You are on page 1of 13

702576

research-article2017
JTEXXX10.1177/0022487117702576Journal of Teacher EducationBallock et al.

Article

Journal of Teacher Education

An Exploration of Professional Knowledge


113
2017 American Association of
Colleges for Teacher Education
Needed for Reading and Responding to Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Student Writing DOI: 10.1177/0022487117702576


https://doi.org/10.1177/0022487117702576
journals.sagepub.com/home/jte

Ellen Ballock1, Vicki McQuitty2, and Scot McNary2

Abstract
This study explored the knowledge elementary teachers need for one core practice: reading and responding to students
writing. Forty-five preservice teachers read and responded to an elementary students narrative writing sample. Using teacher
noticing as a framework, we first decomposed the practice into five components indicative of differences in teachers attention
to writing features, reasoning about those features, and suggested responses. We used multiple correspondence analyses to
investigate potential underlying relationships among components and developed cases to highlight one underlying relationship
that was found. The findings indicate reading and responding draws upon teachers pedagogical content knowledge. More
specifically, it draws on both knowledge of content and students and knowledge of content and teaching.

Keywords
literacy teacher education, preservice teacher education, practice-based teacher education, teacher knowledge, elementary
teacher education, teacher noticing, writing

The widespread adoption of the Common Core State Standards While the CCSS do not directly state the discourse knowl-
(CCSS) has refocused attention on writing instruction in edge children must learn, students cannot produce narrative,
schools. Previous policies, such as No Child Left Behind, informational, and opinion texts without understanding
emphasized reading and mathematics achievement and mar- appropriate genre features (Hayes & Olinghouse, 2015).
ginalized writing in many classrooms (Harper et al., 2007; Another challenge elementary children face as they learn
McCarthey, 2008). In contrast, the CCSS make writing central to write is engaging in the writing process itself. Although
to schooling, provide a blueprint for writing instruction across children can learn to both plan and revise their writing through
grade levels, and establish ambitious learning goals (Graham & explicit strategy instruction (Graham, Harris, & Santangelo,
Harris, 2015). The CCSS include Language Standards, which 2015), these processes do not come naturally. When prompted
address foundational writing skills, such as spelling, grammar, to plan, children often simply begin composing (Bereiter &
conventions, and vocabulary. The CCSS also include Writing Scardamalia, 1987), and planning does not necessarily
Standards, which require that children learn to write narrative, improve the quality of their writing (Olinghouse & Graham,
informational, and opinion texts for different purposes and 2009). Children also do not spontaneously revise, and when
audiences; use the writing processplanning, revising, edit- they do, they tend to make surface-level changes more akin to
ingto develop and strengthen a texts content, organization, editing than substantive changes that significantly improve
and style; use digital tools to produce and publish writing; and text quality (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987). In short, elemen-
use writing to analyze texts and build new knowledge. tary children have a lot to learn to attain the ambitious writing
Although the writing standards appear straightforward at goals established by the CCSS.
first glance, perhaps because they are well-organized and Attaining the CCSSs ambitious writing goals will depend
specify clear developmental progressions (Shanahan, 2015), upon skillful teaching, yet teachers report minimal preparation
learning to write is, in fact, a complex process for elementary for writing instruction during teacher preparation programs
school children. Whereas adults have developed automatic- (Cutler & Graham, 2008; Gilbert & Graham, 2010). While this
ity in basic aspects of transcription (the act of putting words is problematic, it is perhaps unsurprising given the small body
on paper), children must exert conscious effort to form let-
ters, spell words, and adhere to capitalization and punctua- 1
Gordon College, Wenham, MA, USA
tion rules. This can impede writing production (Hayes & 2
Towson University, MD, USA
Olinghouse, 2015). Children are also still developing con-
Corresponding Author:
ceptual knowledge about discourse, including the character- Ellen Ballock, Gordon College, 255 Grapevine Road, Wenham, MA 01984,
istics of good writing generally and genre-specific text USA.
features (McCutchen, 2011; Olinghouse & Graham, 2009). Email: ellen.ballock@gordon.edu
2 Journal of Teacher Education

of research on what writing teachers must know. Disciplines conceptual alignment. Much of the noticing literature ana-
such as mathematics (e.g., Ball, Thames, & Phelps, 2008) and lyzes how teachers attend to and make sense of students and
science (Abell, 2008) have ongoing, substantial lines of their thinking in the midst of classroom instruction or discus-
research into the knowledge needed for teaching. In contrast, sions. Jacobs, Lamb, and Philipp (2010) have extended this
teacher knowledge has not been a major area of inquiry among line of research to analyze how teachers read and respond to
writing researchers (Reutzel et al., 2011). The paucity of students written mathematical tasks. The teacher noticing
empirical findings offers little guidance for teacher education literature highlights three interrelated components: attention,
programs with respect to preparing teachers of writing. reasoning, and determining a response.
A growing number of teacher educators advocate taking a The first two componentsattention and reasoning
practice-based approach to understanding the knowledge and decompose what teachers do while reading student writing. A
skills needed for teaching (Ball & Forzani, 2009; Forzani, piece of writing contains many different features. Writing
2014; Grossman, Hammerness, & McDonald, 2009; McDonald, teachers must be able to focus their attention on the most
Kazemi, & Kavanagh, 2013). A practice-based approach salient features and reason about the instructional signifi-
involves first identifying the work teachers dothe core cance of those features. Current research notes the variability
teaching practices that support student learningthen decom- in writing teachers attention and reasoning while reading stu-
posing those practices to specify the special knowledge, dents writing. Some teachers attend almost exclusively to
skills, and orientations (Ball & Forzani, 2009, p. 500) needed what many teacher educators call surface features (e.g., gram-
for enactment. Decomposition involves breaking down a mar, usage, and length), those features found in the Language
complex practice into its constituent parts (Ball & Forzani, Standards. Other teachers also attend to deep features (e.g.,
2009, p. 504) so that novices can more easily learn it. Because ideas, structure, and style; Ell, Hill, & Grudnoff, 2012;
decomposition provides a template for how preservice teach- Gibson, 2007; Masuda & Ebersole, 2012), features embedded
ers will learn to enact a practice, a practice must be decom- in the Writing Standards. When teachers reason about these
posed in a way that is manageable for novices while preserving features, they may adopt an evaluative lens, focusing on
the aspects that make it effective. Once a core practice has weaknesses and errors (Masuda & Ebersole, 2012; Matsumara,
been decomposed, novices can engage in approximations Patthey-Chavez, Valdes, & Garnier, 2002). Or they might
(Grossman et al., 2009, p. 283) of it through rehearsals and read more generously (Spence, 2010), seeking to interpret
feedback from teacher educators. students purposeful thinking and meaning-making as authors.
One promising practice that is both possible for novice For example, a teacher may read this story is about. . . at
teachers to begin to master and likely to help children prog- the beginning of a childs draft, attend to the lowercase t,
ress toward the CCSSs writing goals is reading and and reason that the child does not know to capitalize the first
responding to student writing. Reading and responding is a word of a sentence. In contrast, another teacher may read the
subject-specific enactment of two core practices identified same text, attend to how the sentence announces the story
by other scholars: analyzing student work and providing topic, and reason that the child is attempting to inform the
feedback to students (Teaching Works, 2016). It is central to audience about the subject of the text.
writing instruction because it occurs regularly in classrooms The third component of teacher noticing is determining an
and can positively impact the quality of students texts appropriate response. The noticing literature focuses on the
(Graham, Hebert, & Harris, 2015). However, little research decision-making aspect of respondinga teachers intended
has examined reading and responding as a practice or how responserather than how a teacher carries out a response
novice teachers become skilled in it. (Jacobs et al., 2010). We also take this approach because
In our work with elementary preservice teachers, we have responses are likely to draw on strategies (e.g., conducting
been investigating what reading and responding to student mini-lessons, modeling with mentor texts), which in turn
writing entails and how to decompose it so that preservice might be decomposed and studied as practices. The literature
teachers can learn to skillfully enact it. In this article, we first on writing teachers highlights at least three components of
describe how we have chosen to decompose reading and response: response topics, response strategies, and response
responding as a practice and how this decomposition has agents. Response topicsthe writing features teachers
allowed us to explore preservice teachers current approxima- choose to address in their responsevary. Teacher feedback
tions of practice (Grossman et al., 2009). Then, we present the may emphasize surface edits (Matsumara et al., 2002) or
current study and how it informs our thinking about the may focus on content, process, and style (DeGroff, 1992).
knowledge needed for reading and responding effectively. Response strategiesthe form of the responsealso vary.
Teachers may suggest very generic response strategies like
editing or additional practice (Gibson, 2007). In contrast,
Reading and Responding Decomposed
they might employ more sophisticated tactics such as teacher
Recent scholarship around teacher noticing (Sherin, Jacobs, modeling, trade book modeling, guided practice, or targeted
& Philipp, 2011) offers a useful starting point for decompos- questioning to help the child consider how to clearly com-
ing reading and responding as a practice because of its strong municate with the audience (Gibson, 2007; Masuda &
Ballock et al. 3

