You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Consumer Marketing

Giving and getting gift cards


Erhard K. Valentin, Anthony T. Allred,
Article information:
To cite this document:
Erhard K. Valentin, Anthony T. Allred, (2012) "Giving and getting gift cards", Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 29 Issue: 4,
pp.271-279, https://doi.org/10.1108/07363761211237344
Permanent link to this document:
https://doi.org/10.1108/07363761211237344
Downloaded on: 04 July 2017, At: 09:34 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 31 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 1320 times since 2012*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
(2013),"Gift-giving among adolescents: exploring motives, the effects of givers' personal characteristics and the use of
impression management tactics", Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 30 Iss 5 pp. 436-449 <a href="https://doi.org/10.1108/
JCM-01-2013-0426">https://doi.org/10.1108/JCM-01-2013-0426</a>
(2002),"Brand choice in gift-giving: recipient influence", Journal of Product &amp; Brand Management, Vol. 11 Iss 4 pp. 237-249
<a href="https://doi.org/10.1108/10610420210435434">https://doi.org/10.1108/10610420210435434</a>

Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-srm:463963 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for Authors service
information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Please visit
www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company manages a portfolio of
more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as well as providing an extensive range of online
products and additional customer resources and services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee on Publication Ethics
(COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation.

*Related content and download information correct at time of download.


Giving and getting gift cards
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred
John B. Goddard School of Business & Economics, Weber State University, Layton, Utah, USA

Abstract
Purpose The reported study was designed to provide insight into gift cards as gifts and their place among gifts of cash and goods. It also was
designed to identify promising avenues for further research.
Design/methodology/approach Data were collected using a structured questionnaire administered to a convenience sample of 317 respondents of
both sexes who varied greatly in age.
Findings Effective liquidity served largely as the basis for categorizing gift cards. The greater a cards effective liquidity, the more its economic impact
on the recipient resembles that of cash. The results indicated the following: face value affects recipient preference for effective liquidity; the giver-getter
relationship affects recipient preference for effective liquidity; the gift cards givers give tend to have less effective liquidity than those they prefer to get;
some gift cards are more appropriate gifts than others and some, but not all, gift cards are more appropriate gifts than cash; and people feel less guilt
when paying for personal luxuries with gift cards than with cash.
Research limitations The study was largely exploratory insofar as its breadth greatly exceeded its depth and findings derived from a convenience
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

sample.
Originality/value The study introduced effective liquidity as a basis for assessing similarities and differences between gift cards and gifts of cash
and goods. Findings enhance scholarly understanding of gift cards and their place among gifts of cash and goods. Moreover, they afford insights into
marketing gift cards and into promising paths for further research.

Keywords Gift cards, Effective liquidity, Preference asymmetry, Consumer behavior, Liquidity, Gifts

Paper type Research paper

An executive summary for managers and executive The study reported in this article was designed to better
readers can be found at the end of this article. understand the gift cards place in the general scheme of gift
giving and getting and to identify promising avenues for
1. Introduction further research. It probed whether card face value and the
giver-getter relationship affect the type of gift card recipients
Gift giving and getting have been studied for more than a prefer to get, whether givers prefer to get the cards they give,
century from diverse disciplinary perspectives, including whether gift cards are more appropriate gifts than cash, and
anthropology, economics, and social psychology. whether paying for personal luxuries with gift cards alleviates
Collectively, findings suggest cash is the best present when guilt that sometimes arise from making such purchases
recipients apply only narrow utilitarian evaluation criteria (Burnett and Lunsford, 1994). The studys conceptual
(Waldfogel, 1993, 2002, 2005), but generally, gift evaluation underpinnings are delineated in the next section, which also
is more complex and emotional than economic theories of summarizes prior research that guided this inquiry.
utilitarian rationality posit (Camerer, 1988; Teigen et al., Afterwards, research methodology, findings, and
2005; White, 2006, 2008). implications are addressed.
The gifting literature, despite its vastness, affords only
sparse insights into gift cards, which account for an enormous
share of the money spent on gifts (Grannis, 2010). Most 2. Conceptual underpinnings and literature
gifting studies were conducted before modern gift cards
invented in 1995 (Offenberg, 2007) became sufficiently review
prominent to attract scholarly attention. Further, much recent Prior research into gifts of cash and gifts of goods is a rich
gifting research has not focused directly on understanding gift source of hypotheses about giving and getting gift cards.
cards. Notable exceptions are studies that examined gift cards Utilitarian economics implies that, when the giver knows
in relation to cash and gifts of goods. While some gift cards exactly what the getter wants and chooses accordingly, then a
are essentially debit cards that, like cash, can be used widely, gifts value to the recipient may exceed its purchase price
others restrict purchases severely. Restricted cards are not because the recipient is spared the burdens of shopping.
nearly as liquid as cash and, in some ways, resemble gifts of Otherwise, cash is the best gift (Waldfogel, 1993, 2002,
goods (White, 2006, 2008). 2005). Giving cash minimizes the givers risk of making a poor
selection, requires minimal shopping, and allows recipients
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at maximal freedom of choice. Nevertheless, giving goods
www.emeraldinsight.com/0736-3761.htm remains popular because neither gift givers nor gift getters
generally evaluate presents solely on utilitarian grounds
(Camerer, 1988; Caplow, 1982, 1984; Khalil, 2004;
Journal of Consumer Marketing Waldfogel, 2002; Webley and Wilson, 1989). Broadly
29/4 (2012) 271 279
q Emerald Group Publishing Limited [ISSN 0736-3761]
categorized, gift selection criteria reflect the givers self-
[DOI 10.1108/07363761211237344] centered intentions, which are egoistic, and recipient-centered

271
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

intentions, or aims to delight the recipient (Belk, 1976; .


