You are on page 1of 10

ARTICLE

Predicting Stuttering Onset by the Age of 3 Years: A


Prospective, Community Cohort Study
Sheena Reilly, PhDa,b,c, Mark Onslow, PhDd, Ann Packman, PhDd, Melissa Wake, MDa,b,c, Edith L. Bavin, PhDe, Margot Prior, PhDb,f,
Patricia Eadie, PhDc, Eileen Cini, BAppScc, Catherine Bolzonello, MSpPathc, Obioha C. Ukoumunne, PhDa,c

aDepartment of Paediatrics, fSchool of Behavioural Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia; bRoyal Childrens Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia;
cMurdoch Childrens Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia; dAustralian Stuttering Research Centre, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia; eSchool of
Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

The authors have indicated they have no nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Whats Known on This Subject What This Study Adds

Stuttering involves repeated movements and xed postures of the speech mechanism. The cumulative incidence of stuttering onset by age 3 years was higher than previ-
Stuttering may severely impair communication, leading to speech-related social anxiety ously reported. Risk factors for stuttering onset explained little of the variation in
and social phobia and failure to attain occupational potential. Although stuttering is a stuttering onset. Language delay and shyness were not associated with stuttering
common disorder, the cause remains unknown. onset.

ABSTRACT
OBJECTIVES. Our goals were to document (1) the onset of stuttering and (2) whether
specific child, family, or environmental factors predict stuttering onset in children
aged up to 3 years. www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/
peds.2007-3219

METHODS. Participants included a community-ascertained cohort of 1619 2-year-old doi:10.1542/peds.2007-3219


Australian children recruited at 8 months of age to study the longitudinal develop- The project was initiated by Drs Reilly,
Onslow, Packman, Wake, Bavin, Prior, and
ment of early language. The main outcome measure was parental telephone report Eadie; together with Ms Bolzonello they were
of stuttering onset, verified by face-to-face expert diagnosis. Preonset continuous responsible for managing the project
measures of the childs temperament (approach/withdrawal) and language develop- including data collection and analysis. Dr
ment were available. Information on a range of predictor measures hypothesized to Ukoumunne provided statistical advice and
conducted the analyses in conjunction
be associated with stuttering onset was obtained (maternal mental health and with Ms Cini. Dr Reilly wrote the article in
education levels, gender, premature birth status, birth weight, birth order, twinning, conjunction with Drs Onslow and
socioeconomic status, family history of stuttering). Packman, and all authors contributed to
planning, reviewing, and editing the
manuscript. Dr Reilly had full access to all
RESULTS. By 3 years of age, the cumulative incidence of stuttering onset was 8.5%. of the data in the study, takes
Onset often occurred suddenly over 1 to 3 days (49.6%) and involved the use of responsibility for the integrity of the data
and the accuracy of the data analysis,
word combinations (97.1%). Children who stuttered were not more shy or with- and is the articles guarantor.
drawn. Male gender, twin birth status, higher vocabulary scores at 2 years of age, and
Key Words
high maternal education were associated with stuttering onset. The multivariable stuttering, longitudinal study, risk factors,
model, however, had low predictive strength; just 3.7% of the total variation in child, preschool, epidemiological studies
stuttering onset was accounted for. Abbreviations
ELVSEarly Language in Victoria Study
CONCLUSIONS. The cumulative incidence of stuttering onset was much higher than LGAlocal government area
SEIFASocio-economic Indexes for Areas
reported previously. The hypothesized risk factors for stuttering onset together RAresearch assistant
explained little of the variation in stuttering onset up to 3 years of age. Early onset CDIMacArthur-Bates Communicative
was not associated with language delay, social and environmental factors, or preon- Development Inventories
CSBSCommunication and Symbolic
set shyness/withdrawal. Health professionals can reassure parents that onset is not Behaviour Scales
unusual up to 3 years of age and seems to be associated with rapid growth in CI condence interval
language development. Pediatrics 2009;123:270277 IQRinterquartile range
OR odds ratio
Accepted for publication Apr 14, 2008

S TUTTERING INVOLVES REPEATED movements and fixed postures of the speech


mechanism and superfluous verbal and nonverbal behaviors.1 In severe cases,
fixed postures can arrest verbal communication, and associated grimacing can be
PhD, Royal Childrens Hospital, Department of
Speech Pathology, Flemington Road, Parkville,
Victoria 3086, Australia. E-mail: sheena.reilly@
Address correspondence to Sheena Reilly,

disfiguring. School-aged children who stutter suffer bullying and teasing. In later life, mcri.edu.au
stuttering can lead to speech-related social anxiety and social phobia, failure to attain PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005;
Online, 1098-4275). Copyright 2009 by the
occupational potential, and severely impaired communication.2 American Academy of Pediatrics
The cause is unknown, although it is commonly believed to be a disorder of the
neural processing underpinning spoken language, with genetic and environmental
influences.3 It is generally accepted that stuttering typically begins between 2 and 4 years of age and coincides with

