You are on page 1of 14

CSR for Sustainable Development: CSR Beneciary

Positioning and Impression Management Motivation

Moon Seop Kim,


1
*
Dong Tae Kim
2
and Jae Il Kim
3
1
Keimyung University, Korea
2
Korea University of Technology Education, Korea
3
Seoul National University, Korea
ABSTRACT
This study aims to seek ways to make corporate social responsibility (CSR) a vehicle for the
sustainable development of companies and society. It examines the effect of CSR beneciary
positioning on purchase intention based on social exchange theory. It also investigates the
moderating roles of product category, situation, and consumers self-monitoring level based
on impression management theory. The results show that consumers will be more likely to
buy products from those companies which employ a self-benet CSR positioning. In addition,
the ndings also suggest that the effect of the CSR beneciary positioning on purchase intention
depends on product type, situation, and self-monitoring level. Specically, when the impression
management motivation is heightened, people will pay more attention to information about
companies CSR efforts. Thus, CSR beneciary positioning will have less effect on purchase
intention. However, the effect of CSR beneciary positioning persists when the motivation is
not heightened. Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment
Received 10 February 2012; revised 25 June 2012; accepted 6 July 2012
Keywords: beneciary positioning; corporate social responsibility (CSR); impression management motivation; self-benet;
self-monitoring; sustainable development
Introduction
C
ORPORATE SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY (CSR) HAS BEEN WIDELY HIGHLIGHTED. GOVERNMENTS HAVE ENCOURAGED COMPA-
nies to pay more attention to CSR. World organizations, such as the World Business Council for Sustain-
able Development (WBCSD), call for companies to shoulder wider social responsibilities. Moreover,
consumers interests in CSR have increased, and therefore, companies are actively investing in CSR
activities as a way to fulll their societal obligations and to enhance their prot and sustainability (Smith, 2003; Cone,
2008, 2010; Babiak and Trendalova, 2011; Lo, 2010). It is likely that companies CSR activities could be an efcient
investment to make more consumers purchase from these companies, leading to increased prot and sustainability.
However, reality is different fromour expectations. Although consumers praise socially responsible companies, they
are reluctant to compensate those companies by purchasing products fromthem. In the USA, green laundry detergents
*Correspondence to: Moon Seop Kim, Professor, Department of Business Administration, Keimyung University, 1095 Dalgubeoldaero, Dalseo-Gu,
Daegu, Korea, 704701. Email: mskim1@kmu.ac.kr

This article is based on the rst authors doctoral dissertation


Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment
Corporate Social Responsibility and Environmental Management
Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
Published online 26 September 2012 in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/csr.1300
and household cleaners accounted for less than 2% of the sales in their categories (Bonini and Oppenheim, 2008).
Usually, to applaud those companies is one matter while to purchase products fromthose companies is another. If con-
sumers do not compensate companies for their pro-social practice, companies will lose their motivation to continue
investing in CSR activities (Babiak and Trendalova, 2011). Eventually, this vicious cycle will lead to a decrease of public
interests, including the consumers interests. Therefore, an effective CSR strategy is sought which triggers a virtuous
circle. Specically, if companies make a substantial commitment to CSR for the good of the public, such a commitment
is acknowledged by consumers in their purchasing decisions. This purchase, then, spurs companies CSR activities,
contributing to the sustainable development of both the company and society.
However, despite this requirement, previous research failed to go further than suggest some reasons why CSR
efforts are not inuential on purchase decisions. Existing research hints that when there is a conict between con-
sumers interests (e.g. to purchase high quality products) and socials interests (e.g. to purchase pro-social products),
consumers hesitate to sacrice their own interests for social interests (De Cremer and Van Dijk, 2002). However,
previous research has not suggested ways to prevent or solve such conicts. Also, although the low diagnosticity
(i.e. low relevance) of CSR information in purchase decisions was pointed out as another reason (Keller, 2003;
Grhan-Canli and Batra, 2004), not enough effort was put forth to nd ways to enhance the diagnosticity so that
CSR information may be considered as relevant by consumers in their purchase decisions. Therefore, this study
aims to suggest ways to exploit CSR efforts as fruitful strategic investments for the sustainable development of
companies and society. For this purpose, our research introduced a beneciary (other vs self) positioning frame
from donation literatures (Pessemier et al., 1977; Holmes et al., 2002; Fisher et al., 2008; White and Peloza,
2009) as well as impression management theory (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1964; Leary and Kowalski, 1990).
Specically, the current study examines the effect of CSR beneciary positioning on purchase intention based
on social exchange theory. Furthermore, this study investigates the moderating role of product category, pur-
chase situation, and consumers dispositional self-monitoring level based on impression management theory.
Literature Review and Hypothesis
The effect of CSR on purchase intention
CSR refers to social-obligation-related activities of a rm (Brown and Dacin, 1997), obligations of a rm to society
(Smith, 2003), and activities of a rm to fulll social responsibility (Kotler and Lee, 2005). There are mixed results
about the effect of CSR activities on purchase decisions, from a positive effect to a null or even negative effect.
Specically, some research argues that consumers tend to give incentives [penalties] to socially responsible
[irresponsible] companies and reveals that consumers are loyal to ethical companies (Matute-Vallejo et al., 2011).
Consumers are willing to purchase products from those companies (Murry and Vogel, 1997) and pay higher prices
to companies whose activities are more ethical than consumers expectations (Creyer and Ross, 1997; Trudel and
Cotte, 2009). Also, consumers are unwilling to purchase or pay lower prices for products from companies who
do not invest in CSR activities (Trudel and Cotte, 2009).
On the other hand, other research shows the opposite results. CSR activities negatively affect purchase intention when
consumers perceive that a company is investing in CSR activities at the expense of the companys core capability (Sen and
Bhattacharya, 2001). CSR activities also decrease satisfaction level of consumers when a company, lacking in capabil-
ity (e.g. innovativeness), participates in CSR activities. This is because CSR efforts are expected to help companies
acquire an institutional legitimacy in society. However, CSR activities of these companies signal problems in priority
and strategy of the companies, thus hurting the legitimacy of the company (DiMaggio and Powell, 1983).
