You are on page 1of 22

Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

The Antecedents and Moderators of Offline Information Search:


A Meta-Analysis
Moutusy Maity a, , Mayukh Dass b,1 , Naresh K. Malhotra c,d
a Marketing, Indian Institute of Management Lucknow, 1309 Faculty Block, Plot B1, Sector 62, Noida, UP 201307, India
b Marketing, Rawls College of Business, Texas Tech University, MS 2101, Lubbock, TX, USA
c Senior Research Fellow, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, South Africa
d Regents Professor Emeritus, Georgia Institute of Technology, Scheller College of Business, 800 West Peachtree St NW, Atlanta, GA 30308-1149, USA

Abstract
An examination of consumer information search literature reveals that the effects of several antecedents (cost, price dispersion, knowledge,
prior experience) on ofine information search vary greatly in terms of the directions and magnitudes of the determinants effect sizes, particularly
indicating the possibility of inverted-U shaped relationships. Also, despite a wide range of studies undertaken, there remains a need for identifying
an overarching framework for consumer information search. This article synthesizes previous empirical studies on ofine information search
literature, and attempts to provide and test a framework that advances our understanding of the current state of this literature. The review identifies
significant antecedents to offline information search, and also tests the moderating effects of demographic variables and study conditions on the
direct effects. In the context of the main effects, findings suggest that the nature of relationships of several antecedents with the central construct
is indeed inverted-U shaped. Further, moderator analyses identify the boundary conditions under which specific theories hold. The closing section
provides future research directions.
2014 New York University. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Offline information search; Offline consumer information search; Inverted-U shaped relationship; Moderators; Meta-analysis

Introduction yield both positive (e.g. Laroche, Cleveland, and Brown 2004)
and negative (e.g. Roedder-John, Scott, and Bettman 1986)
Consumer information search behavior has a long tradition directions to the relationship. While these studies advance our
of being a useful mechanism for understanding consumer shop- understanding of the effects of various factors on offline infor-
ping behavior, including consumer choice, and choice processes mation search, there has been little attempt to collate the findings
(e.g. Beatty and Smith 1987; Bettman 1974; Punj and Staelin of past studies to report the direct effects (between antecedents
1983). Over the last few decades, multiple studies have repeat- and information search) and the moderating effects of potential
edly investigated the effects of different antecedent variables moderators.
on offline information search. However, the studies conclu- To reconcile these issues, and to provide a holistic under-
sions vary greatly in terms of the directions and magnitudes of standing of these existing research findings, we perform a
the determinants effect sizes. For example, past research find- formal meta-analytic review of extant literature in offline
ings on the relationship between cost and information search information search, examine the direct effects, and investigate
the role of study conditions (i.e. moderators). Therefore, the
specific objectives of this study are: (a) to ascertain the strength

and direction of the direct effects of specific antecedents to
Corresponding author. Fax: +91 120 668 1002.
offline consumer information search, (b) to assess the impact of
E-mail addresses: mmaity@iiml.ac.in (M. Maity), mayukh.dass@ttu.edu
(M. Dass), naresh.malhotra@mgt.gatech.edu (N.K. Malhotra). various moderators on the direct effects; and (c) to identify and
1 Fax: +1 806 742 2199. test an overarching framework for categories of antecedents to

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretai.2014.03.001
0022-4359/ 2014 New York University. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
234 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