270 REILLY et al
Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
advances in language development, particularly the de- ories and to underpin stuttering interventions during the
velopment of 2- to 3-word phrases. There are many preschool years.18,19 Prospective information about stut-
causal theories, but for the most part they remain un- tering incidence, antecedents and predictors, and natural
tested.4 recovery rates is urgently required to inform treatment
To date, there have been 2 prospective, community practices with preschoolers. Particularly pressing is the
studies of incidence and natural recovery. Stuttering was need to identify children in whom stuttering is and is not
examined in the 1000-family study conducted in New- likely to persist, so that differing advice (watchful wait-
castle-Upon-Tyne, England,5,6 with all children born in ing versus recommending treatment) can be correctly
that city in May and June 1947 assessed over a number targeted.
of years. Cumulative incidence up to 5 years old (ie, the This report aims to begin to address some of these
proportion of children experiencing onset by age 5) was methodologic issues by prospectively documenting the
3.5%, and 18 (42%) of 43 children recovered naturally onset of stuttering in children up to 3 years of age in a
by 6 years.6 Shortcomings of these data7 include the fact large, community-ascertained cohort of Australian chil-
that stuttering was identified by health visitors rather dren. Specifically, we aimed to describe the cumulative
than by speech pathologists, and it was not clear how incidence, age, and characteristics of stuttering at onset,
long after stuttering onset the children were assessed. and to determine any child, family, or environmental
The cumulative incidence of stuttering in 1042 pre- factors that might predict stuttering onset.
schoolers up to 3 years old born between 1990 and 1991
on the Danish island of Bonholm was 5%.8 By 5 years of METHOD
age, 71% were reported to have naturally recovered.
Limitations of this study included the fact that the chil- Overview of the Early Language in Victoria Study
dren were not assessed until their third birthday, and the The children in this study were participants in a larger
presence of stuttering was not established before the age study of early language development, the Early Lan-
of 3 years. guage in Victoria Study (ELVS), conducted in Mel-
Many antecedents and risk factors have been thought bourne, Australia (population: 3.6 million), and de-
to be implicated in stuttering onset. There is ongoing scribed in detail elsewhere.20,21 Briefly, ELVS is a
debate about the relationship between language and prospective observational longitudinal study commenc-
stuttering.9 Whereas some studies revealed that lan- ing at 8 months, followed by repeated measures at each
guage learning difficulties were not associated with stut- birthday throughout the preschool and into the primary
tering,10 others demonstrated that children who stutter school years. Infants were recruited from 6 of metropol-
have lower language proficiency than children who do itan Melbournes 31 local government areas (LGAs). The
not stutter.11 In addition, they are reported to differ on Disadvantage Index derived from the census-based So-
various temperament characteristics,12 including shy- cio-economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA),22 represents
ness,13 sensitivity,14 adaptability,14 and vulnerability.15 attributes such as low income, low educational attain-
Some have proposed that these temperament character- ment, and high unemployment. The index was used to
istics play a role in the etiology of stuttering.13 To date, stratify the 31 LGAs into 3 tiers, after which 2 noncon-
however, no study we are aware of has measured either tiguous LGAs were selected from each tier to ensure the
language or temperament before stuttering onset. There- study sampled from geographic areas across the spec-
fore, it is not clear whether these factors are associated trum of disadvantage/advantage.
with stuttering onset and, if they are, whether they Between September 2003 and April 2004, maternal
precede or develop as a result of the stuttering. and child health nurses were asked to consecutively
Our review of the literature also identified several approach parents of all infants at their universal
other potential variables thought to be implicated in 8-month visit (attended by over 80% of all Melbourne
stuttering onset. Included were prenatal and perinatal infants), followed by a formal recruitment process. Chil-
factors, birth order, socioeconomic status, parent-child dren were excluded if they had an obvious congenital
relationships, traumatic experiences in early childhood, (eg, Down Syndrome) or developmental (eg, cerebral
and growth and neurologic development.5,16 Again, few palsy) problem or other serious intellectual or physical
of these factors have been measured before stuttering disability diagnosed by 8 months of age. Participants
onset, and the reported associations are therefore not were also excluded if their parents did not speak and
well supported. We identified 1 exception, a study of 93 understand English sufficiently for recruitment and
children in the Netherlands17 in which language devel- completion of questionnaires. The final sample was 1911
opment measured before any of the children started to children. Parents reported on early communication and
stutter did not predict stuttering onset. The only predic- language development at each annual wave, and a broad
tors associated with onset were increased child speech range of other child, family, and environmental mea-
rate and reduced maternal language complexity. Chil- sures was also collected.20,21
dren in the Dutch study were selected on the basis that
at least 1 parent stuttered, and the results may not Procedure
therefore generalize to the large numbers of children ELVS participants were eligible for inclusion in this stut-
who stutter without an overt family history. tering study once they had returned their 2-year-old
Existing knowledge about stuttering onset has been questionnaires. Parents were then sent a letter of invi-
used to inform the development of causal pathway the- tation explaining the aims and procedures of the ELVS