Previous studies have identied some variables which will affect the effect of CSR efforts on consumer buying.
These variables include relatedness of CSR domain to the existing product category, consumers personal values,
corporate reputation, product quality, type of benet valued in a product, perceived sincerity of CSR efforts, and
response mode (Madrigal, 2000; Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001; Collins et al., 2007; Madrigal and Boush, 2008; Irwin
and Naylor, 2009; Luchs et al., 2010).
For example, CSR efforts have a positive effect on purchase decisions when the CSR domain of the company is
related with the companys existing products or when the CSR domain is related with corporate capability (Madrigal,
15 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
2000; Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001). Also, when consumers regard the CSR domain as attending to their personal
values, they are motivated to reward a company for its CSR efforts by purchasing products from the company
(Collins et al., 2007; Madrigal and Boush, 2008). CSR efforts increase consumers preference for such products
as baby shampoos and facial soaps when gentleness-related attributes of the product are valued, whereas CSR efforts
decrease consumers preference for such products as car shampoos and laundry detergents when strength-related
attributes of the product are valued. This is because the ethicality of a company is positively [negatively] associated
with the gentleness [strength] related attributes of products (Luchs et al., 2010). In addition, a response mode deter-
mines the inuence of CSR efforts: people are more inuenced by CSR efforts when they constitute a consideration
set by excluding alternatives rather than including alternatives (Irwin and Naylor, 2009).
Other research found that a conict between the consumers interests and social interests along with the low diag-
nosticity of CSR information in purchase decisions were two major reasons why CSR efforts have little effect on con-
sumers purchases. First, this conict comes from two apparently contradictory goals. That is, consumers purchase
behavior inherently focuses on selsh goals pursuing their own interests, while the purchase of pro-social products
focuses on altruistic goals pursuing social or public interest. Consumers generally purchase products not for social
interest but for themselves, their family, or friends, except when consumers are inherently altruistic or regard ethical
consumption very highly (Collins et al., 2007). Therefore, when consumers come to consider ethical purchasing (i.e.
purchasing products from companies who actively invest in CSR activities), they come into conict between their
interests and socials interests. Furthermore, because consumers are reluctant to sacrice their private interests in fa-
vor of social interests (De Cremer and Van Dijk, 2002), consumers positive CSR associations regarding the company
do not always lead to actual purchase (Keller, 2003; Grhan-Canli and Batra, 2004; Berens et al., 2005). Secondly,
some research, based on an accessibility-diagnosticity framework, suggests that CSR association is not diagnostic
(i.e. relevant) information in purchase decisions and does not lead to purchasing, whereas a companys capability
association is diagnostic information in purchase decisions and does lead to purchasing (Grhan-Canli and Batra,
2004). More specically, CSR is not considered for purchase decisions at hand except for some cases in which the
CSR domain is related to the companys capability, and consumers are altruistic or primarily concerned with values
represented in the CSR activities (Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001; Collins et al., 2007; Madrigal and Boush, 2008).
Even though these two reasons (i.e. the conict and the low diagnosticity) are intermingled and cannot be clearly
distinguished from one another, this research will explore solutions to resolve each reason separately. Specically,
this study investigates the effect of CSR beneciary positioning as a way to solve this conict and then examines the
role of impression management related factors (i.e. product type, purchase situation, and consumers dispositional
self-monitoring level) as a means of enhancing the diagnosticity of CSR information.
CSR beneciary positioning
According to the social exchange theory, people invest in relationships based on the reward-cost ratio. People invest in
relationships only when the rewards outweigh the costs from the relationship (Blau, 1964). Research on donation il-
lustrated that people readily donate to charitable causes when the rewards or benet for the people override the cost,
whereas people hesitate to donate when the cost outweighs the benet. Therefore, self-benet appeals which allow
donors to obtain self-benet through their donation are more effective than other-benet appeals which allow others
in need to obtain benet through their donation in increasing donation because the former appeals help people to
justify to themselves that their donation serves their own self-interest (Holmes et al., 2002; White and Peloza, 2009).
By the same logic, if the CSR beneciary is positioned as self vs other, it will become easier for consumers to justify
their altruistic purchase decision (i.e. purchases of products from companies who are active in CSR initiatives) even
though selsh goals are activated in the purchase situation. That is, the self-benet communication (i.e. you can
obtain benet through this altruistic purchase) prevents or resolves conict between consumers self-interests and
social interests, increasing the pro-social purchase intention, whereas the generic CSR communication, other-benet
communication (i.e. others or society can obtain benet through this altruistic purchase), generates such a conict.
H1: Purchase intention will be higher when the CSR beneciary is positioned as self versus other
However, this effect of CSR beneciary positioning on purchase intention can disappear when impression
management motivation is heightened and CSR information is considered as diagnostic in purchase decisions.
16 M. S. Kim et al.
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
In the following section, heightened impression management motivation will be introduced as a variable which
makes CSR information diagnostic for purchase decisions.
Impression management
Impression management refers to the process by which people manage their impression and adjust their behavior
in order to make a good impression on others. Despite the egoistic human nature, people behave in a manner re-
quired by others or by society in order to manage their impression, enabling people to maintain relationships with
others and to cooperate with others for the public interest (Leary and Kowalski, 1990).
Leary and Kowalskis (1990) two-component model of impression management suggests that impression man-
agement is composed of impression motivation (i.e. the desire to convey a particular self-image) and impression
construction (i.e. the choice of an appropriate self-image and a way to convey that image). When people become
concerned about their public self-image, this concern activates not only impression motivation but also the impres-
sion construction process. Moreover, during this impression construction process, pro-social behavior for an altru-
istic reason, rather than an egoistic reason, is selected because a good deed stemming from an altruistic reason,
rather than from a selsh reason, is acknowledged by social norms (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1964). For example,
when a public self-image concern is heightened, other-benet appeals are more effective to induce donation behav-
ior rather than self-benet appeals (White and Peloza, 2009).
