples relative to the internal standard a s Potter, and K. Wuensch for their help with the
preferred foodstuff. well as the determination of the (relative) total study and A. J . Cooper, S. D. Berry, A. J.
amounts of all monoterpenes in each sample. Pantle, and J. C. Jahnke for reading the manu-
Thus the amount o f a-pinene is impor- Kovats' indices were determined on the glass scriot. Suooorted bv NSF - grants BNS 76-05069
tant in choosing which ponderosa pine capillary Carbowax 20M column at 70°C for the and'^^^ '76.80423.'
monoterpenes, and mass spectra were obtained Present address: Cooperative Institute for Re-
trees are to be used as food sources by from the total essential oil with a Carbowax 20M search in Environmental Sciences and Depart-
glass capillary column in a quadrupole gas chro- ment of Environmental, Population, and Organ-
tassel-eared squirrels. Other factors that matography-mass spectrometer (Finnigan mod- ismic Biology, University of Colorado, Boulder
may affect twig preferences are nutri- el 1015C) with a model 6000 data system [H. 80302.
Maarse and R. E. Kepner, J. Agric. Food
tional quality o f the cortical tissue and Chem. 18, 1095 (1970)l. 23 October 1980
possible synergisms between nutrients
and combinations o f secondary com-
pounds (mono- and sesquiterpenes).
Contextual factors such as the visual
appearance o f the trees, presence o f con- Spatial Knowledge and Geometric Representation in a
specifics, and pheromonal cues may also Child Blind from Birth
act as determinants in feeding tree selec-
tion i f the chemistry o f the tree is accept- Abstract. A series of experiments demonstrated that a congenitally blind 2'/r-year-
able. old child-as well as sighted but blindfolded children and adults-can determine the
R. C. FARENTINOS* appropriate path between two objects aJier traveling t o each of those objects from a
Department of Zoology, Ohio third object. This task requires that the child detect the distances and the angular
State University, Co1umbu.s 43210 relationship qf the jamiliar paths and that she derive therefrom the angle of the new
P. J. CAPRETTA path. Our research indicates that the locomotion ofthe young blind child is guided by
Department of Psychology, Miami knowledge. of the Euc.lidean properties of a spatial layout and by principles j))r
University, Oxjbrd, Ohio 45056 muking inferences based on those properties.
R. E . KEPNER
Department of Chemistry, W e have had the opportunity to study Sighted adults and 3-year-old children,
University of Calij))rnia, Davis 95616 the spatially guided locomotion o f one all blindfolded, performed with similar
V. M . LITTLEFIELD21/2-year-old blind child in several ex- accuracy. These observations demon-
Department of Psychology, perimental settings. After the child had strate that the locomotion o f children,
Miami University been taken along several paths connect- with or without visual experience, is
References and Notes
ing four objects in a small room, she was guided by metric knowledge o f space.
able to move directly between the ob- This knowledge makes possible the deri-
1. J. 0. Keith, Ecology 46, 150 (1965); D. R.
Patton, U.S. For. Srrv. Res. Note R M 272 jects along paths she had never taken. vation o f further spatial information.
(1974). These observations were undertaken
2. D. R. Patton and W. Green, U.S. For. Serv.
Re.$. Note R M 169 (1970). to address a classical issue in psycholo-
3. Feeding trees were determined in the field by Fig. 1. Room layout gy, the development o f human knowl-
extensiveness of defoliation and number of
freshly clipped needle clusters; the nearest for spatial inference edge o f space. Descartes (1) suggested
neighbor of the same age and size a s the feeding experiment. The
tree that did not show cvidence of squirrel- room measured 2.44 that the geometric principles underlying
caused defoliation was designated a s a nonfeed- m by 3.05 m. Dashed spatial knowledge are innate and accessi-
ing tree. In choice experiments and monoter-
pene analysis, twigs from a specific feeding tree lines, trained routes; ble to any perceptual mode. He offered
were compared with twigs from its counterpart solid lines, test the example o f a blind man exploring
nonfeeding tree. routes. Landmarks:
4. P. J. Capretta and R. C. Farentinos, in Prefer-
M, mother; P, pil- objects with a stick. For the man to
ence Behuviour und Chemorrception, J. H. A. discover the shapes and arrangement o f
Kroeze. Ed. (Information Retrieval Limited, lows; T, table; B , bas-
London, 1979), pp. 205-218; . , V. M. ket. those objects, he must refer each tactual
SCIENCE, VOL. 213. I 1 SEPTEMBER 1981 0036-807518110911- 1275$01.0010 Copyright 0 1981 AAAS 1275
impression to a unitary spatial frame- they apply these inference rules to the tal fibroplasia. She is totally blind (5).
