You are on page 1of 16

Child Development, May/June 2013, Volume 84, Number 3, Pages 891905

Longitudinal Relations Among Language Skills, Anger Expression, and


Regulatory Strategies in Early Childhood
Caroline K. P. Roben, Pamela M. Cole, and Laura Marie Armstrong
The Pennsylvania State University

Researchers have suggested that as childrens language skill develops in early childhood, it comes to help chil-
dren regulate their emotions (Cole, Armstrong, & Pemberton, 2010; Kopp, 1989), but the pathways by which
this occurs have not been studied empirically. In a longitudinal study of 120 children from 18 to 48 months of
age, associations among child language skill, observed anger expression, and regulatory strategies during a
delay task were examined. Toddlers with better language skill, and whose language skill increased more over
time, appeared less angry at 48 months and their anger declined more over time. Two regulatory strategies,
support seeking and distraction, explained a portion of the variance in the association between language skill
and anger expression after toddlerhood.

Emotion regulation, dened as processes that con- This developmental decline in anger reactivity
tribute to the monitoring, evaluating, and modifying coincides with other changes that should contribute
of emotional reactions (Thompson, 1994), is broadly to childrens ability to self-regulate. By the 3rd year,
regarded as critical to childrens socioemotional com- children are thought to draw on new cognitive and
petence and mental health. In toddlerhood, quick, linguistic skills to regulate emotions (Kopp, 1989).
intense, and sustained angry reactions are typical of From this perspective, self-regulation is dened by a
toddlers tantrums (Potegal, Kosorok, & Davidson, childs ability to initiate regulatory strategies in the
1996). However, when preschool and school-age chil- absence of adult instruction (Kopp, 1982). It is com-
dren have frequent angry reactions that are quick, monly assumed that language development contrib-
intense, or sustained, they are viewed as emotionally utes to the development of self-regulation although
dysregulated or as having behavior problems (Cole, this premise has not been studied to the degree that
Zahn-Waxler, & Smith, 1994; Shaw, Bell, & Gilliom, attention control has been. Although there is no
2000). Thus, the transition from toddlerhood to pre- overarching theory that has specied the varied
school age in which angry reactions typically ways that language might contribute to self-regula-
decrease in frequency, immediacy, duration, and tion of anger, language skills arguably enhance a
intensity (Bridges, 1932; Cole et al., 2011) can be childs ability to express needs with words rather
viewed as crucial to emotional development. than with emotion, to think before acting in a frus-
trating situation, and to generate and sustain ideas
Present address: Caroline K. P. Roben, Department of Psy- that serve attention control (Cole, Armstrong, &
chology, University of Delaware. Pemberton, 2010). That is, young children who
Present address: Laura Marie Armstrong, Department of Psy-
chiatry and Human Behavior, The Warren Alpert Medical School acquire language quickly and well should be able to
of Brown University. think about rules (Mommy said wait), to commu-
This research was supported by the National Institute on Men- nicate needs calmly (language mitigates the need to
tal Health with R01-061388 awarded to the second author, the
rst and third authors fellowships on T32-070327 (PI: Pamela M. express needs nonverbally), and, when needed, to
Cole), and an individual fellowship F31-088015 to the rst sustain a shift of attention rather than focus on
author, and by the National Institute on Child Health & Human something they cannot have (language enriches the
Development with an individual fellowship F31-063318 to the
third author. We thank Keith Crnic and Clancy Blair for their content of the activities, such as pretend play, that
contributions to the study and comments on early versions of distract the child from the desired object or activity).
this manuscript. We also thank the many graduate and under-
graduate students who contributed to the data collection and
reduction, as well as the commitment and contributions of the
families that participated.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to 2012 The Authors
Pamela M. Cole, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania Child Development 2012 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
State University, 141 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2013/8403-0011
Electronic mail may be sent to pmc5@psu.edu. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12027
892 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

Language develops rapidly between the rst social competence (Cassidy, Werner, Rourke, Zub-
birthday and preschool age (Gleason, 2005) in par- ernis, & Balaraman, 2003). In terms of longitudinal
allel with the normative decline in quick, intense, evidence, toddler verbal skills predicted childrens
and prolonged angry reactions and the develop- behavior during a later mental development test
ment of regulatory strategies. Several lines of (Ayoub, Vallotton, & Mastergeorge, 2011). Vocabu-
research suggest that these two domains are linked. lary size at age 24 months, above and beyond
First, child language delays and behavior problems, general cognitive skill, predicted the level and rate of
especially those involving poorly regulated anger, change in self-regulation from ages 14 to 36 months
are associated (Baker & Cantwell, 1992; Cohen & for boys and the level at each age for girls (Vallotton
Mendez, 2009; Irwin, Carter, & Briggs-Gowan, & Ayoub, 2011). These ndings support the premise
2002). Second, child use of emotion language pre- that early language development inuences self-
dicts their socioemotional competence (e.g., Den- regulation, but their assessment of self-regulation
ham et al., 2003; Dunn, Bretherton, & Munn, 1987; was limited to behavior during the administration of
Fabes, Eisenberg, Hanish, & Spinrad, 2001). Third, the Bayley Scales, a task that is not designed to tax
childrens self-directed speech can guide their self-regulation specically. Moreover, the ndings
actions (Berk, 1992; Luria, 1961; Vygotsky, 1962; underscore the need to consider cognitive skill and
Winsler & Naglieri, 2003). Fourth, preschool inter- gender when assessing the relations between lan-
ventions that teach children to use words to deal guage and self-regulation. To further understand
with frustration improve their socioemotional com- these relations we chose to examine the degree to
petence (Domitrovich, Cortes, & Greenberg, 2007; which language skills predicted changes in the
Izard et al., 2008). immediacy, intensity, and duration of young chil-
In addition to language skill, a childs disposition drens angry reactions, the degree to which changes
or temperament is associated with the development were associated with child-initiated regulatory strat-
of emotion regulation. For example, effortful control egies, and whether these strategies had a stronger
or the ability to shift attention and inhibit a prepo- link to reduced anger at certain ages. In early stages
tent response contributes to a childs ability to mod- of language learning, such as rst words and vocab-
ulate negative emotional reactions (Kochanska, ulary spurt, children may be unable to integrate
Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Rothbart, Ellis, Rueda, & verbal and emotional expression (Bloom, 1993). At
Posner, 2003). Between 24 and 36 months, children later ages, however, we assume that they can more
are rst observed to engage in effortful control of readily rely on words instead of nonverbal
their behavior when frustrated (e.g., Calkins, Gill, emotional expressions to convey their needs, which
Johnson, & Smith, 1999; Feldman, 2009), albeit with is the premise of several interventions for promoting
limited effectiveness (Buss & Goldsmith, 1998). preschoolers emotional competence (Domitrovich
Thus far, effortful control has mainly been attrib- et al., 2007; Izard et al., 2008). Kopp (1989) sug-
uted to the development of attention control (Gilli- gested that the ability to draw on language in the
om, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, & Lukon, 2002; Sethi, toddler years, the period of heightened anger reac-
Mischel, Aber, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 2000). For tivity, should contribute to the decline in anger
example, when young children are required to wait reactivity because children can draw on language
for a reward, those who shift their attention away skills to regulate anger. Conversely, children who
from the reward and focus on another activity express less anger as toddlers may receive more
appear less angry than children who do not distract language input because they are easier conversation
themselves in this way. This use of distraction is partners for parents (Stern, 1985). Linking language
regarded as an important self-regulatory strategy development to emotional development in these
for decreasing anger expression in a reward-delay various ways is consistent with the growing under-
situation (Cole et al., 2011). As effortful control is standing of the development of the brains role in
also related to a childs language ability (Blair & the integration of domains of psychological function-
Razza, 2007), we controlled for it in this study of ing (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007; Johnson & Munakata,
developmental links among childrens language 2005).
skills, anger expressions, and strategy use. In sum, in addition to appreciating the impor-
Direct evidence for relations among growth in tance of effortful control to the development of self-
language skills, decline in anger reactivity, and regulation, it is necessary to test the premise that a
increase in self-regulatory strategy use is limited. childs language ability, conceptualized either as an
Preschoolers verbal skills are related concurrently to individual difference or in terms of intraindividual
their expressions of positive emotions and their growth, contributes to the development of anger
Language and Anger Expression 893