By the same logic, when people consider public self-image, their impression management motivation
increases and this motivation allows people to consider social image-related aspects of products (Lee and Shavitt,
2006), including CSR information. That is, when impression management motivation is low, CSR information
is considered for purchase decisions only by some people who are inherently altruistic or who support the do-
main of CSR initiatives. Conversely, when impression management motivation is high, CSR information is
diagnostic for purchase decisions even by other people who are not altruistic or do not support the domain
of CSR initiatives. Furthermore, because pro-social behavior for altruistic (vs egoistic) reasons is normatively ap-
proved of (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1964; White, 1984; Fisher et al., 2008), heightened impression management
motivation makes altruistic (vs egoistic) reasons more inuential and dilutes the relative inuence of self (vs other)
benet positioning (White and Peloza, 2009). Therefore, it could be argued that the effect of CSR beneciary (other
vs self) positioning on purchase intention is inuenced by heightened impression management motivation. More-
over, the role of product category, purchase situation, and consumer disposition, which are related to this motivation,
will be investigated.
Product category
People use certain products to express their social identity and infer users social identity (Burroughs et al., 1991;
Berger and Heath, 2007). Thus, the symbolic, visible, and identity-relevant products are more frequently used
than the instrumental, invisible and identity-irrelevant ones for this purpose (Bearden and Etzel, 1982; Shavitt,
1990; Berger and Heath, 2007). When people purchase social identity-related products such as wrist watches,
jackets, or T-shirts, they consider whether their chosen products appropriately express their social identity.
Further, people consider the social image-related aspects of products in order to manage their social image
and impression (Shavitt et al., 1994; Lee and Shavitt, 2006). For instance, one of the success factors of the
hybrid vehicle, Prius, was that it is an appropriate medium to express a drivers identity as an environmental
leader (Maynard, 2007). As a result, CSR information, which is related to the users social image, becomes more
diagnostic or considered for the purchase of social identity-related products rather than social identity-unrelated
products. Furthermore, because good deeds for altruistic reasons are normatively approved of (Berkowitz and
Daniels, 1964; White, 1984; Fisher et al., 2008), it is normatively more highly recommended to purchase
social-identity-related products from socially responsible companies for altruistic reasons rather than for selsh
reasons.
Putting it together, it is expected that when people purchase products related to social identity, the spontaneously
heightened impression management motivation increases the diagnosticity of CSR information. Such motivation
also makes altruistic (vs egoistic) reasons more inuential, diminishing the relative inuence of self (vs other)
17 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
benet positioning of CSR on purchase intention. In contrast, when people purchase products unrelated to social
identity (e.g. detergent, socks), the impression management motivation would be low. This low impression
management motivation may neither increase the diagnosticity of CSR information nor make altruistic reasons
more inuential, thus maintaining the effect of CSR beneciary positioning.
H2: Product category will moderate the effect of CSR beneciary (self vs. other) positioning on purchase intention.
Specically, the effect of CSR beneciary positioning will disappear in social identity-relevant products while such an
effect will remain in social identity-irrelevant products.
Purchase situation and disposition of consumer
A choice situation inuences impression management motivation. When people make a decision publicly or
account for their decision, impression management motivation increases (Reis and Gruzen, 1976; Leary and
Kowalski, 1990; Ratner and Kahn, 2002). More specically, when others are watching their decision or their
decision might be known to others, and when they present their choice publicly, people are motivated to manage
their impression (Arkin et al., 1980; Schlenker and Weigold, 1992). Thus, people consider social-image-related
aspects of products including CSR information for purchase decisions. Furthermore, as shown above, when
impression management motivation is heightened by public choice situation, a good deed stemming from
altruistic reasons is selected because of its normative appropriateness (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1964; Leary and
Kowalski, 1990).
Putting it together, it is expected that when the purchase situation is public, heightened impression manage-
ment motivation increases the diagnosticity of CSR information and emphasizes altruistic (vs egoistic) reasons,
thus eliminating the relative inuence of positioning CSR beneciary as self vs other on purchase intention. In
contrast, when the purchase situation is private, the low impression management motivation may not increase
the diagnosticity of CSR information or emphasize altruistic reasons, thus maintaining the effect of CSR bene-
ciary positioning.
However, the inuence of purchase situation on impression management motivation might vary with
peoples disposition. Some people carefully monitor how they project themselves to others and adjust their behavior
in order to give a good impression to others, whereas other people pay little attention to their public image. According
to the literature on individual differences in impression management motivation, high self-monitors who are
high in public self-awareness or high in self-monitoring, are more conscious of their impressions on others,
thus being affected by social inuence (Snyder, 1974; Fenigstein et al., 1975; Scheier and Carver, 1985). In other
words, high self-monitors behave differently depending on whether they are in public or in private situations.
Specically, these high self-monitors are conscious of others presence, ask themselves about the behavior
required in a situation, and tend to behave based on these requirements in order to give a good impression
(Leary, 1995). Conversely, low self-monitors who are low in public self-awareness or low in self-monitoring are
indifferent to required behavior cues, lack the capability to manage their impression, and are reluctant to follow
the norm (Snyder, 1974).
To summarize, purchase situation has an inuence on the impression management motivation of high
self-monitors, thus moderating the effect of CSR beneciary positioning on purchase intention. Specically, in
a private purchase situation where their impression management motivation is low, they are more sensitive to
quality or price information connected with their self-interests rather than CSR information connected with their
social image. As a result, their purchase decision is inuenced by the CSR beneciary positioning. However, in
a public purchase situation where their impression management motivation is high, CSR information is considered
for purchase decisions and moreover, altruistic reasons become more inuential than egoistic reasons, eliminating
the effect of CSR beneciary positioning. On the other hand, purchase situation does not inuence the impression
management motivation of low self-monitors and hence, the effect of CSR beneciary positioning remains for the
low self-monitors.
H3: The effect of CSR beneciary positioning on purchase intention will be moderated by the purchase situation and the level
of self-monitoring.