work, structured by the principles of sensory information that the environ- After spending the first 5 months in the
Euclidean geometry. Descartes proposed ment provides. Second, Gibson (4) pro- hospital, primarily in an isolette, Kelli
that any perceiver, exploring the world posed that no geometric principles what- was discharged wlth a developmental
through any mode, faces the blind man's ever need structure one's sensory im- status of newborn.
problem, and must draw on tacit knowl- pressions. 'The spatial properties of ob- When Kelli was 32 months old, she
edge of geometry to solve it. Descartes jects are specified by higher-order was brought into an unfam~liarlabora-
further reasoned that geometric princi- relationships in spatially and temporally tory playroom, 2.44 m by 3.05 m. The
ples must be innate: since those princi- extended arrays of stimulation. Space room contained four landmarks in a dia-
ples structure any perceptual experi- perception depends on mechanisms that mond shaped array: her mother seated
ence, they cannot themselves be ac- detect these relationships. on a chair (M), a stack of pillows (P), a
quired through experience. These argu- We have attempted to address these basket of toys (B), and a table (T) (Fig.
ments form part of the core of the issues by extending Descartes's exam- I). In experiment 1, we placed her at M
rationalist tradition in psychology (2). ple. We focused on the attempts of a and walked her from there to P and back,
Descartes's analysis can be contrasted young blind child to discover the spatial twice; from M to T and back, twice; and
with two major subsequent develop- layout of objects in a room from limited, from M to B and back, twice. Each time,
ments. First, Helmholtz (-?), extending temporally extended encounters with Kelli felt the object while facing it, and
the arguments of British empiricism, re- those objects and with paths between she might have thus determined the ori-
jected Descartes's claim of innateness. them. We investigated whether this child entation of the object relative to the
Helmholtz suggested that the geometric could make spatial deductions that rely trained path. We then induced her to find
principles underlying space perception on information about Euclidean angles the routes between P, T, and B on her
can be acquired. Perceivers need be en- and distances. own, by giving her such simple com-
dowed only with a set of general, non- Our principal subject was Kelli, a child mands as "Go to the toybasket" or "Go
spatial inference rules. They will deduce born 3 months prematurely and blinded to the pillows." Kelli was followed as
the principles of Euclidean geometry as shortly after birth as a result of retrolen- she moved until she reached the goal or

9:PtoB 10: B to P 11: p to B 12: B to P


Fig. 2. Performance on test trials. Solid line, independent rnovernent by child: dashed line. experimenter aided child.
1276 SCIENCE, VOL. 213
expressed confusion. In the latter case, of eight trials (P = .0008). More impor- rather, her actions may have been guided
the trial was ended, and she was taken to tant, on only one of the eight trials could by a perceptual mechanism that detected
the goal. Each route was tested twice, her direction of movement be accounted invariant tactual relationships as she ac-
for a total of 12 trials, in the following for by the orientation of the room, a tively locomoted. Such a mechanism
order: T-B, B-T, T-B, B-T; T-P, P-T, nonsignificant proportion (P = .38). might be sensitive to Euclidean relation-
T.-P, P-T; P-B, B-P, P-B, B-P. Her A third experiment was conducted ships, yielding the perception of further
route was plotted from a videotaped rec- with five sighted 3-year-old children and Euclidean relationships without produc-
ord by observing her position and frontal six sighted adults, all of whom wore ing a spatial representation to which in-
direction at 3-second intervals, and join- opaque goggles to block their vision of ference rules are explicitly applied. The
ing these points with a line representing the room. The accuracy of the child Gibsonian burden is to provide a general
her path of movement. subjects was similar to that of Kelli, with characterization of the invariants that
Kelli's performance on each test trial a mean of 8.2 successes in 12 trials. The perceivers detect and of the mechanisms
is shown in Fig. 2. To test the null adult subjects performed somewhat bet- that detect them-a characterization that
hypothesis that Kelli moved randomly to ter, with a mean of 11.0 successes. can encompass the performance of the
some stopping point, we measured her These experiments indicate that a blind child traveling along a limited set of
position just before reaching the target or young blind child is able to set a course paths.