Ebersole, 2012). Response agentswho are actively partici- picture book, and an argumentative essay. In contrast, 27 par-
pating in the responsevary as well. Teachers might offer ticipants were nearing degree completion. They had com-
teacher-centered responses that promote students depen- pleted a series of literacy courses focused on childrens
dence on the teacher or child-centered responses that pro- literacy development, literacy methods, and literacy assess-
mote students use of self-regulated writing strategies ment and were participating in their year-long Professional
(Glasswell, Parr, & McNaughton, 2003). Development School internship in several different school
The variability of teachers reading and responding systems with exposure to a variety of different literacy cur-
reported in the literature is troubling. While there is unlikely ricula. Transcripts of face-to-face interviews served as data
to be a single right way to read and respond to a given for this study. During the interview, each participant read and
piece of writing, some approaches are more likely than oth- responded to Reptile Story, a typed narrative authored and
ers to support the goals of the CCSS. For example, teachers illustrated by a second-grade student. We chose this writing
who attend and respond to a broad range of writing features sample because it was interesting to read and included
will help children develop not only language and transcrip- numerous features participants could notice and respond to
tion skills but also conceptual knowledge of writing. Teachers with respect to the expectations of the second grade and even
whose reasoning takes childrens goals, thinking, and pro- a few of the third-grade standardsfor example, characters,
cess into consideration have a broader evidence base for plot, dialogue, action, details, and conventions. We asked
diagnosing childrens learning needs. Teachers who engage participants two questions about the sample: (a) What do you
children as active participants in learning and equip them notice about the students writing? (b) What would be your
with effective writing strategies are likely providing more next steps with this student?
targeted and meaningful learning opportunities.
Our own research (Ballock & McQuitty, 2014) suggests
that preservice teachers nearing the end of their elementary
Data Analysis
education program exhibit similar variability in attention, The statistical analysis conducted for this study drew on a
reasoning, and response to that found in the literature qualitative analysis conducted for a previous study (Ballock &
reviewed above. It is tempting to jump from the identifica- McQuitty, 2014). Therefore, in this section, we first describe
tion of this problem to immediate implementation of a solu- our previous qualitative analysis and then describe the analysis
tion: to design a pedagogical response within teacher conducted for this study. In our initial analysis, we used quali-
education courses. However, we desired to pursue deeper tative content analysis (QCA; Schreier, 2012), a process used
empirical support for our decision making. Research to date to describe the content of qualitative data by systematically
allows us to describe the variability in teachers attending, applying coding frames that are both concept- and data-driven.
reasoning, and responding as discrete components. What is Drawing on the teacher noticing literature reviewed above
still unclear is the relationship among these components. (e.g., Sherin et al., 2011), we developed a coding frame with
Does each component follow an idiosyncratic learning tra- five main categories (Table 1). We identified subcategories for
jectory, or do two or more components develop in concert? Is each main category through consultation of relevant literature
each component distinctive, requiring a unique set of knowl- followed by selective coding (Schreier) of the data.
edge and skills that preservice teachers need to learn in isola- The first two main categoriesTopic and Stance
tion from the other components, or is there a common focused on participants reading of the narrative. Topic
underlying knowledge base upon which all components addressed participants attention: the writing features they
rely? The research presented in this article begins to address noticed while reading. Following precedent set by previous
these questions. In this study, we explore what, if any, rela- research in literacy teacher education (e.g., Ell et al., 2012;
tionship exists among the various components of reading and Glasswell et al., 2003), we coded two Topic subcategories: (a)
responding to student writing and what such a relationship Surface Features, such as grammar, spelling, and punctuation
might tell us about the professional knowledge or skill the and (b) Deep Features, such as ideas, sequence, details, word
practice requires and how it might best be decomposed so choice, and narrative features. Stance referred to participants
preservice teachers can learn it. reasoning about the writing features they noticed. Four sub-
categories, adapted from previous noticing research (van Es
& Sherin, 2006), included (a) Description, (b) Interpretation,
Research Design (c) Positive Evaluation, and (d) Negative Evaluation.
This study included 45 preservice teachers in an elementary The remaining three coding categoriesResponse Topic,
education program at one Mid-Atlantic state university. Response Strategy, and Response Agentfocused on the
Eighteen participants were just beginning their degree pro- participants response to the narrative. Response Topic
gram and were enrolled in either Principles of Learning, referred to the focus of the next steps participants suggested.
Development, and Diversity, an introductory education As with Topic, we applied two subcategories: Surface
course, or Writing for Elementary Educators, a course that Features and Deep Features. Response Strategy addressed
required them to write a personal narrative, an informational the teaching strategies participants suggested for their next
4 Journal of Teacher Education

Table 1. Qualitative Content Analysis Coding Frame.