PEDIATRICS Volume 123, Number 1, January 2009 271


Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
Stuttering Study, an opt-out letter, and a refridgerator drawn children receive higher, and outgoing children
magnet defining stuttering with examples (see Appen- lower, scores.
dix). Reminder letters were then sent every 4 months for Maternal mental health was measured using the Non-
12 months to all families who had not opted out. All specific Psychological Distress Scale (Kessler-6, or K-6),29
letters asked parents to telephone the research team if which yields scores from a possible 0 (no distress) to 24
their child displayed any of the stuttering behaviors de- (maximum distress). The remaining parent-reported
scribed (see Appendix). predictor variables were child gender, premature birth
When a parent telephoned, the nature of the speech status, twin birth status, birth weight, existence of an
disruption was first clarified on the telephone by a re- older sibling, mothers education level, and family his-
search assistant (RA). If the RA verified that the speech tory of speech, language, reading, and stuttering prob-
characteristics reported resembled stuttering or was un- lems. Finally, the continuous SEIFA Index of Relative
sure, a 45-minute home visit was arranged as soon as Disadvantage based on the participants postcode was
possible. All RAs were qualified speech pathologists and used as the indicator of socioeconomic status, with lower
consulted regularly with 2 of the authors (Drs Onslow scores representing greater disadvantage (Australian
and Packman). At the home visit, parents were inter- normative mean: 1000; SD: 100).22
viewed to clarify the onset and characteristics of stutter- Almost all of the putative risk factors identified in our
ing and to give a detailed family history of stuttering. review of the literature were collected within ELVS and
This was followed by a videotaped 25-minute play ses- in most cases before the onset of stuttering. ELVS was
sion. Parents were asked to play with their child as they designed as a study of early language development.
normally would, avoid asking too many questions, and Therefore, data on some variables hypothesized to be
reduce 1-word utterances. A standard set of toys, pic- implicated in stuttering onset were not measured (eg,
tures, and questions were used to prompt verbal output. traumatic experience before stuttering onset, physical
growth and neurologic development, and parent chil-
drearing skills).
Outcome Measure
The main outcome measure was the presence of RA-
confirmed stuttering by 3 years of age. The Lidcombe Analysis
10-point stuttering severity scale was completed, with a Logistic regression was used to calculate odds ratios
score of 1 corresponding to no stuttering and 10 to very (ORs) for the relationship between stuttering onset by 3
severe stuttering.23 Where possible, home visits were years old and the predictor variables specified earlier.
conducted within 2 weeks of the parent report. A con- Information sandwich estimates of standard error30 that
sensus panel considered all cases for whom there was allow for the correlation between responses from twins
uncertainty about the presence or absence of stuttering. were obtained. Crude ORs from bivariable models, in
The panel, comprising 2 speech pathologists experienced which each risk factor is used as a predictor on its own,
in the diagnosis and rating of stuttering (Drs Onslow and and adjusted ORs from multivariable models, in which
Packman), independently reviewed the videorecorded all the risk factors are used simultaneously as predictors,
play sessions and completed the Lidcombe stuttering were obtained. The squared Pearson correlation measure
severity scale. Stuttering was determined to be present if was used to quantify explained variation (R2).31 Scores
the parent reported stuttering and the RA and the 2 on the variables of interest for children born prema-
experienced raters judged stuttering to be present (ie, turely, defined as 36 weeks gestation, were age-cor-
scores 2 on the Lidcombe severity scale). rected before analysis. All analyses were implemented
by using Stata 9.2.32

Predictor Measures
RESULTS
Predictor measures were largely drawn from the ques-
tionnaire completed by parents as children turned 2 The Sample
years of age. Raw (quantitative) vocabulary scores were Figure 1 summarizes the participant flow between ELVS
calculated from the MacArthur-Bates Communicative and the Stuttering Study. At 2 years of age, 57 of the
Development Inventories (CDI), with children below the original 1911 ELVS participants were unavailable (with-
10th percentile for vocabulary production identified as drawn, lost contact, or moved out of Victoria), and an
late talkers.24,25 The development of communicative be- additional 235 parents contacted us and opted not to
haviors up to 24 months of age was summarized using participate in the study, leaving 1619 participants (85%
the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales of the original sample). Table 1 compares the study
(CSBS) Infant-Toddler Checklist26; standardized Total participants with the nonparticipants, with larger pro-
Scores (normative mean: 100, SD: 15) were calculated portions of children whose mothers had a degree or
according to the manual. Parents completed the 5-item postgraduate qualification among the participants than
Approach/Withdrawal scale, 1 of the 5 temperament the nonparticipants.
dimensions that comprise the Australian Temperament A total of 158 (9.8%) of 1619 parents called to report
Project-Short form suitable for toddlers aged 1 to 3 years the onset of stuttering by 3 years of age. The presence of
(Toddler version).27 Each item was rated on a scale from stuttering was then confirmed in 137 children (8.5%
1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always) and summed to [95% confidence interval (CI): 7.2%9.9%]) by a
provide a total score (maximum score: 30).28 Shy/with- trained research assistant during the initial home visit.