Circumstances contextual particulars. The giver-getter
Camerer, 1988; Sherry, 1983). relationship is among the most salient contextual
influences on gift evaluation (Burgoyne and Routhb,
2.1 Self-centered intentions 1991). It may be close or distant and may or may not be
Common self-centered intentions, or motives, include making romantic. The occasion, custom, and innumerable other
a good impression, but without spending too much money or circumstantial factors also may affect the value recipients
time on acquiring a gift. Cash generally is the most convenient place on gifts (Otnes et al., 1993; Sherry, 1983; Webley
gift. However, it has been stigmatized widely, albeit not and Wilson, 1989).
universally, as a thoughtless, lazy givers gift, while nicely .
Liquidity the ease with which a gift can be exchanged
wrapped goods have been portrayed as potentially more without penalty. Of all conceivable gifts, cash is most
personal, endearing, respectful, and socially acceptable liquid in modern industrialized societies because
presents (Burgoyne and Routhb, 1991; Khalil, 2004; recipients can spend it as they please when they please.
Waldfogel, 2002; Webley et al., 1983; Webley and Wilson, Further, cash generally can be exchanged more readily for
1989). goods or gift cards than vice versa.
Common self-centered intentions also include fulfilling
obligations; maintaining relationships; wanting to appear The most liquid gift cards are pure debit cards that can be
generous, knowledgeable, or sophisticated; exerting influence spent wherever debit cards are accepted. They are fungible
and nearly indistinguishable from cash. The effective liquidity
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

or control; and signaling social standing, respect, affection, or


criticism (Camerer, 1988; Sherry, 1983). Affection varies of restricted cards, which are recognized only by specific
greatly in kind, as do gifts that signal the intended kind of vendors, may or may not approach the liquidity of cash. A gift
affection (Belk and Coon, 1993). Jewelry and lingerie send cards effective liquidity depends on its instrumentality in
more intimate messages than cash and toasters, for example, putting cash in the recipients pockets. Walmart cards, for
while a gift of bold apparel may serve to criticize or influence instance, have high effective liquidity because they can be
the recipients tastes. used to buy items that most households can use readily and
purchase routinely. In effect, they are cash because they free
2.2 Recipient-centered intentions up cash that recipients ordinarily would spend on household
Recipient-centered intentions are altruistic, or agapic (Belk necessities. Jewelry cards, in contrast, have less effective
and Coon, 1993); they motivate givers to seek gifts recipients liquidity because they less often purchase merchandise that
want and value (Sherry, 1983). To varying degrees, the value card recipients considered buying with money at hand.
recipients place on gifts including cash, traditional non-cash Offenbergs (2005, 2007) study of gift cards auctioned on
items, and gift cards depends on the following: eBay by their original recipients suggests that the bids sellers
.
Symbolic and sentimental value emotions aroused by are willing to accept depend on both effective card liquidity
inferences derived from a gift about the givers intentions and face value. On average, sellers accepted bids that netted
or disposition toward the recipient (Camerer, 1988; them 80 percent of face value, which implies a 20 percent
Khalil, 2004). White (2006, 2008) found that purchases welfare loss, defined as follows (Waldfogel, 1993):
made with gift cards tend to be more memorable and have
greater symbolic value than those made with gifted cash. Gift purchase price 2 Cash equivalent
Welfare loss 100
Giver concerns about a gifts symbolic or sentimental Gift purchase price
value may be self- or recipient-centered.
.
Serendipity pleasant surprise, which sometimes is a gifts The cash equivalent equals the amount of cash that renders
most valued benefit (Caplow, 1984). For instance, a gift of the recipient indifferent between the gift and cash.
opera tickets that the recipient would not have bought Welfare loss was least for cards redeemable at home
might lead unexpectedly to a pleasant experience and improvement and general merchandise discount stores. It was
discovery of a musical genre that becomes a favorite. greatest for cards redeemable only at jewelry or clothing
Likewise, receiving a boutique- or restaurant-specific gift stores. Accordingly, differences in effective liquidity seem to
card may culminate in a joyful experience the recipient explain differences in welfare loss, at least in part. The greater
would have missed. a cards effective liquidity, the greater its cash value to the
.
Guilt relief the extent to which paying for personal recipient and the lower the welfare loss.
luxuries, such as jewelry, with a gift card or a gift of cash Welfare loss increased with card size, or face value, which
alleviates guilt (Kivetz and Keinan, 2006). Tensions may reflect differences in the ease with which recipients can
commonly exist between prudence and indulgence. They spend a gift card on things they would buy without it. The
are moderated by regret for having been imprudent and, smaller the card, the easier. Hence, the relationship between
thus, having jeopardized future well-being or by regret for card size and welfare loss indicated by Offenbergs eBay study
having been too prudent to enjoy life (Burnett and may reflect differences in effective liquidity.
Lunsford, 1994; Keinan and Kivetz, 2008).
Inducing consumers to precommit to an indulgent purchase 2.3 Risk and motives
mitigates guilt when actually purchasing (Kivetz and Waldfogel (1993) found that recipients of traditional
Simonson, 2002a, b). For instance, in advance of occasions Christmas gifts of goods would have realized substantially
on which they anticipate getting gift cards, prospective more value from cash equal to the purchase price. In his
recipients may precommit to using cards received for self- words, between a tenth and a third of the value of holiday
indulgent purposes. Imagining the joy of using a gift card to gifts is destroyed by gift-giving (p. 1336). Accordingly, gift
buy hedonic goods or services may intensify precommit and givers have ample recipient- and self-centered reasons to fear
alleviate guilt further (Zaltman, 2000). their gifts will miss the intended mark and may give cash or

272
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

readily spendable gift cards not because they are lazy or Table I Respondent demographic
uncaring, but because they want to be safe (Waldfogel, 2002).
Whether recipient- or self-centered, concerns about giving a Sex
gift that will be well-received are apt to be more intense when Age group Female Male Totals
the giver-getter relationship is close rather than distant (Belk, 18-20 24 7 31
1982). 21-25 43 40 83
26-30 20 34 54
2.4 Gift preference asymmetries 31-40 22 31 53
Gift givers frequently give gifts different from those they 41-50 29 13 42
prefer to get. Differences, or asymmetries, between the gifts
> 50 35 18 53
givers prefer to give and those they prefer to get are
Totals 173 143 316a
potentially attributable to differences in givers and getters
preferences, errors in givers judgments, and self-centered Notes: aOne usable questionnaire did not provide age information
motives (Teigen et al., 2005).