Categories Subcategories Sample transcript segment


Topic/ Response Topic Surface Features It says Me and my snake, it should be my snake and I
Deep Features He ended the story with somebody living happily ever after.
Stance Description He spoke in first person the whole time.
Interpretation He may have recognized something from science and made a connection.
Positive Evaluation The content is really good.
Negative Evaluation His writing is very choppy and can come off a little bit awkward.
Response Strategy No Strategy He needs to work on more of the escalation to the problem.
Basic Strategy Focus more on the conventionsmini-lessons on those.
Specialized Strategy Do a web . . . to help focus on the main idea.
Response Agent Teacher I would show him paragraph formatting.
Student Have the kids first go through themselves and edit.
Both Make an outline with him . . . Say, Where do you want to start out with it? What do you
want to get across to your reader? And where do you want to end with it?

steps. To identify subcategories, we began with Cutler and Features and one Deep Feature), or a Broad Range (Surface
Grahams (2008) list of instructional strategies for primary- Features and two Deep Features). We determined whether
grade writing. Through selective coding of the data, we nar- each participant identified No Strategy, only Basic Strategies,
rowed this list down to two subcategories: (a) Basic or at least one Specialized Strategy. We determined whether
Strategies, which any layperson might suggest, such as edit- participants were Teacher-Focused (teacher as sole agent of all
ing, teaching foundational writing skills, and mini-lessons next steps) or Student-Focused (student as collaborator or pri-
with no clearly specified learning activities and (b) mary agent in at least one next step).
Specialized Strategies, which would require more special- This analysis approach was effective for our initial
ized knowledge, such as graphic organizers, reading to sup- research, allowing us to describe the characteristics of each
port writing, writing prompts, goal-oriented writing individual participants reading and responding and to com-
conferences, and clearly specified learning activities. Finally, pare participants within each coding category. However, it
Response Agent referred to who would carry out the sug- was insufficient for the purposes of this study: exploring the
gested next steps. For this category, we identified three sub- interrelationship of the five coding categories to look for
categories, adapted from noticing research (van Es & Sherin, underlying structures that might shed light on reading and
2006): Teacher, Student, or Both. responding as a practice. Several factors guided our choice
Focusing on one category at a time, the first two authors of method for this analysis. First, because we entered the
independently segmented each transcript into coding units study with the goal of discovery instead of confirmation, we
using thematic criterion (Schreier, 2012). Then, we coded required a procedure that could reveal multivariate structure
each transcript, applying only one code to each segment. rather than confirm a priori hypotheses, as one might with
Overall interrater reliability was 90%. We resolved coding structural equation modeling. Second, because we were
conflicts through consensus. interested in exploring both the clustering of participants and
Coded transcripts served as the basis for describing partici- categories, we needed a procedure that could be both person-
pants skill in each component of reading and responding, the centered and variable-centered in approach. Neither Factor
goal of the previous study. We first focused on Topic. Because Analysis nor Cluster Analysis would allow us to uncover
every participant noticed a variety of Surface Features, we person and variable clusters simultaneously. Third, because
focused on describing participants attention to Deep Features. our data are categorical instead of continuous, we needed a
We identified whether each participant attended to a Narrow method that was flexible enough to accommodate the granu-
Range (0-2 Deep Features), a Medium Range (3-4 Deep larity of our data. Factor Analysis and Cluster Analysis
Features), or a Broad Range (5 or more Deep Features). We assume data are at least continuous in some sense and gener-
then determined each participants primary stance toward the ally require larger samples sizes than what we have. In con-
narrative: (a) a Negative Stance (50% or more of word count trast, multiple correspondence analysis (MCA) is useful for
coded as Negative Evaluation), (b) a Positive Stance (50% or discovering clusters of persons/variables and accommodates
more of word count coded as Positive Evaluation), or (c) a categorical data. Therefore, we elected to use MCA for this
Broad Stance (all subcategories accounted for less than 50% study. We used each coding category from the qualitative
of the word count). Next, we characterized each participants analysis (e.g., Stance), and the values assigned to transcripts
response. We identified whether each participant suggested for each category (e.g., Negative, Positive, Broad) as cate-
No Topic, only Surface Features, a Medium Range (Surface gorical variables for the MCA.
Ballock et al. 5

MCA is a variant of correspondence analysis (CA), an Response Topic, Response Strategy, and Response Agent),
analysis technique useful for discovering whether particular we treated each coding category as an active categorical vari-
categories of the variables tend to co-occur in a way that able in the MCA. We carefully assessed the meaningfulness
reveals common meaning. It is sometimes referred to as a of each identified dimension through triangulation of MCA
principal components analysis for nominally scaled variables outputs, QCA results, and careful re-reading of select tran-
(Clausen, 1998). MCA is an extension of CA from the case scripts. Finally, we added two supplementary variables to the
of two variables in a contingency table to the case in which MCA to aid interpretation: (a) Transcript Length (four cate-
there are multiple categorical variables in higher order con- gories; quartiles of total transcript word count) and (b)
tingency tables. Program Status (Early, meaning enrolled in initial course-
Any underlying structure between two variables discov- work, or Late, meaning enrolled in the final internship year).
ered through CA is based on the association between the
various categories of one variable and the categories of a sec-
ond variable. The observed association may reflect a shared
Findings
feature, or features. Extending CA to MCA, categories of The purpose of this study was to explore what preservice
multiple variables may collectively be associated, because of teachers need to learn to effectively read and respond to stu-
shared features across multiple variables. Whereas shared dent writing. To do so, we explored possible interrelation-
variable associations are called factors in Factor Analysis, in ships among categories of attending, reasoning, and
CA/MCA, these shared features are called dimensions. responding. MCA allowed us to investigate the likelihood
MCA proves advantageous to this study because it can that certain characteristics of reading and responding would
represent each categorical variable and participant simulta- coexist in the same participant. For example, would partici-
neously in a multivariate space which spans the number of pants who notice a Broad Range of Topics be likely to also
dimensions that are extracted. However, careful interpreta- suggest a Broad Range of Response Topics? Or would par-
tion is necessary to assess the meaningfulness of each identi- ticipants who take a Positive Evaluative Stance toward the
fied dimension, because it is possible that one or more narrative be likely to recommend similar Response
dimensions identified through MCA may simply be the result Strategies? Are there particular ways of attending, reasoning,
of chance associations among the data. Clausen (1998) sug- and suggesting responses that cohere?
gests that quantitative indices, such as Contribution and
Squared Correlation, are the first tools to aid interpretation.
Categories with large Contribution values strongly influence
MCA Dimensions
the placement of dimension axes, while categories with large The five QCA categories (i.e., Topic, Stance, Response
values of Squared Correlation are those categories particu- Topic, Response Strategy, Response Agent) were submitted
larly well explained by a dimension. Visual displays, such as as active variables to MCA. Each variable consisted of two
scatterplots, also guide interpretation. CA/MCA results are to five categories, collectively creating a 45 (person) 16
often displayed visually as a scatterplot with dimensions as (categories of variables) table for analysis. Extraction of two
axes and variable categories and/or participants as points on dimensions accounted for 37% of the variance across partici-
the scatterplot. This results in a spatial representation of (a) pants, 22% for Dimension 1 and 15% for Dimension 2.
the interrelationships among variables and categories and (b) While not large, the proportion of variance explained sug-
how participants relate to those interrelationships. For exam- gests that intercorrelations among categories are potentially
ple, variable categories which tend to co-occur in the same interpretable. MCA output identified a third dimension; how-
interview transcript would, through MCA, be located in ever, it did not explain sufficient variance to warrant inter-
close proximity to each other on the scatterplot, whereas cat- pretation. Table 2 displays quantitative indices of the
egories unlikely to co-occur would be located further away relationship between variable categories and dimensions,
from each other. Similarly, associations of individual partici- whereas Figure 1 visually represents the MCA output as a set
pants with categories or dimensions are observable by virtue of scatterplots. In each scatterplot, categories are plotted to
of their location in the scatterplot. The alignment of variable show how strongly associated they are with each dimension.
categories with dimensions reveals structure because it is Confidence ellipses reflect differences between centroids of cat-
assumed that correlations among variables and categories egories with respect to dimension association. Nonoverlapping
reflect common threads of meaning. Supplementary vari- ellipses imply categories which are spatially significantly
ables, though not direct contributors to the MCA dimension different from each other on the two dimensions. Finally,
coordinate solutions, can also be plotted and used as confirma- points indicate where individual participants fall with respect
tory evidence of dimension meanings. We used FactoMineR to the dimensions.
(Husson, Josse, Le, & Mazet, 2015), a software package for Because our interpretation of Dimension 2 is tentative and
R (R Core Team, 2016), to conduct the MCA. needs further investigation, we focus this report of findings
As we sought to explore underlying structures or associa- on Dimension 1. We describe how we used a combination of
tions among QCA coding categories (Topic, Stance, MCA outputs and three cases selected based on their MCA
6 Journal of Teacher Education