information search. A search on the term consumer information Studies included in the review
search on EBSCO confirms that recent research on the topic is
increasingly focused on the phenomenon in the online (personal For identifying eligible studies, a keyword search in the
computer, tablet, mobile) context. While consumers are under- ABI/Inform database was followed by a search for conference
taking information search online (e.g. Pauwels et al. 2011), papers. Finally, letters requesting published and unpublished
offline information search remains an important component of studies on this topic were mailed to approximately 100 authors.
a consumers purchase behavior. Businesses are increasingly This search resulted in approximately 350 studies, including
interested in learning about a consumers entire journey along those that were purely theoretical, concentrated on economet-
the purchase path, which includes both offline and online search ric modeling, carried out estimation and simulation, and did
(e.g. MSIs Shopper Marketing initiative). Therefore, while not directly measure information search. A search on Google
online information search has gained more visibility in recent Scholar also yielded similar results. Studies that suggest mod-
years, a review of offline information search literature is timely. els (without quantitatively testing them), present derivations
Consumer information search in the context of this meta- only, or simply suggest the hypothesized direction between an
analysis is the effort that a consumer expends in order to acquire antecedent and information search (e.g. Gemunden 1985), were
information from the external environment (e.g. Srinivasan and not included in this review. Sixty-two pertinent studies were
Ratchford 1991) for a specific purchase (Beatty and Smith 1987). identified, which investigated 79 antecedent variables (the com-
Consumer information search has been largely measured along plete list can be obtained from the authors), and yielded a sample
two dimensions: (1) amount of search (defined as: number of size (n) of 94,302, with 357 effect sizes (r). A requirement for
alternatives/brands, number of attributes, number of times a a meaningful meta-analysis is a minimum of ten reported stud-
particular source is examined, or the extent of search) (Gregan- ies per variable (Hunter and Schmidt 1990). Ten antecedents
Paxton and Roedder-John 1995; Jacoby, Speller, and Berning report ten or more effect sizes per variable, and were included
1974; Malhotra 1982), and (2) time spent on search (defined for further analysis.
as: total time required to search) (Brucks 1985). Hunter and In order to identify moderators of offline consumer informa-
Schmidt (1990) and several other researchers (e.g. Glass 1976) tion search four different moderator categories (Assmus, Farley,
suggest that the same construct (though measured variously) can and Lehmann 1984; Sultan, Farley, and Lehmann 1990) are
be aggregated in a single meta-analysis. Therefore, this study considered: (1) measurement methods, (2) research context,
pools together all eligible studies with the dependent variable (3) estimation procedure, and (4) model specification. Units of
information search. analyses in this study are bivariate correlations, and therefore,
This paper provides insights that advance both theoretical are unaffected by the estimation procedure or model specifica-
and conceptual knowledge of offline information search. In par- tion. Hence, this review focuses on two moderator categories,
ticular, in the context of direct effects, findings suggest that namely measurement methods (study design and respondent
the relationships between information search and antecedents type) and research context (product type, age, gender, educa-
cost, price dispersion, knowledge, and prior experience (each) tion and income), and assesses their impact on the homogeneity
are inverted-U shaped. Study findings also suggest that bene- of direct effects. Research in marketing and consumer behavior
t, instead of cost of search, is more appropriate for modeling suggests that the direct or mediating effects of these variables are
consumer information search. In the context of moderators, the small and that these variables are best treated as moderators (see
study finds that the moderating effects of age and product type Chen 2012; Cheon, Cho, and Sutherland 2007 for the specific
on direct effects are significant. use of moderators in meta-analyses).
The paper addresses the following gaps in the information
search literature. First, this research conducts a much-needed Antecedents: Theoretical foundations
meta-analytic review of a mature area, and addresses the need
to reconcile inconsistent findings among studies conducted Researchers examining consumer information search have
in the past. Second, this study separates chance effects from tried to identify an overarching framework explaining this phe-
moderator effects in the offline information search literature nomenon. Several researchers have suggested specific categories
and identifies conditions under which specific theories hold. of antecedent variables that affect consumer information search
Third, it identifies a framework with specific categories of vari- (see Table 1). The categories identified in these investigations
ables that affect consumer information search. The rest of the are usually prompted by the authors assessment of the current
paper is organized as follows. First, we present the procedure state of information search literature and the variables that have
adopted for identifying studies, and report the sampling frame. been investigated prior to the categorization attempts. For exam-
Next, we discuss the theoretical foundations of categories of ple, in their categorization, Moore and Lehmann (1980) split the
antecedents to information search, and present the hypotheses Properties of Choice Situation category suggested by Bettman
on the direct effects. Third, we present the research methods, (1979) into two categories Market Environment and Situational
and discuss the findings on the direct effects. Next, we put Variables, and Newmans (1977) Potential Payoff category into a
forth the hypotheses on the moderators, present the data analy- number of other categories. Beatty and Smith (1987), in turn, add
sis procedures and discuss the findings on the moderators. We a category Cost of Search to the scheme suggested by Moore
conclude with research implications and directions for future and Lehmann (1980). Many studies also test holistic models of
research. offline information search (e.g. Srinivasan and Ratchford 1991;
Table 1
Categories of antecedents to information search.
Author(s) Variables/categories of variables suggested in extant literature

Newman (1977) Cost Buying Strategies (brand Potential Payoff (price, Situational Variables Personality Variables Other Variables
and store preference, style, perceived (urgency, financial (household role,
satisficing vs. optimizing, difference, perceived risk, pressure, special buying party of major
strategy of information knowledge, experience, opportunities, location of influence, social
acquisition) education, income) residence) class, occupation,
age, stage of life
cycle)
Bettman (1979) Properties of Choice Costs vs. Benets of Conict and Conict Individual Differences Knowledge
Situation (availability of Information Response Strategies (in-store vs.

M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254


information, difficulty of prior-processing, abilities,
choice task, time concern with optimality
pressure) of choice)
Moore and Lehmann Market Environment Situational Variables Potential Payoff/Product Knowledge and Individual Differences Conict and
(1980) (number of alternatives, (time pressure, social Importance (perceived Experience (usage of (involvement, ability, Conict-Resolution
complexity of pressure, financial risk, price, social product) training) Strategies
alternatives, information pressure, ease of access of visibility)
availability) information)
Punj and Staelin Cost Benet Factors Unique to a
(1983) Consumer (e.g. usable
prior knowledge, prior
memory structure)
Bloch et al. (1986) Market Environment Situational Factors Involvement
Beatty and Smith Market Environment Situational Variables Potential Payoff/Product Knowledge and Individual Differences Conict and Cost of Search
(1987) (number of alternatives, (time pressure, social Importance (perceived Experience (usage of (involvement, ability, Conict-Resolution (psychological
complexity of pressure, financial risk, price, social product) training) Strategies costs)
alternatives, information pressure, ease of access of visibility)
availability) information)
Srinivasan and Size of evoked set Interest in cars Perceived risk Perceived benefits Amount of Cost
Ratchford (1991) experience, positive
experience, product
knowledge
Moorthy et al. Relative brand uncertainty Involvement Individual brand Risk aversion Search cost Experience
(1997) uncertainty
Authors (based on Costs vs. Benets of Potential Payoff Knowledge and Individual Differences
EoI theory) Information (number of (perceived risk, financial Experience (usage of (involvement, ability,
alternatives, time pressure, price, social product) training)
pressure, psychological visibility)
costs)

Categories of variables as identified by the authors (in italics). Specific variables as identified/used by the authors (in regular font).