272 REILLY et al
Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
ELVS cohort
1911 subjects

Stuttering-study
FIGURE 1 Lost to follow-up participants Opted out
Flowchart of ELVS participants and relationship to the stuttering 57 subjects 1619 subjects 235 subjects
study.

Confirmed stutterers Nonstutterers


137 subjects 1482 subjects

TABLE 1 Characteristics of Participants Versus Nonparticipants 0.10

Variables Participants Nonparticipants


(N 1619)a (N 292)b 0.08

Gender

Proportion
Male, n (%) 824 (50.9) 142 (48.6) 0.06
Female, n (%) 795 (49.1) 150 (51.4)
Twin birth, n (%) 42 (2.6) 11 (3.8) 0.04
Premature birth, n (%) 47 (2.9) 12 (4.1)
Birth weight, mean (SD), kg 3.4 (0.5) 3.4 (0.6)
0.02
Birth order
First child, n (%) 820 (50.8) 134 (46.5)
Second child or more, n (%) 795 (49.2) 154 (53.5) 0.00

Family history 0 10 20 30 36
Age, mo
No problem, n (%) 1213 (74.9) 222 (76.0)
Speech/language/reading problems 312 (19.3) 60 (20.5) FIGURE 2
only, n (%) Proportion of study participants who stuttered by a given age. Data shown for children
Stuttering problem, n (%)c 94 (5.8) 10 (3.4) up to 3 years of age.
SEIFA disadvantage score, mean (SD) 1037 (61) 1033 (60)
Mothers education level
Did not complete year 12, n (%) 359 (22.4) 72 (25.2) when the child was stringing 3 or more words together
Completed year 12, n (%) 628 (39.2) 130 (45.5) (133 of 137 [97.1%]). In more than half the children,
Degree/postgraduate qualication, n (%) 615 (38.4) 84 (29.4) stuttering started suddenly; 51 parents (37.2%) reported
Maternal mental health score, mean (SD) 3.2 (2.9) 3.1 (2.9)
stuttering commenced during a period of 1 day, and 17
a The number of participants ranged from 1469 to 1619.
b The
parents (12.4%) over 2 to 3 days. In some children,
number of nonparticipants ranged from 242 to 292.
c Children with a family history of stuttering may also have reported family histories of speech/ stuttering was reported to emerge more slowly: 1 to 2
language and/or reading problems. weeks (37 [27.0%]), 3 to 4 weeks (19 [13.9%]), and
during a period of 5 weeks in 8 children (5.8%). Four
parents (2.9%) were unsure about the time it took for
The panel agreed that there were 21 borderline chil- stuttering to emerge. For 1 case, the data were missing.
dren in whom stuttering reports were ambiguous, and Family histories were obtained from all the children
these children were classified as nonstuttering for the in the ELVS cohort (Table 1). A more detailed family
main analyses. The median age of onset was 29.9 history of stuttering and related communication prob-
months (interquartile range [IQR]: 27.0 33.1; range: lems was elicited after onset from the families of children
12.0 36.9) and of parent telephone to report stuttering who stuttered, which revealed a specific family history
onset was 31.8 months (IQR: 29.0 34.9; range: 24.9 of stuttering in 71 (51.8%) parents of children who
42.4). The median period from reported onset to tele- started to stutter. Between 20% and 30% reported family
phone contact was 1.4 months (IQR: 0.6 3.2; range: histories of other related conditions including speech prob-
0.113.1), and between reported onset and the home lems, problems at school, difficulty with math, and diffi-
visit was 2.1 months (IQR: 1.0 3.9; range: 0.218.1). culty with reading and/or writing. Less than 20% reported
Figure 2 plots the proportion of the ELVS cohort who problems in a variety of domains including epilepsy, intel-
had stuttered by any age up to 3 years. The constant lectual impairment, language problems, motor difficulties,
slope from 25 months emphasizes that there was no and attention and concentration problems.
obvious shorter age window during which stuttering The majority of parents described the nature of their
characteristically commenced. Eleven parents reported childs stuttering as episodic (125 [91.2%]) rather than
that their children started to stutter before 2 years of age. continuous. The most commonly reported stuttering be-
Most parents reported that they first noticed stuttering havior was whole word repetition (97 [71%]). Table 2