Table 2. Multiple Correspondence Analysis Coefficients for Two Extracted Dimensions.

Dimension 1 Dimension 2

Squared Squared
Coordinate Contribution correlation V test Coordinate Contribution correlation V test
Topic
Narrow range 0.54 0.04 0.13 2.42 0.78 0.11 0.27 3.47
Medium range 0.41 0.02 0.09 1.94 0.83 0.14 0.34 3.88
Broad range 0.86 0.11 0.41 4.25 0.10 0.00 0.01 0.47
Stance
Negative 0.16 0.01 0.06 1.62 0.15 0.01 0.05 1.43
Positive 1.08 0.07 0.22 3.07 0.49 0.02 0.04 1.39
Interpretive 0.41 0.00 0.01 0.73 0.80 0.03 0.05 1.42
Broad 0.93 0.03 0.08 1.92 1.38 0.10 0.18 2.85
Response Topic
No topic 0.75 0.01 0.03 1.08 0.72 0.01 0.02 1.03
Surface features 0.63 0.08 0.38 4.07 0.27 0.02 0.07 1.72
Medium range 0.12 0.00 0.00 0.44 0.99 0.15 0.32 3.75
Broad range 1.40 0.18 0.56 4.98 0.65 0.06 0.12 2.32
Response Strategy
No strategy 0.99 0.09 0.28 3.52 0.87 0.10 0.22 3.08
Basic strategies 0.25 0.01 0.07 1.69 0.72 0.16 0.54 4.85
Specialized strategies 1.31 0.19 0.62 5.22 0.65 0.07 0.15 2.59
Agent
Teacher focused 0.53 0.07 0.39 4.12 0.14 0.01 0.03 1.05
One or more student 0.73 0.09 0.39 4.12 0.19 0.01 0.03 1.05
focused

dimension coordinates as the basis for interpreting Dimension further along in their degree program (Program Status, Late
1. Then, we articulate our conclusions as to the meaning of Program Status; Figure 1) and, therefore, had completed
Dimension 1. more literacy coursework and field placements. We would
expect these participants to have more professional knowl-
Dimension 1.The spatial organization of categoriesindi- edge and experience to draw on when reading and respond-
cated by the sign and magnitude of the Coordinate column ing and therefore to exhibit greater sophistication. Participants
(Table 2) and visual inspection of categories plotted across plotted near the positive pole also had more to say about the
the horizontal axis of each scatterplot (Figure 1)provides narrative sample, as indicated by longer transcript length
initial evidence for interpreting Dimension 1. Five categories (Transcript Length, Figure 1). While verbosity should not be
are clearly associated with the positive poleBroad Topic, conflated with sophistication, review of the data shows that
Broad Stance, Broad Response Topic, Specialized Strategies, participants speaking fewer than 300 words attended and
and Student-Focused Response Agentwhile another six responded to a narrow or medium range of features and only
categories are clearly associated with the negative pole one suggested a specialized student-focused strategy, while
Narrow and Medium Topic, Positive Evaluative Stance, No participants speaking over 1,000 words all attended to a
Response Topic, Surface Features Only, No Response Strat- broad range of features and suggested specialized student-
egy, and Teacher-Focused Response Agent. The positioning focused strategies.
of the most sophisticated categories and least sophisticated Contribution and Squared Correlation coefficients (Table
categories of each variable at opposite poles leads us to infer 2) also aid interpretation. Although Contribution and Squared
that Dimension 1 represents a continuum of sophistication in Correlation coefficients tend to vary together, they reflect
reading and responding. different points of view. Since Contribution values sum to
Supplemental variables add support to this interpretation. 1.00 down columns, they reflect the relative importance of
This is important as supplementary variables were not used categories to a dimension. Two categories are the strongest
to compute dimension coordinates but were plotted after determinants of Dimension 1: Specialized Response
dimension solutions had already been established. First, par- Strategies (0.19) and Broad Response Topic (0.18). In con-
ticipants plotted near the positive pole were more likely to be trast, Squared Correlations indicate the extent to which
Ballock et al. 7

Figure 1. Multiple Correspondence Analysis Scatterplots.

Table 3. Reading and Responding Profiles.