235
236 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

Moorthy, Ratchford, and Talukdar 1997), but do not identify While the above-stated relationship is the dominant view in
specific categories of variables. We suggest a framework that information search literature, Bodur and Matyas (2008) find that
is parsimonious and is explicitly grounded in the Economics of there is an inverted-U shaped relationship between assortment
Information (EoI) theory. size and consumers evaluation of choice tasks for promotion
Despite these seemingly ad hoc categorizations, most of the focus individuals. Findings in psychology and conflict man-
authors agree that the phenomenon of consumer information agement suggest that sometimes cognitive cost (in the form of
search is primarily grounded in the costbenefit concept of Eco- cognitive conflict) has an inverted-U shaped relationship with
nomics of Information (EoI) by Stigler (1961). Therefore, we decision making (e.g. Parayitam and Dooley 2011). A highly
take the EoI as the framework for identifying categories of complex task will have a higher level of cognitive cost and lower
antecedent variables that affect consumer information search, level of performance compared to a moderately complex task.
and for guiding our theoretical discussion and empirical inves- This phenomenon occurs because complex tasks involve greater
tigations. We identify four categories of antecedent variables demand and greater memory load and hence, is more vulnerable
that affect information search: Cost vs. Benet of Information, to breakdowns in the process. A simple task does not provide the
Potential Payoff, Knowledge and Experience and Individual Dif- adequate motivation required in order to reach optimal levels of
ferences. decision-making, as a moderately complex task does. We may
We present propositions about the relationships of the cate- represent the relationship between cost and benefit as a curve,
gories of antecedent variables with information search, and then such that the rising (falling) part of the curve represents the por-
present the hypotheses for the ten variables that are included in tion of search where benefits (costs) outweigh costs (benefits),
the meta-analysis (Table 2). which correspond to the postulations of the EoI theory. (The hor-
izontal axis represents the cost of search while the vertical axis
Cost versus benet of information represents the benefits accrued from search as well as the amount
of information search undertaken.) Therefore, as cognitive costs
It is widely acknowledged that there are costs associated with rise, information search will increase and then decrease. Hence,
acquiring and processing information (e.g. Punj and Staelin we propose that:
1983; Srinivasan and Ratchford 1991), and one of the cen-
Proposition 1a. The relationship between information search
tral arguments of the EoI (or, cost theory) is that consumers
and internal cost of search is inverted-U.
search for information till marginal benefits continue to exceed
marginal costs (Smith, Venkataraman, and Dholakia 1999). As The benefit obtained from information search, represents the
a consequence of these costs, Stigler (1961) argues that con- rising part of the curve. Therefore, we expect that:
sumers will continue to search for additional information until
Proposition 1b. The relationship between information search
the benefit obtained from additional information is smaller than
and benet is positive.
the cost of obtaining it. Since the EoI theory is conceptualized
as a costbenefit trade-off that a consumer undertakes while The latter category of search costs is exogenous in nature,
searching for information, therefore, in keeping with Bettman and is influenced by the situation that a consumer is in. For
(1979), we suggest the category of Cost vs. Benet of Informa- example, Smith, Venkataraman, and Dholakia (1999) identify
tion as one of the categories of variables affecting consumer urgency (or, time pressure) as one such cost given which the con-
information search. sumer may choose to continue (discontinue) her search. External
The overall directions of the relationships of the specific search costs by definition are not under the consumers con-
antecedent variables (which may belong to this category), trol. Therefore, it is expected that these kinds of search costs
with the central construct, need to be ascertained. Researchers will not allow the consumer to be in a situation where marginal
suggest that search costs can be categorized as internal or benefits exceed marginal costs. The consumer will experience
external costs, where internal search costs are those that are a decrease in the amount of information search undertaken as
cognitive costs and can be controlled by the consumer, while costs increase.
external search costs are not within the consumers direct con- A large number of studies on decision making have advanced
trol (Smith, Venkataraman, and Dholakia 1999), and may be the important role of cognitive capacity (e.g. Miller 1956; Simon
situational. 1957) and the limited resource models of Kahneman (1973)
Extant literature suggests that cognitive cost (psychological and Norman and Bobrow (1975). Research in decision mak-
cost) is negatively related to information search (e.g. Beatty and ing acknowledges that while arousal may be a basic operator,
Smith 1987; Punj and Staelin 1983). Humans have limited cog- reductions in cognitive ability are often proposed as the central
nitive capabilities, and as cognitive costs increase, an individual explanatory element. For example, Eysenck and Calvo (1992)
is likely to limit him/herself to satisficing (Simon 1987, p. 243) asserted that individuals under stress demonstrate lower cog-
as opposed to optimizing. Information overload provides a sim- nitive efficiency due to intrusive cognitions competing for the
ilar argument, and occurs when the amount of input to a system limited pool of cognitive resources. Other studies demonstrated
exceeds its processing capacity (Malhotra 1982; Milford and that time pressure (e.g. Payne, Bettman, and Luce 1996), and
Perry 1977; Simon 1979). When information overload occurs, noise (e.g. Hockey 1970), as well as naturally occurring life-
it is likely that a reduction in decision quality will occur, thus event stressors (e.g. Baradell and Klein 1993), pose a burden
leading to lower information search. on cognitive capacity, increasing errors made on cognitive tasks
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 237

Table 2
Directions of the hypothesized relationships and the effects reported in studies included in the meta-analysis.
Antecedent variables Theories explaining the Hypotheses Hypothesized Number of Range of Cumulative n
relationships relationship effects reported
reported effects (r)

Cost vs. Benet of Information


Number of alternatives Cost Theory; Information H1 Inverted-U 11a /17b 0.31 to 0.60 5,561
Overload; Utility Theory
Cost Cost Theory; Information H2 Inverted-U 33/37 0.61 to 0.54 9,264
Overload; Utility Theory;
EoI Theory
Price dispersion Cost Theory; EoI Theory H3 Inverted-U 16/21 0.40 to 0.54 5,666
Time pressure Cost theory H4 16/21 0.88 to 0.36 4,829
Potential Payoff
Perceived risk Risk Theory H5 + 13/16 0.33 to 0.33 3,567
Financial constraint Risk Theory H6 + 24/26 0.38 to 0.43 10,872
Uncertainty Risk Theory H7 + 18/22 0.060.53 4,821
Knowledge and Prior Experience
Knowledge Memory; Theory of H8 Inverted-U 30/43 0.34 to 0.55 12,090
Learning; Involvement
Theory; Risk Theory;
Effort Theory;
Satisfaction Theory
Prior experience Memory; Theory of H9 Inverted-U 24/36 0.54 to 0.52 10,360
Learning; Involvement
Theory; Risk Theory;
Effort Theory;
Satisfaction Theory
Individual Difference
Involvement Involvement Theory H10 + 20/25 0.17 to 0.80 8,186