regulation in early childhood. The purpose of this families. Withdrawn families did not differ from
study was to investigate the contributions of early those who completed on any demographic charac-
language development, over and above effortful teristic. Children were seen within 2 weeks of their
control, gender, and intellectual ability in predicting half or full birthday at four age points; Mage in
(a) the normative decline in anger expressions and months at each time were 18.44 (SD = 0.57), 24.39
(b) the use of regulatory strategies (child-initiated (SD = 1.30), 36.44 (SD = 0.80), and 48.33 (SD = 0.67)
support seeking and distractions). months. Most (93.3%) children were identied as
On the basis of the framework described by Cole White by their mothers; 6.7% were biracial. Includ-
et al. (2010), we predicted that (a) better language ing all sources of income, the average household
skills and language skills that increase at a greater annual income when the child was age 18 months
rate over time would predict a developmental was $40,502.94 (SD = 14,480.73).
decrease in anger expressions, (b) better language
skills would predict use of support seeking to
Procedures
mother because language allows children to specify
needs through words rather than affect, (c) better Observations and questionnaires were conducted
language skills would predict quicker and longer at four home visits (at child ages 18, 30, 36, and
distractions because language should enrich the 42 months) and four lab visits (18, 24, 36, and
focus of redirected attention, and (d) toddler-age 48 months). Each lab visit varied somewhat due to
language skills should have a prospective rather child age but always included an alternating series
than concurrent inuence on anger regulation of tasks to elicit anger (Cole, Teti, & Zahn-Waxler,
because the inuence of language should occur once 2003; Goldsmith, Reilly, Lemery, Longley, & Pres-
childrens language development is more established cott, 1993) and standard relief tasks such as free
in early preschool, rather than in early language play. Only the lab task used to assess child anger
learning. This study tested the prediction that and child-initiated regulatory strategies is described
language status inuenced developmental changes in detail. Home observations were 90120 min vis-
in anger expressions between ages 24 and 48 months its, conducted by trained research assistants, and
during a situation designed to frustrate young chil- scheduled for a time when most family members
dren, and that this relation was at least partly would be present and they were encouraged to do
explained by the quality of young childrens regula- as they would normally. Observers limited their
tory strategies. interactions with family members and no specic
instructions that constrained family interactions and
activities were given. Video recording was not used
Method based on prior experience that cameras distracted
toddlers from their normal routines more than
Participants
observers did.
A multistage strategy was used to recruit rural Speech sample collection. The home observer
and semirural families whose incomes were below used a digital audio recorder to record naturally
the national median income but above the national occurring speech samples. In addition, the obser-
poverty threshold. This strategy allowed variation ver conducted four 10-min observations of family
in income, which is associated with socioeconomic interaction; the ratings from these observations
status (Hart & Risley, 1995), without the many were not included in this study. The audio-
confounding inuences of poverty or above aver- recorded speech samples were subsequently tran-
age economic advantage. In addition to public scribed by trained research assistants using the
advertisements in targeted census tracts, letters CHILDES system (MacWhinney, 2000). To derive
were sent to families in those communities with scores for these speech samples, two or more
children in the recruitment age, identied through 10-min epochs were transcribed to reach 50 child
birth announcements. utterances per epoch. A graduate student trained
The aim of recruitment was to enroll 125 families in CLAN supervised transcription and checked
based on power analysis. Using this recruitment 100% of the narratives for formatting accuracy
strategy, 128 families enrolled; 124 were income eli- and 20% for content accuracy.
gible at Time 1 when the child was 18 months old. Anger expression and regulatory strategies. Child
At 48 months, 120 families remained enrolled (65 anger expressions and regulatory strategies were
boys), a retention rate of 96.8% that we attribute to observed during a boring 8-min wait, often used
our efforts and to the values of the participating to observe child self-regulation (Dennis, 2006;
894 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, average j for emotion = .88 (range = .81.94). For
1996). Each mother was instructed to tell her child analyses, we created the following variables for
that the child must wait to open a gift until she anger: average intensity of bout of anger, latency to
nished her work (adapted from Vaughn, Kopp, & rst bout, and average bout duration. The number
Krakow, 1984). Waiting is frustrating for typical of anger bouts at each age were 3.50 (SD = 1.91),
children in the ages studied (e.g., Cole et al., 2003; 3.13 (SD = 2.20), and 1.68 (SD = 1.95) at 24, 36, and
Cole et al., 2011). The task was administered in the 48 months, respectively. More details about the
following manner at each age point. First, mothers developmental changes in anger are included in
were briefed in advance to avoid mothers laughing another article (Cole et al., 2011), but the numbers
and in that way inadvertently inserting humor into suggest that children were frustrated by this task at
a frustrating situation for the child. First, the RA all ages and that changes in this expression of anger
gave the mother work (questionnaires) and the over time in response to the boring wait warrants
child a boring toy: a at rubber lilypad (18 months), investigation.
one of a pair of cloth cymbals (24 months), a small Regulatory behavior. In the same 15-s epochs, an
toy car with missing wheels (36 months), and a toy independent team coded 10 child behaviors typi-
horse with missing legs (48 months). Then the RA cally studied in emotion regulation studies. For
placed a shiny gift-wrapped bag, tied tightly with a this study, we focused on 2: (a) support-seeking
ribbon, on the childs table, saying This is a sur- bids to mother about the challenge (e.g., Mom,
prise for you. As the RA left, the mother told the are you almost done? or I wonder whats in it),
child to wait until she nished her work to open and (b) distraction, which was further distin-
the gift. After 8 min, the RA returned and the guished as either (a) focused, that is, child became
mother let the child open the gift. absorbed in alternative activity (e.g., making silly
faces in the one-way mirror) or (b) unfocused, that
is, child diverted attention from focus on challenge
Coding Systems
but did not become absorbed in an alternative
Two independent coding systems generated activity. Each behavior was classied as child initi-
data for this study. The rst system involved a ated, mother initiated (and child complied), or
time-sensitive assessment of emotion expressions. disruptive. For this study, only nondisruptive,
The second system involved an independent but child-initiated behaviors were examined. In addi-
time-linked assessment of several behaviors, tion, we only included calm support seeking to
including regulatory strategies. For each system, a mother about the challenge because these bids
different coding team was trained to 80% accuracy were not disruptive and could arguably be
with master coders; once trained, 15% of cases regarded as appropriate regulatory strategies
were checked for reliability at random times. (Gilliom et al., 2002). The average kappa across
Anger expressions. To determine the total amount ages was j = .82 (range = .73.91.). We analyzed
of anger displayed and its temporal qualities, we total time, bout frequency, latency to rst bout,
coded emotion expressions in 15-s epochs in all and average bout duration for each code. For these
challenging tasks. For this study, we only report analyses, latency to the rst and duration of sup-
data from the boring wait. At 8 min long, it pro- port seeking and latency and duration of focused
vides a lengthier behavioral record than the other distractions were included.
tasks. In addition, it was the only task that was
used at all four ages. The focus of this study was
Language
on anger, but we also recorded neutral, nonangry
periods in which the child was either happy or neu- Spontaneous sampling procedures. Linguistic com-
tral. Each emotion was coded in terms of its pres- plexity was assessed using spontaneous sampling
ence and intensity (except neutral) in each 15-s procedures (e.g., Miller, 1981), which captured
epoch. We inferred emotion expressions from facial unconstrained speech during natural interactions
(brow and mouth movements), vocal, and select between parent and child in the home or during the
postural or gestural cues (Cole et al., 1994). For unstructured free play, reading task, and clean-up
example, anger was coded if the brow was fur- task in the laboratory. The index of linguistic com-
rowed, the jaw clenched or set, lips were pressed, plexity derived from transcriptions of this natural
vocalizations conveyed protest, irritation, or fussi- speech was the mean length of utterance (MLU). The
ness, and the body appeared restless, squirmy, with MLU is a measure of syntactic complexity, rst intro-
agitated arm and leg movements. Across ages, the duced by Brown (1973), and is computed as the
Language and Anger Expression 895