Profile 1 (n = 18) Profile 2 (n = 14) Profile 3 (n = 13)


Topic Narrow or Medium Range Medium or Broad Range Broad Range
Stance Positive or Negative Evaluation Negative Evaluation Negative Evaluation
Response Topic None or Surface Features Surface Features or Medium Range Broad Range
Response Strategy None or Basic Basic Specialized
Response Agent Teacher-Focused Teacher-Focused or Student-Focused Student-Focused

dimensions explain categories. Three categories are particu- within each profile, it is clear that Profile 2 participants focus
larly well explained by Dimension 1: Specialized Response on more deep features than Profile 1 participants, and Profile
Strategies (0.62), Broad Response Topic (0.56), and Broad 3 participants are characterized by an even broader focus on
Topic (0.41). V test values greater than |2.00| indicate coordi- deep features. Profile 3 participants are unique in suggesting
nates significantly different from zero (Le, Josse, & Husson, specialized response strategies. Here, we present case
2008). These data suggest that three variables are particu- descriptions to illustrate each profile. Then, we present and
larly key to interpreting Dimension 1: Topic, Response discuss our conclusions as to what Dimension 1 represents.
Topic, and Response Strategies.
As a final aid to Dimension 1 interpretation, we devel- Profile 1 case. Typical of participants in Profile 1, Megan
oped three case descriptions. To select the three cases, we attended to a narrow range of writing features:
examined the location of individual participants along the
Some of the sentences needed periods, and he didnt add plurals
Dimension 1 axis. Paying particular attention to the three to the ends of some nouns. He needs to work on the present tense
variables noted above (i.e., Topic, Response Topic, and and the past tense. And endings. I saw something that needed an
Response Strategies), we noted substantive qualitative dif- ed at the end. He would say me and my snake pretty often.
ferences among participants at two points along the But . . . he understands how to use I sometimes . . . He knows
Dimension 1 axis. This allowed us to group participants into how to use dialogue, I think. He knows to put the quotation
three profiles (Table 3). Despite the considerable diversity marks after a punctuation mark. And then he knows to capitalize
8 Journal of Teacher Education

the letter. He knows how to use his commas when hes going and usage errors. However, she took a more interpretive
into a dialogue. stance toward the picture. She did not evaluate it even though
she did not fully understand it. Instead, she wanted to know
Megans comments exclusively addressed surface fea- more about the childs intended meaning and suggested ask-
tures such as punctuation, plurality, tense, capitalization, and ing him what he intended the picture to communicate. This
sentence structure. She seemed to interpret the research task shows a beginning acknowledgment of the child author as a
as a directive to assess the writings correctness, evaluating communicator with an intentional thought process.
strengths and errors almost equally. Although Megan had Lisas remaining instructional suggestions addressed sur-
recently studied features of effective narratives to support face features. Noteworthy, however, is Lisas identification
her own narrative writing in the Writing for Elementary of grammar as the main issue. This language suggests a
Teachers course, she did not note any of these deep features weighing and prioritizing of features that might lead to
in the students narrative. improvement. In the previous case, Megan prioritized certain
Given Megans focus on surface features and evaluative surface features over others in her suggested response, but
stance, it is not surprising that her next steps emphasized gave no indication she noticed any deep features. In contrast,
instruction to correct mistakes: Lisa noticed other features, but indicated she felt the conven-
tions were the most important moving forward. Lisa indi-
When he said, Can I borrow this snake for a week, and then he cated she would go over, have a mini-lesson . . . where he
says nowhen he says no he needs to say who is saying could try to make it better. Then she added, I think pulling
that. I would help him with making complete sentences, not
him aside and going over the tenses and when to use plurals
run-on sentences. I would help him with run-on sentences,
or just figuring that all out. Like Megan, Lisa focused on
because I think he has some of those. And then I would help him
with using the past and present tense. correcting the writing to make it better. She offered basic
unelaborated teaching strategies, simply indicating she
Megan identified only three response topicsidentifying would use a mini-lesson and go over tenses and plurals
the speaker in dialogue, sentence structure, and verb tense without indicating any particular role for the student.
and each of these involved conforming the writing to con-
ventional rules. She indicated she would help the student Profile 3 case. Attention to a wide range of deep features
but provided no instructional strategies and gave no indica- and identification of specialized instructional strategies are
tion of the role the student might play in making these cor- unique characteristics of participants in Profile 3. Keris
rections. Thus, her instructional ideas were confined to interview illustrates this sophistication. In addition to notic-
surface features and unclear strategies. ing surface features (capitalization, punctuation, paragraph
indentations, articles, and plurality), Keri attended to quite a
Profile 2 case. While Lisa primarily addressed surface fea- range of deep features. For example, she began by noticing
tures, such as tense, punctuation, and articles, she also high- a stylistic feature, stating, I notice that his writing is very
lighted a few deep features, including the sequence of ideas choppy and can come off a little bit awkward. She contin-
in the story: ued by highlighting the narratives structure:

My friend eat all the donuts. Everyone go home. And I He has five paragraphs, and its clear that he has an introduction,
open all my presents, my gifts. He definitely needs the tenses. and a conclusion, and a body, so maybe there was an organizer
And being able to know when to use commas . . . Then he used used . . . Me and my snake live happily ever after. So he has
a a couple of times. I gave my snake a snake food. It sounds the endings. I think its a good conclusion, actually.
weird. All the grammar rules are wrong . . . I also thought it was
a random story. Its a pretty decent sequence because it talks Most notable was Keris extensive consideration of the
about making a party and then what the snake can do. And then authors ideas:
hes bringing the snake for show and tell. Then he went home. I
think he has a good gist of how to write a story . . . I just think He seems very persistent on someone stealing his snake, which
that the grammar and the tenses and all of that is the main issue. is cute. Im not sure what the exact prompt was, but if they were
doing a story based on what they were learning in science, I
Lisa also commented on the illustration drawn underneath think that this is probably more fantasy than realistic because
the text of the story: snakes do not jump. I think its good that he used the name
Python. It means he may have recognized something from
I guess thats the snake. Maybe thats snake food, but I have no science and made a connection . . . I wonder what the snake food
idea the background to it. I would have asked the student what is. Just out of curiosity, because of science . . . He talks about
that was. Maybe when he turned it in, or when I gave it back to making a doughnut, a cupcake, a cherry pie, and a cake for his
him, Could you describe whats going on in the picture? birthday, but then he doesnt ever mention the pie again. And its
interesting that he made donuts, because thats hard . . . When
Im reading it, I would like to know more. He says, I get lots of
Lisas stance toward the writing was primarily negatively presents. Like, what? So, I think that hes lacking some details
evaluative, as demonstrated in her description of grammar that could maybe engage the reader more.
Ballock et al. 9