Several common statistical tests were used to analyze data


3. Research questions and aims
(Kanji, 2006). They are noted as results are presented and
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

The preceding paragraphs raise issues about gift card giving discussed.
and getting that are too numerous to address in any single
study. Accordingly, the present study was limited to probing
the following questions:
5. Results and discussion
Q1. Does card face value affect recipient preference for The ensuing discussions address each research question. Card
effective liquidity? size refers to face value; denominations are in US currency.
Q2. Does the giver-getter relationship close or distant
affect recipient preference for effective gift-card 5.1 Q1: Does card face value affect recipient preference
liquidity? for effective liquidity?
Q3. Are givers inclined to give the kinds of gift cards they Respondents were asked to consider two hypothetical gift
prefer to get? cards, named the Everyday Card and the Special Card.
Q4. Are even highly liquid gift cards more appropriate gifts As described, the two cards differed, respectively, in their
than cash? capacity to purchases utilitarian necessities and discretionary
Q5. Do gift cards relieve spending guilt? personal non-necessities, also called hedonic, special, and
luxury items (Kivetz and Simonson, 2002a). The Everyday
Offenberg (2005, 2007) alluded to Q1 in a study that raised
Card could be used to buy household mainstays, such as
many further questions. Much prior research has addressed
gasoline and groceries, but nothing special. In contrast, the
giver-getter relationships and their impact on gift evaluation.
Special Card could be used only to purchase enjoyable non-
Moreover, a few studies have focused on give-get
necessities that the recipient seldom, if ever, would buy
asymmetries. But Q2 and Q3, specifically, have not yet been
without a gift card. Since the Everyday Card could be spent
answered. Inquiries into Q4 and Q5 were intended to further
on items nearly every household bought routinely, it
explore recipients perceptions of gift cards in relation to cash.
effectively put cash in recipients pockets and, in effect, was
much more liquid than the Special Card.
4. Research methodology Respondents answered several preference questions by
choosing from the following replies:
Data were collected using a four-page questionnaire that .
1 Strongly prefer the Everyday Card.
instructed respondents to read questions carefully, mark their .
2 Slightly prefer the Everyday Card.
answers, and return the completed questionnaire to the survey .
3 No preference.
administrator. Respondents resided in a metropolitan area of .
4 Slightly prefer the Special Card.
the western USA. They were selected on a convenience basis .
5 Strongly prefer the Special Card.
and provided 317 usable questionnaires. A breakdown of the
sample by age and sex is shown in Table I. Thus, values below 3.0 denote preference for the Everyday
Two sections of the questionnaire solicited information Card, while values above 3.0 denote preference for the Special
about respondents gift card giving and getting preferences. Card.
Some questions centered on preferences for two hypothetical Two preference questions stipulated card denominations of
gift cards, one having greater effective liquidity than the other. $50 and $100 and portrayed the giver as a casual
Additional preference questions probed into restricted cards acquaintance. The topmost portion of Table II shows
identified by vendor name (e.g. Dillards) or description (e.g. (x $50A 2:359) , (x $100A 2:632) , 3.0, which indicates
restaurant). Card face values were specified as US$50 and that, regardless of card size, respondents preferred getting
US$100, and three giver-getter relationships were stipulated. the comparatively liquid Everyday Card from casual
A third section included questions about the appropriateness acquaintances. However, the smaller the card, the stronger
of gift cards in relation to gifts of cash and questions about their preference for the more liquid option (p , 0:01).
guilt experienced when buying personal luxuries with gift As reported in Table II, comparable results were obtained
cards. The last section solicited common demographic when respondents were asked to choose between an Everyday
information about sex, age, education, marital status, and Card and a Special Card under the assumption they had won
financial status. a gift card. Of 317 participants, 59.62 percent chose the $50

273
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

Table II Effects of face value and source on recipient preference for effective gift card liquidity
Test statistic and p for
Question Null hypothesisa,b Sample meansa,b n differencesc
Q1: Does card face value affect recipient H0: m$50A m$100A x $50A 2.359 315 t 4.167 p , 0.01
preference for effective liquidity? x $100A 2.632
Difference 0.273
H0: p$50E p$100E p^ $50E 0.5962 317 z 4.807 p , 0.01
p^ $100E 0.4826
Difference 0.1136
Q2: Does the giver-getter relationship H0: m$50A m$50F x $50F 3.590 315 t 12.841 p , 0.00
affect recipient preference for effective x $50A 2.359
liquidity? Difference 1.231
Notes: am and x denote hypothesized and sample means, respectively, of items measured on a scale ranging from 1 Strongly prefer Everyday Card to
5 Strongly prefer Special Card, with 3 indicating no preference. Respectively, p and p^ denote hypothesized and sample relative frequencies. bSubscripts denote
card denominations ($50 or $100) and source (A=casual acquaintance, F=dear friend) or card choice (E=Everyday Card). cFrom z-test for proportions with
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

