You are on page 1of 10

Educational Researcher

http://er.aera.net

''Where Is the Action?'' Challenges to Studying the Teaching of Reading in Elementary Classrooms
Robert G. Croninger and Linda Valli
EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER 2009 38: 100
DOI: 10.3102/0013189X09333206

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://edr.sagepub.com/content/38/2/100

Published on behalf of

American Educational Research Association

and

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Educational Researcher can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://er.aera.net/alerts

Subscriptions: http://er.aera.net/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.aera.net/reprints

Permissions: http://www.aera.net/permissions

Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010


“Where Is the Action?” Challenges to Studying the
Teaching of Reading in Elementary Classrooms
Robert G. Croninger and Linda Valli

The authors argue that part of the difficulty in studying the teaching relationship with desirable outcomes for students. We argue that
of reading in elementary classrooms is determining where “the part of the difficulty in studying the teaching of reading is
action” occurs in reading instruction. Based on their 5-year longitu- determining, or delimiting in our research designs, where
“the action” actually occurs in reading instruction—which
dinal study of fourth- and fifth-grade teachers in moderate- and high-
instructional interactions actually constitute the teaching
poverty elementary schools, they describe three challenges:
of reading. The three challenges we wish to discuss are
(a) determining key factors in the complex instructional environment (a) determining the key factors in the complex instructional
that should be the focus of study, (b) determining who teaches read- environment that should be the focus of study; (b) determining
ing to specific students, and (c) determining the boundaries of reading the sources of instruction—who teaches reading to specific
instruction or when it occurs during the school day and year. The students within a school; and (c) determining the boundaries of
reading instruction—that is, determining when it occurs during
authors argue that these challenges, although not insurmountable, are
the school day and year. Although each of these challenges has
difficult to resolve and that they are becoming more pervasive
been identified to varying degrees by others (see, e.g., Good &
because of current policies that promote high-stakes accountability. Brophy, 2003; Raudenbush, Martinez, & Spybrook, 2007), we
argue that they are more difficult to resolve than is often
acknowledged and that they are becoming progressively more
Keywords: high-stakes accountability; reading instruction; research difficult in the current, high-stakes accountability environment.
methodology; teacher effectiveness
Background
We draw our observations from a 5-year longitudinal study of
teaching that we conducted in moderate- to high-poverty ele-

M
any educators and policy makers who are eager to mentary schools. The primary research goals for the study were
improve teaching focus on reading instruction in par- to understand (a) how teachers in fourth- and fifth-grade class-
ticular, because reading generally is seen as fundamen- rooms help students acquire foundational skills in reading and
tal to learning other skills and subjects. However, there is less mathematics1 and (b) how various aspects of a school’s organiza-
consensus about what constitutes high-quality reading instruc- tion and culture can facilitate or hinder a teacher’s ability to
tion, or even what is the best way of studying it (Fuhrman, engage in positive forms of teaching in the classroom. For the
Cohen, & Mosher, 2007). A range of reasons are given for the purposes of this article, we focus primarily on the first goal of the
difficulty of developing a reliable, agreed-upon knowledge base study. Using a cognitive framework, we define reading as the abil-
from which to fashion instructional strategies and education ity of students to “understand what is in text” (Pressley, 2000,
policies in this area. They highlight the multiplicity of scholarly p. 545), and we consider some of the challenges that we experi-
perspectives that characterize the field of reading research, includ- enced studying teaching within this framework.
ing different definitions of what constitutes reading; examina- All of the schools that participated in the study were part of a
tions of different populations of students, for whom different single school system, the Stevenson Public School District,2 a
reading approaches may be more or less effective; and interests in large and diverse district that was facing a wide range of
different aspects of reading, such as genre, comprehension, educational challenges associated with significant demographic
achievement, motivation, and engagement (Allington & changes and heightened expectations for student performance. In
Cunningham, 1996; Allington & Johnston, 2002; Fenstermacher & the past two decades, the enrollment of low-income students had
Richardson, 2005; Lampert, 2001). doubled in Stevenson. At the time of our study, the district
Although we acknowledge the difficulty of arriving at enrolled nearly 140,000 students and employed roughly 19,000
consensus, we identify three methodological and practical employees, making Stevenson one of the largest school districts
challenges that complicate attempts to develop a reliable in the nation. Approximately 30% of the students in Stevenson
knowledge base about the nature of reading instruction and its participated in free and reduced-price meals services (FARMS),