PEDIATRICS Volume 123, Number 1, January 2009 273


Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
TABLE 2 Comparison Between Stutterers and Nonstutterers on the CDI and CSBS at 2 years than children who did
Variables Stutterers Nonstutterers not stutter, but mean shyness, birth weight, and mater-
(N 137)a (N 1482)b nal mental health scores were similar (Table 2).
Table 3 summarizes the results of the logistic regres-
Male gender, n (%) 84 (61.3) 740 (49.9)
Premature birth (36 wk), n (%) 2 (1.5) 45 (3.0) sion analyses of stuttering status on the risk factors of
Twin birth, n (%) 7 (5.1) 35 (2.4) interest showing both crude (bivariable) and adjusted
Has older siblings, n (%) 62 (45.3) 733 (49.6) (multivariable) ORs. No evidence of nonlinearity in the
Family history relationships between the quantitative predictors and
No problem, n (%) 105 (76.6) 1108 (74.8) the log odds of stuttering onset by 3 years was detected
Speech/language/reading 20 (14.6) 292 (19.7) using the fractional polynomials approach.33 In the ad-
problems only, n (%) justed model male gender, twin birth status, a mother
Stuttering problem, n (%)c 12 (8.8) 82 (5.5) with a degree or postgraduate qualification, and having
Birth weight, mean (SD), kg 3.5 (0.5) 3.4 (0.5)
a high CDI raw vocabulary score at 2 years were all
SEIFA disadvantage score, mean (SD) 1047 (50) 1036 (62)
associated with stuttering. An increase of 100 words on
Mothers education level
Did not complete year 12 18 (13.3) 341 (23.2) the vocabulary score corresponds to an increase of 17%
Completed year 12 38 (28.1) 590 (40.2) in the odds of beginning to stutter. The findings for twin
Degree/postgraduate 79 (58.5) 536 (36.5) birth should be interpreted cautiously; the OR estimate for
Maternal mental health score at 2 y, 3 (1, 5) 2 (1, 4) this variable may be unstable, because the number of twins
median (IQR) who started to stutter was small. There was weak evidence
Temperament score at 2 y, mean (SD) 15.5 (4.6) 15.7 (5.0) that children with a family history of stuttering were more
CDI raw vocabulary score at 2 y, mean 299 (157) 259 (162) likely to develop stuttering in comparison to those with no
(SD) problems or those with only a history of general language/
CDI percentile rank at 2 y, mean (SD) 48.8 (27.6) 40.6 (28.3)
speech/reading problems. Overall, the multivariable model
Late talker (score 10th percentile on 11 (8.0) 268 (20.0)
had low predictive strength, accounting for only 3.7% of
CDI) at 2 y
CSBS total standardized score at 2 108 (15) 104 (15) the total variation in stuttering onset. A sensitivity analysis
years, mean (SD) in which the 21 borderline cases were classified as stut-
a Sample size ranged from 134 to 137 for stutterers.
terers rather than nonstutterers provided similar results to
b Sample size ranged from 1308 to 1482 for nonstutterers. the main adjusted analysis.
c Children with a family history of stuttering may also have reported family histories of speech/

language and/or reading problems. DISCUSSION


The cumulative incidence of stuttering onset by 3 years
old was 8.5%, which is almost twice the percentage
shows the stuttering group had higher proportions of previously reported in other studies. Although we con-
children with each of the following characteristics than firmed several of the hypothesized predictive associa-
the nonstuttering group: boys, twins, and parents edu- tions, very little of the variation in stuttering onset
cated at or above degree level. The stuttering group had (3.7%) could be explained by the 12 risk factors we
a lower proportion of late-talkers and higher total scores investigated.

TABLE 3 Logistic Regression of Stuttering Status (Stuttering Versus not Stuttering)


Variable Crude Adjusted
OR 95% CI P OR 95% CI P
Male gender 1.59 1.102.29 .01 1.64 1.102.45 .02
Premature birth (36 wk) 0.47 0.111.98 .31 0.54 0.122.45 .43
Twin birth 2.23 0.816.09 .12 3.28 1.208.99 .02
Birth weight, kg 1.21 0.861.70 .28 1.08 0.721.63 .71
Has older siblings 0.84 0.591.19 .33 0.89 0.601.32 .56
Family history .14 .06
No problem Reference Reference
Speech/language/reading problems only 0.72 0.441.19 0.82 0.481.41
Stuttering problema 1.54 0.812.93 2.15 1.064.37
SEIFA disadvantage score (per 100-unit increase) 1.45 1.052.00 .03 1.10 0.781.54 .59
Mothers education level .001 .001
Did not complete year 12 Reference Reference
Completed year 12 1.22 0.692.17 1.06 0.581.95
Degree/postgraduate 2.79 1.644.74 2.17 1.233.81
Maternal mental health score 0.99 0.931.05 .81 1.02 0.951.09 .67
Temperament score of child at 2 y 0.99 0.961.03 .70 1.01 0.971.04 .64
CSBS total score at 2 y (per 15-unit increase) 1.30 1.081.56 .005 1.17 0.931.48 0.19
CDI raw vocabulary score at 2 y (per 100-unit increase) 1.16 1.051.29 .005 1.17 1.021.34 .02
The number ranges from 1442 to 1619 for unadjusted analyses and is 1335 for adjusted analysis.
a Children with a family history of stuttering may also have reported family histories of speech/language and/or reading problems.