All summary statistics (e.g. F, t, Z, chi-square) obtained across studies are converted to the common correlation coefficient metric, r.
a Number of statistically significant (at *p < 0.05) effects reported.
b Total number of effects reported.

and altering the way information is processed. In his review of Cost of search is the consumers perception of the effort
the literature, Staal (2004) concluded that stressors lead to two expended in information search, and has been measure through
specific changes in decision making: causing information pro- multi-item scales (e.g. DeSarbo and Choi 1999; Dowling and
cessing to become more rigid with fewer alternatives scanned Staelin 1994), opportunity cost and wages obtained (e.g. Urbany,
and inducing the tendency to persist with a method or problem- Dickson, and Kalapurakal 1996) in extant literature. In our
solving strategy even after it has ceased to be helpful (p. 68). meta-analysis, we find that the ranges of effect sizes for the
Hence, we propose that: relationship between information search and the antecedent vari-
ables, number of alternatives (0.31 to 0.60) and cost (0.61 to
Proposition 1c. The relationship between information search 0.54) contain both negative and positive valences. An increase in
and external cost of search is negative. these two variables increases internal search costs. Such diver-
gent findings indicate that the relationships of these variables
Number of alternatives, cost. Past research (Jacoby et al. with information search are probably inverted U-shaped. Also,
1974; Malhotra 1982) has demonstrated that as the amount as discussed above, some researchers find an inverted-U shaped
of information available to consumers increase, the amount of relationship between cognitive cost and decision-making (e.g.
search undertaken decrease. Number of alternatives is defined Bodur and Matyas 2008; Parayitam and Dooley 2011). There-
as the total number of alternatives available from which the con- fore, information search is high when cost of search is medium,
sumer has to make a choice (e.g. Ratchford and Srinivasan 1993; and information search is low when cost of search is high or low.
Srinivasan and Ratchford 1991). The variable is operational- We hypothesize that:
ized by directly asking consumers or, by objectively counting
the number of options that are available to the consumer. An H1. The relationship between information search and number
individual has limited information processing capabilities (finite of alternatives is inverted-U.
cognitive capability), which limit the number of alternatives con-
H2. The relationship between information search and cost is
sidered (Roedder John and Cole 1986). Therefore, based on cost
inverted-U.
theory (Stigler 1961), large number of alternatives should reduce
the level of information search, that is, have a negative effect on Price dispersion. Perceived price dispersion is the judgment
information search. that a consumer makes about the relative width (i.e. the variance)
238 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

of the available prices from which the consumer can make a information search. Stigler (1961) calls for systematically (p.
choice (e.g. Urbany, Dickson, and Kalapurakal 1996). In the 224) accounting for all those factors that may affect consumers
studies included in the meta-analysis, price dispersion has been information search, attributing to her ignorance. Ignorance
variously conceptualized as inter-brand difference, and price may refer to both risk and uncertainty (Vriend 1996). Perceived
range for homogeneous as well as for heterogeneous goods. risk is the uncertainty and the consequences associated with a
Recall that we conceptualize information search as acquiring choice; it affects the extent of information search (Bauer 1960).
information from the external environment, which encompasses We, therefore, offer Potential Payoff as a category of antecedent
a variety of possible reasons for price dispersion, including prod- variables affecting information search.
uct heterogeneity (e.g. Pan, Ratchford, and Shankar 2003). High The uncertainty and risk experienced by a consumer while
levels of perceived price dispersion will lead to higher expected making a decision will affect the potential payoff expected from
savings from the search activity (Claxton, Fry, and Portis 1974). that decision. In such situations information is a valuable asset
Perceived price dispersion, too, is an internal search cost. that may be used to reduce uncertainty. Consumers perceive
Since it is a necessary condition for consumers to undertake a wide variety of risks in the marketplace. Perceived risk is
a search activity (Stigler 1961), perceived price dispersion or defined as the consumers perception of the uncertainty and the
information variation is a basic premise of the EoI theory. Con- adverse consequences of buying a product (Dowling and Staelin
sumers who perceive wide dispersions in list prices would search 1994). Risk theory predicts that consumers who perceive a high
more extensively than buyers who perceive a narrow dispersion risk involved in choosing a product, may proactively search for
(Urbany 1986). We observe that a range of effect sizes have more information thereby trying to reduce the risk associated
been reported for price dispersion (0.40 to 0.54) in extant lit- with the outcome of making that particular choice (Mitra, Reiss,
erature. Price dispersion has been measured through subjective and Capella 1999). Therefore, according to risk theory (e.g.
scales (items measured through likert scales or bi-polar adjective Dowling and Staelin 1994), as perceived risk increases, there
scales) (e.g. Putrevu and Ratchford 1997; Urbany, Dickson, and is an increase in the amount of information search undertaken.
Kalapurakal 1996), and has also been manipulated in experimen- Hence, we propose that:
tal settings (e.g. Urbany 1986). Low information asymmetry (i.e.
Proposition 2. The relationship between information search
similar levels of information among sellers and buyers) reduces
and potential payoff is positive.
perceived price dispersion in the markets, which reduces cost
of search (Rothschild 1974), and hence is expected to reduce Perceived risk. Perceived risk can be defined as the proba-
information search since benefits are limited from undertaking bility of any loss (financial or otherwise) that can occur as a
more information search. Similarly, higher information asym- result of using a product, multiplied by the importance of that
metry (i.e. the information sellers may have that buyers lack) is loss (Srinivasan and Ratchford 1991). Information search is an
expected to increase information search. We further argue that at attempt to reduce the risk associated with the choice process.
very high price dispersion the cost associated with information Perceived risk has been measured via multiple items as risk aver-
search will increase to an extent where search costs outweigh sion (e.g. DeSarbo and Choi 1999), or, as perceived risk (e.g.
the benefits obtained, and information search will be low, which psychological risk, social risk, financial risk Dholakia 2001).
may happen in the context of product heterogeneity or when In the meta-analysis we consider perceived risk as a single con-
consumers have a high personal search cost (in case of homo- struct and acknowledge that this construct may have various
geneous products). Therefore, we hypothesize that information dimensions. Based on the above discussion, we hypothesize that:
search will be the highest at medium levels of price dispersion:
H5. The relationship between information search and per-
H3. The relationship between information search and price ceived risk is positive.
dispersion is inverted-U.
Financial constraint, uncertainty. Financial constraint is con-
Time pressure. Time pressure has been conceptualized as the ceptualized as having a budget within which the consumer has
consumers perception of whether she had adequate time to com- to make a choice (e.g. Hill 2001; Laroche et al. 2000). Also, con-
plete a task and has been operationalized through multi-item sumers may have a range within which they would like to restrict
scales (e.g. Avery 1996), or, has been manipulated in exper- their expenditures (Urbany, Dickson, and Kalapurakal 1996).
iments. Time pressure is an external search cost (e.g. Smith, Some studies find that nancial constraint is positively related to
Venkataraman, and Dholakia 1999). Hence we hypothesize that: external information search (e.g. Claxton, Fry, and Portis 1974).
As per risk theory, this restriction would result in searching for
H4. The relationship between information search and time more information. Therefore, we expect that greater financial
pressure is negative. constraint leads to greater amount of information search.
H6. The relationship between information search and nancial
Potential payoff
constraint is positive.
Stigler (1961) points out that consumers may suffer from Uncertainty is the amount of information that a buyer brings
ignorance (p. 224) while undertaking information search. to the search process (e.g. Urbany 1986). The variable is often
However, the EoI does not identify the factors that may con- manipulated by providing (not providing) subjects with addi-
tribute to a consumers ignorance in the context of undertaking tional information (e.g. Urbany 1986), or measured in the
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 239