Keris response to the ideas in the writing sample suggests mini-lesson to brainstorm at other times. Thus, she seems
she read the narrative through two lenses. First, as a reader, cognizant of not only helping the child improve this story but
she enjoyed the story and sought to make meaning as she also equipping the child with strategies to use over time.
read. She seemed to enjoy the students persistence in indi- Even when Keri suggested the need for editing the narra-
cating the snake was stolen. She was interested in the inclu- tive, her approach was very different from Megans and
sion of making donuts, an activity she considered hard. Lisas:
Furthermore, she reflected on her experience as a reader and
her desire for more details. She treated the narrative as an What else could we work on? Forming our sentences . . . maybe
authentic text, intended to engage an audience. Second, she even having the child read the story after he wrote it down, out
read through the eyes of a teacher assessing the students loud a few times, and seeing if by taking away or adding certain
written piece. She noted how the listed party foods were not articles, like a or the or and it makes it flow better, or it
makes more sense . . . doing it in pairs, so that they can both give
integral to the story, questioned whether the ideas fit the
each other feedback.
assigned genre, and considered interdisciplinary connections
reflected in the narrative.
This strategy would actively and meaningfully involve
In addition to noticing a wide range of features, Keri was
the student, either experimenting independently with adding
one of just four participants who took a broad stance toward
and removing different articles or getting feedback from a
the writing. She did make evaluative comments: His writing
partner. Again, this frames the student as the active agent in
is very choppy, He should have a comma and a question
writing.
mark after right, and Its a good conclusion. However,
she also employed an interpretive stance, attempting to
Dimension 1 interpretation.The differences across these
understand the logic in what the student did. For example,
three cases led us to conclude that Dimension 1 reveals
she wondered about the snakes name: I give him a name
something about participants underlying knowledge for
called Python. I wonder if hes confusing Python as a name
reading and responding to student writing. First, these cases
or if it is the type of snake . . . She went on to hypothesize
seem to highlight differences in participants knowledge of
that the use of Python may have come from something the
content and students (KCS; Ball et al., 2008). KCS is a type
child learned in science. She inferred maybe there was an
of knowledge integrating knowledge of students with knowl-
organizer used, acknowledging a process that may have led
edge of content. It includes knowledge about childrens
to the narratives clear organization. Keri seemed to view the
typical patterns of understanding, including common con-
child as an author with agency. She moved beyond evalua-
ceptions, misconceptions, and error patterns at various ages
tion of text features to consider the students intentions and
or points of development. Megan demonstrated minimal
process.
KCS. Despite successfully writing her own narrative incor-
In addition, Keris suggested instructional strategies stand
porating dialogue, action, and chronological sequence just
out for their detailed elaboration and clear student roles. For
weeks before her interview, Megan attended only to surface
example,
features. She did not seem to recognize these deep features
in the childs writing. Perhaps she did not expect children
I would do a mini-lesson on colorful language. I had to do this
could demonstrate genre knowledge while still developing
with my students, when I taught a personal narrative in second
grade. Id hand out paint strips for different words that they have
their language and transcription skills. Or perhaps she did
in their narrative. So, when he uses the big upper boy, instead not recognize how genre features manifested themselves in
of using the word big, he would write big at the bottom in the childs narrative. In fact, Megan did not acknowledge the
the lightest color of the paint strip, and you would look up and student author at all except to suggest working with the child
write down different descriptive words that have the same to correct errors.
meaning, so he has a variety to pick from that make his writing In contrast, Keri seemed to have a deeper understanding
more colorful. But he could also use it with different times. He of what children can accomplish as writers. She recognized a
could brainstorm with other things. variety of genre-specific features, treating the students writ-
ing as an authentic narrative. More sophisticated KCS was
Keri highlighted a strategy very specific to enhancing the also particularly evident in Keris stance toward the writing
vocabulary used in the narrative, and she communicated sample. Viewing the student as an author, communicator,
details about how she would orchestrate the mini-lesson. meaning-maker, and thinker, she used her KCS to consider
This strategy implies the teachers role is to equip rather than possibilities about the childs thought processes. She pointed
correct. Keri would create the opportunity for the child to out possible connections to science and questioned whether
develop a tool, but the decisions about words used in the nar- the student understood Python as a name of a snake or a type
rative would belong to the student author. This strategy of snake. Thus, KCS appears to undergird effective reading
would likely improve this narrative, but Keri also suggested and responding by helping teachers to (a) recognize novice
the student author could use the words he generated in this forms of various writing features, (b) recognize that students
10 Journal of Teacher Education

are engaged in intentional thought processes, and (c) inter- Identifying the critical components of a core practice is
pret and diagnose students misunderstandings. important because, given the time constraints of teacher edu-
Second, these cases seem to highlight differences in par- cation programs, preservice teachers may not have enough
ticipants knowledge of content and teaching (KCT; Ball et time to learn all of a practices nuances. However, by learning
al., 2008). KCT integrates knowledge of teaching with the most crucial and powerful components, they can enact the
knowledge of content. Teachers use this knowledge to make practice in a way that still supports student learning.
instructional decisions about how to make content accessible The three cases demonstrate the importance of the Topic,
to studentsto support students in developing understand- Response Topic, and Response Strategy components of read-
ing and proficiency. Participants drew on KCT as they ing and responding. For example, Megan attended to only
weighed the importance of the various strengths and weak- the surface features of the childs writing, and her only sug-
nesses they noticed in the writing sample and determined gested response was to edit mechanical errors. Thus, the stu-
response topics and strategies. Whereas Megan and Lisa dent had no opportunity to develop his narrative using
demonstrated minimal KCT by suggesting vague basic strat- effective techniques, details, or event sequences (Standard 3)
egies (e.g., do a mini-lesson, pull the child aside) aimed at or to consider the texts clarity, coherence, development,
correcting errors in the current draft, Keri demonstrated organization, or style (Standard 4). In addition, Megans
greater KCT by identifying specific strategies to address response that she would simply help the student edit makes
both surface and deep features (e.g., peer editing, detailed it unclear whether he would have developed an understand-
word choice mini-lesson). She also took a long-term view of ing of language skills that he could apply to his future writ-
instruction, suggesting the development of a tool that would ing. Her response was troubling in light of evidence that
simultaneously improve the current draft as well as support children need explicit instruction about revision and editing
the student in future writing. Thus, KCT undergirds effective strategies (Graham, Harris, & Santangelo, 2015) so they
reading and responding because it helps teachers determine learn strategies that will guide their writing of future texts.
when and how to teach students the knowledge, skills, and In contrast to Megan, Keri attended to the both the surface
strategies that can support their ongoing growth as writers. and deep features of the childs writing and suggested specific,
KCS and KCT are two smaller components of a broader specialized instructional strategies. In addition to mechanics,
category of teacher knowledge: pedagogical content knowl- she considered the writers ideas, the narratives organization,
edge (PCK). Although researchers have described PCK in and whether the details were appropriate for the genre
different ways (Ball et al., 2008), Shulman (1986) first defined (Standards 3 and 4). This attention to genre features is impor-
it as the subject matter knowledge needed for teaching, an tant because childrens understandings of discourse knowl-
amalgam of subject matter knowledge and pedagogical edge contribute to their success as writers (Hayes &
knowledge, the content knowledge that embodies the aspects Olinghouse, 2015; McCutchen, 2011; Olinghouse & Graham,
of content most germane to its teachability (p. 9). It is the 2009). Keri also considered the process that might have led the
specialized content knowledge that distinguishes a good writ- child to produce a text with these features (Standard 5).
ing teacher from a good writer, including an understanding of Furthermore, she suggested instructional responses that would
what makes certain topics or skills easy or difficult to learn, allow the child to learn strategies that he could apply not just
the ability to diagnose and respond to misunderstandings to the current text but also to his future writing as well.
when they arise, and a repertoire of representations of content Our findings provide evidence that differences in PCK
that can support student learning. Because KCS and KCT contribute to differences in participants reading and respond-
explain key differences among cases, we conclude that the ing. While PCK has been examined extensively in science
best interpretation for Dimension 1 is that it represents PCK and mathematics, this study is among the first to empirically
and plots participants along a PCK continuum. In other words, identify how PCK pertains to writing instruction. Unlike
the closer a participant is plotted toward the positive pole of many studies, we did not set out to describe or measure PCK.
Dimension 1, the more developed his or her PCK. Therefore, the emergence of PCK from our analysis is par-
ticularly important, adding credibility to PCK as a relevant
construct for the teaching of writing.
Discussion
More specifically, this study suggests that at least two
The purpose of this research was to investigate the relation- subsets of PCK undergird skillful reading and responding:
ship among the components of reading and responding to stu- KCS and KCT. Teachers draw on KCS when they read stu-
dent writing to better understand the underpinnings of the dents writing. Well-developed KCS supports teachers in
practice and how teacher educators might decompose it so attending to a broad range of features and reasoning about
that preservice teachers can learn to skillfully enact it. MCA those features to identify childrens learning needs. In con-
results suggest that all five components are interrelated, but trast, teachers draw on KCT when responding to students
that three may be particularly interrelated and central to sup- writing. Well-developed KCT supports teachers in selecting
porting children in meeting the CCSSs learning goals for targeted instructional strategies to address childrens learn-
writing: Topic, Response Topic, and Response Strategy. ing needs. Our data suggest that KCS and KCT, while related,
Ballock et al. 11