before the trial was ended. The circle between objects along a route she has A young blind child exploring an envi-
that surrounded her starting position was never followed, after moving to each ronment was able to gain knowledge of
divided into nine 40" segments, and each object from a third point. In order to certain spatial relationships between ob-
trial was treated as a success if her final accomplish this, the child must have jects. Moreover, she could use these
position fell within the 40" segment con- access to information about the lengths relationships to derive further knowl-
taiining the target (which subtended an of the two connecting routes travelled edge about the spatial properties of that
angle of about that magnitude). The ran- during training and the angular separa- layout. Our observations indicate that
dom probability of success is .I 1, but tion of those routes. From this informa- metric properties of space can be appre-
Kelli's actual performance was much tion, the child can derive new angular ciated by children at an early age. They
higher: there were eight successes and relationships: the angular direction of further indicate that vision plays no es-
f o ~ failures
~r (binomial test, P = .0001). one object from the other. Angle and sential role in the early development of
Two of the failures were errors of IS0 or distance information are properties that knowledge of such properties.
less. are preserved in metric geometries, such BARBARA L ANDAU
Kelli did not move ballistically toward as Euclidean geometry; they are not HENRYGLEITMAN
the target. Instead, she seemed to adjust properties of nonmetric geometries, such ELIZABETH SPELKE
her movements, as she went, towards as the projective or topological. Further- Department of Psychology,
the target. At successive intervals of 0.61 more, the axioms and theorems of Eu- University of Pennsylvania,
m, from her starting position, we com- clidean geometry are sufficient for the Philadelphia 19104
pared her frontal direction at the begin- derivation of the new angular informa-
ning of the interval to her position at the tion, whereas those of the other geome- References and Notes
end of the interval and computed the tries are not (7). We conclude that this 1. R. Descartes, Discourse on Method, Optics,
average degrees of self-correction to- blind child, and sighted controls, know Geometty, and Meteorology P. J . Olscamp,
Transl. (Bobbs-Merrill, Indianapolis, 1965; orig-
wa,rd the target for each trial. She adjust- about some of the metric properties of inally published in 1637).
ed her direction toward the target on 11 space, probably Euclidean properties. 2. N. Chomsky, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax
(MlT Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1965); I. Kant,
trials, and away from the target on 1 of Our findings do not distinguish conclu- Critique of Pure Reason (Macmillan, New
the 12 trials (P = .003) (6). sively between the Cartesian, Helmholt- York, 1929; originally published in 1781); C. W.
Leibniz, New Essays Concerning Human Un-
Kelli's performance might have been zian, and Gibsonian approaches, but derstanding (Open Court, Chicago, 1916; orig-
caused by the use of subtle acoustic cues inally published in 1704).
they do help to sharpen the theoretical 3. H , von Helmholtz, Treatise on Physiological
to orient her toward the different land- issues and clarify the empirical tasks Optics (Dover, New York, 1962; originally pub-
lished in 1867), vol. 3.
marks. Experiment 2 tested for this pos- facing proponents of each tradition. The 4. J. J. Gibson, The Perception of the Visual World
sibility. Its procedure was identical to Cartesian psychologist might propose (Houghton-Mifflin, Boston, 1950); The Senses
Considered as Perceptual Systems (Houghton-
that of experiment 1 with one exception: that Euclidean geometry is innate. The Mifflin, 1966); The Ecological Approach to Visu-
after the first six test trials, Kelli was task is then to discover the psychologi- al Perception (Houghton-Mifflin, 1979). See R.
Shaw and J. Bransford, in Perceiving, Acting,
carried out of the room, and the array of cally appropriate axiomatization of that and Knowing, R. Shaw and J. Bransford, Eds.
objects was rotated 90". Kelli was then (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., 1977) for an analysis
geometry and to characterize the pro- In the Gibsonian tradition of the blind-man-with-
carried back in and placed facing one of cesses of inference based on a Euclidean stick problem.
5. Cases of severe prematurity are at risk for a
the objects. She then received eight test representation. The Helmholtzian psy- variety of problems. Kelli has been assessed
trials with the rotated array. The rotation chologist might propose that Euclidean yearly since the beginning of the study and does
not show any significant deficiencies relative to
preserved the spatial relationships principles can be induced in the first 2% norms for sighted or blind children.
among objects, but it changed the abso- years by a child who applies innate non- 6. Had Kelli been moving constantly straight
ahead, she would not have performed well,
lute position of each object within the spatial inference rules to correlated pat- since her starting position varied on each trial.
room. Thus, if Kelli used acoustic infor- terns of sensation, whether visual or For example, on trial 2, she would have moved
toward M from her starting position; on trial 3,
mation from the room as spatial land- nonvisual. The burden of a Helmholtzian she would have moved straight into T from her
starting position.