dependent samples
Source: Kanji, 2006, pp. 57-8

Everyday Card over the $50 Special Card. But preference for card will have lost 50 percent of its value. Medium-sized cards
the relatively liquid Everyday Card dropped to 48.26 percent may be too large, yet too small, to raise the aforementioned
when respondents were asked to choose between a $100 utilitarian issues.
Everyday Card and a $100 Special Card. The result
( p^ $50E 0:5962) . ( p^ $100E 0:4826), also, indicates a 5.2 Q2: Does the giver-getter relationship affect
significant inverse relationship between card size and recipient preference for effective liquidity?
preference for effective liquidity (p , 0:01). One question identified the giver as a dear friend and the card
In sum, these results indicate effective liquidity is valued amount as $50. The last row of Table II shows the following:
more in small than large gift cards when the source is (x$50F 3:590) . 3.0 . (x$50A 2:359), where x $50F and
impersonal. Accordingly, they imply that the percentage x $50A , respectively, denote mean preference ratings for a $50
discount from face value recipients would accept if offered card from a dear friend and a $50 card from a casual
cash for their cards varies inversely with card size. The smaller acquaintance. Since preference for the Special Card is
the card, the larger the acceptable percentage discount. indicated by scores above 3.0, these results imply
Offenbergs (2005, 2007) study of gift cards sold on eBay respondents preferred to get the $50 Special Card from
indicated the opposite relationship. The larger the card, the close friends, despite its low effective liquidity. However, they
greater the accepted percentage discount. Correspondingly, preferred to get the comparatively liquid $50 Everyday Card
the larger the card, the greater the welfare loss and preference from casual acquaintances (p , 0:0001). The closer the giver-
getter relationship, the less recipients appear to value effective
for liquid media of exchange.
liquidity.
Although results from the present study do not support
Offenbergs (2005, 2007) findings, they do not refute them 5.3 Q3: Are givers inclined to give the cards they prefer
either. Plausible reconciliatory explanations include the to get?
following. Replies to several questions facilitated assessing whether
First, Offenbergs sample of 2002 gift cards sold on eBay givers are inclined to give the cards they prefer to get. One
seems decidedly unrepresentative because people who trade question asked respondents to choose between giving an
on eBay are very different from the population at large and, Everyday Card and a Special Card to a particular friend with
thus, may have very different preferences than typical gift card who they are not romantically involved and who is not a
recipients (Hou and Elliott, 2010). Further, the mean face relative. Card size was not specified. As shown in Table III,
value of cards in Offenbergs eBay sample was $192 while gift only 38.29 percent of 316 respondents chose to give the
cards sold in the U.S.A. average around $40 (Grannis, 2010). Everyday Card. Yet, when the two gift cards were framed as
Very few cards near average size were represented in prizes, 59.62 percent of the sample preferred getting a $50
Offenbergs sample. The present study focused on $50 and Everyday Card to getting the $50 Special Card, while 48.26
$100 denominations. percent preferred getting the $100 Everyday Card to getting
Second, the relationship between card size and preference the $100 Special Card.
for liquid media of exchange may be U-shaped. Recipients Both give-get differences shown in Table III are statistically
may devalue very large illiquid cards because they fear they significant at p , 0:01. However, the give-get difference was
can redeem such cards only for items priced well above their more pronounced when the card won had a $50 face value
cash equivalent. And they may devalue small illiquid cards (z 6:55; p , 0:001) than when it had a $100 face value
because, without cash supplements, such cards seem unlikely (z 2:98; p , 0:005).
to purchase anything truly appealing. If the recipient of a $20 Whether givers give the kinds of cards they prefer to get was
gift card, for example, paid $40 for an item he would buy explored further by asking respondents to consider getting
without a card only if it cost $30 or less, then, in effect, the and giving the following gift cards:

274
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

Table III Giver preferences and give-get comparisons


Research question Null hypothesisa Sample meansa n z and pb
Q3: Are givers inclined to give the cards H0: pE(Give) p$50E(Get) p^ E(Give) 0.3829 316 z 6.552 p , 0.001
they prefer to get? p^ $50E(Get) 0.5962
Difference 0.2133
H0: pE(Give) p$100E(Get) p^ E(Give) 0.3829 316 z 2.982 p , 0.005
p^ $100E(Get) 0.4826
Difference 0.0997
Notes: apE(Give) denotes the relative frequency of card givers who would give the Everyday Card rather than the Special Card; p$50E(Get) and p$100E(Get) denote the
relative frequencies, respectively, of card winners who would choose the $50 and the $100 Everyday Card. bFrom z-test for proportions with dependent samples
Source: Kanji, 2006, pp. 57-8

.
Card A: Good for $100 of groceries or gasoline from your favored Card D as givers (49 percent) than as getters (27
favorite grocery retailer or gas station. percent).
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

.
Card B: Good for $100 at Dillards, Macys, Nordstroms, Respondents also were asked, Which gift card would you be
and all jewelry stores. least likely to give a close friend who is not a relative and with
.
Card C: Good for $100 on all cameras, audio systems, and who you are not romantically involved? That question was
video equipment at all stores that carry electronics. followed by, Which gift card from a close friend would thrill
.
Card D: Good for $100 at any nice restaurant where you least? Results are reported in Table V. The overall last-
dinner costs at least $15 per adult. choice give-get differences, too, are statistically significant
(x2 13:05; p , 0:005), which further affirms that give and
The Card A description was intended to depict a card much get preferences differ. However, the last-choice differences are
like the Everyday Card one high in effective liquidity and less striking than the first-choice differences.
nearly as impersonal as cash. In contrast, the Card C and D Present findings are consistent with results from a survey of
descriptions were intended to suggest much less liquid cards Norwegian students (Teigen et al., 2005). Respondents were
cards similar to the Special Card. The Card B description presented with pair-wise choices between distinctive items
was intended to suggest a card akin to the Everyday Card and (e.g. one bottle of excellent Bordeaux) and more mundane
cash in some aspects and akin to the Special Card in others. gifts of equal cost (e.g. two bottles of good Chilean wine).
Accordingly, the card was described as one that could be Participants usually favored giving the relatively special item,
redeemed at several department stores that sold various even though they preferred getting the other gift. Further,
personal and household necessities, as well as a wide variety of respondents favored getting cash, but were reluctant to give it.
hedonic merchandise, including jewelry. Neither differences between givers and getters wants nor
Respondents were asked first: Which gift card would you misconceptions about recipients preferences accounted for
give a close friend who is not a relative and with who you are the Norwegian give-get preference asymmetries. Instead, the
evaluation mode, joint or separate, seemed to account
not romantically involved? Then they were asked: Which gift
substantially for give-get preference asymmetries, which
card from a close friend would thrill you most? Results are
were much greater when respondents were presented with a
reported in Table IV. choice of gifts (joint evaluation) than when they were asked to
Overall, the first-choice give-get differences are statistically
evaluate gifts one at a time (separate evaluation).
significant (x2 63:84; p , 0:000). This result, like those Teigen et al. (2005) argued that recipients generally evaluate
shown in Table III, also indicates givers are apt to give cards gifts separately. Hence, relevant asymmetries are much
unlike the ones they prefer to receive. Especially interesting smaller than those detected in studies that offer both givers
are the Card B and D give-get differences. As card recipients, and getters choices. However, Teigen et al. (2005) noted, also,
the largest number of respondents, 33 percent, said Card B that this generalization does not apply when gifts evoke
would thrill them most. Yet, only 21 percent said they were comparisons. A damaged item, for example, evokes
most likely to give Card B. Conversely, more respondents comparisons between that item and its undamaged