18 M. S. Kim et al.
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
For high self-monitors, the effect of CSR beneciary positioning on purchase intention will be moderated by a
purchase situation. Specically, purchase intention will be higher when the CSR beneciary is positioned as self
vs other in the private purchase situation, whereas purchase intention will not vary with CSR beneciary positioning
in the public purchase situation. Conversely, for low self-monitors, the effect of CSR beneciary positioning on the
purchase intention will not be moderated by a purchase situation.
A description of the theoretical model and the hypotheses are presented in Figure 1.
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 intends to test whether purchase intention increases when the CSR beneciary is positioned as self
versus other (hypothesis 1) and to examine whether this CSR beneciary positioning effect is moderated by product
category (hypothesis 2).
Method
Pretest
A pretest was conducted to validate the CSR beneciary positioning information and to select social-identity-related
versus unrelated products. Fair-trade was chosen as a CSR domain because of consumers growing interest in fair-
trade products; cotton was chosen as a fair-trade product because various products (e.g. T-shirt, underwear, socks)
can be manufactured from cotton. Forty-six participants were recruited for extra credit in an introductory marketing
course and were randomly given a booklet containing either the other-benet or self-benet version of CSR infor-
mation. Participants responded to questions such as how much of the given information was related to other-benet
(To what degree is this information associated with interests of others? and To what degree is this information
focused on helping others?) and to self-benet (To what degree is this information associated with the interests
of the purchaser? and To what degree is this information focused on helping the purchaser?) on a seven-point
scale (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). These items were adapted from White and Peloza (2009). The rst
two items were averaged for the other-benet index and the second two items were averaged for the self-benet
index. Afterwards, participants rated the identity relevance level of products (i.e. T-shirt, pants, hat, and socks)
(Does a T-shirt greatly express your identity?; Can you tell a person by his or her T-shirt selection?) on a seven-
point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). These items were adapted from Berger and Heath (2007)
and were averaged for the identity relevance index.
Participants showed a tendency to evaluate the other-benet (vs self-benet) version of CSR information as being
more focused on other-benets, and the self-benet (vs other-benet) version as being more focused on self-benets.
However, such a tendency was not statistically signicant (for each, M
other vs. self
= 5.29 vs. 4.75; t(44) = 1.64, p >.1;
M
other vs. self
= 3.06 vs. 3.45; t(44) = 1.11, p >.1). Therefore, based on additional interviews, other-benet and self-
benet versions were modied to be distinctively different from each other in their relatedness with self-benet.
This modied version was used for the main experiment. The means (with standard deviations in parentheses)
Figure 1. Proposed model and hypotheses
19 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
of the identity relevance levels of the products were T-shirt = 5.13 (1.05); hat = 4.53 (1.17); pants = 4.42 (1.22); and
socks = 3.01 (1.48), respectively. T-shirts [socks] were chosen as identity relevant [irrelevant] products, and the
identity relevance level of T-shirts was signicantly greater than that of socks (t(45) = 9.15, p = .000)
Participants and design
Eighty-nine undergraduate students from a university in Korea were recruited in exchange for extra credit in an
introductory marketing course. They were randomly assigned to the conditions of a 2 (CSR beneciary: other vs self)
2 (product category: socks vs T-shirt) between-subjects design.
Procedure
Participants were given a survey booklet and either read other-benet or self-benet version of CSR information.
The text of the other-benet [self-benet] CSR information was as follows:
FT. Co. is a clothing company sourcing fair-trade certied cotton. We pay a fair price to our farmers in seven
countries including India, Peru, and Cameroon. The fair-trade agreement of FT. Co. guaranteed farmer
groups sustainable incomes, thus improving the quality of farmers lives. Also, cotton farmers receive benets
including water supplies, education, and hygiene from F.T. Co. [thus improving the quality of cotton. You can
purchase clothes made from high quality cotton from F.T. Co.].
The purchase intention of T-shirts [socks] from this company was assessed using two items (I will purchase the
T-shirt [socks] and It is likely that I will buy the T-shirt [socks]) adopted from Putrevu and Lord (1994) on a seven-
point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). Afterward, participants responded to the CSR beneciary
manipulation check items (i.e. items for other-benet; items for self-benet; and the identity relevance manipulation
check items). After answering the demographic questions, participants were debriefed.
Results
Manipulation checks
To examine the effectiveness of the CSR beneciary manipulation, a 2 (CSR beneciary: other vs self) 2
(product category: socks vs T-shirt) ANOVA was conducted on the perceived relatedness to other-benet and
self-benet, respectively. The results revealed a main effect of CSR beneciary only on the perceived relatedness
to self-benet (F(1,85) = 7.00, p <.01), whereby participants in the self-benet conditions considered the given
CSR information as more related to their self-interests than did participants in the other-benet condition
(M
other vs. self
= 3.23 vs 3.90). As expected, there was no signicant main effect or signicant interaction effect of
CSR beneciary and product category on the perceived relatedness with other-benet. Therefore, the manipulation
of CSR beneciary was successful. Also, to examine the effectiveness of product category manipulation, a 2
(CSR beneciary: other vs self) 2 (product category: socks vs T-shirt) ANOVA was conducted on the perceived
social identity relevance of each product. As expected, there was only a main effect of the product category
(M
socks vs. T-shirt
= 3.29 vs. 4.52; F(1,85) = 14.79, p = .000), indicating that participants perceived identity relevance
of T-shirts as higher than that of socks.
Test of hypotheses
To test hypotheses 1 and 2, a 2 (CSR beneciary: other vs self) 2 (product category: socks vs T-shirt) ANOVA was
conducted on the purchase intention. The main effect of product category was not signicant, but the main effect of
CSR beneciary was marginally signicant and more importantly, this effect was manifested by the predicted inter-
action effect of CSR beneciary and product category (for each, F(1,85) <1; F(1,85) = 2.79, p <.1; F(1,85) = 4.85,
p <.05). In addition, as shown in Figure 2, follow-up contrasts showed a difference in the identity irrelevant product
(i.e. socks), whereby participants displayed greater intent to purchase products in the self-benet condition (M
self
=
5.00) rather than the other-benet condition (M
other
= 3.94); this difference was statistically signicant
20 M. S. Kim et al.
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
(F(1,85) = 7.60, p <.01). Conversely, in the identity relevant product (i.e., T-shirts), purchase intention did not vary
with CSR beneciary positioning (M
other vs. self
= 4.65 vs. 4.50; F(1,85) <1). Hypotheses 1 and 2 were supported.