Educational Researcher, Vol. 38, No. 2, pp. 100–108


DOI: 10.3102/0013189X09333206
© 2009 AERA. http://er.aera.net

100 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
and 12% participated in English programs for speakers of other 1998), to develop our instruments and research design. These
languages (ESOL). We selected the school district because of its principles indicate that good teaching promotes deep, principled
racial and economic diversity, its reliable database on students learning of content; encourages the development of cognitive and
and schools, and its well-publicized commitment to standards- metacognitive skills; motivates students to engage deeply in
based reforms. subject matter; addresses individual and developmental differences
The study design called for identifying a group of moderate- among students; and creates inclusive, affirming, and successful
to high-poverty elementary schools with greater-than-expected learning environments.
achievement gains and then following these schools and their We recognized that differences in subject areas make different
fourth- and fifth-grade teachers for 3 to 4 years.3 When a school demands on teachers’ content and pedagogical expertise
agreed to participate in the study, we attempted to recruit all of (Alexander & Fives, 2000; Shulman, 1986). Although the
the teachers in those two grades. When identified schools declined learner-centered psychological principles complement standards
to participate or withdrew from the study, we replaced them with derived from reading research (see, e.g., International Reading
schools in the district that had similar demographics and, when Association & National Council of Teachers of English, 1996),
possible, reputations for good teaching. During the 4-year period the manifestations of “exemplary” teaching are shaped in part by
for which we collected data, approximately 20 elementary lesson topics, students, and curricular materials. They may also
schools, 160 teachers, and 4,300 students participated in the differ according to the goals and values that drive the instruction
study. Most of the data used in this article to support our (Chambliss & Graeber, 2003). In general, however, much of the
observations came from the 2003–2004 school year. As a group, research on exemplary reading instruction at the fourth- and
the participating schools had higher-than-average achievement fifth-grade levels suggests the importance of large amounts of
levels in reading and mathematics on the state’s mandated fourth- reading and writing, connections across the curriculum, and
and fifth-grade assessments—higher than those of other schools skills instruction embedded in meaningful interaction with text.
with comparable FARMS enrollments in the district, although According to this body of research, high-quality instruction
the range in achievement levels varied substantially across provides students with choices about interesting and challenging
participating schools. texts associated with a variety of genres and encourages students
We developed a series of instruments and research protocols to respond personally to those texts. Teachers also promote
to capture the occurrence and consequences of a wide range of dialogue about reading and writing through teacher questioning,
pedagogical practices using a complementary mixed-methods grouping practices, cooperative learning, and informative
research design (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 2002). Our instruments feedback (Morrow, Wamsley, Duhammel, & Fittipaldi, 2002;
included a time-sampling protocol in which observers recorded Pressley, Yokoi, Rankin, Wharton-McDonald, & Mistretta,
what teachers and students were doing every 3 minutes during a 1997; Taylor, Pearson, Clark, & Walpole, 2002).
reading lesson. The 3-minute intervals served two functions. One indicator of high-quality teaching evident in both the
First, they gave observers enough time to record seven core learner-centered psychological principles and the reading
dimensions of instruction (teacher activity, student activity, standards that we used is high cognitive demand. As noted in the
instructional organization, teacher attention, lesson content, standards, teachers should help students critically analyze texts
context of episode, classroom behavior). Second, they provided and engage in purposeful and challenging language experiences
enough data points (approximately 20 per lesson) to capture (International Reading Association & National Council of
patterns and sequences of instructional interactions. We used this Teachers of English, 1996). We defined high cognitive demand as
instrument, along with pre- and post-lesson interviews, to observe teachers’ press for student reasoning, reflection on learning, and
6–8 reading lessons each year per teacher. In the pre-lesson higher order thinking, as well as the extent to which students are
interviews, observers asked the teacher about lesson goals, exposed to new and challenging content. In particular, we looked
content, planning, and materials to situate and help focus the at (a) teacher requests for complex forms of thinking, (b) student
observation. In the post-lesson interviews, teachers were asked if responses that signaled cognitive complexity, and (c) the
they had accomplished or departed from their goals and why.4 prevalence of content that required greater cognitive engagement
Our conceptualization of high-quality reading instruction by students. To be considered high cognitive demand, the reading
shaped our instruments and our study design, as well as the content had to focus on genre characteristics, themes, or main
challenges that we experienced in studying teaching.5 Most ideas and supporting details. We use this construct of high
pertinent to our study was the research in the areas of reading and cognitive demand as an example of one of the challenges associated
cognitive psychology, which depicts high-quality teaching as a with studying the quality of reading instruction in classrooms
complex, multidimensional phenomenon best studied through a and schools discussed below.
variety of overlapping, complementary strategies. This research Although we studied reading instruction only in the fourth
encompasses a set of general theories about how children learn and fifth grades and only in the schools that participated in our
and, correspondingly, how successful teachers teach. What study, we have no reason to believe that the challenges we describe
distinguishes these theories about teaching is that they are based are unique to either these classes or these schools. On the contrary,
on a body of cognitive research about learning (Bransford, Brown, as we explain elsewhere (Valli, Croninger, & Buese, 2006; Valli,
& Cocking, 1999) that is linked to rich descriptions of classroom Croninger, Chambliss, Graeber, & Buese, 2008; Valli, Croninger,
practice (Ball & Cohen, 1999; Lampert, 2001). We used a set of & Walters, 2007), there is good reason to believe that these
relevant principles, the American Psychological Association’s challenges have been magnified by the demands of high-stakes
learner-centered psychological principles (Alexander & Murphy, testing. Nonetheless, we acknowledge that such challenges may