Table IV Q3 supplement: give-get differences by type of card


Most likely to Most thrilled to
give get Differencea
2
First choice by type of card (x 5 63:84; p < 0:000) n (%) n (%) z p<
A: Good for groceries or gasoline 54 17 67 21 1.642 0.100
B: Good at Dillards, Macys, and Nordstroms 66 21 104 33 4.841 0.000
C: Good for cameras, audio, and video 41 13 60 19 2.469 0.015
D: Good at any nice restaurant 154 49 86 27 7.266 0.000
Notes: aFrom z-test for proportions with dependent samples
Source: Kanji, 2006, pp. 57-8

275
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

Table V Q3 supplement: give-get differences by type of card


Least likely to Least thrilled to
give get Differencea
Last choice by type of card (x2 5 13:05; p < 0:005) n (%) n (%) z p<
A: Good for groceries or gasoline 118 37 89 28 3.314 0.002
B: Dillards, Macys, Nordstroms, jewelry stores 70 22 85 27 2.190 0.030
C: Good for cameras, audio, and video 103 32 108 34 0.662 0.550
D: Good at any nice restaurant 26 8 34 11 1.374 0.200
Notes: aFrom z-test for proportions with dependent samples
Source: Kanji, 2006, pp. 57-8

equivalent. Seemingly, gift cards, whose face values inevitably 6. Synopsis of findings and implications
become known to recipients, evoke spontaneous comparisons
to cash. Inspired by Offenbergs (2005, 2007) work, the reported
study introduced effective liquidity explicitly as a construct
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

that facilitates understanding the gift cards place among gifts


5.4 Q4: Are even highly liquid gift cards more of cash and goods (Waldfogel, 1993; White, 2006, 2008). It
appropriate gifts than cash? revealed that face value and the giver-getter relationship affect
Cash has been characterized widely as a lazy, impersonal gift recipient preference for effective liquidity; givers tend to give
because giving it requires little deliberation or shopping gift cards more liquid than those they prefer to receive; even
(Camerer, 1988; Webley et al., 1983). Many gift cards, among highly liquid gift cards, some cards are more
including Target and Walmart cards, resemble cash gifts appropriate gifts than others; some, but not all, gift cards,
because they, too, are convenient and readily spendable. are more appropriate gifts than cash; and people tend to feel
Hence, recipients may evaluate them much as they do cash. less guilt when paying for personal luxuries with gift cards
Whether that is the case was explored by asking respondents than with cash. These findings have both managerial and
to rate the appropriateness of Target and Walmart cards in research implications.
relation to cash. On a scale ranging from 1 strongly agree to
4 strongly disagree, respondents were asked to indicate the 6.1 Managerial implications
extent to which they agreed with the statement, Usually a Preliminarily, results from this study suggest the following
Target gift card is a perfectly appropriate gift. Like questions when viewed in light of prior research. Gift cards may be
were asked with respect to Walmart gift cards and cash. The promising solutions to dilemmas spawned by conflicting self-
lower the score, the more appropriate the gift. centered intentions, such as wanting to minimize shopping
Overall, the rating differences were found statistically time and wanting to make a good impression. Buying them
significant via repeated-measures ANOVA generally requires little time or effort, and their social
(F 16:91; p , 0:000). Further, Tukey HSD tests rendered acceptability and sentimental value may exceed that of cash.
the Target-cash and Target-Walmart differences significant However, effective gift card promotion requires tailoring
(p , 0:01). Respondents deemed the Target gift card most messages to fit the givers motives and relationship with the
appropriate ( x 2:176) and the Walmart card least recipient. For instance, promotions that advise buying a gift
appropriate (x 2:465), but not significantly less appropriate card to save time are more likely to resonate with givers
than cash (x 2:381). Evidently, adjudged appropriateness seeking gifts for casual acquaintances than with givers seeking
does vary among gift cards, even those that are readily gifts for dear friends. Accordingly, merchants seem well-
available and exceedingly liquid. advised to aim communications that emphasize the ease with
which gift cards can be bought at givers who value
5.5 Q5: Do gift cards relieve spending guilt? convenience as much as correctness and are in search of
On a four-point scale ranging from 1 strongly agree to gifts for casual acquaintances. Marketing messages should
4 strongly disagree, which has a midpoint of 2.5, remind anxious givers that giving goods can be hazardous and
respondents were asked to indicate the extent to which they giving cash is crass.
agreed with the following statements: Purveyors of gift cards redeemable for personal luxuries
If I bought something that I really did not need, I would feel guilty if I paid might emphasize that recipients welcome opportunities to
for it with cash or a credit card. indulge. Gift cards that facilitate indulgent spending seem
If I bought something I really did not need, I would feel guilty if I paid for it especially appropriate when they are intended for close
with a gift card that I could have used to buy groceries or gasoline. friends. Accordingly, promotions aimed at givers seeking gifts
for close friends might emphasize a gift cards hedonic
Respondents agreed somewhat with the first statement purchasing power and the variety of hedonic choices it
( x 2:304) and disagreed somewhat with the second affords. To prompt prospective recipients to ask for the
statement (x 2:677); the difference was statistically featured card, promotions might encourage likely recipients to
significant (t 7:04; p , 0:000). Apparently, people feel anticipate receiving the card and envision prospective
somewhat less guilt when buying personal luxuries with gift purchases.
cards even gift cards that purchase household necessities Some gift cards seem more acceptable than others under
than they do when buying them with cash or a credit card. particular or even general circumstances. For instance,