Discussion
These results support our prediction that the positioning of CSR beneciary as self enhances consumers purchase
intention (hypothesis 1). This is due to the fact that self-benet CSR positioning helps consumers justify their
purchase decisions and therefore, prevents or resolves conicts between their own interests and social interests
(Holmes et al., 2002; White and Peloza, 2009), thus increasing purchase intention toward socially responsible
products. In addition, this experiment supports our proposition that the effect of CSR beneciary positioning on
purchase intention is moderated by product type (hypothesis 2). Specically, self-benet positioning rather than
other-benet positioning more effectively enhances the purchase of identity irrelevant products, whereas CSR
beneciary positioning does not change the purchase intention of identity relevant products. This occurs because
when people purchase social identity-related products, they impulsively consider the image-related aspects of
products including CSR information in order to manage their social image (Shavitt et al., 1994; Lee and Shavitt,
2006). Moreover, altruistic reasons versus selsh reasons for good deeds are emphasized due to social approval
(Berkowitz and Daniels, 1964; Leary and Kowalski, 1990). Therefore, the relative inuence of self benet
positioning on purchase intention disappears. In contrast, when people purchase social identity-unrelated products,
impression management motivation is not activated and therefore, positioning of CSR beneciary as self is
inuential on purchase intention.
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 aims to examine whether CSR beneciary positioning effect is moderated by purchase situation and
individual disposition (i.e. dispositional self-monitoring level) (hypothesis 3). In this experiment, fair-trade coffee
beans were selected as a fair-trade product because fair-trade coffee beans have been attracting consumers interest
with the increase of coffeehouse chains selling certied fair-trade coffee.
Method
Participants and design
Seventy-four undergraduate students from a university in Korea were recruited in exchange for extra credit in an
introductory marketing course. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the four groups in a 2 (CSR bene-
ciary: other vs self) 2 (purchase situation: private vs public) between-subjects design. Responses of two participants
Figure 2. Purchase intention as a function of CSR beneciary and product type
21 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
who did not respond to some questions were removed. Thus, responses from 72 participants were used for the
subsequent analysis. Participants dispositional self-monitoring levels were measured as a continuous variable
and used as an independent variable.
Procedure
Participants were given a booklet and were informed that the study was composed of two unrelated tasks. Each task
had a different heading and font style in order to make each task look as if it pertained to a different study. In order
to manipulate purchase situation, participants were given verbal and written instructions modeled after the
accountability manipulation of Ratner and Kahn (2002) and White and Peloza (2009). Half of the participants in
the public purchase condition were told that their purchase intentions would be made public (i.e. discussed with
other students) and were reminded of this fact on the rst page of the booklet, whereas the participants in the private
purchase condition were told that their purchase intentions would be private and were reminded of this fact on the
rst page of the booklet.
In the rst task, participants read other-benet or self-benet versions of the CSR information of a coffeehouse
chain. This CSR beneciary manipulation was similar to the CSR beneciary manipulation used in experiment 1,
except that coffee beans were used instead of cotton. After reading the CSR information, participants reported their
purchase intention by responding to two questions which were used in experiment 1. Afterward, participants
responded to the CSR beneciary (other vs self) manipulation check items, which were used in experiment 1, and
purchase situation manipulation check items (I was not conscious of others when I responded to the questions;
I thought my response would not be made public; I was conscious of others when I responded to the questions;
I thought my response would be made public) on a seven-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). A
purchase situation index was created by reversing the scores of the rst two items and averaging these reversed
items and the last two items.
In the latter task, participants completed a ller task consisting of several unrelated questionnaires. They then
completed the 18-item version of the self-monitoring scale (Snyder and Gangestad, 1986) on a seven-point scale
(1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). A self-monitoring index was created by reversing the scores of ten low
self-monitoring items (e.g. I would not change my opinions or the way I do things in order to please someone else
or win their favor) and averaging these reversed items and eight high self-monitoring items (e.g. In different
situations and with different people, I often act like a very different person).
Results
Manipulation checks
To test the effectiveness of the CSR beneciary manipulation, a 2 (CSR beneciary: other vs self) 2
(purchase situation: private vs public) ANOVA was conducted on the perceived relatedness to other-benet
and self-benet. As expected, there was a main effect of CSR beneciary only on the perceived relatedness
to self-benet (F(1,68) = 7.29, p <.01), whereby participants in the self-benet conditions considered the given CSR
information as more related to their self-interest than did participants in the other-benet condition (M
Other vs. Self
=
3.21 vs 3.96). On the other hand, there was no signicant main effect or interaction effect of CSR beneciary and
purchase situation on the perceived relatedness to other-benet, as expected. Therefore, the manipulation of
CSR beneciary appeared effective. To assess the effectiveness of the purchase situation manipulation, a 2
(CSR beneciary: other vs self) 2 (purchase situation: private vs public) ANOVA was conducted on the pur-
chase situation index. The signicant main effect was neither evident nor any signicant interaction effect of
CSR beneciary and purchase situation (for all, F(1,68) <1). This result will be discussed in the following general
discussion section.
Test of hypothesis
To test hypothesis 3, a multiple regression was performed on purchase intention with CSR beneciary (dummy
coded as 0= other and 1 = self), purchase situation (dummy coded as 0= private and 1 = public), mean-centered
self-monitoring level, their three two-way interactions, and one three-way interaction, as predictors. Based on the
recommendations and procedures of Aiken and West (1991) and Fitzsimons (2008), self-monitoring level was used
22 M. S. Kim et al.
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
as a continuous variable instead of as a dichotomized variable (i.e. by a median split). As shown in Table 1, a simple
effect of CSR beneciary, interaction effect of CSR beneciary self-monitoring, and interaction effect of purchase
situation self-monitoring were signicant (for each, b = .43, t = 2.64, p <.05; b = .50, t = 2.80, p <.01; b= .48,
t = 2.78, p <.01). More importantly, these effects were qualied by the predicted three-way interaction (b = .43,
t = 2.31, p <.05). No other effects were signicant (ps >.1). These results indicate that positioning of CSR
beneciary as self is more effective to enhance consumers purchase intention and the effect of CSR beneciary
positioning is moderated by purchase situation and self-monitoring level.