MARCH 2009 101


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
vary with variations in the press to meet annual achievement presented to students, and orientation of classroom management)
goals, the resources available to teachers and principals to meet moderated the relationship between teacher requests and student
those goals, and beliefs about what constitutes good and effective responses. To examine these relationships, we constructed a three-
teaching of reading. level hierarchical model (lessons nested within classrooms, nested
within schools).6
Determining Key Factors in the Complex
Instructional Environment Methodological and Practical Challenges
One of the greatest challenges in studying the quality of reading Our analysis of these data helps to characterize the methodologi-
instruction is understanding how “quality” can be dependent on cal and practical challenges associated with capturing the com-
multiple and simultaneous factors (Cohen, Raudenbush, & Ball, plexity of reading instruction. First, when we examined variation
2003; Good & Brophy, 2003; Fenstermacher & Richardson, in the quality of teacher–student oral exchanges across observa-
2005). Some researchers have framed this challenge as under- tions, we discovered that most of the variance in these exchanges
standing how teacher actions interact with students and content occurred among lessons enacted by the same teachers. Nine per-
(Good & Brophy, 2003; Lampert, 2001); others have discussed cent (9%) of the variation in the quality of oral exchanges was
how teacher actions interact with available resources (broadly associated with schools, and 7% was associated with classes; by
conceived) to realize desirable student outcomes (e.g., contrast, 84% was associated with the lessons conducted by the
Raudenbush, 2008). In either case, teaching is portrayed as a same teachers. Although schools and teachers varied in the extent
complex process that requires an investigation of how instruc- to which we were able to observe more cognitively demanding
tional effects vary across multiple factors, such as prior knowl- exchanges surrounding texts, the preponderance of variation was
edge, the difficulty of text, a teacher’s instructional repertoire, associated with lessons taught by the same teacher in the same
and student expectations. Capturing this complexity was a fun- classroom. One aspect of complexity, therefore, has to do with
damental goal and challenge for our study. the lesson content itself and the extent to which content lends
itself to the study of particular teacher practices—in this case,
Quality of Teacher–Student Oral Exchanges
fostering cognitively rich dialogue between teachers and students
By way of illustration, we sought to determine what teachers do about text.
to encourage students to engage with reading content in greater Methodologically, this means that studies of reading must
cognitive depth. We examined the results of the time-sampling observe multiple lessons by the same teacher to determine the
protocol for 412 reading lessons that we observed in 62 reading extent to which practices occur and their effects on student
classrooms (roughly 7 lessons per teacher). We then coded obser- outcomes. In our study we attempted to observe 6–8 reading
vations that characterized the quality of students’ oral exchanges lessons per teacher, a relatively heavy burden for data collection,
with their teachers about reading content. We coded an oral given the number of teachers in our study. However, our own
exchange as reflecting greater cognitive demand when students experience suggests that this range for number of observations
posed a hypothesis about the text, provided an alternative answer may represent the lower limit of the number required to portray
to a question, justified or explained an answer, or elaborated on accurately the practices of teachers in reading classrooms. Even
an answer already given. When students provided a simple answer relatively straightforward and prevalent practices, such as the use
or made simple statements about text, we coded these responses of whole-group instruction, varied more among the lessons
as reflecting less cognitive demand and complexity. In a similar taught by the same teachers than among teachers or schools.
fashion, we coded teachers’ oral exchanges with students during Rarer and more complex interactions between students and
lessons. When teachers asked students to reflect on text or an teachers require observations across even more lessons to portray
answer, assess a statement, or elaborate on an answer, or responded accurately the nature of reading instruction in classrooms (see
to a question with an alternative question, we coded these the Rowan & Correnti article in this issue of Educational
exchanges as having greater cognitive demand. Simple statements Researcher).
or a failure to respond we coded as exchanges having less cogni- As we had anticipated, we also found a very strong relationship
tive demand and complexity. between the quality of teacher requests and the quality of student
We then used these new codes to examine the relationship responses about reading content. The more teachers encouraged
between the cognitive complexity of teacher requests and the students to think deeply about text, the more students appeared
cognitive complexity of student responses as a characteristic of to do so, at least as measured by the nature of their oral exchanges
reading instruction. Based on our conceptual framework and with their teachers. The effect of teacher requests on student
related research (Good & Brophy, 2003; Nystrand & Gamoran, responses was nearly half (0.48) a standard deviation. Moreover,
1991), we assumed that students were more likely to engage in the effect of teacher requests varied among teachers and schools.
cognitively complex discussions of what they were reading if The range in effects associated with teachers was nearly a full
teachers encouraged them to do so. We also assumed, however, standard deviation, whereas the range in effects associated with
that the effectiveness of these requests might vary with the schools was approximately half a standard deviation. In other
characteristics of lessons, curriculum materials, students, and words, teachers, even when they made the same types of requests
other teacher practices. To this end we considered whether aspects for students to engage text, had different levels of success in
of classroom and school composition (e.g., percentage ESOL soliciting more cognitively complex responses from students.
students, percentage FARMS students) and average teacher Other factors in and outside the classroom moderated the
practices (e.g., organization of classes, difficulty of content effects.