average number of morphemes in a childs utter-


Covariates
ances. Morphemes include each word in the sentence
(including repetitions), as well as afxes or grammat- Child gender. Boys were coded as 0 and girls as 1.
ical inections such as un-, -s, -ed. Each morpheme Effortful control. Child temperament was assessed
represents linguistic knowledge, and thus children annually by mother report. For this study, the
with similar number of morphemes per utterance are Toddler Behavior Questionnaire (TBAQR; Gold-
thought to have language at the same level of com- smith, 1996) Effortful Control scale, dened by 5 of
plexity or maturity (Gleason, 2005). MLU begins to the TBAQRs 14 subscales, was used. The Effortful
lose value as an index of language development at Control scale comprises inhibitory control, attention
around the fourth year and thus is not used as an shifting, low-intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitiv-
index of complexity after 36 months of age. ity, and attention focusing, but preliminary analy-
Speech samples were transcribed from home vis- sis for this sample indicated that perceptual
its when the child was 18 months and 30 months of sensitivity and low-intensity pleasure had inade-
age and in the lab when the child was 24 and quate internal consistencies. We removed them and
36 months. Previous research has shown MLU to be use an Effortful Control scale with Cronbachs alpha
reliable across home and laboratory settings at of 0.82.
24 months (Bornstein, Haynes, Painter, & Genevro, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence
2000). Moreover, for the sample being used in this (WPPSI) Performance IQ. Childrens intellectual
study, MLU was reliable across settings, as evi- status was assessed at 36 and 48 months during
denced by a signicant relation between the 30- laboratory visits with the WPPSIIII (Wechsler,
month home visit and 36-month lab visit, suggesting 2002). The Performance IQ (Block Design and Object
that MLU is related across setting and time for these Assembly subtests), a composite of 36 and 48 months
ages, r(99) = .52, p < .001. The MLU for each child (r = .44, p < .01), was used as an index of general
was calculated using CLAN, a language analysis intellectual functioning to avoid shared variance
software package (MacWhinney, 2000). between language skills and Verbal IQ. The correla-
tions of Performance IQ and Full Scale IQ for the
sample were 0.87 at 36 months and 0.80 at
Laboratory Tasks Assessing Language
48 months.
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory Income-to-needs ratio (INR). The INR is an index
(MCDI). The MCDIWords and Gestures (Fenson of household income relative to national norms. A
et al., 1993) was administered at 18 months. The middle-income family has an INR of 3.0, and by de-
MCDI is an 889-item form on which mothers indi- nition, an INR of 1.0 indicates that the household
cate how many gestures, words, and phrases the income represents poverty. The mean INR was 2.37
child understands and uses. Although the children (SD = 0.94), indicating households ranged between
were 18 months of age, the Words and Gestures poverty and middle income.
form, which is normed up to 16 months of age,
was used. We used this form of the MCDI because
the version for older children was normed on Results
children from advantaged households, unlike the
Descriptive Data and Overview of Analyses
children from homes that are economically strained.
Because the MCDIWords and Gestures was The means and standard deviations for variables
not normed for 18-month-olds, only the raw scores used in data analyses are presented in Tables 1
for words understood and words produced were (language status indices) and 2 (anger expression,
used in the analysis (alphas of .95 and .96, respec- support seeking, and distraction). Log transforma-
tively). tions were used to improve skewed distributions of
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 3 anger expression and regulatory behaviors. Descrip-
(CELF3). The Sentence Structure Standard Score tive statistics and zero-order correlations were con-
from the CELF3 (Semel, Wing, & Secord, 1995) ducted in SPSS 18.0 (IBM Software, Armonk, NY).
was administered at the 36-month lab visit. To examine how levels and change in lan-
TOLDP:3. The Sentence Imitation and Gram- guage skill inuenced levels and change in anger
matical Understanding Scores from the Test of expression, linear growth of each construct was
Language DevelopmentPrimary Third Edition modeled using LISREL 8.80 (Jreskog & Srbom,
(TOLDP:3; Newcomer & Hammill, 1997) was 1996). Structural equation models were executed
administered at the 48-month lab visit. with covariance matrices of observed variables,
896 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