context of an actual purchase (e.g., Moorthy, Ratchford, and search for more information. For those consumers who have low
Talukdar 1997). Uncertainty may be knowledge uncertainty prior knowledge/experience, the cost of acquiring information
(i.e. uncertainty about the information on alternatives, Urbany, outweighs the benefit that they associate with acquiring the cur-
Dickson, and Wilkie 1989) or choice uncertainty (i.e. uncer- rent information, and so undertake limited information search.
tainty about which alternative to choose, Laroche et al. 2004; In contrast, those consumers who have moderate information
Urbany, Dickson, and Wilkie 1989). Greater uncertainty should do assume that their state of uncertainty may change by acquir-
lead to greater information search undertaken. Therefore, we ing more information, and are motivated to undertake a greater
hypothesize that: amount of information search. Based on the above discussion,
we propose that the relationship between information search and
H7. The relationship between information search and uncer- knowledge/prior experience will be inverted-U shaped:
tainty is positive.
Proposition 3. The relationship between information search
and knowledge/prior experience is inverted U-shaped.
Knowledge and prior experience
In our review, we treat the two variables separately depending
We theorize that the ignorance (Stigler 1961, p. 224) upon how the authors of the specific studies have operationalized
referred to in EoI also refers to the level of knowledge or expe- them in their investigations. Therefore, we hypothesize:
rience an individual may have regarding the decision-making
situation or task. Therefore, we offer Knowledge and Expe- H8. The relationship between information search and knowl-
rience as another category of antecedent variables that affect edge is inverted U-shaped.
information search. H9. The relationship between information search and prior
Product class knowledge has been defined as subjective experience is inverted U-shaped.
knowledge or information that consumers have related to the
particular product class (Brucks 1985). Knowledge and prior
Individual difference variables
experience have often been interchangeably used to mean a
consumers familiarity with a buying situation (Bettman and
EoI acknowledges the difference in tastes (Stigler 1961,
Park 1980; Brucks 1995), and researchers argue that the two
p. 216) among consumers which may affect information
are conceptually similar. Past research examining the rela-
search. We offer the category individual differences as another
tionship between knowledge/prior experience and information
antecedent category that affect consumer information search.
search has yielded equivocal results. Researchers (e.g. Jacoby,
We expect that the direction of the relationships of different
Chestnut, and Fisher 1978) have found a positive relation-
individual difference variables with information search will be
ship between purchasing experience and information search.
different, and hence we do not offer any specific proposition for
Weiss and Heide (1993) argue that a lack of prior experi-
the overall category of variables.
ence necessitates extensive search on the part of the buyer
Involvement. Involvement theory was originally introduced
(Anderson, Chu, and Weitz 1987). Contrary to these findings,
in social psychology, but has been adopted extensively in con-
many other researchers (Katona and Mueller 1955; Newman
sumer behavior. Laaksonen (1994) defines involvement as the
and Staelin 1971) have found that increased positive experi-
amount of arousal, interest, or activation a person experiences
ence led to less information search. Moore and Lehmann (1980)
in a specific situation. Involvement (i.e. an arousal or concern
found no relationship between prior experience and informa-
about the purchase decision) figures prominently in consumer
tion search, and a negative relationship with product knowledge
search effort theories. Researchers have argued that involvement
and the central construct. These conflicting findings and argu-
can be conceived in behavioral terms such as the time or energy
ments can probably be explained by a curvilinear relationship
spent on information search, the number of alternatives consid-
between knowledge/prior experience and information search
ered, and the extent of the decision process (Laaksonen 1994;
(Malhotra 1983). Johnson and Russo (1984) also postulate an
Stone 1984).
inverted-U shaped relationship, which is validated by Moorthy,
Mitchell (1981) reports that, at higher involvement levels,
Ratchford, and Talukdar (1997). Bettman and Park (1980) pro-
consumers will aggressively search for relevant information.
vide a possible explanation for this relationship, Those low
Petty and Cacioppo (1981) assert that high product involvement
in knowledge/prior experience lack the ability to acquire and
motivates consumers to examine carefully product advantages
process much current information, while those high in knowl-
and disadvantages, which means greater information search.
edge/prior experience lack the motivation to do so. The moderate
Consumers are thought to engage in systematic or comprehen-
group, however, may possess enough ability and have enough
sive search under high-involvement conditions, but minimize
motivation to devote substantial processing effort to the task
search activity and rely on simple schemas or cognitive heuristics
(Malhotra 1983, p. 129).
under low-involvement conditions (Engel, Kollat, and Blackwell
Information is acquired to satisfy a need for change in
1973). Hence, greater extent of involvement will lead to greater
uncertainty (Burnkrant 1976). Following this argument, those
amount of information search. We hypothesize that:
consumers who have a high prior knowledge/experience, assume
that they do not need more information to change the state of H10. The relationship between information search and involve-
uncertainty they may be in, and therefore, are not motivated to ment is positive.
240 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