102 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
We examined a range of possibilities that might help us better conducting our study, we anticipated that students would inter-
understand how different factors interacted to produce different act with multiple adults in the process of reading instruction even
outcomes—in this case, cognitively rich dialogue about text in within a single grade, but we did not anticipate the extent and
classrooms. The strongest influence that we found was teachers’ variety of these student–adult interactions. A major challenge for
classroom management style. The more teachers focused on the study was delineating and characterizing these more complex
organizing tasks and materials during a lesson, the more effective instructional interactions in and outside the classroom. Identifying
they were at soliciting cognitively complex discussions about text; who is teaching reading required the consideration of multiple
the more teachers focused on student behaviors, the less successful sources of instruction that occurred during the school day and
teachers were in soliciting these oral exchanges with students across the school year.
(controlling for students’ FARM status, English language
Unintentional Shared Designs
proficiency, and a measure of prior knowledge). The differences
in the effects of teachers’ requests for greater cognitive engagement As we watched students and adults move in and out of class-
ranged from 0.61 standard deviation for teachers who focused on rooms, we began to categorize the different ways in which mul-
managing materials and activities to 0.35 for teachers who tiple adults shared responsibility for teaching reading (Valli et al.,
focused on managing student behavior. 2007). We first divided these patterns broadly into two categories:
The above illustration demonstrates one of the challenges unintentional and intentional shared instructional designs.
associated with the examination of the teaching of reading in Unintentional designs included typical accommodations made in
elementary classrooms. Practices vary substantially across lessons, response to disruptions in the daily routines of students and
often more so than they do across teachers or classrooms. As a teachers. For example, teacher absences require some sharing of
result, studies of reading need to examine what teachers do across teaching responsibility. Because teachers are absent for a wide
multiple lessons (we aimed at 6–8 lessons per teacher in the range of reasons, from staff development sessions to family cir-
examples that we have provided), especially if the practices of cumstances, they may require sharing instruction for one class
interest are relatively rare or complex. The number of observations period in the school day or for a significant part of a school year.
required to accurately portray instruction poses a considerable Studies have demonstrated a strong link between teacher absen-
practical burden on studies of reading, often forcing reading teeism and student achievement, particularly at the school level
researchers to choose between enhancing the internal validity of (Miller, Murnane, & Willett, 2008); less is known, however,
a study (by increasing the number of lessons observed) and about how routine accommodations for absences affect teaching
enhancing the external validity of a study (by increasing the and learning. The data from our study indicate that teachers, on
number of teachers and classrooms included in a study). average, had assistance covering reading instruction 7% of the
Moreover, although it is well known that teachers who engage time, or for roughly 2.5 weeks of instruction during the school
students more deeply in text are more successful in facilitating year. Although these instances of shared instruction are modest,
both comprehension and the development of literacy skills, less there is no reason to believe that they have no effect on instruc-
is known about how teachers do so effectively (Allington & tion or student learning.
Cunningham, 1996; Allington & Johnston, 2002). The examples Even when teachers carry out their instructional responsibilities
that we provided suggest that teachers are most effective when without major absences, student mobility can interfere with the
efforts to elicit cognitively demanding dialogue from students are one-to-one relationship between students and teachers,
combined with a classroom management style that focuses on necessitating some unintentional sharing of teaching responsibility
managing instructional activities more than on student behaviors. (Lash & Kirkpatrick, 1990). The average student mobility rate
But certainly there are other factors that may influence these was 20% per year in the schools that we studied. This meant that
relationships. For example, the moderating effects of classroom a significant number of students had a teacher from another
management style may be an indication of how successful a school responsible for part of their instruction during any
teacher is in forming normative expectations for student discourse particular year of the study (and, of course, some classrooms had
and engagement in a classroom. Alternatively, it may be an higher rates of student mobility than others). Although it is
indication of how fortunate a teacher is to have a classroom of tempting to drop students who enter classrooms during the
students who share his or her expectations, possibly as the result school year from achievement gain models, doing so undoubtedly
of reading instruction experienced in prior grades or other distorts any estimations of instructional effects because changes
settings. In either case, the complexity of reading instruction in classroom membership can also affect the achievement of
poses a serious challenge to researchers. Even when we as nonmobile students (see Monk & Ibrahim, 1984, for an
researchers seek to develop designs that capture complexity, our interesting study of how changes in classroom membership can
theories and methods often prove inadequate to the task (Ball & affect achievement, although from the perspective of student
Lampert, 1999; Cohen et al., 2003; Raudenbush, 2008). absenteeism).
A third form of unintentional shared design involved the
Determining the Sources of Reading Instruction
public notification mandate introduced by the No Child Left
Education researchers and policy makers generally are aware of Behind Act of 2001. According to this mandate, parents must be
many of the challenges associated with isolating teacher effects on given the results of testing prior to the end of the school year so
student learning (see, e.g., McCaffrey, Lockwood, Koretz, & that families can make “informed” decisions about school
Hamilton, 2003), including the recognition that students experi- transfers and tutoring. In the district that we studied, state tests
ence reading instruction across multiple grades and adults.7 In were given in March to satisfy the end-of-year notification

MARCH 2009 103


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
requirement. Based on this single factor, every teacher in the classroom may miss important instructional influences occurring
study shared responsibility for achievement gains (or losses) with elsewhere in school.
at least one teacher from the previous year. Given the district’s
Simultaneous shared instruction. The second most prevalent form
9.5-month school calendar, this amounted to roughly one quarter
of shared teaching that we observed was simultaneous shared
of a student’s “tested” instructional time. Although there is
instruction. In this kind of design, teaching responsibility is
growing interest in examining instructional effects across multiple
directly shared within the boundaries of a lesson in a particular
years and classrooms (see Raudenbush, 2008), very few studies
time and place. More than one adult in the classroom influences
have actually done so.
student learning. For example, in team-taught classes, two teach-
Intentional Shared Designs ers, sometimes with equal status and expertise, are assigned the
same group of students during the same time period. But respon-
We also observed teachers using a range of more complex instruc- sibility can also be shared among adults with varying degrees of
tional designs during their reading lessons, quite different from expertise, such as a classroom teacher and instructional assistants,
those of the traditional “egg-crate” classroom, where one teacher resource teachers, student teachers, or parent volunteers. These
works with her class of students apart from other adults (Lortie, shared arrangements can include full-time, intensive support for
1975). Instead of having one teacher responsible for their yearly one student; occasional help for a small group of students; or the
progress in reading, many students had multiple adults who occasional modeling of instruction by a reading specialist. Nearly
shared responsibility for their instruction, especially if the stu- two thirds of the teachers in the study used an instructional
dents were considered to belong to “at-risk” populations, such as design that involved at least one additional adult in the classroom
special education students or English language learners. These (e.g., a teaching assistant, student teacher, or resource teacher).
more complex instructional designs were neither rare nor unin- What these designs have in common is that the instructional
tentional in the schools that participated in our study; rather, responsibility for the lesson is shared by two or more adults
they represented deliberate and systematic attempts by teachers within the same classroom and class period. Although assigned
and principals to reallocate instructional resources to address per- teachers may vary in the extent to which they share or delegate
ceived instructional challenges, particularly those posed by high- instructional responsibilities in the classroom, it was not
stakes testing. uncommon in the classrooms that we observed for someone
As we examined these multiple influences on student learning, other than the assigned teacher to assume major responsibility for
we began to see three broad patterns of intentional shared designs. reading instruction during a lesson, especially for an individual
We labeled these intentional designs supplemental, simultaneous, student or group of students with similar instructional needs.
and sequential. Although all three patterns are, to varying degrees, Even when a study’s design captures the relevant instructional
implicitly or explicitly recognized in the literature, there has been interactions in classrooms, simultaneous instruction raises
little discussion about their prevalence or their consequences for questions about who represents the primary source of instruction,
studies of teaching. In the 2003–2004 school year, every school the answer to which may vary by students, and how to characterize
in our study (n  18) practiced at least one of the forms of shared the mix of instructional opportunities provided to students in the
instruction, and all but two reading teachers (n  68) reported same classroom. Based on our experience, answers to these
using some form of supplemental, simultaneous, or sequential questions are neither simple nor straightforward.
instruction in their classrooms.
Sequential shared instruction. In the third pattern of shared
Supplemental shared instruction. The most prevalent form of responsibility, sequential shared instruction, a teacher is assigned,
shared responsibility was supplemental reading instruction. temporarily or permanently, to students who were previously
Nearly three quarters of the reading teachers reported that one or assigned to a different classroom teacher. Nearly one fifth of the
more of their students received supplemental reading instruction teachers reported using a sequential design during the 2003–
outside their classrooms. Although the number of affected stu- 2004 school year. Although this form of shared teaching was
dents varied substantially among classrooms, roughly one quarter observed less often in the classrooms in our study, specific schools
of the teachers reported that at least one third of their students used this instructional design more often than other schools did.
received additional reading instruction from a different teacher For example, one school reassigned nearly half of its fourth- and
in their school. fifth-grade students for reading instruction during the year;
In this form of shared teaching, the originally assigned teacher another school reassigned more than one quarter of its students.
continues to influence student learning. However, that influence In short, although the usage of specific instructional designs var-
is supplemented with additional instruction outside the regular ied within and among schools, teachers were far more likely to
classroom, for individuals or small groups, through homework share instructional responsibilities with at least one other adult
clubs, tutorials, remedial instruction, and a variety of intervention than to have sole responsibility for the students in their class-
programs. Although classroom and supplementary teachers may rooms. As a result, we found our ability to isolate the sources of
collaborate on instructional goals, the structural pattern itself reading instruction in schools far more complicated than we had
distributes responsibility for teaching reading between two or anticipated.
more teachers. The more pervasive the use of supplemental
instruction, the more difficult it is to disentangle the effects Conclusions on intentional shared designs. These patterns of shared
of instruction associated with different teachers. Study designs instructional responsibility also affected a large proportion of the
that focus exclusively on interactions that occur in the regular 1,541 students who participated in the study that year. Two thirds