Table 1 Table 2
Minimums, Maximums, Means, and Standard Deviations for Lan- Means and Standard Deviations for Temporal Dynamics of Anger
guage Variables Expression and Regulatory Strategies

Min. Max. M SD Age in months

Toddler age 24 36 48
18m Home Visit MLU 1.00 2.25 1.43 0.29
18m MCDI 14.00 396.00 242.19 89.75 Anger expression
Vocabulary Latency to 1st bout
Comprehension M 4.11 10.38 18.04
18m MCDI 0.00 323.00 75.01 71.79 SD 7.51 11.47 12.56
Vocabulary Production Average intensity
24m Lab Visit MLU 1.00 2.98 1.45 0.38 M 2.22 1.53 0.94
Early Preschool age SD 0.78 0.92 0.85
30m Home Visit MLU 1.02 3.29 1.84 0.47 Average length
36m Lab Visit MLU 1.20 3.78 2.35 0.55 M 6.01 1.87 1.21
36m CELF-3 0.00 19.00 9.38 3.45 SD 8.36 1.91 1.47
Sentence Structure Support seeking
Preschool age Latency to 1st bout
TOLD-P:3 Grammatical 4.00 15.00 9.64 2.43 M 15.90 5.31 3.73
Understanding SD 12.87 8.50 8.51
TOLD-P:3 Sentence 5.00 16.00 9.05 3.07 Average length
Imitation M 0.89 1.51 1.81
SD 0.78 0.69 0.95
Note. m = month; MLU = mean length utterance; MCDI = Focused distractions
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory; CELF3 = Latency to 1st bout
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 3; TOLDP:3 = Test M 10.34 7.25 3.23
of Language DevelopmentPrimary Third Edition. SD 10.23 8.32 4.82
Average length
M 1.95 2.44 3.26
SD 1.40 1.92 3.24
using THEIL (Molenaar, 1996), a FORTRAN
program for robust covariance matrix estimation.
It improves the condition of a covariance matrix
Model Fit
and handles missing data using the maximum
likelihood estimate while leaving its underlying Model t was assessed using the root mean square
structure intact. error of approximation (RMSEA), the comparative t
Analyses rst examined whether (a) the initial index (CFI), and the normal theory weighted least
level and change of language skill inuences the squares chi-square (v2). The RMSEA index includes
end level and change of anger expression (see adjustments for model complexity and is less inu-
Figure 1), or (b) the initial level and change of enced by the number of parameters in the model
anger expression inuences the end level and than other indicators of t. The CFI compares the
change of language skill. In the presence of signi- covariance matrices predicted by the model to the
cant associations, we next tested whether the path- observed covariance matrix, and compares the null
ways could be partially associated with support model with the observed covariance matrix. Like
seeking and focused distractions. To understand at RMSEA, CFI is among the measures least affected by
what time the strategies most mattered, we looked sample size (Fan, Thompson, & Wang, 1999). For
at each assessment as an independent predictor RMSEA, a value less than .05 is considered a good
(see Figure 2). In all models, child gender, child t, and below .08 an adequate t (Kline, 2004). By
effortful control, child Performance IQ, and INR convention, the CFI should be equal to or greater
were included as covariates. Covariates were than .90 to accept the model. Chi-square is inuenced
entered into the model as predictors of both pre- by sample size and indicates good t when p > .05.
dictor and outcome initial level and growth vari-
ables. Nonsignicant covariates were systematically
Composites
removed to improve power, whereas marginal and
signicant covariates were included in the nal Associations between growth models for both
model. child language skill and anger expression were
Language and Anger Expression 897

Figure 1. Example full model of language initial level and slope predicting anger end level and slope.
Note. Circles represent latent variables and squares represent manifest variables. Numbers on the unidirectional arrows from latent vari-
ables to manifest variables indicate loadings in the lambda matrix for the creation of linear growth models.

Figure 2. Example full model of language initial level and slope predicting strategies at all three ages, which in turn predict anger end
level and slope.
Note. Bidirectional arrows between latent language and anger levels and manifest strategy variables represent correlations between vari-
ables measured at the same assessment. Unidirectional arrows among latent language variables, strategy variables, and latent anger
variables represent predictions. Note that there was no unidirectional or bidirectional association tested between 48mo strategy and
anger slope, because a later event cannot predict or be correlated with change preceding it in time.

examined using composites of each construct created ease of reading. We took this approach to generate a
at three ages: 24, 36, and 48 months. Variables in the robust index of child language skill at each age for
Language Skill composite changed across ages to that 6-month period. This avoided basing estimates
reect important elements of developmental change of language skill on a single index, especially one
in language skills (vocabulary size, MLU, receptive such as MLU, which could vary across child mood
and expressive grammatical understanding). The and situational context. Finally, at 48 months, pre-
toddler-age Language composite included vocabu- school age, we used a developmentally appropriate
lary size (receptive and expressive based on the test of receptive and expressive language, the TOLD
MCDI) at 18 months and natural speech MLU at 18 Grammatical Understanding and Sentence Imitation
and 24 months, and the early preschool-age compos- subtests. MLU is a less sensitive index of language
ite included natural speech MLU at 30 and skill by this age. Anger Expression composites
36 months and receptive grammar at 36 months. We included latency to the rst bout, duration, and
refer to these as 24- and 36-month composites for intensity of anger expressions at each age.
898 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