To further interpret these interactions, simple slope tests were performed on purchase intention when
self-monitoring level was centered at 1 standard deviation above (i.e. high self-monitors) and below the mean
(i.e. low self-monitors) (Aiken and West, 1991; Fitzsimons, 2008). For low self-monitors (1SD), as shown in
Figure 3a, simple effect of CSR beneciary and purchase situation, and interaction effect of CSR beneciary
situation were not signicant (ps >.10). However, for high self-monitors (+1SD), as depicted in Figure 3b, simple
effects of CSR beneciary and purchase situation were signicant, respectively (for each, b = .87, t = 3.29, p <.01;
b= .61, t = 3.09, p <.01). More importantly, the two-way interaction of CSR beneciary situation was signicant
(b = .64, t = 2.18, p <.05). Specically, self-benet (vs other-benet) CSR positioning was more effective in
increasing purchase intention in the private condition (M
other vs. self
= 3.25 vs. 5.86; t = 3.29, p <.01), whereas CSR
beneciary positioning did not signicantly affect purchase intention in the public condition (M
other vs. self
= 5.08
vs. 5.49; t <1). In addition, the other-benet CSR positioning was more effective in the public condition (M
public
=
5.08) rather than in the private condition (M
private
=3.25). This difference was statistically signicant (t =3.09,
p <.01). However, the effect of self-benet CSR positioning on purchase intention did not vary with the purchase
situation (M
private vs. public
=5.86 vs. 5.49; t <1). Putting it together, hypothesis 3 was partially supported.
Discussion
The results of the second experiment reveal that the effect of CSR beneciary positioning is moderated by the
interaction of purchase situation and consumers dispositional self-monitoring level (hypothesis 3). More specically,
the effect of CSR beneciary positioning depends on the purchase situation only for high self-monitors. Such depen-
dency occurs because high self-monitors adjust their behavior in order to manage their social image while low self-
monitors are less inclined to change their behavior in order to manage their social image (Snyder, 1974; Snyder and
Gangestad, 1986). Specically, for high self-monitors, self-benet (vs other-benet) CSR positioning is more
inuential on purchase intention in the private situation, whereas this beneciary positioning effect disappears in
the public situation. In addition, the other-benet CSR positioning is more inuential on purchase intention in
the public purchase situation rather than in the private purchase situation, while the inuence of self-benet CSR
positioning on purchase intention is constant, regardless of purchase situation.
Standardized b coefcients t-value p- value
(Constant) 10.99 .000
CSR Beneciary .43 2.64 .010
*
Purchase Situation .15 .93 .358
Self-Monitoring -.21 1.34 .184
CSR Beneciary Purchase Situation -.11 -.51 .612
CSR Beneciary Self-Monitoring .50 2.80 .007
**
Purchase Situation Self-Monitoring .48 2.78 .007
**
CSR Beneciary Purchase Situation Self-Monitoring -.43 2.31 .024
*
Table 1. Regression analysis for purchase intention
Note.
R
2
= .54 (Adjusted R
2
= .29), F(7, 64) = 3.67, p <.01
*p <.05
**p <.001
23 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
General Discussion
This research makes several contributions to CSR literature by suggesting some ways to enhance the positive effect
of CSR on consumers purchase decisions. Most of all, our research enriches CSR literature by introducing a CSR
beneciary positioning frame which was studied in donation literatures (Pessemier et al., 1977; Holmes et al., 2002;
Fisher et al., 2008; White and Peloza, 2009). Even though previous research argues that consumers hesitate to pur-
chase products from socially responsible companies because they are not inclined to sacrice their own interests for
social interests when they experience conicts between those two interests (De Cremer and Van Dijk, 2002), there
was not enough effort put forth to nd ways to prevent or resolve those conicts. Consistent with the claims of the
social exchange theory (Blau, 1964), the current study illustrates that purchase intention increases when the CSR
beneciary is positioned as self. This result indicates that self-benet CSR communication helps consumers justify
their pro-social purchases and resolve conict between their own interests and social interests (Holmes et al., 2002;
White and Peloza, 2009). Thus, the probability of purchasing from socially responsible companies increases,
whereas other-benet CSR communication is not effective in preventing or resolving such a conict, and thus,
has little inuence on purchase intention.
Our ndings also provide a boundary condition under which CSR beneciary positioning inuences purchase
decisions, stipulating the moderating role of product type, purchase situation, and the self-monitoring level of con-
sumer. Although some research presents that CSR information is not inuential in purchase decision due to its lack
of diagnosticity for product evaluation (Keller, 2003; Grhan-Canli and Batra, 2004) and suggests some conditions
under which CSR information is diagnostic (e.g. exceptional consumers who are altruistic or concerned with values
represented in the CSR activities) (Collins et al., 2007; Madrigal and Boush, 2008), the ways to enhance the
Figure 3. Purchase intention as a function of CSR beneciary, purchase situation, and self-monitoring
24 M. S. Kim et al.
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
diagnosticity of CSR information are not adequately presented. This research introduces the impression manage-
ment theory and examines various means (i.e. product type, purchase situation, and consumers self-monitoring
level) to increase the diagnosticity of CSR information and the inuence of altruistic reasons.
Specically, this study demonstrates that the effect of CSR beneciary positioning on purchase decisions
depends on product type. When people purchase social-identity-related products, the social-image-related aspects
of products, including CSR information, become immediately diagnostic (Shavitt et al., 1994; Lee and Shavitt,
2006). Moreover, altruistic reasons become more inuential in purchase decisions because pro-social behavior
stemming from altruistic reason is publicly acknowledged (Berkowitz and Daniels, 1964; Leary and Kowalski,
1990), eliminating the relative inuence of self-benet positioning on purchase intention. In contrast, when people
purchase social-identity-unrelated products, the diagnosticity of image-related aspects or the relative inuence of
altruistic reasons do not change, maintaining the effect of CSR beneciary positioning.