104 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
of the students experienced one or more of these instructional vary substantially during an instructional day and in the course
designs during the course of the year. In other words, only one of the school year.
third of the students could be said to be solely linked to an indi-
Scope of Reading Instruction
vidual teacher for reading instruction—the typical assumption in
many study designs. The most prevalent form experienced by Because of its correlation with achievement, the amount of time
students was simultaneous instruction involving an instructional students have for reading instruction has long been of interest to
assistant (30%), student teacher (17%), staff developer/resource reading researchers (Allington & Cunningham, 1996; Barr &
teacher (15%), and/or in-class help assigned specifically to them Dreeben, 1983; Byrnes, 2000). At the beginning of the school
(8%). Nearly one fifth of the students received additional reading year, administrators typically establish a simple schedule that
instruction outside classrooms, and 7% were reassigned to a dif- shows which teachers teach which subjects, at which grade levels,
ferent teacher during the course of the year. and at what times of the day and week. But we soon discovered
Although these practices could be regarded as trivial variations that we could not rely on official documents to determine the
in a typical instructional day, we found little evidence to warrant times set for reading lessons. There were official times and unofficial
such a stance. When viewed cumulatively and across the course times, and, in some schools, the unofficial times accounted for
of a year, they underscore the potential complexity of linking more instructional time than the official times.
teacher actions to student learning in classrooms and schools. Variations across schools and changes over time in reading/
Moreover, we suspect that determining the sources of reading language arts instruction made consistency in identifying and
instruction has become increasingly difficult in the current high- recording lessons more difficult than we had imagined. Most of
stakes accountability environment, where teachers and principals our schools set aside between 90 and 120 minutes per day for a
look for ways to meet accelerating expectations for student solid block of reading/language arts instruction. In a few schools,
achievement. The main reasons given by principals for using 60 minutes were set aside for reading during one part of the day
these more complex instructional designs were to provide extra and another 60 minutes for writing in a separate part of the day.
support to both students and teachers, particularly in classes Teachers, however, also engaged in practices that extended
where numerous students were below grade level in reading, and instructional time in reading/language arts into other areas of the
to maximize the distribution of the staff ’s reading expertise in curriculum or school day. In many schools, teachers incorporated
ways that would improve student test scores (Valli et al., 2007). reading instruction into social studies or science classes, sometimes
As a result, in an era of high-stakes accountability, we should emphasizing reading more than the scheduled subject and skill
anticipate more rather than less sharing of responsibility for set. Homeroom teachers also supplemented reading/language
teaching in tested subject areas. arts instruction with what teachers called “bell work”: generally, a
review of grammar and sometimes oral reading while administrative
Determining the Boundaries of Reading Instruction
tasks such as lunch count took place.
The third challenge in studying reading instruction that we dis- In schools at greatest risk of failing to meet annual
cuss in this article is determining when and where it occurs dur- performance standards, principals circumvented the official
ing the school day.8 Although all school subjects can intersect curriculum to add supplementary reading instruction during
with each other, even in curricula that are not explicitly interdis- some part of the day. These supplementary blocks for reading
ciplinary, the boundaries between reading instruction and other typically involved populations of students thought to be most
subject areas are particularly porous (Allington & Johnston, at risk of failing to make proficiency in reading, with some
2002; Chambliss & Graeber, 2003). Students read mathematics students receiving two (“double dipping”) and three (“triple
problems, directions for science experiments, current events, the dipping”) times as much reading instruction as called for by the
calendar of daily events, and so forth. During these times, the official schedule. In one school that was on the state “watch
reading teacher, homeroom teacher, instructional assistant, par- list,” reading instruction could take up to half the day for all
ent volunteer, or even another student may give students explicit students, with specific students getting even more instruction
instruction in reading. Even when the instruction is not explicit, during the time typically allocated for specials (e.g., music,
assistance in reading or practice in reading may also influence chorus, physical education).
student literacy skills and knowledge (Byrnes, 2000; Taylor et al., As testing time drew near, more explicit teaching of reading
2002), underscoring the importance of identifying these various occurred beyond the boundaries of the official schedule in most
sources of instruction in our research designs and analytical schools. In addition to scheduled supplementary classes, some
models. homeroom teachers would conduct “test prep” reading sessions
Because of the diffuseness of reading throughout the school during bell work time, drilling students on content expected to
day, simply locating “reading” on the week’s instructional calendar appear on the state reading assessment tests. Although the school
is not a sufficient strategy for selecting times to study the teaching district encouraged schools to develop local strategies for
of reading. Researchers need to gather more data in situ before addressing the instructional needs of students, none of the
delimiting their research designs. That is, they should determine principals recorded these practices as part of the official schedule
whether reading classes or clubs systematically meet during other for reading instruction. As the time for the state-mandated
parts of the school day, who teaches them, for what purposes, assessments approached, practically all of the students received
with what programs, and for what students. Based on our some form of additional instruction, and one fifth of the students
experience, these preliminary data collection efforts are critical received intensive supplemental instruction outside the official
because both the amount and the type of reading instruction can reading/language arts block. In some schools these extended