Table 3
Zero-Order Correlations Among Variables in the Language Composites

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. 18m MLU
2. 18m MCDI Comprehension .06
3. 18m MCDI Production .19 .58**
4. 24m MLU .29** .32** .51**
5. 30m MLU .29** .24* .42** .53**
6. 36m MLU .22* .16 .26** .40** .53**
7. 36m CELF .12 .22* .33** .10 .20 .12
8. 48m TOLD Grammatical Understanding .12 .17 .25** .25** .13 .11 .23*
9. 48m TOLD Sentence Imitation .11 .23* .39** .26** .41** .33** .38** .31**

Note. m = month; MLU = mean length utterance; MCDI = MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory; CELF = Clinical Evalua-
tion of Language Fundamentals; TOLD = Test of Language Development.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

Composites were created by standardizing each


Initial Level and Change in Language Skill Predicting
measure into a Z score and then summing the
End Level and Change in Anger Expression
standardized variables. Language measures were
generally moderately correlated at each age In this model, the initial level at 24 months and
(24 months, rs = .06.58; 36 months, rs = .12.53; change in language skill from 24 to 48 months were
and 48 months, r = .31; see Table 3). Not unexpect- tested as predictors of the end level at 48 months
edly, standardized measures were more highly and change in anger expression. Growth models of
correlated with other standardized measures than language skill and anger expression each indicated
with MLU. The lowest correlations were between that variance estimates of level and slope were sig-
those measures without shared method variance. nicant (p < .05). After rst testing the model with
We hope that by including both types of measures, all possible paths (Figure 1), nonsignicant paths
we are including the most accurate picture of chil- were systematically removed. The trimmed model
drens language skill as possible and including and its standardized betas are presented in Fig-
several different constructs within language devel- ure 3. Please note that although some standardized
opment. Anger expression variables were signi- betas are larger than 1, this does not indicate an
cantly correlated with each other at each age error. Unlike in classical exploratory factor analysis
(24 months, rs = .39.67; 36 months, rs = .57.86; where factors are standardized and uncorrelated, if
and 48 months, rs = .61.92). the factors are correlated, the factor loadings are

Figure 3. Trimmed model of language skill predicting anger expression.


Note. For clarity, covariates are not pictured.
*p < .05.
Language and Anger Expression 899

regression coefcients and not correlations and can (b = .45, p < .10). Neither Language Skill level and
be larger than 1 in magnitude (Jreskog, 1999). It slope nor Anger Expression level and slope were sig-
may indicate a degree of multicollinearity in the nicantly correlated. Therefore, we concluded that
data, which would not be unexpected in this model language skill was a stronger predictor of anger
where we have related constructs of both initial expression across toddlerhood than was the reverse
level and growth all in one model. The tested hypothesis. Our next steps were to examine whether
model was an excellent t by all measures, each of two specic regulatory strategiessupport
RMSEA = .0, CFI = 1.0, v2(26) = 25.07, p > .05. Lan- seeking and distractioncould contribute to the
guage Skill initial level predicted Anger Expression pathway between language and the decline in anger
end level (b = .72, p < .05) and change in Anger expression.
Expression over time (b = 1.02, p < .05). Language
Skill change over time predicted Anger Expression
Support Seeking as a Factor in the Path Between
end level (b = .80, p < .05) and change in Anger
Language Skill and Anger Expression
Expression over time (b = 1.29, p < .05). Language
Skill level and slope were negatively correlated The latency to and duration of support seeking to
(r = .63, p < .05), and the relation between Anger mother adequately loaded as a single latent Support
Expression level and slope was not signicant. Seeking factor, which was used at each age (range
of loadings = .461.0, all signicant). After rst
testing the model with all possible paths (as seen in
Initial Level and Change in Anger Expression
Figure 2), nonsignicant paths were systematically
Predicting End Level and Change in Language Skill
removed. The trimmed model is presented with
To determine whether an alternative path between standardized betas (Figure 5) and was an adequate
early language skills and change in anger expression t by most measures, RMSEA = .05, CFI = 0.93,
could be shown, that is, early anger expression pre- v2(94) = 122.30, p = .027. There was moderate stabil-
dicting language skills, a reverse model was built. ity in Support Seeking across time (bs = .29 and .25,
The initial level at 24 months and change in Anger respectively, both ps < .05). A steeper increase in
Expression from 24 to 48 months were tested as pre- Language Skill across time was associated with
dictors of the end level at 48 months and change in quicker and longer Support Seeking to mother at
Language Skill. The trimmed model was an excellent 36 months. Support Seeking at 24 months was asso-
t (see Figure 4), RMSEA = .0044, CFI = 0.99, ciated with an increase in Anger Expression across
v2(28) = 28.06, p > .05. However the reverse paths toddlerhood (b = .32, p < .05), but Support Seeking
did not reach the p < .05 level of signicance. There at 36 months was associated with a lower end level
were trends for Anger Expression change over time of Anger Expression (b = .72, p < .05) and a
to predict to both Language Skill end level (b = .42, decrease in Anger Expression across time (b = .97,
p < .10) and change in Language Skill over time p < .05).

Figure 4. Trimmed model of anger expression predicting language skill.


Note. Dashed lines indicate marginal ndings where p < .10. For clarity, covariates are not pictured.
900 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

v2(51) = 78.40, p = .0081. Higher initial levels of


Distraction as a Factor in the Path Between Language
Language Skill were associated with longer distrac-
Skill and Anger Expression
tions at 36 months (b = .69, p < .05), as was steeper
Latency to and average duration of distractions increase in slope in Language Skill (b = .98,
did not load as a single latent factor. Although p < .05). Steeper increase in Language Skill slope
being able to quickly distract in the context of wait- was also marginally associated with longer distrac-
ing is important (Cole et al., 2011), the longer a tions at 24 months (b = .21, p < .10), and signi-
child can sustain a distraction the less likely the cantly associated with longer durations at 48 months
child will become frustrated again. Therefore, we (b = .76, p < .05), as was marginally the initial level
used distraction duration in this model. of language (b = .40, p < .10). Longer distractions at
The trimmed model was an adequate t by most 24 months were associated with a steeper increase
measures (Figure 6), RMSEA = .07; CFI = 0.90, in Anger Expression over time (b = .75, p < .05).

Figure 5. Trimmed model for language skill, support seeking, and anger expression.
Note. Covariates and manifest variables that create the latent support seeking factor (latency to rst bout of support seeking and aver-
age length of support seeking) are not pictured for clarity.
*p < .05.