274 REILLY et al
Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
To our knowledge, this is the largest epidemiological majority of those with a family history of stuttering were
study of early stuttering onset to date, and the first to unaware of it (ie, this could not have been the reason for
recruit a cohort and measure putative risk factors before self-selection into either ELVS or the stuttering study).
onset and to incorporate reliable, expert diagnoses. Finally, the study successfully captured families from
Some of our findings confirm previous reports about across the entire socioeconomic spectrum to quantify
early stuttering. For example, boys were more likely to internal predictor-outcome relationships, for which a
stutter than girls. In addition, we confirmed the reported representative sample is not essential.
rapid onset of stuttering, its episodic nature, and the This report includes onset only up to 3 years. The
prominence of repeated words at onset. Consistent with cumulative incidence of stuttering onset by 4 years will
existing retrospective evidence, the majority of parents be higher. We may have missed some cases because
reported that stuttering onset occurred during the 2- or parents elected not to call or stuttering duration was so
3-word stage of language development. brief that it did not elicit a parent report. As we continue
Reports of a family history of stuttering varied accord- our observation of this cohort into the preschool years,
ing to when the information was obtained. Before stut- we will be able to ascertain any missed cases of persistent
tering onset, 12 (8.8%) of 137 families with stuttering stuttering that were not notified by parents.
children reported a family history of stuttering. When Four factors were significantly associated with stut-
family history data were collected after stuttering onset, tering onset at the 5% level. The association with gender
many more of these families recalled a family history (71 is the best-recognized factor in the literature, but the
of 137 [51.8%]). Similar findings have been reported in associations with both twinning and maternal education
studies involving screening for hearing impairment,34 were surprising and, as noted earlier, should be inter-
and the finding may simply reflect parents being un- preted cautiously. Mothers with a degree or postgradu-
aware of the risk factor at the time of questioning (eg, ate qualification were more likely to have children that
before onset). The finding, however, does raise concern stuttered by 3 years. It could be that well-educated
about using a potential risk factor such as family history mothers were more likely to be aware of (and therefore
to identify children at risk of stuttering onset. to report) stuttering onset. It is also possible that the
The high cumulative incidence of stuttering onset of association with maternal education may be an artifact;
8.5% by 3 years old may seem surprising. We are con- stuttering onset typically occurs with the development of
fident about the estimate, because the 95% CIs were 3-word combinations, and such combinations may sim-
narrow (7.2%9.9%). The response rate was high (85% ply occur earlier in children of more highly educated
of parents in the original ELVS cohort), and stuttering mothers. Against this, vocabulary scores at 2 years were
was rigorously verified by experts using strict criteria, so significantly associated with onset of stuttering but not
we can be reasonably confident that both the majority of with maternal education,21 and clearly this unexpected
children in the population who started to stutter before association requires additional elucidation as the chil-
age 3 were identified and that the high rate was not dren mature. Our results do not support the premise that
because of false-positives. Finally, the high incidence is language problems are associated with stuttering onset
plausible: it is likely we have identified stuttering that but that communication skills and vocabulary are more
would have been missed in previous studies, either be- highly developed in children who start to stutter by 3
cause it was mild or because it was short-lived. In the years of age.
Newcastle-Upon-Tyne study,5 for example, stuttering Of clinical and theoretical interest is our finding that
sometimes seemed to last only a few months. the dimension of temperament we measured did not
Whenever a major study reports more extreme predict stuttering onset children who started to stutter
findings than previous research, the possibility of bias were neither more shy nor more outgoing than their
must be considered. We acknowledge that our cumula- peers who did not stutter. Importantly, our measures
tive incidence estimate may be somewhat inflated, be- were taken before stuttering onset by using a well-vali-
cause disadvantaged parents and infants were underrep- dated parent report instrument normed on Australian
resented in this cohort (a common limitation in children.27 Temperament is regarded as a relatively sta-
longitudinal population studies), whereas children of ble trait, but is nonetheless receptive to environmental
highly educated mothers were both more likely to stut- experiences.27,35,36 It is possible that when children begin
ter and to be included in the study. However, this does to stutter they become more vigilant and more reticent,
not alter our conclusions. Australian census data on as described by Anderson et al.12 Alternatively, it may be
mothers of children born in 2001 indicate that 43.8% that parents, who are the ones that typically fill out
did not complete school, compared with only 22.4% in temperament questionnaires, perceive their children to
our stuttering study. When we applied inverse probabil- be so simply because they stutter. Regardless, our results
ity weights accordingly as a sensitivity check, the esti- do not suggest that innate shyness has a causal role in
mated cumulative incidence fell from 8.5% to 7.5% stuttering.
(95% CI: 6.2% 8.8%), which is still considerably higher Additional study of this cohort will enable estimation
than previously reported. Other contributors to selection of the cumulative incidence of stuttering onset during
bias seem unlikely: the ELVS cohort was not originally the preschool years and identify whether stronger pre-
recruited to a study of stuttering, recruitment occurred dictors emerge with stuttering onset after 3 years of age.
well before any concern about stuttering onset might We will also be able to estimate the natural recovery rate
have arisen, and our findings indicate that the great during the preschool years and provide a much-needed