MARCH 2009 105


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
forms of instruction occurred daily; in others they occurred two highlighted the manner in which many studies overlook
or three times a week. substantial amounts of reading instruction that students
experience (and perhaps not always to their benefit). An important
The Nature of Reading Instruction
challenge that we face as reading researchers, then, is to understand
If the nature of reading instruction remained constant across all how informal instruction interacts with official instruction to
time points, determining the scope of reading instruction would influence reading outcomes.9
be less important (although not inconsequential) to reading
Concluding Remarks
research. However, the schools in the school district we studied
offered a variety of reading programs or special reading interven- The data from our study, along with our experiences in collecting
tions that departed, often quite dramatically, from the district’s the data, indicate that it is more difficult to determine “where the
curriculum frameworks and preferred instructional practices. action” resides in reading instruction than typically is acknowl-
The conflict between preferred reading instructional practices edged by many researchers and the designs that inform their
and supplemental instructional practices became even more per- studies. Many research designs assume a simple one-to-one rela-
vasive 2 months into the 2003–2004 school year, when a new tionship among teachers, students, and reading instruction. From
associate superintendent encouraged the school district to sup- this perspective, the teaching of reading occurs during regularly
plement the reading curriculum with a reading intervention pro- scheduled blocks of time. At those times students meet with a
gram used in her former school district. The schools with the single teacher responsible for their reading instruction, and they
lowest reading test scores received a mandate to incorporate the rely on that teacher for instruction during the course of the year.
program; other schools were “encouraged” to use it. Most note- If teaching is constructed as a routine, isolated activity, such a
worthy was how the implemented program deviated from the research design makes sense—it is straightforward and undoubt-
official curriculum and preferred practices, which tended to edly a cost-effective way to study the teaching of reading. But our
encourage a cognitively demanding, constructivist approach to own work suggests that such a construction is unwarranted, espe-
reading instruction. The methodological challenge created by the cially in a high-stakes accountability environment where notifica-
intervention program was not only that it created an extended tion requirements complicate fall–spring comparisons of test
block of time for reading instruction but also that it created scores and principals redeploy resources in ways that increasingly
expectations for a set of instructional interactions very different make the teaching of reading a shared responsibility.
from those experienced by students and teachers in their regular Data from our study also indicate that the temporal boundaries
classrooms. of reading instruction are porous and become increasingly more
To illustrate the importance of studying instruction as it difficult to identify as state-mandated assessments approach
occurs outside the official time blocks, we draw on data from (Valli et al., 2006; Valli et al., 2008). The official instructional
three schools in which we conducted in-depth case studies during schedule blurs as principals and teachers seek additional
the 2004–2005 school year (Valli et al., 2008). In these schools opportunities to prepare students for the reading assessments.
we observed 11 intervention classes, 8 of which were taught by a Research designs that rely solely on the official schedule to
regular classroom teacher. A comparison between 143 regular identify temporal frameworks for data collection run the risk of
reading lessons and 56 intervention lessons indicated that the missing a substantial proportion of the reading instruction
intervention classes were less likely to engage students in experienced by students. For some students in our study, informal
cognitively demanding work, a practice encouraged in the district instructional time exceeded formal instructional time during the
and encouraged by the reading theories that informed our study school day. The informal instructional experiences of students
(Allington & Johnston, 2002; Taylor et al., 2002). Teacher– warrant further investigation, for they may well influence the
student interactions that occurred during the regularly scheduled effectiveness of practices that occur during the formally scheduled
instructional time were nearly twice as likely to involve cognitively time for teaching reading.
demanding tasks that required students to construct understanding Our experiences also underscored the challenges associated
as were the teacher–student interactions that occurred during with the complexity of reading instruction in classrooms. We
unofficial, supplemental instruction (17% as opposed to 10% of were especially interested in understanding how teachers and
lesson time). During the unofficial instructional times, students students interacted in relation to text, so we developed a mixed-
were more likely to be engaged in repetitive drill and practice methods design that allowed us to capture classroom interactions,
work designed to prepare them for the state reading assessments curriculum coverage, and teachers’ beliefs about what constituted
(40% as opposed to 26% of lesson time). exemplary forms of reading instruction. Even though we
Our growing awareness of reading instruction outside the acknowledged the importance of capturing the complexity of
classroom made us realize the inadequacy of our original data reading instruction, we were left with a fundamental challenge:
collection design, which was to record the 60- to 90-minute Which constellation of factors and conditions should we
reading lessons presented to students during the officially investigate to advance our knowledge about reading instruction?
scheduled reading block. If we were going to get a comprehensive As Raudenbush (2008) has remarked, more elaborate models of
picture of reading instruction, we would also need to study what instruction that attempt to incorporate all of the possible
went on outside the official block designated for reading, interaction effects associated with the use of resources by teachers
especially as the state reading assessments approached. Although ultimately collapse under the weight of their own ambitions. The
we were able to capture many aspects of instructional experiences task of identifying and assessing all of the ways in which the
occurring outside the official instructional schedule, our efforts consequences of reading instruction may potentially vary with