Also, this study found that the effect of CSR beneciary positioning varies with purchase situation and
consumers dispositional self-monitoring levels. High self-monitors, who are consistently monitoring their social
impression and control their behavior in order to manage their social image, are inuenced by purchase situation
(Snyder and Gangestad, 1986; Leary, 1995). That is, high self-monitors tend to purchase from socially responsible
companies when CSR beneciary is positioned as self vs other in private purchase situations, whereas their pur-
chase intention does not change with CSR beneciary positioning in public purchase situations. CSR information
positioned as other-benet can be inuential only if high self-monitors are placed in a public purchase situation
compared to private purchase situation. In contrast, low self-monitors, who are inherently indifferent to their
impression on others, are not inuenced by purchase situation (Snyder and Gangestad, 1986; Leary, 1995; White
and Peloza, 2009).
Managerial Implications
This research suggests some managerial implications for companies who want to make their CSR efforts a more
protable investment for their sustainable development. Most of all, companies can use CSR as a strategic invest-
ment to increase their sales and prots by communicating the beneciaries of CSR as the consumers themselves.
Also, current research provides some multilateral CSR communication guidelines encompassing product category,
purchase situation, and consumers disposition which could be used to maximize the positive effect of CSR
beneciary positioning on purchase decision. For example, companies with social-identity-irrelevant products
(vs identity-relevant products) can make CSR efforts more inuential on the purchase by presenting the CSR
beneciary as consumers themselves in the advertisement. Also, companies need to differentiate the CSR
beneciary depending on the purchase situation (private vs public) in the point of purchase (POP). More specically,
a communication strategy to make the perception of purchase situations as public could efciently encourage high
self-monitors (vs low self-monitors) to participate in pro-social purchasing. Moreover, pro-social purchases by high
self-monitors in private purchase situations could be increased by presenting the CSR beneciary as consumers
themselves as well as others.
Limitations and Future Directions
The current research has some limitations. First, undergraduate students were recruited for this study and such
samples might constitute a limitation in testifying real-life consumption behaviors. Even though undergraduate
students are one of the major consumer segments of coffeehouse chains in Korea, further research using other
samples could enhance the generalizablility of our ndings to real consumer settings. Also, the current research
was conducted in Korea (i.e. interdependent culture) where people are more conscious of the presence of others
and are likely to follow the norm and behavior of group members more than the people in western countries,
including the EU and the USA (i.e. independent culture). Given these cultural idiosyncrasies, future research might
be expanded to examine the moderating role of purchase situation in independent cultures. It is expected that
purchase situation propels people into being more conscious of others in the interdependent culture than in the
independent culture.
25 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
In addition, the purchase situation manipulation in experiment 2, which was modeled after the manipulations of
Ratner and Kahn (2002) and White and Peloza (2009), was not effective. It is assumed that the manipulation may
have contradicted participants self-esteem to acknowledge that they were conscious of others during their responses
to the questionnaire, and thus, their responses were inuenced by the presence of others. Therefore, there is the
possibility that even though this manipulation was effective, manipulation checks, using self-reporting, may have
inadequately portrayed the effectiveness of this manipulation. Furthermore, Ratner and Kahn (2002) and White
and Peloza (2009, p.113) did not report this manipulation check and related statistics. Moreover, similar to some
research using implicit or subconscious priming (Epley and Gilovich, 1999; Berger and Fitzsimons, 2008), manip-
ulation related to self-esteem or unconsciousness dispenses with manipulation checks because the effectiveness of
such manipulation cannot be checked through self-reporting. Further research needs to use other methods to com-
plement the limitation of self-reporting manipulation checks.
Furthermore, this study used ctitious brands in order to control extraneous variables. Given that CSR activities
become inuential in consumers responses through accumulated communication over a long period of time, it is
necessary to carefully interpret consumers responses to CSR information, shown only once in this study. Future
research using eld studies with real brand name products could enhance the external validity of these results.
References
Aiken LS, West SG. 1991. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interaction. Sage: Newbury Park, CA; 4961.
Arkin RM, Appelman AJ, Berger JM. 1980. Social anxiety, self-presentation and the self-serving bias in causal attribution. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 38: 2335.
Babiak K, Trendalova S. 2011. CSR and environmental: Responsibility: Motives and pressures to adopt green management practices. Corporate
Social Responsibility and Environmental Management 18(1): 1124. DOI: 10.1002/csr.229
Bearden WO, Etzel MJ. 1982. Reference group inuence on product and brand purchase decisions. Journal of Consumer Research 9: 183194.
Berens G, Van Riel CBM, Van Bruggen GH. 2005. Corporate associations and consumer product responses: The moderating role of corporate
brand dominance. Journal of Marketing 69(3): 3548.
Berger J, Fitzsimons GM. 2008. Dogs on the street, pumas on your feet: How cues in the environment inuence product evaluation and choice.
Journal of Marketing Research 45: 114.
Berger J, Heath C. 2007. Where consumers diverge from others: Identity signaling and product domains. Journal of Consumer Research 34:
121134.
Berkowitz L, Daniels LR. 1964. Affecting the salience of the social responsibility norm: Effects of past help on the response to dependency rela-
tionships. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 68(3): 275281.
Blau PM. 1964. Exchange and Power in Social Life. John Wiley and Sons: New York.
Bonini S, Oppenheim J. 2008. Cultivating the green consumer. Stanford Social Innovation Review 6(4): 5661.
Brown TJ, Dacin PA. 1997. The company and the product: Corporate associations and consumer product responses. Journal of Marketing 61(1):
6884.
Burroughs WJ, Drews DR, Hallman WK. 1991. Predicting personality from personal possessions: A self-presentational analysis. Journal of Social
Behavior and Personality 6(6): 147163.
Collins CM, Steg L, Koning MAS. 2007. Customers values, beliefs on sustainable corporate performance and buying behavior. Psychology and
Marketing 24: 555577.