Figure 6. Trimmed model for language skill, distraction, and anger expression.
Note. Dashed lines indicate marginal ndings where p < .10. For clarity, covariates are not pictured.
*p < .05.
Language and Anger Expression 901

However, by 48 months, distraction duration was 1990; Stern, 1985). Thus, it appeared that early lan-
negatively correlated with the end level of Anger guage abilities increased the likelihood that the
Expression (r = .27, p < .05). child would calmly seek support from mother,
during an age when support seeking appears to be
a bridge between complete reliance on parents for
regulating negative affect and more self-reliance
Discussion
(Kopp, 1989).
Childrens growth in language development should By 48 months, children seem to rely less on their
contribute to the decline in anger expression in early mothers for support and more on themselves, and
childhood and enhance the strategies they can initi- this too may be helped by a childs language
ate to regulate frustration (Cole et al., 2010; Kopp, development. The fact that toddler language con-
1989). These ndings are the rst to examine pro- tributed to the duration of child-initiated distrac-
spective associations among childrens language tions at 48 months, and not at earlier ages when
skills, the quality of child anger expression, and children are more reliant on their parents for regu-
child-initiated regulatory strategy use. Specically, lation (Grolnick, Bridges, & Connell, 1996), supports
individual differences in language skill in the the view that a fund of words and the ability to
toddler years (1824 months), as well as a childs put words together to generate ideas helps children
language growth, were associated with less quick, distract themselves from something they want but
intense, and sustained anger by preschool age and must wait to have. Possibly, a verbally rich inner
with a decline in anger over time. In addition, the life enriches the degree to which children become
rate of language growth contributed to better anger absorbed in new activities when they shift attention
regulation. Children whose language skills increased away from the thing they desire (Cole et al., 2010).
more than other childrens appeared less frustrated Although these analyses did not reveal the qualita-
by a delay for a reward by preschool age, and the tive aspects of how children used words to distract
nature of their anger expressions improved more themselves, we can perhaps gain more insight
over time. Moreover, these relations between lan- using an anecdotal example taken from a 48-
guage skill and anger expression were partially month-old during the Wait Task. After approxi-
explained by childrens initiation of regulatory strat- mately 3 min of waiting, he began to express
egies. Language skills were associated with initia- frustration. Then he started pushing chairs around
tion of calm support seeking at 36 months of age the table, but this only briey interrupted his frus-
and of distraction at 48 months of age, both of tration. He began to count the chairs next, soon
which were associated with less angry expressions counting more generally. I counted! he said. Then
by preschool age. he turned attention back to the gift, Now can I
The ndings support the view that language open my prize? When told no, he resumed count-
skills in toddlerhood could help children regulate ing, up to 38, until he said, I cant count anymore.
their emotion as they reach preschool age (Kopp, I need my prize. This child was able to distract
1989), and lead to new questions about the specic himself for a full minute, longer than the average
ways that these skills may inuence the decline in duration of toddlers in this task, which is less than
anger reactivity. Understanding the timing and role 30 s. The longer duration of distraction may be
of language skill and specic regulatory strategies aided by the ability to verbally elaborate the activ-
could better inform the creation of developmentally ity of counting. Thus, it is possible that children
tailored interventions for emotion regulation dif- with early verbal skills may have more opportuni-
culties. The present evidence is consistent with the ties to learn how to use language in this way, such
view that the child who has a vocabulary from that his ability to shift attention is sustained by his
which to draw, and the ability to put words ability to use language while distracted.
together to communicate, may learn to regulate An alternative hypothesis to language develop-
anger well because they can verbalize their needs ment inuencing emotional development is that
without expressing frustration nonverbally during early individual differences in anger expression
early childhood (Cole et al., 2010; Vallotton & inuence the development of language skills
Ayoub, 2011). Moreover, the steeper the childs lan- (Bloom, 1993). Trend level associations between the
guage growth across early childhood the sooner the degree of decline in anger and subsequent language
child may be able to reect on and understand skill suggested that being high in anger reactivity
emotional experience and to share meaning with may interfere with language acquisition beyond
caregivers (Cole et al., 2010; Prizant & Wetherby, rst words and vocabulary spurt (Bloom, 1993).
902 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

Greater anger may interfere with interactions or exert attention control during a delay task. Lan-
relationships between caregivers and children, lim- guage skills and attention control both develop in
iting language learning experiences (e.g., Claussen, this age period (Rueda, Acosta, & Santonja, 2007;
Mundy, Mallik, & Willoughby, 2002). Poorly regu- Ruff & Rothbart, 1996) and are related to each other
lated anger may also compromise higher order cog- (Blair & Razza, 2007). Language may facilitate dis-
nitive and linguistic processing, recruiting resources traction as an emotion regulation strategy only after
needed for different stages of language learning executive attention has matured, which may explain
(Blair, 2002; Bloom, 1993). However, in this study, why infant and toddler regulatory efforts are lim-
associations between early anger and later language ited in effectiveness (Buss & Goldsmith, 1998; Stifter
skills were not as strong as prospective relations & Braungart, 1995) and more dependent upon
between early language ability and a decline in maternal warmth and responsiveness (Graziano,
anger reactivity. These ndings and studies of con- Calkins, & Keane, 2011). Future research should
current relations between emotion regulation and investigate both of these skill domains, including
language skill (Graziano, Reavis, Keane, & Calkins, more direct assessment of executive function, how
2007; Stansbury & Zimmerman, 1999) underscore they inuence one another over time, and how they
the need to more closely examine reciprocal rela- contribute, uniquely or conjointly, to better anger
tions between language and emotional develop- regulation skills. Carefully planned intervention
ment in early childhood. studies could play a key role in delineating the lon-
It is notable that regulatory strategies did not con- gitudinal inuences of one domain on another.
tribute to the associations between language skill and Given the present ndings, the next step is to
anger expression at all ages. The steeper the childrens investigate the specic aspects of language develop-
growth in language, the more they sought support ment that contribute to emotion regulation. Vocabu-
from their mothers at 36 months, which then contrib- lary size, rather than talkativeness, predicted
uted to a steeper decline in anger reactivity and less quality of self-regulation in one study (Valloton &
expressed anger by age 48 months. In this study, Ayoub, 2011). Such work must grapple with the
mothers were instructed to do as they normally did challenge of using developmentally sensitive mea-
when requiring their children to wait. Mothers tended sures across a period of rapid language growth. A
to become absorbed in completing questionnaires and single index, for example, MLU, may be more sen-
typically ignored childrens bids about the demands sitive at early ages but less so at later ages. We
of the wait. They may have thought that attending to used both naturalistic and standardized assess-
the child would make it more difcult to complete the ments appropriate to each child age as a robust
work or for the child to tolerate the wait. When they method of assessing individual differences in lan-
did interact with the child, mothers usually reminded guage over time, which yielded stability in lan-
the child to wait. For toddlers, this may render sup- guage level. However, this approach impeded our
port seeking ineffective in reducing anger. However, ability to determine which specic aspects of lan-
as children mature, support seeking may change, sup- guage (e.g., grammatical understanding, receptive
ported by better established language skills and cogni- vs. expressive skill) contribute at which ages.
tive skills such as working memory (Wolfe & Bell, Understanding the specic aspects of language that
2007). Indeed, childrens bids for support with the aid anger regulation would contribute to both basic
challenge become calmer and less angry by age and applied research. Furthermore, disentangling
36 months, suggesting change in how bids function the associations between language, attention, inhibi-
(Cole et al., 2011). Parent-focused interventions could tory control (Carlson & Wang, 2007), and tempera-
use this line of study to better inform suggested strate- mental factors such as effortful control will be
gies for parents of toddlers and children who struggle important for creating effective intervention in these
with challenging situations like waiting. early years and understanding how these processes
Similarly, toddler language skill predicted the work together. Our measure of effortful control is
duration of childrens distractions at both 36 and limited in that it is a parent-rated questionnaire,
48 months. Moreover, distraction duration was and future work could use multiple measures
associated with less anger expression only at across domains to create more robust comparisons.
48 months, perhaps because at this age childrens Because we studied a group of children who are
distractions were becoming more effective. This under represented in the literaturechildren whose
relation was found over and above the inuences family income was above the poverty threshold but
of effortful control, suggesting both language and less than middle incomethe generality of the nd-
effortful control contribute to a childs ability to ings is not known. We focused on families with
Language and Anger Expression 903