106 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
other known and unknown factors is overwhelming. Without a other hand, if desirable forms of instruction are seen as enhancing stu-
thoughtful, systematic approach to examining the complexity of dents’ depth of understanding or comprehension of text, then the chal-
reading instruction, we are not likely to build a very reliable or lenges that we describe complicate our ability to understand how
useful knowledge base, at least not in the foreseeable future. teachers facilitate such outcomes.
6We demonstrate the challenges of complexity by using a quantita-
A final challenge, therefore, and one not unique to under-
tive paradigm; however, we do not believe that those challenges are
standing the importance of complexity, is to develop a systematic
unique to quantitative methods. Both quantitative and qualitative
agenda for assessing the status of our knowledge of teaching, first researchers are concerned with making reliable and accurate observa-
reviewing the strategies that have been developed to measure tions about phenomena (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 2002).
reading instruction and then setting our priorities for future 7For a more detailed discussion of the data presented in this section,

research. We suggest at least three priorities for research: see “Who (Else) Is the Teacher?” (Valli, Croninger, & Walters, 2007).
8We limit ourselves to the complexities of studying reading instruc-
v Characterize the temporal boundaries of reading instruction,
tion within the official school day. However, outside the scope of this
and determine how best to examine the fluidity of these article, there is a broad range of scholarship from sociocultural perspec-
boundaries in terms of teacher practices and reading tives on the importance of literacy development in out-of-school com-
outcomes. munities. This literature extends back at least to Heath’s (1983) Ways
v Characterize more fully the multiple sources of reading With Words and includes work by James Gee (1996), Carol Lee (2001,
instruction that students receive, and determine how best to 2007), and Luis Moll and colleagues (Moll, Amanti, Neff, & Gonzalez,
incorporate these sources into our theories and analytical 1992). For a useful conceptualization of multiple literacies (school, com-
models. munity, and personal), see Gallego and Hollingsworth (2000).
9Although there is a body of work that examines the use of “pull outs”
v Develop strategies for examining more systematically the
complexity of reading instruction and the constellation of in schools (i.e., pulling students out of classrooms to receive specialized
instruction), particularly schools that receive Title I funding (see, e.g.,
factors and conditions that influence reading outcomes.
Chambers et al., 2000), we believe that additional work needs to be done
These are some of the priorities that we have set for ourselves as to fully understand how different instructional designs create both infor-
we continue to analyze data on the quality of reading instruction mal and formal opportunities for reading instruction and how those
that we observed in the moderate- to high-poverty elementary opportunities influence reading outcomes, including student motivation
schools that participated in our study. and interest in reading.

NOTES REFERENCES

This article is based on a paper that was originally presented at the Alexander, P., & Fives, H. (2000). Achieving expertise in teaching read-
symposium “Measuring Classroom Instruction: The State of the Art,” at ing. In L. Baker, M. Dreher, & J. Guthrie (Eds.), Engaging young
the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association readers: Promoting achievement and motivation (pp. 285–308). New
in New York City, March 2008. The work reported herein was sup- York: Guilford.
ported by the Interdisciplinary Educational Research Initiative (IERI Alexander, P., & Murphy, P. (1998). The research base for APA’s learner-
Grant No. 0115389), a combined effort of the National Science centered psychological principles. In N. Lambert & B. McCombs
Foundation, the U.S. Department of Education, and the National (Eds.), How students learn: Reforming schools through learner-centered
Institutes of Health. The opinions expressed in this article are our own education (pp. 25–60). Washington, DC: American Psychological
and do not reflect the positions or policies of the National Science Association.
Foundation, the U.S. Department of Education, or the National Allington, R., & Cunningham, P. (1996). Schools that work: Where all
Institutes of Health. Please direct all correspondence to Robert G. children read and write. New York: HarperCollins.
Croninger at croninge@umd.edu. Allington, R., & Johnston, P. (2002). Reading to learn: Lessons from exem-
1
Although in this article we focus on the challenges that we experi- plary fourth-grade classrooms. New York: Guilford.
enced in studying reading instruction, very similar challenges applied to Ball, D., & Cohen, D. (1999). Developing practice, developing practi-
the study of mathematics instruction in these classrooms and schools. tioners: Toward a practice-based theory of professional education. In
2All names used in this article are pseudonyms.
L. Darling-Hammond & G. Sykes (Eds.), Teaching as the learning
3We decided to study fourth- and fifth-grade classrooms because
profession: Handbook of policy and practice (pp. 3–32). San Francisco:
these grades represent an important transition point for students in Jossey-Bass.
terms of curricular demands. Students who fall behind in core subject Ball, D., & Lampert, M. (1999). Multiples of evidence, time, and per-
areas in these grades, particularly reading, are at considerable risk of spective: Revising the study of teaching and learning. In E. Lagemann
failure in subsequent grades (Chall & Jacobs, 2003; Chall, Jacobs, & & L. Shulman (Eds.), Issues in education research (pp. 371–398). San
Baldwin, 1990). Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
4We also developed an attribution scale for characterizing lessons, a
Barr, R., & Dreeben, R. (1983). How schools work. Chicago: University
curriculum log for teachers in which to describe the daily content of of Chicago Press.
lessons, and a series of individual and focus-group interviews to capture Bransford, J., Brown, L., & Cocking, R. (1999). How people learn: Brain,
teachers’ beliefs about teaching and the policy environment. We do not mind, experience, and school. Washington, DC: National Academy
discuss them here because they are not directly relevant to this article. Press.
5Although we believe that the challenges we discuss in this article are
Byrnes, J. (2000). Using instructional time effectively. In L. Baker,
prevalent, their importance varies with one’s beliefs about what consti- M. Dreher, & J. Guthrie (Eds.), Engaging young readers: Promoting
tutes “high-quality” reading instruction. For example, if high-quality achievement and motivation (pp. 188–208). New York: Guilford.
instruction is seen as increasing time on task, then some of the challenges Chall, J., & Jacobs, V. (2003). Poor children’s fourth-grade slump.
that we describe may be thought of as indicators of high quality. On the American Educator, 27(1), 14–17.