may not develop simultaneously. For example, teachers can or completing assignments requiring both analysis of and
attend and respond to deep features in the writing sample response to a piece of writing. Rehearsals should also occur
without identifying targeted strategies likely to improve within field placements. These might include structured con-
those features. The reverse does not seem true. Participants versations about student texts with cooperating teachers or
who attended and responded only to surface features did not grade-level teams, cofacilitating student writing conferences,
identify any specialized response strategies. Thus, it appears videotaping and analyzing the enactment of reading and
that the application of KCT depends, at least in part, upon the responding during student writing conferences, and reflecting
development of KCS. Future research should further explore on the results of lessons planned in response to student texts.
the relationship between KCS and KCT within the context of A final recommendation is that rehearsals of reading and
reading and responding. responding place particular emphasis on the deep features of
The findings of this study have several implications for childrens writing. Research with both preservice (Ballock &
teacher education. First, it is clear that content knowledge, McQuitty, 2014) and inservice (Matsumara et al., 2002)
such as that represented in the first three Writing CCSS, is teachers demonstrates the tendency of some to attend and
insufficient for effective practice. Preservice teachers can respond to primarily surface features. Yet, the CCSS require
know the features of narratives and use them proficiently in children to produce texts with substantive ideas, clear orga-
their own writing without recognizing them in students writ- nization, and genre-specific featuresattributes that extend
ing. While content courses, such as Freshman Composition far beyond conventions. Thus, meeting the expectations of
or the Writing for Elementary Educators course taken by par- the CCSS requires teachers to both recognize deep features
ticipants in this study, provide an important foundation by (or lack thereof) in childrens writing and to respond with
teaching genre features and writing strategies generally, pedagogies that teach deep features. Furthermore, because
teacher education programs must extend this knowledge by the CCSS address narrative, informational, and persuasive/
helping teacher candidates become familiar with how these argumentative writing, preservice teachers must learn to read
features typically appear in childrens writing (KCS). and respond in genre-specific ways. This study focused on
Analyzing childrens writing is one way to bridge preser- the knowledge needed to read and respond to a narrative, but
vice teachers content knowledge about writing to the KCS ultimately, teacher education courses must address KCS and
needed for reading and responding because it provides KCT for reading and responding to other genres as well.
opportunity to examine childrens novice, emerging attempts We recommend that future research examine how teach-
at writing. Preservice teachers can analyze childrens writing ers learn to read and respond to different genres at different
with respect to research and theory on childrens writing grade levels. Given the importance of KCS to the practice,
development and with respect to exemplars provided in we wonder how much difference exists between, for exam-
Appendix C of the CCSS (http://www.corestandards.org/ ple, reading and responding to kindergarteners personal nar-
assets/Appendix_C.pdf). Studies have shown that preservice ratives and reading and responding to third graders personal
teachers can learn to analyze childrens writing in increas- narratives. Similarly, how much difference exists between
ingly sophisticated ways over time (Davenport, 2006; reading and responding to fifth graders personal narratives
Gibson, 2007; Moore & Seeger, 2009). However, more and fifth graders opinion texts? Is there a point at which a
research is needed to further clarify how teachers develop teacher is skillful enough in reading and responding that she
skill in analyzing childrens writing. can easily learn to apply the practice to new genres and grade
Similarly, instructional strategies cannot only be taught in levels, or must she specifically learn how each genre mani-
isolation if preservice teachers are to learn to read and respond fests in the writing of students at each level? Answers to
effectively. This prompts a rethinking of traditional methods these questions could provide useful additional guidance for
courses that teach pedagogical skills without necessarily con- literacy teacher educators.
necting to childrens writing development. Our research makes In summary, this study highlights reading and responding
clear that reading and responding to student writing is one to student writing as a core practice that when enacted skill-
integrated practice: instructional responses are contingent fully is likely to support children in meeting the CCSSs
upon a prioritization of the features teachers recognize in a challenging demands. This study provides insight into the
childs text. We assert that preservice teachers need opportuni- nature of reading and responding, specifically in terms of its
ties to rehearse reading and responding as an integrated prac- reliance on the development of teachers PCK. As teacher
tice so they can receive feedback on their attempts to link educators work to develop pedagogies of teacher education
responses with identified student needs and further develop to teach reading and responding as a practice, it will be
their PCK in context. Rehearsals of this integrated practice important not only to decompose reading and responding
may take on a variety of forms in a methods course, such as into component parts that can be rehearsed in isolation but
using protocols to guide structured collaborative discussions also to support novice teachers in enacting reading and
of writing samples in class, collaboratively planning lessons responding as an integrated whole, connecting their knowl-
based on a class set of student writing samples, planning and edge of teaching and content with their knowledge of teach-
rehearsing writing conferences in response to writing samples, ing and students to make targeted instructional decisions.
12 Journal of Teacher Education