economic strain to capture variability that is associ- stranger at home and in the laboratory: A methodologi-
ated with income, but in a range of income that is cal study. Journal of Child Language, 27, 407427.
not correlated with the signicant advantages of Bridges, K. B. (1932). Emotional development in early
wealth and profound disadvantages of poverty (Hart infancy. Child Development, 3, 324341. doi:10.2307/
1125359
& Risley, 1995). Studies that embrace the full diver-
Brown, R. (1973). A rst language: The early stages. Cam-
sity of conditions in which childrens language and
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. doi:10.1126/
emotion regulation develop are critically needed. science.184.4143.1275
We did not investigate mothers contributions to Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger
child emotion regulation and language development regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics
in this study given our focus on self-regulation and of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359374.
the childs ability to initiate and maintain regulatory doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06195.x
strategies. Hart and Risley (1995) noted that care- Calkins, S. D., Gill, K. L., Johnson, M. C., & Smith, C. L.
giver responsiveness to young childrens verbal bids (1999). Emotional reactivity and emotion regulation
declined rapidly once children acquired language; strategies as predictors of social behavior with peers
the modal caregiver response to child bids by during toddlerhood. Social Development, 8, 310334.
doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00098
36 months was ignoring. However, a large literature
Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and
on the role of emotion discourse and parental sensi-
emotion regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Devel-
tivity underscores the need to understand the role opment, 22, 489510. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002
of caregivers in relations among language skills, Cassidy, K. W., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L.
anger regulation, and strategy use in early child- S., & Balaraman, G. (2003). The relationship between
hood. Further research about parental input in this psychological understanding and positive social behav-
process as well as additional third variables will iors. Social Development, 12, 198221. doi:10.1111/1467-
further determine the extent and process of how lan- 9507.00229
guage inuences emotion regulation development. Claussen, A. H., Mundy, P. C., Mallik, S. A., & Willoughby,
This study establishes longitudinal relations among J. C. (2002). Joint attention and disorganized attachment
language development, anger expression, and regu- status in infants at risk. Development and Psychopathology,
14, 279291. doi:10.1017/S0954579402002055
latory strategy use that can guide future studies that
Cohen, J. S., & Mendez, J. L. (2009). Emotion regula-
examine caregiver contributions.
tion, language ability, and the stability of preschool
childrens peer play behavior. Early Education & Devel-
References opment, 20, 10161037. doi:10.1080/10409280903305716
Ayoub, C., Vallotton, C. D., & Mastergeorge, A. M. Cole, P. M., Armstrong, L. M., & Pemberton, C. K. (2010).
(2011). Developmental pathways to integrated social The role of language in the development of emotion
skills: The roles of parenting and early intervention. regulation. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Devel-
Child Development, 82, 585600. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624. opment at the intersection of cognition and emotion. Wash-
2010.01549.x ington, DC: APA. doi:10.1037/12059-004
Baker, L., & Cantwell, D. P. (1992). Attention-decit dis- Cole, P. M., Tan, P. Z., Hall, S. E., Zhang, Y., Crnic, K.
order and speech/language disorders. Comprehensive A., Blair, C. B., et al. (2011). Developmental changes in
Mental Health Care, 2, 316. anger expression and attention focus: Learning to wait.
Berk, L. E. (1992). Childrens private speech: An overview Developmental Psychology, 47, 10781089. doi:10.1037/
of theory and the status of research. In R. M. Diaz & a0023813
L. E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to Cole, P. M., Teti, L. O., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). Mutual
self-regulation (pp. 85100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. emotion regulation and the stability of conduct prob-
Blair, C. B. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition lems between preschool and early school age. Develop-
and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of ment and Psychopathology, 15, 118. doi:10.1017/
childrens functioning at school entry. American Psychol- S0954579403000014
ogist, 57, 111127. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.57.2.111 Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Smith, K. D. (1994).
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, exec- Expressive control during a disappointment: Variations
utive function, and false belief understanding to emerging related to preschoolers behavior problems. Developmen-
math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Develop- tal Psychology, 30, 835846. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.30.6.
ment, 78, 647663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x 835
Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to language. Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J.,
New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., et al. (2003). Preschool
CBO9780511752797 emotional competence: Pathway to social competence.
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Painter, K. M., & Gene- Child Development, 74, 238256. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.
vro, J. L. (2000). Child language with mother and with 00533
904 Roben, Cole, and Armstrong