MARCH 2009 107


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010
Chall, J., Jacobs, V., & Baldwin, L. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the
children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. instruction of? In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, &
Chambers, J., Lieberman, J., Parrish, T., Kaleba, D., Van Campen, J., & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 545–561).
Stullich, S. (2000). Study of education resources and federal funding: Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Final report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Pressley, M., Yokoi, L., Rankin, J., Wharton-McDonald, R., & Mistretta,
Planning and Evaluation Service. J. (1997). A survey of the instructional practices of Grade 5 teachers
Chambliss, M., & Graeber, A. (2003, April). Does subject matter matter? nominated as effective in promoting literacy. Scientific Studies of
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Reading, 1, 145–160.
Research Association, Chicago. Raudenbush, S. (2008). Advancing policy by advancing research on
Cohen, D., Raudenbush, S., & Ball, D. (2003). Resources, instruction, instruction. American Educational Research Journal, 45, 206–230.
and research. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 25, 119–142. Raudenbush, S., Martinez, A., & Spybrook, J. (2007). Strategies for
Fenstermacher, G., & Richardson, V. (2005). On making determina- improving precision in group randomized experiments. Educational
tions of quality in teaching. Teachers College Record, 107, 186–213. Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 29, 5–29.
Fuhrman, S., Cohen, D., & Mosher, F. (2007). The state of education Rowan, B., & Correnti, R. (2009). Studying reading instruction
policy research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. with teacher logs: Lessons from the Study of Instructional
Gallego, M., & Hollingsworth, S. (Eds.). (2000). What counts as literacy: Improvement. Educational Researcher, 38, 120–131.
Challenging the school standard. New York: Teachers College Press. Shulman, L. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in
Gee, J. (1996). Social linguistics and literacies: Ideology in discourses (2nd teaching. Educational Researcher, 15(2), 4–14.
ed.). London: Falmer. Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (2002). Handbook of mixed methods in
Good, T., & Brophy, J. (2003). Looking in classrooms (9th ed.). Boston: social and behavioral research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Allyn & Bacon. Taylor, B., Pearson, P., Clark, K., & Walpole, S. (Eds.). (2002). Teaching
Heath, S. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in communi- reading: Effective schools, accomplished teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
ties and classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Erlbaum.
International Reading Association & National Council of Teachers of Valli, L., Croninger, R., & Buese, D. (2006, April). Studying high-quality
English. (1996). Standards for the English language arts: A project of teaching in a high-stakes policy environment. Paper presented at the
International Reading Association and National Council of Teachers of annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association,
English. Newark, DE, and Urbana, IL: Authors. San Francisco.
Lampert, M. (2001). Teaching problems and the problems of teaching. Valli, L., Croninger, R., Chambliss, M., Graeber, A., & Buese, D.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. (2008). Test driven: High-stakes accountability in elementary schools.
Lash, A., & Kirkpatrick, S. (1990). A classroom perspective on student New York: Teachers College Press.
mobility. Elementary School Journal, 91, 177–191. Valli, L., Croninger, R., & Walters, K. (2007). Who (else) is the teacher?
Lee, C. (2001). Is October Brown Chinese? A cultural modeling activity Cautionary notes on teacher accountability systems. American Journal
system for underachieving students. American Educational Research of Education, 113, 635–662.
Journal, 38, 97–142.
Lee, C. (2007). Culture, literacy, and learning: Taking bloom in the midst of AUTHORS
the whirlwind. New York: Teachers College Press. ROBERT G. CRONINGER is an associate professor in the Department
Lortie, D. (1975). Schoolteacher: A sociological study. Chicago: University of Education Policy Studies, University of Maryland, 2110D Benjamin
of Chicago Press. Building, College Park, MD 20742-1175; croninge@umd.edu. His
McCaffrey, D., Lockwood, J., Koretz, D., & Hamilton, L. (2003). research focuses on research methods, the social context of schooling,
Evaluating value-added models for teacher accountability. Santa Monica, teaching, teacher qualifications, and the distribution of educational
CA: RAND. opportunities.
Miller, R., Murnane, R., & Willet, J. (2008, August). Do teacher absences
impact student achievement? Longitudinal evidence from one urban LINDA VALLI is the Jeffrey and David Mullan Professor in Teacher
school district. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 30, 181–200. Education–Professional Development in the Department of Curriculum
Moll, L., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzalez, N. (1992). Funds of knowl- and Instruction, University of Maryland, 2311 Benjamin Building,
edge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect homes and College Park, MD 20742-1175; lrv@umd.edu. Her research focuses on
classrooms. Theory Into Practice, 31, 132–141. professional development, teacher learning, teaching practices, school
Monk, D., & Ibrahim, M. (1984). Patterns of absence and pupil achieve- improvement, and cultural diversity.
ment. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 295–310.
Morrow, L., Wamsley, G., Duhammel, K., & Fittipaldi, N. (2002). A case
study of exemplary practice in fourth grade. In B. Taylor, P. Pearson,
K. Clark, & S. Walpole (Eds.), Teaching reading: Effective schools, accom-
plished teachers (pp. 289–307). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (1991). Instructional discourses, student Manuscript received June 3, 2008
engagement, and literature achievement. Research in the Teaching of Revision received November 20, 2008
English, 25, 261–290. Accepted December 24, 2008

108 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER


Downloaded from http://er.aera.net by guest on August 23, 2010

You might also like