276
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

relatively liquid cards appear to be better gifts for casual the voucher (Teigen et al., 2005). For reasons that remain to
acquaintances than dear friends, and cards issued by be determined, give-get preference gaps found in this study
downscale vendors seem no more appropriate than cash. were significantly wider for $50 than $100 cards. Seemingly,
Restaurant cards, while popular among givers and some get preferences approach give preferences as gift card size
recipients, are less preferred, on average, than department increases, at least over some range.
store cards. These findings suggest some gift card vendors
face formidable challenges. For instance, identifying likely 6.2.4 Gift card appropriateness
buyers of their cards and developing compelling promotional Many gift cards resemble cash insofar as they require little
messages seems more difficult for downscale than upscale shopping time and, in effect, are nearly as liquid as cash. This
vendors. The give-get preference gap for restaurant cards may study suggests, however, that some such cards are more
portend a decline in sales of such cards, unless restauranteurs appropriate gifts than others. For example, Target cards were
reposition their offerings in the minds of gift card recipients. adjudged more appropriate gifts than Walmart cards, and
Walmart cards were no more appropriate than cash.
6.2 Limitations and implications for further research Why one card is considered more appropriate than another
The noted managerial implications are preliminary because is unclear and in need of further investigation. The most
the reported study was largely exploratory insofar as its apparent difference between Target and Walmart is store
breadth greatly exceeded its depth and findings derived from a image. Accordingly, adjudged gift card appropriateness may
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

convenience sample of 317 people residing within one depend on the image of the vendor whose name appears on
metropolitan area. Although both sexes and all relevant age the card: The more upscale the vendor, the more appropriate
groups were represented amply, greater geographic diversity the card, at least to a point. How pure debit cards are
would have rendered findings more conclusive and perceived as gifts in relation to cash and highly liquid
generalizable. But whether random sampling or quota restricted gift cards, too, requires further investigation.
sampling would have produced a sample more Adjudged card appropriateness also may depend on a cards
representative of the relevant population is unclear because aptness to be seen as cash or evoke comparisons to cash.
the bounds of the relevant population are obscure. Cards seen as cash may be stigmatized much like cash.
While this study shed some light on gift cards as gifts, it also Seemingly, gift cards evoke spontaneous comparisons to cash
made clear that understanding the gift cards place among because their face values are transparent (Teigen et al., 2005).
gifts requires much additional research. In general, it requires Nevertheless, some cards may be more susceptible than
further research by type of gift card, type of giver, type of others to such comparisons. Further, a cards capacity to
recipient, and type of situation, including culture, the giver- engender memorable purchases rich in symbolic meaning may
getter relationship, and the occasion. More specific research depend on its perceived resemblance to cash (White, 2006,
implications derived from present findings are enumerated in 2008).
the following.
6.2.5 Gift cards and guilt
6.2.1 Preference for liquidity and card size
Respondents felt less guilty about buying personal luxuries
This study and prior research suggest card size affects
with gift cards, including cards that could be spent on
recipient preference for effective liquidity. Yet unclear is
household necessities, than they felt about buying them with
whether the relationship between card size and preference for
cash or a credit card. Since prior research has shown gifts and
liquidity is direct, inverse, or U-shaped.
windfalls are largely exempt from the mental accounting that
6.2.2 Preference for effective liquidity and the giver-getter governs how people spend earned income and savings (Arkes
relationship et al., 1994; Thaler, 1989), the finding that gift cards alleviate
The closer the relationship, the less the recipient values guilt was not surprising. But, it raises the question, Do gift
effective liquidity. Perhaps, utilitarian concerns give way to cards alleviate guilt more readily than gifts of cash? While gift
non-utilitarian considerations, such as the givers intent or cards and gifts of cash seem fungible at first glance, White
disposition toward the recipient, as the giver-getter (2006, 2008) found they are different in the minds of
relationship becomes closer. Prior research also suggests recipients and are spent differently.
close relationships differ in kind and that the type of close
relationship bears on gift evaluation (Otnes et al., 1993).
Preference for effective gift card liquidity was examined in this References
study only with respect to two impersonal sources and one Arkes, H.R., Joyner, C.A., Pezzo, M.V., Nash, J.G., Siegel-
personal source described as a dear friend. Further research is Jacobs, K. and Stone, E. (1994), The psychology of
needed to understand how diverse close relationships , windfall gains, Organizational Behavior and Human
e.g. spouse-to-spouse, friend-to-friend, parent-to-child, child- Decision Processes, Vol. 59, pp. 331-47.
to-parent affect recipients gift card evaluations. Belk, R.W. (1976), Its the thought that counts: a signed
6.2.3 Give-get preference asymmetries digraph analysis of gift-giving, Journal of Consumer
Respondents who would give a comparatively liquid Everyday Research, Vol. 3 No. 3, pp. 155-62.
Card to a close friend were significantly fewer than those who Belk, R.W. (1982), Effects of gift-giving involvement on gift
would pick an Everyday Card had they won their choice of selection strategies, Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 9
cards. This result is consistent with findings from a No. 1, pp. 408-12.
Norwegian study that revealed give-get preference Belk, R.W. and Coon, G.S. (1993), Gift giving and agapic
asymmetries with respect to several gift choices, including love: an alternative to the exchange paradigm based on
one between a gift voucher and cash. Subjects preferred dating experiences, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20
receiving the more liquid option (cash), but preferred giving No. 3, pp. 393-417.