Dennis, T. (2006). Emotional self-regulation in preschool- Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of
ers: The interplay of child approach reactivity, parent- stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology,
ing, and control capacities. Developmental Psychology, 42, 58, 145173.
8497. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.84 Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in
Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R. C., & Greenberg, M. T. the everyday experience of young American children. Balti-
(2007). Improving young childrens social and more: Brookes.
emotional competence: A randomized trial of the pre- Irwin, J. R., Carter, A. S., & Briggs-Gowan, M. J. (2002).
school PATHS curriculum. Journal of Primary The social-emotional development of late-talking
Prevention, 28, 6791. doi:10.1007/s10935-007-0081-0 toddlers. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 13241332. doi:10.1097/
about feeling states between mothers and their young 00004583-200211000-00014
children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 132139. doi:10. Izard, C. E., King, K. A., Trentacosta, C. J., Morgan, J. K.,
1037//0012-1649.23.1.132 Laurenceau, J., Krauthamer-Ewing, E. S., et al. (2008).
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Hanish, L. D., & Spinrad, T. Accelerating the development of emotion competence
L. (2001). Preschoolers spontaneous emotion vocabu- in Head Start children: Effects on maladaptive behav-
lary: Relations to likeability. Early Education & Develop- ior. Development & Psychopathology, 20, 369397. doi:10.
ment, 12, 1127. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1201_2 1017/S0954579408000175
Fan, X., Thompson, B., & Wang, L. (1999). Effects of Johnson, M. H., & Munakata, Y. (2005). Processes of
sample size, estimation methods, and model specica- change in brain and cognitive development. Cognitive
tion on structural equation modeling t indexes. Sciences, 9, 152158.
Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 5683. doi:10.1080/ Jreskog, K. G. (1999). How large can a standardized
10705519909540119 coefcient be? Retrieved May 15, 2011, from http://
Feldman, R. (2009). The development of regulatory func- www.ssicentral.com/lisrel/techdocs/.
tions from birth to 5 years: Insights from premature Jreskog, K. G., & Srbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: Users
infants. Child Development, 80, 544561. doi:10.1111/j. reference guide. Lincolnwood, IL: SSI.
1467-8624.2009.01278.x Kline, R. B. (2004). Principles and practice of structural equa-
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., tion modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). The MacArthur Communica- Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effort-
tive Development Inventories: Users guide and technical ful control in early childhood: Continuity and change,
manual. San Diego, CA: Singular. antecedents, and implications for social development.
Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232. doi:10.1037//
Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged 0012-1649.36.2.220
preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the devel- Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L.,
opment of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 38, & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young
222235. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.38.2.222 children and its role in emerging internalization. Child
Gleason, J. B. (2005). The development of language. Boston, Development, 67, 490507. doi:10.2307/1131828
MA: Pearson. Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A
Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament via con- developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology,
struction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Question- 25, 343354. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.18.2.199
naire. Child Development, 67, 218235. Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative
Goldsmith, H. H., Reilly, J., Lemery, K. S., Longley, S., & emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psy-
Prescott, A. (1993). Manual for the Preschool Temperament chology, 25, 343354. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.25.3.343
Assessment Battery. Madison, WI: University of Wiscon- Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of nor-
sin. mal and abnormal behavior. Oxford, England: Liveright.
Graziano, P. A., Calkins, S. D., & Keane, S. P. (2011). Sus- MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for
tained attention development during the toddlerhood analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
to preschool period: Associations with toddlers emo- Miller, J. (1981). Assessing language production in children:
tion regulation strategies and maternal behavior. Infant Experimental procedures. Baltimore: University Park Press.
and Child Development, 20, 389408. doi:10.1002/icd.731 Molenaar, P. (1996). THEIL: A FORTRAN program for
Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. robust covariance matrix estimation. Unpublished pro-
D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation and childrens gram manual.
early academic success. Journal of School Psychology, 45, Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (1997). Test of
319. doi:10.1016/j.jsp. 2006.09.002 Language Development3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Grolnick, W. S., Bridges, L. J., & Connell, J. P. (1996). Potegal, M., Kosorok, M. R., & Davidson, R. J. (1996). The
Emotion regulation in two-year-olds: Strategies and time course of angry behavior in the temper tantrums of
emotional expression in four contexts. Child Develop- young children. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
ment, 67, 928941. doi:10.2307/1131871 ences, 794, 3145. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1996.tb32507.x
Language and Anger Expression 905

Prizant, B. M., & Wetherby, A. M. (1990). Towards an Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New
integrated view of early language and communication York: Basic Books.
development and socioemotional development. Topics Stifter, C. A., & Braungart, J. M. (1995). The regulation of
in Language Disorders, 10, 116. doi:10.1097/00011363- negative reactivity in infancy: Function and development.
199009000-00003 Developmental Psychology, 31, 448455. doi:10.1037//0012-
Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. 1649.31.3.448
(2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in
effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71, 11131144. search of denition. Monographs of the Society for
doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106009 Research in Child Development, 59(23), 2552. doi:10.
Rueda, M. R., Acosta, A., & Santonja, M. (2007). Atten- 1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x
tion and motivation interdependence in self-regula- Vallotton, C., & Ayoub, C. (2011). Use your words: The
tion: A neurocognitive approach. In L. V. Brown (Ed.), role of language in the development of toddlers self-
Psychology of motivation (pp. 2945). Hauppauge, NY: regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 169
Nova Science. 181.
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early Vaughn, B. E., Kopp, C. B., & Krakow, J. B. (1984). The
development: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford emergence and consolidation of self-control from eigh-
University Press. teen to thirty months of age: Normative trends and
Semel, E., Wing, E., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical evalua- individual differences. Child Development, 55, 9901004.
tion of Language FundamentalsThird Edition (CELF3). doi:10.2307/1130151
San Antonio, TS: Psychological Corp. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Oxford, Eng-
Sethi, A., Mischel, W., Aber, J. L., Shoda, Y., & Rodri- land: Wiley. doi:10.1037/11193-000
guez, M. L. (2000). The role of strategic attention Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale
deployment in development of self-regulation: of Intelligence (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Predicting preschoolers delay of gratication from Corporation.
mother-toddler interactions. Developmental Psychology, Winsler, A., & Naglieri, J. (2003). Overt and covert verbal
36, 767777. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.36.6.767 problem-solving strategies: Developmental trends in
Shaw, D. S., Bell, R. Q., & Gilliom, M. (2000). A truly use, awareness, and relations with task performance in
early starter model of antisocial behavior revisited. children aged 5 to 17. Child Development, 74, 659678.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3, 155172. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00561
doi:10.1023/A:1009599208790 Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2007). The integration of cog-
Stansbury, K., & Zimmerman, L. K. (1999). Relations nition and emotion during infancy and early childhood:
among child language skills, maternal socialization of Regulatory processes associated with the development
emotion regulation, and child behavior problems. Child of working memory. Brain and Cognition, 65, 313.
Psychiatry and Human Development, 30, 121142. doi:10. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.01.009
1023/A:1021954402840
This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy.
Users should refer to the original published version of the material.

You might also like