PEDIATRICS Volume 123, Number 1, January 2009 275


Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
evidence base regarding symptom clusters that are likely REFERENCES
to persist and resolve with time. The study did not iden- 1. Teesson K, Packman A, Onslow M. The Lidcombe Behavioral
tify risk factors or antecedents that could be used by Data Language of stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(4):
health professionals in the clinical case history to reliably 1009 1015
predict children at risk of stuttering onset. The known 2. Messenger M, Onslow M, Packman A, Menzies R. Social anx-
iety in stuttering: measuring negative social expectancies. J
prevalence of stuttering in adults is around 1%,37 so one
Fluency Disord. 2004;29(3):201212
significant implication of our high incidence is that nat- 3. Buchel C, Sommer M. What causes stuttering? PLoS Biol. 2004;
ural recovery could be much higher than previously 2(2):E46
thought. Parents presenting to health professionals with 4. Packman A, Attanasio JS. Theoretical Issues in Stuttering. London,
concerns about stuttering onset should be reassured England: Psychology Press; 2004
about the following points: first, stuttering onset is not 5. Andrews G. The epidemiology of stuttering. In: Curlee RF,
unusual in children up to 3 years of age. Second, stut- Perkins WH, eds. Nature and Treatment of Stuttering: New Direc-
tering onset was associated with rapid growth in lan- tions. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press; 1985:112
guage development, most noticeably the combining of 6. Andrews G, Harris M. The Syndrome of Stuttering. London,
words into phrases and short sentences. Third, children England: Heinemann; 1964
7. Ingham RJ. Onset, prevalence, and recovery from stuttering: a
who start to stutter do not seem to be more shy or
reassessment of findings from the Andrews and Harris study. J
withdrawn compared with other children of the same
Speech Hear Disord. 1976;41(2):280 281
age who do not stutter. Finally, parents should be reas- 8. Mansson H. Childhood stuttering: incidence and development.
sured that there is no strong evidence that the social and J Fluency Disord. 2000;25(1):4557
environmental factors measured in this study are related 9. Nippold MA. Concomitant speech and language disorders in
to stuttering onset. stuttering children: a critique of the literature. J Speech Hear
Disord. 1990;55(1):51 60
10. Watkins RV, Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering
APPENDIX: INFORMATION PROVIDED TO PARENTS ABOUT III: initial status of expressive language abilities. J Speech Lang
THE ONSET OF STUTTERING Hear Res. 1999;42(5):11251135
Children who stutter have trouble getting their words 11. Anderson JD, Wagovich S, Hall NE. Nonword repetition skills
out. Stuttering is when children: in young children who do and do not stutter. J Fluency Disord.
2006;31(3):177199
12. Anderson JD, Pellowski MW, Conture EG, Kelly EM. Temper-
Repeat words or syllables over and over (eg, can amental characteristics of young children who stutter. J Speech
can canI go) Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(5):12211233
Make long prolonged sounds (eg, caaaaaaaaaaaaaan I 13. Guitar B. Stuttering: An Integrated Approach to its Nature and
go) Treatment, 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins; 1998
14. Riley G, Riley J. A revised component model for diagnosing
Have speech stoppages or blocks where no sounds and treating children who stutter. Contemp Issues Commun Sci
come out Disord. 2000;27(2):188 189
Stuttering can start quite suddenly or it can begin 15. Oyler M. Vulnerability in stuttering children. Diss Abstr Int.
1996;56 (UMI No. 9602431):3374
gradually over days, weeks, or months.
16. Yairi E, Ambrose NG, Niermann R. The early months of
If you think your child is stuttering please contact the stuttering: a developmental study. Journal of Speech and Hearing
ELVS team as soon as you notice it. Research. 1993;36(3):521528
17. Kloth S, Janssen P, Kraaimaat F, Brutten GJ. Child and mother
variables in the development of stuttering among high-risk
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS children: a longitudinal study. J Fluency Disord. 1998;23(4):
This study was funded by Australian Research Council 217230
18. Onslow M, Harrison L, Rousseau I, eds. The Lidcombe Program
grant DP0557291. The ELVS was funded by Australian
2007: Clinical Tales and Clinical Trials. 3rd ed. New York, NY:
National Health and Medical Research Council Thieme; 2007
(NHMRC) grant 237106. The project was supplemented 19. Jones M, Onslow M, Packman A, et al. A randomised con-
by an NHMRC program grant held by Professors Onslow trolled trial of the Lidcombe Program for early stuttering inter-
and Packman (402763) and additional grants from the vention. BMJ. 2005;331(7518):659 661
Murdoch Childrens Research Institute and the Faculty of 20. Reilly S, Eadie P, Bavin EL, et al. Growth of infant communi-
Healthy Sciences at La Trobe University. Drs Onslow and cation between 8 and 12 months: a population study. J Paediatr
Wake were funded by NHMRC Research Fellowship Child Health. 2006;42(12):764 770
302101 and NHMRC Population Health Career Develop- 21. Reilly S, Wake M, Bavin EL, et al. Predicting language at 2
ment Award 284556, respectively. Dr Okoumunnes years of age: A prospective community study. Pediatrics. 2007;
120(6). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/120/
postdoctoral position is funded by NHMRC Population
6/e1441
Health Capacity Building grant 436914. Ethical ap-
22. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Socio-economic Indexes for Areas.
proval was obtained from the Royal Childrens Hospi- Canberra, Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2001
tal Melbourne (23018) and La Trobe University Hu- 23. Onslow M, Packman A, Harrison E, eds. Lidcombe Program for
man Ethics Committee (03-32). Early Stuttering Intervention: Clinicians Guide. Austin, TX: Pro-
We sincerely thank all the participating children and Ed; 2003
parents. 24. Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS. The MacArthur Communicative