277
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

Burgoyne, C.B. and Routhb, D.A. (1991), Constraints on Waldfogel, J. (2005), Does consumer irrationality trump
the use of money as a gift at Christmas: the role of status consumer sovereignty?, Review of Economics and Statistics,
and intimacy, Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 12 No. 1, Vol. 87 No. 4, pp. 691-6.
pp. 47-69. Webley, P. and Wilson, R. (1989), Social relationships and
Burnett, M.S. and Lunsford, D.A. (1994), Conceptualizing the unacceptability of money as a gift, Journal of Social
guilt in the consumer decision-making process, Journal of Psychology, Vol. 129 No. 1, pp. 85-91.
Consumer Marketing, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 33-43. Webley, P., Lea, S.E.G. and Portalska, R. (1983), The
Camerer, C. (1988), Gifts as economic signals and social unacceptability of money as a gift, Journal of Economic
symbols, American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 94, Psychology, Vol. 4, pp. 223-8.
pp. S180-S214. White, R.J. (2006), Format matters in the mental accounting
Caplow, T. (1982), Christmas gifts and kin networks, of funds: the case of gift cards and cash gifts, University of
American Sociological Review, Vol. 47, pp. 383-92. Waterloo, Waterloo, discussion paper.
Caplow, T. (1984), Rule enforcement without visible means: White, R.J. (2008), The mental accounting of gift card
Christmas gift giving in Middletown, American Journal of versus cash gift funds, Advances in Consumer Research,
Sociology, Vol. 89 No. 6, pp. 1306-23. Vol. 35, p. 722.
Grannis, K. (2010), Gift givers listening to recipients as gift Zaltman, G. (2000), Consumer researchers: take a hike!,
card spending expected to rise, National Retail Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 26 No. 4, pp. 423-8.
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

Federation, available at: www.nrf.com/modules.php?na


meNews&opviewlive&sp_id1033 About the authors
Hou, J. and Elliott, K. (2010), Profiling online bidders,
Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, Vol. 18 No. 2, Erhard K. Valentin is Professor of Marketing and
pp. 109-26. Management, Weber State University. He earned his Ph.D.
Kanji, G.K. (2006), 100 Statistical Tests, Sage Publishing, in Marketing from the University of Utah in 1979 and has
London. extensive business experience, including consulting, and
Keinan, A. and Kivetz, R. (2008), Remedying hyperopia: the teaching experience at the undergraduate and MBA levels.
effects of self-control regret on consumer behavior, Journal His research interests center on consumer behavior,
of Marketing Research, Vol. 45, pp. 676-89. marketing management, and market strategy. Erhard
Khalil, E.L. (2004), The gift paradox: complex selves and K. Valentin is the corresponding author and can be
symbolic good, Review of Social Economy, Vol. 62 No. 3, contacted at: evalentin@weber.edu
pp. 379-92. Anthony T. Allred is a Professor of Marketing at Weber
Kivetz, R. and Keinan, A. (2006), Repenting hyperopia: an State University. He earned his Ph.D. in Marketing from
analysis of self-control regrets, Journal of Consumer Oklahoma State University in 1997. His research interests
Research, Vol. 33 No. 2, pp. 273-82. include marketing, sales management, and entrepreneurship.
Kivetz, R. and Simonson, I. (2002a), Earning the right to He has substantial small business and corporate consulting
indulge: effort as a determinant of customer preferences experience.
toward frequency program rewards, Journal of Marketing
Research, Vol. 39, pp. 155-70. Executive summary and implications for
Kivetz, R. and Simonson, I. (2002b), Self-control for the
righteous: toward a theory of precommitment to managers and executives
indulgence, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 29 No. 2, This summary has been provided to allow managers and executives
pp. 199-217. a rapid appreciation of the content of this article. Those with a
Offenberg, J.P. (2005), Essays on Market Pricing, Purdue particular interest in the topic covered may then read the article in
University, doctoral thesis, available from ProQuest toto to take advantage of the more comprehensive description of the
Dissertations and Theses. research undertaken and its results to get the full benefits of the
Offenberg, J.P. (2007), Markets: gift cards, Journal of material present.
Economic Perspectives, Vol. 31 No. 2, pp. 227-38.
Otnes, C., Lowery, T.M. and Young, C.K. (1993), Gift For over a century, many disciplines have studied gift giving
selection for easy and difficult recipients: a social roles and receiving. Cash is commonly identified as the most
interpretation, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20 No. 2, preferred option because it involves minimal effort, eliminates
pp. 229-44. giver fear of making the wrong choice, and recipients enjoy
Sherry, J. (1983), Gift giving in anthropological the freedom to purchase what they desire. However, the
perspective, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 10 No. 2, evaluation of gifts is considerably more complex and
pp. 157-68. emotionally-laden than economic theories recognize. Gift
Teigen, K.H., Olsen, M.V.G. and Solas, O.E. (2005), Giver- cards account for a sizeable percentage of the money spent on
receiver asymmetries in gift preferences, British Journal of gifts, but research attention in this area remains limited. Few
Social Psychology, Vol. 44, pp. 125-44. studies have focused on better understanding the gift card
Thaler, R. (1989), Mental accounting matters, Journal of phenomenon.
Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 12 No. 3, pp. 183-206. Various academics note that some individuals adopt a self-
Waldfogel, J. (1993), The deadweight of Christmas, centered approach when giving presents. Making a good
American Economic Review, Vol. 83 No. 5, pp. 1328-36. impression might be one driving factor, as could a need to
Waldfogel, J. (2002), Gifts, cash, and stigma, Economic spend minimal time selecting what gift to give. Cash fits the
Inquiry, Vol. 40 No. 3, pp. 415-27. purpose in the latter scenario, although some view that choice