Declaration of Conflicting Interests Graham, S., Hebert, M., & Harris, K. R. (2015). Formative assess-
ment and writing: A meta-analysis. Elementary School Journal,
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
115, 523-547.
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Grossman, P. L., Hammerness, K., & McDonald, M. (2009).
Redefining teaching, re-imagining teacher education. Teachers
Funding and Teaching: Theory and Practice, 15(2), 273-289.
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author- Harper, C., Platt, E., Naranjo, C., & Boynton, S. (2007). Marching
ship, and/or publication of this article. in unison: Florida ESL teachers and No Child Left Behind.
TESOL Quarterly, 41, 642-651.
References Hayes, J. R., & Olinghouse, N. G. (2015). Can cognitive writ-
Abell, S. K. (2008). Twenty years later: Does pedagogical content ing models inform the design of the Common Core State
knowledge remain a useful idea? International Journal of Science Standards? Elementary School Journal, 115, 480-497.
Education, 30, 1405-1416. doi:10.1080/09500690802187041 Husson, F., Josse, J., Le, S., & Mazet, J. (2015). FactoMineR:
Ball, D. L., & Forzani, F. M. (2009). The work of teaching and the Multivariate exploratory data analysis and data mining (R pack-
challenge for teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, age version 1.29) [Software]. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-
60, 497-511. doi:10.1177/0022487109348479 project.org/package=FactoMineR
Ball, D. L., Thames, M. H., & Phelps, G. (2008). Content knowl- Jacobs, V. R., Lamb, L. L. C., & Philipp, R. A. (2010). Professional
edge for teaching: What makes it special? Journal of Teacher noticing of childrens mathematical thinking. Journal of
Education, 59, 389-407. doi:10.1177/0022487108324554 Research in Mathematics Education, 41, 169-202.
Ballock, E., & McQuitty, V. (2014, April). Preservice teachers Le, S., Josse, J., & Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: An R pack-
noticing in elementary student writing. Presented at the Annual age for multivariate analysis. Journal of Statistical Software,
Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, 25(1), 1-18. Retrieved from http://factominer.free.fr/docs/arti-
Philadelphia, PA. cle_FactoMineR.pdf
Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written Masuda, A., & Ebersole, M. (2012). Beginning teachers study
composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. groups: A place to continue critical conversations about teach-
Clausen, S. (1998). Quantitative applications in the social sci- ing writing. The New Educator, 8, 160-176. doi:10.1080/1547
ences, 121: Applied correspondence analysis: An introduction. 688X.2012.670570
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. Matsumara, L. C., Patthey-Chavez, G. G., Valdes, R., & Garnier,
Cutler, L., & Graham, S. (2008). Primary grade writing instruction: H. (2002). Teacher feedback, writing assignment quality, and
A national survey. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, third-grade students revision in lower and higher achieving
907-919. doi:10.1037/a0012656 urban schools. Elementary School Journal, 103, 3-25.
Davenport, N. A. (2006). Connecting preservice teachers with stu- McCarthey, S. J. (2008). The impact of No Child Left Behind on
dents using email to build skills for teaching writing. Journal teachers writing instruction. Written Communication, 25, 462-
of Reading Education, 31(2), 13-19. 505. doi:10.1177/0741088308322554
DeGroff, L. (1992). Process-writing teachers responses to fourth-grade McCutchen, D. (2011). From novice to expert: Implications of
writers first drafts. Elementary School Journal, 93, 131-144. language skills and writing-relevant knowledge for memory
Ell, F., Hill, M., & Grudnoff, L. (2012). Finding out more about during the development of writing skills. Journal of Writing
teacher candidates prior knowledge: Implications for teacher Research, 3, 51-68. doi:10.17239/jowr-2011.03.01.3
educators. Asia-Pacific Journal of Teacher Education, 40(1), McDonald, M., Kazemi, E., & Kavanagh, S. S. (2013). Core prac-
55-65. doi:10.1080/1359866X.2011.643760 tices and pedagogies of teacher education: A call for a common
Forzani, F. M. (2014). Understanding core practices and language and collective activity. Journal of Teacher Education,
practice-based teacher education: Learning from the past. 64, 378-386. doi:10.1177/0022487113493807
Journal of Teacher Education, 65, 357-368. doi:10.1177/ Moore, R. A., & Seeger, V. (2009). Dear sincerely: Exploring lit-
0022487114533800 erate identities with young children and preservice teachers
Gibson, S. A. (2007). Preservice teachers knowledge of instruc- through letter writing. Literacy Research and Instruction, 48,
tional scaffolding for writing instruction. Mid-Western 185-205. doi:10.1080/19388070802226246
Educational Researcher, 20(2), 9-15. Olinghouse, N. G., & Graham, S. (2009). The relationship between
Gilbert, J., & Graham, S. (2010). Teaching writing to elementary the discourse knowledge and the writing performance of ele-
students in grades 4-6: A national survey. Elementary School mentary-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Journal, 110, 494-518. doi:10.1086/651193 101, 37-50.
Glasswell, K., Parr, J. M., & McNaughton, S. (2003). Four ways to R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statis-
work against yourself when conferencing with struggling writ- tical computing [Computing software]. Vienna, Austria: R
ers. Language Arts, 80, 291-298. Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available from http://
Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2015). Common Core State Standards www.R-project.org/
and writing: Introduction to the special issue. Elementary Reutzel, D. R., Dole, J. A., Read, S., Fawson, P., Herman, K., Jones,
School Journal, 115, 457-463. C. D., . . .Fargo, J. (2011). Conceptually and methodologically
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Santangelo, T. (2015). Research-based vexing issues in teacher knowledge assessment. Reading and
practices and the Common Core: Meta-analysis and meta-syn- Writing Quarterly, 27, 183-211. doi:10.1080/10573569.2011.
thesis. Elementary School Journal, 115, 498-522. 560098
Ballock et al. 13

Schreier, M. (2012). Qualitative content analysis in practice. Author Biographies


Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Ellen Ballock is an associate professor in the Department of Early
Shanahan, T. (2015). Common Core State Standards: A new role
and Elementary Education at Gordon College in Wenham, MA. Her
for writing. Elementary School Journal, 115, 464-479.
research focuses on preservice and inservice teacher professional
Sherin, M. G., Jacobs, V. R., & Philipp, R. A. (Eds.). (2011).
development with a specific focus on learning through collabora-
Mathematics teacher noticing. New York, NY: Routledge.
tively examining artifacts of practice.
Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth
in teaching. Educational Researcher, 15, 4-14.
Spence, L. K. (2010). Generous reading: Seeing students through Vicki McQuitty is an assistant professor in the Department of
their writing. The Reading Teacher, 63, 634-642. doi:10.1598/ Elementary Education at Towson University in Towson, MD. Her
RT.63.8.2 research and teaching center on preparing preservice and inservice
Teaching Works. (2016). High leverage practices. Retrieved from teachers to teach writing.
http://www.teachingworks.org/work-of-teaching/high-lever-
age-practices Scot McNary is associate professor in the Department of
van Es, E. A., & Sherin, M. G. (2006). How different video club Educational Technology and Literacy at Towson University in
designs support teachers in learning to notice. Journal of Towson, MD. His research and teaching are focused on quantitative
Computing in Teacher Education, 22, 125-135. methods and social interactions in learning environments.

You might also like