276 REILLY et al
Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
Development Inventories: Users Guide and Technical Manual. San Equations: an orientation. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(4):
Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group; 1993 364 375
25. Fenson L, Pethick SJ, Renda C, Cox JL, Dale PS, Reznick JS. 31. Hosmer D, Lemeshow S. Applied Logistic Regression. New York,
Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative De- NY: Wiley; 2000
velopment Inventories. Appl Psycholinguistics. 2000;21(1): 32. Stata Corporation. Stata Statistical Software [computer program].
95116 Release 9.2. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation; 2005
26. Wetherby A, Prizant B. Communication and Symbolic Behaviour 33. Royston P, Ambler G, Sauerbrei W. The use of fractional poly-
Scales. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 2002 nomials to model continuous risk variables in epidemiology.
27. Prior M, Sanson A, Oberklaid F. The Australian Temperament Int J Epidemiol. 1999;28(5):964 974
Project. In: Kohnstamm GA, Bates JE, Rothbart MK, eds. Tem- 34. Russ R, Rickards F, Poulaki Z, Barke M, Saunders K, Wake M. Six
perament in Childhood. Chichester, Sussex, United Kingdom: year effectiveness of a population based two tier infant hearing
Wiley; 1989:537554 screening programme. Arch Dis Child. 2002;86(4):245250
28. Prior M, Sanson A, Smart D, Oberklaid F. Pathways From 35. Sanson A, Hemphill S, Smart D. Connections between temper-
Infancy to Adolescence: Australian Temperament Project ament and social development: a review. Soc Dev. 2004;13(1):
19832000. Melbourne, Australia: Australian Institute of 142170
Family Studies; 2000 36. van den Boom D. Do first-year intervention effects endure?
29. Kessler R, Mroczek D. Final Version of Our Non-specific Psycho- Follow-up during toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable
logical Distress Scale [memo]. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social infants. Child Dev. 1995;66(6):1798 1816
Research; 1994 37. Craig A, Hancock K, Tran Y, Craig M, Peters K. Epidemiology
30. Hanley JA, Negassa A, Edwardes MD, Forrester JE. Statistical of stuttering in the community across the entire life span. J
analysis of correlated data using Generalized Estimating Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(6):10971105

HEALTH CLINICS INSIDE STORES LIKELY TO SLOW THEIR GROWTH

The boom in walk-in health clinics located inside pharmacies, supermarkets


and big-box retailers is showing signs of slowing. Hailed as an inexpensive
option for treating minor health ailments like sore throats and rashes, the
retail clinics have grown in number to 963 as of May 1 from just 125 three
years ago. The clinics typically feature nurse practitioners who can prescribe
basic drugs, and the price for a visit ranges from $50 to $75. But in recent
months, retail health-clinic operators based in New York, Nevada, Indiana
and Alabama have closed their doors, shuttering 69 clinics in 15 states. Now,
the biggest retail-clinic operator, CVS Caremark Corp., says it is scaling back
expansion plans for its MinuteClinic brand. The cost of setting up an in-store
clinic runs about $500 000. That is one reason why much of the future
growth in walk-in health centers is expected to come from big companies
with deep pockets and from hospital systems that are already well-known
within a community and dont have to spend so much on marketing.
Armstrong D. Wall Street Journal. May 7, 2008
Noted by JFL, MD

PEDIATRICS Volume 123, Number 1, January 2009 277


Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017
Predicting Stuttering Onset by the Age of 3 Years: A Prospective, Community
Cohort Study
Sheena Reilly, Mark Onslow, Ann Packman, Melissa Wake, Edith L. Bavin, Margot
Prior, Patricia Eadie, Eileen Cini, Catherine Bolzonello and Obioha C. Ukoumunne
Pediatrics 2009;123;270
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2007-3219
Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services /content/123/1/270.full.html
References This article cites 22 articles, 4 of which can be accessed free
at:
/content/123/1/270.full.html#ref-list-1
Citations This article has been cited by 6 HighWire-hosted articles:
/content/123/1/270.full.html#related-urls
Post-Publication 2 P3Rs have been posted to this article /cgi/eletters/123/1/270
Peer Reviews (P3Rs)
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
the following collection(s):
Infectious Disease
/cgi/collection/infectious_diseases_sub
Epidemiology
/cgi/collection/epidemiology_sub
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2009 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017


Predicting Stuttering Onset by the Age of 3 Years: A Prospective, Community
Cohort Study
Sheena Reilly, Mark Onslow, Ann Packman, Melissa Wake, Edith L. Bavin, Margot
Prior, Patricia Eadie, Eileen Cini, Catherine Bolzonello and Obioha C. Ukoumunne
Pediatrics 2009;123;270
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2007-3219

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
/content/123/1/270.full.html

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2009 by the American Academy
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from by guest on April 14, 2017

You might also like