278
Giving and getting gift cards Journal of Consumer Marketing
Erhard K. Valentin and Anthony T. Allred Volume 29 Number 4 2012 271 279

as thoughtless and lazy. In contrast, a nicely wrapped western USA. The 317 subjects recruited were aged from 18
present is considered much more personal and endearing. to over 50, with 173 being female. In addition to different
Givers could also be motivated by a need to fulfill a certain sections relating to gift giving and receiving, the questionnaire
obligation, sustain a relationship, or be perceived as asked respondents to provide demographic information.
generous, knowledgeable or sophisticated. A gift can Among the indications from the data were:
reveal desire to manipulate or control, while the intention of .
the face value of the gift card influences recipient
some people might be to indicate such as status, respect, preference for effective liquidity. for impersonal sources,
affection or even disapproval. Where affection is being liquidity is valued more for small than large gift cards;
signaled, the level of intimacy will be determined by the .
nature of the relationship between giver and receiver also
type of gift offered. impacts on liquidity preference. with this factor, liquidity
In contrast, the reason for giving a gift can be more seems to be less valued as the relationship becomes closer;
recipient-centered. Researchers view this behavior as altruistic .
give and receive preferences tend to differ and people
as it typically involves gifts which receivers wish for and value. generally opt to give cards more liquid than they prefer to
The value recipients place on a gift, be it cash, item or gift receive themselves;
card can be influenced by factors which include: .
where highly liquid gift cards are concerned, certain gift
.
Symbolic and sentimental value. Giving a gift might indicate cards appear more suitable gifts than others;
intentions towards the recipient and arouse certain .
some gift cards are regarded as a more appropriate gift
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

emotions. It was found in some studies that the than cash; and
symbolic value was greater for purchases made with gift .
people feel less guilty when they buy luxury personal items
cards than those made with cash given as a gift. with a gift card than with cash or credit card.
.
Serendipity. The surprise factor is significant here such as
Based on this evidence, Valentin and Allred conclude that gift
when recipients are given gifts that enable them to
cards offer a solution to people faced with conflicting self-
experience something unexpected and stimulate a new
centered intentions like saving time and aiming to make a
interest.
favorable impression. They point out that cards appear more
.
Guilt relief. It is normal for people to struggle to balance
socially acceptable and contain greater sentimental value than
being prudent and indulgent and experience guilt when
cash. Vendors are advised to adopt different focus depending
taking the latter course. An assumption here is that using a
on whether the gift card is being purchased for a casual
gift card or gift cash to buy jewelry or other personal
acquaintance or a close friend. With the former, it is
luxuries can help alleviate any guilt an individual might
acceptable to emphasize the ease of purchase and convenience
feel about making extravagant purchases. When people
offered. Such messages can be reinforced by reminding buyers
anticipate receiving gift cards, they might even
of the risks involved in choosing goods and that a cash gift
precommit to this form of self-gratification.
shows insensitivity. Gift cards which can be used to purchase
.
Circumstances. The giver-recipient relationship is one
luxury items are more fitting for intimate friends and
aspect that also includes contextual factors like custom
marketers of such cards should accentuate this. Pointing out
or the occasion. These and many other circumstantial
the hedonic choices a card enables is likewise
issues help determine the value recipients place on gifts.
recommended.
.
Liquidity. This refers to how easy it is to redeem a gift
The authors note the appropriateness of some gift cards for
without incurring a penalty. Cash is the most liquid
specific occasions. On the other hand, downscale vendors
present as it can be easily exchanged for something else
offer cards that have no greater merit than cash. Marketers of
such as goods or gift cards. The liquidity of gift cards
these cards may struggle to persuade consumers of their
varies depending on whether they effectively function as
value. That restaurant cards seem to have lost some
debit cards or are restricted to certain stores or product
popularity suggests a need to reposition the experience on
categories.
offer to the card recipient.
Fear of making an inappropriate choice of present prompts Additional study using more geographically diverse samples
people to opt for the safer option of cash or gift card. Such may render these findings more generalizable. Further
anxiety becomes likelier when the giver enjoys a close investigation of issues examined here is suggested, while
relationship with the recipient. Research also suggests a researchers might also consider giver and receiver types, gift-
tendency for people to give different gifts from those they giving occasions, situational differences and the impact of
want to receive. Dissimilar preferences, poor judgment or self- culture.
centeredness on the givers part are reasons cited for this.
Various issues relating to gift-card giving and receiving were (A precis of the article Giving and getting gift cards. Supplied by
addressed in a study conducted in a metropolitan area of Marketing Consultants for Emerald.)

To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com


Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints

279
This article has been cited by:

1. Jingming Pan, Wenqing Shi, Xiaowo Tang. 2017. Pricing and ordering strategies of supply chain with selling gift cards. Journal
of Industrial and Management Optimization 13:2, 50-50. [CrossRef]
2. Daniel Horne, Neil Bendle. 2016. Gift cards: a review and research agenda. The International Review of Retail, Distribution and
Consumer Research 26:2, 154-170. [CrossRef]
3. Thyra Uth Thomsen, Judith Lynne Zaichkowsky. 2015. Gifting from the closet: thoughtful or thoughtless?. Journal of Consumer
Marketing 32:6, 450-458. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
4. Suri Weisfeld-Spolter, Cindy B. Ripp, Stephen Gould. 2015. Impact of Giving on Self and Impact of Self on Giving. Psychology
& Marketing 32:1, 1-14. [CrossRef]
5. Qing Yao, Rong Chen. 2014. Gift Cards and Gifted Cash: The Impact of Fit between Gift Type and Message Construal. Journal
of Retailing 90:4, 481-492. [CrossRef]
6. Hannele Kauppinen-Risnen, Johanna Gummerus, Catharina von Koskull, ke Finne, Anu Helkkula, Christian Kowalkowski,
Anne Rindell. 2014. Am I worth it? Gifting myself with luxury. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management: An International
Journal 18:2, 112-132. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
Downloaded by Nirma University of Science and Technology At 09:34 04 July 2017 (PT)

You might also like