Antecedents: Findings will be the highest. The other two categories represent medium
search costs.
Antecedents (direct effects) In order to test the hypotheses for knowledge and prior
experience, we need to identify proxy variables that mean-
For each of the ten antecedent variables, Table 2 summa- ingfully represent knowledge about the product class. For the
rizes the underlying theories, the hypothesized relationships variables knowledge and prior experience, the parameters cho-
with information search, number of effects reported, range of sen for creating the pertinent categories are the extent of
reported effects, and the cumulative n. We do not include the knowledge/experience (high/low), as reported in the original
antecedent benet in the meta-analysis since the number of effect studies, and product type (infrequently purchased/frequently
sizes obtained for this antecedent is less than ten. The findings purchased). If participants are regular buyers (or have made
on the direct effects are discussed below. The conceptualization a purchase) of a product category, they are designated as
and measurement of all the ten variables are fairly consistent high in knowledge/experience, and vice versa. The rationale
across all the studies included in the meta-analysis. for including product type is that the definition of knowl-
edge/prior experience in extant literature is in the context of
Methodology: Direct effects product class. Therefore, we choose a meaningful proxy vari-
able that represents consumers knowledge about a product
Although correlations are the most common metrics found class.
in the 62 studies, many studies also report F, t, and chi-square A consumer buying a frequently purchased product regu-
statistics. We convert these summary statistics to the common larly is likely to be quite knowledgeable about the product
correlation coefficient metric, r (e.g. Wolf 1986) (Table 2), class (frequently purchased and high knowledge/experience),
therefore ensuring the use of bivariate correlations in the meta- and the level of knowledge is likely to be higher than in
analysis. In order to test the hypotheses on the five inverted-U case of an infrequently purchased product with which the user
shaped relationships (H1 , H2 , H3 , H8 , H9 ), ideally, we need may have prior experience (infrequently purchased and high
to identify at least three levels (i.e. low, medium, and high) of knowledge/experience). The level of knowledge is even lower
each antecedent. However, different levels are not operational- for an infrequently purchased product with which the user
ized for any of the five variables in the studies included in our has little experience (infrequently purchased and low knowl-
meta-analysis. We, therefore, need to create post hoc categories edge/experience). The consumer is expected to have the least
in order to test our hypotheses. knowledge about a frequently purchased product with which
The relationships of the antecedent variables number of alter- the user has low experience (frequently purchased and low
natives, cost and price dispersion, with information search knowledge/experience). These four categories represent differ-
are explained by cost theory. In order to test the hypotheses, ent levels of decreasing knowledge/prior experience.
therefore, we need to identify proxy variables that meaning- The weighted mean effect size is calculated for the four sub-
fully represent cost of search (since the observed effect sizes categories identified for each of the five antecedents H1 , H2 ,
by themselves are inadequate for testing an inverted-U shaped H3 , H8 , and H9 , and for the other five antecedents H4 , H5 , H6 ,
relationship). For these three antecedents, the parameters we H7 , H10 . This study weights the effect size r using the relevant
choose for creating post hoc categories that represent varying sample size information (Table 3). For computational details,
levels of these antecedents are product category (i.e. infre- see Hedges and Olkin (1985).
quently/frequently purchased), and the similarity/dissimilarity
among products in each product category included in the stud- Findings: Direct effects
ies. We argue that for infrequently purchased product categories,
searching for an additional product gives the consumer a larger Number of alternatives and cost. For cost, as expected,
benefit and thus a greater incentive to search. Therefore, con- the weighted r associated with each of the four categories
sumers are expected to search more for infrequently purchased demonstrate an inverted-U shaped curve (CostF S = 0.190;
products and search less for frequently purchased products. CostF DS = 0.090, CostIF S = 0.035, CostIF DS = 0.023),
We define the similarity/dissimilarity among products as the whereby, information search is high for medium levels of cost,
extent to which the products are homogeneous/heterogeneous and low for the high and low levels of cost, thus lending support
in that particular product category. We argue that the cost of to H2 . For number of alternatives, contrary to expectation, we
search is lower for homogeneous products than that associated do not find an inverted-U shaped curve, and hence, no support
with the search for heterogeneous products. Four post hoc cat- for H1 . However, note that the effect sizes for the various levels
egories emerge representing four levels of increasing cost for are significant and different from each other. One possible
each antecedent variable: frequently purchased and similar, fre- reason for this finding could be that extant studies have failed
quently purchased and dissimilar, infrequently purchased and to include adequate number of alternatives that capture the
similar, and infrequently purchased and dissimilar. It is expected complete spectrum of search costs.
that the cognitive cost, and hence, the cost associated with infor- Price dispersion. In accordance with our hypothesis, the
mation search for frequently purchased and similar products relationship between price dispersion and information search
(e.g. groceries) will be the lowest, and that associated with infre- displays an inverted-U shape. Findings for three subgroups
quently purchased and dissimilar products (e.g. cars, house) (PriceDF S = 0.200, PriceDF DS = 0.320, PriceDIF DS = 0.146, no
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 241