Cone LLC. 2008. Past, present, future: The 25th anniversary of cause marketing. http://www.coneinc.com [2 August 2009].
Cone LLC. 2010. 2010 Cause Evolution Study. http://www.coneinc.com/research/archive.php [5 December 2011].
Creyer EH, Ross WT. 1997. The inuence of rm behavior of purchase motive: Do consumers really care about business ethics? Journal of
Consumer Marketing 14: 421432.
De Cremer D, Van Dijk E. 2002. Perceived criticality and contributions in public good dilemmas: A matter of feeling responsible to all?. Group
Processes and Intergroup Relations 5: 319332.
Dimaggio PJ, Powell WW. 1983. The iron cage revisited: Institutional isomorphism and collective rationality in organization elds. American
Sociological Review 46: 147160.
Epley N, Gilovich T. 1999. Just going along: Nonconscious priming and conformity to social pressure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35:
578589.
Grhan-Canli Z, Batra R. 2004. When corporate image affects product evaluations: The moderating role of perceived risk. Journal of Marketing
Research 41: 197205.
Fenigstein A, Scheier MF, Buss AH. 1975. Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology 43: 522527.
Fisher RJ, Vandenbosch M, Antia KD. 2008. An empathy-helping perspective on Consumers responses to fund-raising appeals. Journal of
Consumer Research 35(3): 519531.
26 M. S. Kim et al.
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr
Fitzsimons GJ. 2008. Death to Dichotomizing. Journal of Consumer Research 35: 58.
Holmes JG, Miller DT, Lerner MJ. 2002. Committing altruism under the cloak of self-interest: The exchange ction. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology 38(2): 144151.
Irwin JR, Naylor RW. 2009. Ethical decisions and response mode compatibility: Weighting of ethical attributes in consideration sets formed by
excluding versus including product alternatives. Journal of Marketing Research 46(2): 234246.
Keller KL. 2003. Strategic Brand Management: Building, Measuring and Managing Brand Equity, 2nd edition. Prentice-Hall: New Jersey.
Kotler P, Lee N. 2005. Corporate Social Responsibility: Doing the Most Good for Your Company and Your Cause. John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ.
Leary MR, Kowalski RM. 1990. Impression management: A literature review two-component model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
107(1): 3447.
Leary MR. 1995. Self-presentation: Impression management and interpersonal behavior. Brown and Benchmark: Madison, WI.
Lee K, Shavitt S. 2006. The use of cues depends on goals: Store reputation affects product judgments when social identity goals are salient.
Journal of Consumer Psychology 16(3): 260271.
Lo SF. 2010. Performance evaluation for sustainable business: A protability and marketability framework. Corporate Social Responsibility and
Environmental Management 17(6): 311319. DOI: 10.1002/csr.214
Luchs MG, Naylor RW, Irwin JR, Raghunathan R. 2010. The sustainability liability: Potential negative effects of ethicality on product preference.
Journal of Marketing 74(5): 1831.
Madrigal R. 2000. The inuence of social alliances with sports teams on intentions to purchase corporate sponsors products. Journal of
Advertising 29(4): 1324.
Madrigal R, Boush DM. 2008. Social responsibility as a unique dimension of brand personality and consumers willingness to reward. Psychology
and Marketing 25: 538564.
Matute-Vallejo J, Bravo R, Pina JM. 2011. The inuence of corporate social responsibility and price fairness on customer behaviour: Evidence
from the nancial sector. Corporate Social Responsibility and Environmental Management 18(6): 317331. DOI: 10.1002/csr.247
Maynard M. 2007. Say Hybrid and Many People Will Hear Prius. http://www.nytimes.com/2007/07/04/business/04hybrid.html?
ex=1188273600anden=6ca51be8ad9134ccand ei =5070 [2 January 2010].
Murry KB, Vogel CM. 1997. Using a hierarchy of effects approach to gauge the effectiveness of CSR to general goodwill towards the rm:
Financial versus nonnancial impacts. Journal of Business Research 38: 141159.
Pessemier EA, Bemmaor AC, Hanssens DM. 1977. Willingness to supply human body parts: Some empirical results. Journal of Consumer
Research 4(3): 131140.
Putrevu S, Lord KR. 1994. Comparative and noncomparative advertising: Attitudinal effects under cognitives and affective involvement
conditions. Journal of Advertising 23(2): 7790.
Ratner RK, Kahn BE. 2002. The impact of private versus public consumption on variety-seeking behavior. Journal of Consumer Research 29(2):
246257.
Reis HX, Gruzen J. 1976. On mediating equity, equality and self-interest: The role of self-presentation in social exchange. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology 12: 487503.
Scheier MF, Carver CS. 1985. The self-consciousness scale: A revised version for use with general populations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
15(8): 687699.
Schlenker BR, Weigold MF. 1992. Interpersonal processes involving impression regulation and management. Annual Review of Psychology 43:
133168.
Sen S, Bhattacharya CB. 2001. Does doing good always lead to doing better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility. Journal of
Marketing Research 38: 225243.
Shavitt S. 1990. The role of attitude objects in attitude functions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 26: 124148.
Shavitt S, Swan S, Lowrey T. 1994. The interaction of endorser attractiveness and involvement in persuasion depends on the goal that guides
message processing. Journal of Consumer Psychology 3(2): 137163.
Smith NC. 2003. Corporate social responsibility: Whether or how? California Management Review 46(4): 5273.
Snyder M. 1974. Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 30: 526537.
Snyder M, Gangestad S. 1986. On the nature of self-monitoring: Matters of assessment, matters of validity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 51: 125139.
Trudel R, Cotte J. 2009. Does it pay to be good. MIT Sloan Management Review 50(2): 6168.
White MJ. 1984. Social expectations for prosocial behavior and altruism. Academic Psychology Bulletin 6(1): 7193.
White K, Peloza J. 2009. Self-benet versus other-benet marketing appeals: Their effectiveness in generating charitable support. Journal of
Marketing 73(4): 109124.
27 Csr for Sustainable Development
Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and ERP Environment Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Mgmt. 21, 1427 (2014)
DOI: 10.1002/csr

You might also like