marks or beacons, she would be expect- is to describe these rules and the learning 7. Three different metric geometries could support
ed to move incorrectly between objects, processes that lead to the development the inferences of the blind child: Euclidean,
hyperbolic, and Riemannian geometries. These
since spatial information about the ob- of a Euclidean representation and its geometries are empirically indistinguishable
jects and about the room conflicted. Kel- associated inference rules, using sense over the range of distances that humans can
negotiate. Other geometries, such a s topology
li's level of accuracy after this rotation information. Finally, the Gibsonian psy- and projective geometry, have been proposed to
was close to what it had been before. She characterize the child's spatial knowledge; see
chologist might propose that the child J. Piaget and B. Inhelder, The Child's Concep-
moved successfully to the target on five made no inferences at all in our task; tion of Space (Norton, New York, 1967; orig-

I I SEPTEMBER 1981
inally published in 1948). These geometries can- man for extensive overall conceptual guidance; rial disease [K~llner'srule ( 4 ) ] , indicat-
not support the inferences we have studied, U. Neisser for comments on a previous draft of
however, for they preserve no metric properties this paper; and K. Feldman for running sighted ing an undue vulnerability to retinal in-
IR. Courant and H. Robbins, What is Mathe- contfol sub'ects and transcribing the videotaped sult,
matics? (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1941)l. sess~ons.$his work was aided by a social and
8. We thank C. R. Gallistel, for focusing our atten- behavioral sciences research grant from the Na- In 1970, Laties and Liebman (5) re-
tion on formal geometric analysis of our experi- tional Foundation-March of Dimes and a Wil-
ment, for suggesting important controls and liam T. Carter Foundation grant, both to L. R. ported that the intravitreal injection of a
variations on the basic experimental paradigm, Gleitman. tissue-reactive fluorescent dye, Procion
and for suggesting a statistical analysis derived
from the avian navigation literature; L. R. Gleit- I5 December 1980; revised 22 May 1981 yellow, stained the outer segments of
cones, but not of rods, in the amphibian
retina. Using greater amounts of the dye,
we have obtained a striking result. In the
retina of the monkey, not only are all
Staining of Blue-Sensitive Cones of the Macaque cone outer segments stained with Pro-
Retina by a Fluorescent Dye cion yellow, but the entire soma of some
cones is completely stained by the dye,
Abstract. Intravitreal injection of aJluorescent dye, Procion yellow, results in the producing a Golgi-like silhouette. Such
complete and systematic staining of a cone population in the monkey retina. These cones are organized in a rather regular
cones form an approximately regular array whose separation varies with retinal array and have a characteristic retinal
eccentricity. They are absent in the very center of the fovea, and their density peaks distribution.
at I". The distribution of stained cones resembles that reported for blue-sensitive Procion yellow M4RAN (Polysci-
cones of other primates and, consistent with such an identijication, they are found ences), 5 to 7 percent in deionized water,
with less incidence in species having lower concentrations of blue cones. was injected (0.15 ml) intravitreally into
the eye of anesthetized rhesus and cyno-
The neural retina is a highly organized caque monkeys, the green and red cone molgus monkeys. Leakage of dye was
structure with a crystalline-like array of systems have similar electrophy siologi- reduced by the slow removal of the nee-
tightly packed cones and rods in a two- cal and psychophysical properties, dle. In some animals we also injected
dimensional matrix. Microspectrophoto- whereas those of the blue cone system Lucifer yellow (Polysciences) ( 6 ) simul-
metric work has provided evidence of are different (2). There are also differ- taneously with or subsequently to the
three cone types in the primate retina, ences between dysfunctions of these Procion dye in a weight ratio of 1 : 50 to
each having peak sensitivity at a differ- cone systems leading to color vision dis- 1:100 of Lucifer to Procion yellow. Ex-
ent part of the spectrum (1)-"blue-," orders. Congenital disorders of the green cept for one monkey, which was kept in
'6

green-," and "red-sensitive" cones. and red cone systems correspond to sex- the dark during and after the injection,
To our knowledge, morphological differ- linked inheritance, whereas those of the the animals were kept in a normal light
ences among these cone types have not blue cone system correspond to autoso- (200 trolands): dark cycle for 18 to 30
been reported in primates. Functionally, ma1 inheritance (3). In addition, blue hours (7). The animals were then killed
however, blue cones have unique prop- cones are often involved in acquired with an overdose of pentobarbital. The
erties. In humans and (Old World) ma- color vision disorders secondary to reti- eyes were fixed, often by arterial perfu-

Fig. 1. (A) Radial section of rhesus monkey retina (- 20' eccentricity) showing a cone completely stained by Procion yellow among other cones
unstained except for their outer segments. (B) Tangential section passing through the outer limiting membrane showing a regular array of stained
cones; unstained cones and rods appear as holes in the stained mesh of the outer limiting membrane.
1278 0036-807518110911-1278$01,0010 Copyright O 1981 AAAS SCIENCE, VOL. 213. I 1 SEPTEMBER 1981

You might also like