Table 3
Antecedent variables testing hypotheses for direct effects.
Antecedent variables Levels of antecedent K Unweighted r Weighted r Nature of
(support for hypotheses) variables (where applicable) (observed) (corrected) relationship

Number of Alternatives* NAlternativesF S 10 0.009 0.046** Linear


H1 (No)
NAlternativesF DS
NAlternativesIF S 3 0.160 0.138
NAlternativesIF DS 3 0.192 0.302
Cost CostF S 10 0.236 0.190** Inverted-U
H2 (Yes)
CostF DS 10 0.077 0.090
CostIF S 7 0.017 0.035
CostIF DS 10 0.013 0.023
Price Dispersion PriceDF S 8 0.195 0.200** Inverted-U
H3 (Yes)
PriceDF DS 6 0.282 0.320
PriceDIF S
PriceDIF DS 7 0.121 0.146
Time Pressure 21 0.20 0.14 Linear
H4 (Yes)
Perceived Risk 16 0.11 0.13 Linear
H5 (Yes)
Financial Constraint 26 0.13 0.04 Linear
H6 (Yes)
Uncertainty 22 0.27 0.24 Linear
H7 (Yes)
Knowledge KnowledgeF LK 18 0.078 0.093** Inverted-U
H8 (Yes)
KnowledgeIF LK 9 0.213 0.184
KnowledgeIF HK 11 0.151 0.120
KnowledgeF HK 5 0.033 0.076
Prior Experience ExperienceF LK 6 0.281 0.276** Inverted-U
H9 (Yes)
ExperienceIF LK 2 0.135 0.135
ExperienceIF HK 19 .023 0.016
ExperienceF HK 9 0.089 .080
Involvement 25 0.30 0.26 Linear
H10 (Yes)

indicates that no study has been found in these categories. *Product Category not reported in one study. K = No. of Studies. All weighted and unweighted rs
are significant at <0.0001. The extensions appearing in subscript indicate: For antecedents Number of Alternatives, Cost, and Price Dispersion: (a) F S: frequently
purchased; similar; (b) F DS: Frequently Purchased; Dissimilar; (c) IF S: Infrequently Purchased; Similar; (d) IF DS: Infrequently Purchased; Dissimilar For
antecedents Knowledge and Prior Experience, (a) F LK: Frequently Purchased; Low Product Knowledge; (b) IF LK: Infrequently Purchased; Low Product Knowl-
edge; (c) IF HK: Infrequently Purchased; High Product Knowledge; (d) F LK: Frequently Purchased; High Product Knowledge. **The correlation coefficients
associated with the different levels are different (sig. at p < 0.05).

study was found for the category PriceDIF S ) bear out that con- perceive high price dispersion as they might find the cost of
sumers undertake different amounts of information search at search too high. There could be supply-side issues as well. If
different levels of price dispersion, as hypothesized, providing the available alternatives are perceived to be similar to each
support for H3 . Medium levels of perceived price dispersion other, the amount of search will be limited, unless it is a search
lead to the highest amount of information search. Since cost for price. The amount of information search is high when dis-
of search is a function of price dispersion, it is expected that persion is medium (PriceDF DS = 0.320), and low for low price
at medium levels of dispersion, consumers obtain the highest dispersion (PriceDF S = 0.200).
amount of information than at high or low levels of price disper- Time pressure. As expected, the direction of the relation-
sion. Therefore, though the overall relationship between price ship between time pressure and information search is negative
dispersion and information search is positive, following cost (rTP = 0.14). This finding provides support to H4 .
theory, we find that the amount of information search is low Perceived risk, nancial constraint, uncertainty. The direc-
when price dispersion is high (PriceDIF DS = 0.146). It is likely tion of the relationships between each of the variables
that consumers do not search for more information when they perceived risk, nancial constraint, and uncertainty are positive
242 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

(rPR = 0.13, rFC = 0.04, rU = 0.24), as expected. These example, for the antecedent cost, we obtain positive and nega-
results provide support to H5 , H6 , and H7 . tive valences for the two clusters obtained (Table 4). This study
Knowledge, prior experience. For knowledge, the effect weights the effect size r using the relevant sample size informa-
sizes for the four categories demonstrate an inverted-U shaped tion, and also estimates the associated file drawer N (Table 4).
curve (KnowledgeF LK = 0.093, KnowledgeIF LK = 0.184, (Note that the results reported in Table 4 are those based on all
KnowledgeIF HK = 0.120, KnowledgeF HK = 0.076), provid- studies that measure information search in terms of number of
ing support for H8 . For frequently purchased products, the brands and time. We report the results of a separate meta-analysis
risk associated with decision-making is low, and therefore, conducted on the findings based on studies that measure infor-
even when the level of knowledge is low, users undertake mation search in terms of time only in Web Appendix B. The
low information search (KnowledgeF LK = 0.093), while the results of the two findings are similar, demonstrating that our
information search is even lower when level of knowledge findings are robust.) Homogeneity tests are carried out on a total
is high (KnowledgeF HK = 0.076), probably demonstrating a of fifteen clusters of direct effects, following the recommen-
lack of motivation for gathering more information. We obtain dations of Hedges and Olkin (1985), and results indicate that
similar findings for prior experience (ExperienceF LK = - moderator analysis should be carried out on thirteen relation-
0.276, ExperienceIF LK = -0.135, ExperienceIF HK = 0.016, ships (Web Appendices C and D).
ExperienceF HK = -0.080), providing support for H9 . We make specific suppositions regarding the different clus-
Involvement. The relationship between information search ters of effect sizes that emerge for the four antecedent variables.
and involvement is positive and strong (rI = 0.26). We find For cost, the cluster that displays a weighted r of 0.09
support for H10 . (CostHigh/Low ) is assumed to represent either high or low level of
We find support for nine out of the ten hypotheses tested. The cost of search (or, both), and a weighted r of 0.29 (CostMedium ) is
findings on the relationships between information search and assumed to represent the medium level of cost of search. These
cost and price dispersion provide new insights. The findings on assumptions are reasonable since theory as well as our find-
knowledge and prior experience agree with extant literature, and ings on direct effects demonstrate that the amount of search
provide additional support to our findings. undertaken is the highest when cost of search is medium. Sim-
ilar suppositions are made for the variables price dispersion,
Moderators: Theory and ndings knowledge and prior experience.

Moderator analyses provide deeper insight into the Hypotheses: Effect of moderators
antecedents relationships with the central construct. Recall that
four of the ten antecedents have inverted-U shaped relationships Hypotheses are presented on thirteen relationships that are
with information search. Ideally, a moderator analysis should be moderated by seven moderators.
carried out on each of the levels of the antecedent variables. How- Age. Extant literature on information processing and deci-
ever, note that for testing the hypotheses on the direct effects, sion making has extensively investigated the effects of age,
we used proxy parameters for creating categories that represent with conflicting findings. For example, Otnes (1990) finds that
the cost of search for three antecedents, and the extent of knowl- age positively affects information search, while others (e.g.
edge/prior experience of the participants for two antecedents, Schaninger and Sciglimpaglia 1981) report the opposite effect.
which reduces the number of studies in each sub-group, and Examining the social psychology literature on the impact of cog-
moderator analyses may not be meaningfully carried out. nitive aging on the ability of decision-making, Mata, Schooler,
Therefore, for conducting moderator analysis we follow and Rieskamp (2007) find that search costs are higher for older
clustering of effect sizes, as discussed by Hedges and Olkin adults. Age-related cognitive decline is attributed to decreased
(1985), where the authors suggest the use of overlapping or attentional capabilities (McDowd 1997). Kirasic et al. (1996)
non-overlapping clusters. We follow the latter approach to show that age negatively affects information-processing speed
decompose the effect sizes for each antecedent variable into and working memory. This finding is in line with the theory of
smaller subsets, by rank-ordering u (where r is transformed into cognitive competence as outlined by Schaie and Willis (1993),
Fishers z, and then multiplied by the square root of the sam- which suggests that the ability to perform tasks decline with age.
ple size to obtain u). We examine the ranges of the weighted The studies that report the age of respondents are categorized
mean effect sizes obtained for each antecedent variable that as follows: <30 years, and >30 years, as per extant practice
demonstrates an inverted-U shaped relationship with informa- (Irwin and McClelland 2003). In line with the theory of cognitive
tion search, and find that the effect sizes for the variables cost, competence, we predict the following:
price dispersion and prior experience may be categorized into
H11a. For the moderator age, the effect size for information
two subgroups, and those associated with the antecedent knowl-
search and each of the antecedents is high for consumers <30
edge/experience may be categorized into three subgroups (Fig.
years and low for consumers >30 years.
A in Web Appendix A; the x-axis represents that effect sizes, and
the y-axis represents the studies included in the meta-analysis). On the other hand, older consumers tend to be more risk
The effect sizes for the categories that emerge through the averse and are expected to undertake more information search
clustering approach, display valences that are similar to those than the younger ones. Also, there are studies that suggest that
obtained for testing the hypotheses of the direct effects. For younger adults take less time to perform tasks predominantly
Table 4
Search for moderators.
Antecedent variables Levels of antecedent K Unweighted r Weighted r Observed Sampling Residual variance (% File drawer Nf Q (d.f.) Moderator
variables (where (observed) (corrected) variance error variance age of total variance) (rc = .05) analysis

M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254


applicable)

Number of Alternatives 16 0.072 0.19 0.082 0.003 0.079 (96.72) 45 486.488a

Cost CostHigh/Low 27 0.16 0.09 0.02 0.004 0.003 (76.60) 23 115.38a (26)

CostMedium 10 0.27 0.29 0.018 0.005 0.013 (72.51) 48 37.3a (9)

Price Dispersion Price DispersionHigh/Low 15 0.10 0.08 0.073 0.004 0.03 (88.58) 11 131.33a (14)

Price DispersionMedium 6 0.41 0.41 0.006 0.002 0.005 (74.10) 42 22.90a (5)

Time Pressure 21 0.20 0.14 0.06 0.004 0.05 (92.58) 39 283.13a (20)

Perceived Risk 16 0.11 0.13 0.01 0.004 0.01 (53.84) 26 34.64a (15)

Financial Constraint 26 0.13 0.04 0.05 0.002 0.05 (95.58) 44 587.36a (25)

Uncertainty 22 0.27 0.24 0.01 0.004 0.01 (67.70) 82 67.84a (21)

Knowledge KnowledgeHigh/Low 12 0.14 0.13 0.097 0.004 0.07 (57.12) 18 27.97a (11)
KnowledgeHigh/Low 26 0.16 0.15 0.003 0.003 0.000 (6.54) 51 27.74 (25)
KnowledgeMedium 5 0.51 0.51 0.005 0.003 0.002 (55.90) 46 7.84 (4)

Prior Experience Prior ExperienceHigh/Low 28 0.18 0.14 0.009 0.003 0.006 (63.47) 53 76.59a (27)

Prior ExperienceMedium 8 0.25 0.24 0.019 0.002 0.017 (87.45) 31 63.39a (7)

Involvement 25 0.30 0.26 0.03 0.003 0.03 (92.31) 103 322.72a (24)
a Heterogeneous.

Nf = Fail-Safe N. rc = critical value of r.

243
244 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

due to their adaptability in information search (Gregan-Paxton be discerned only after purchase and consumption. Darby and
and Roedder-John 1995). Some researchers (Davids and Falkoff Karni (1973) extend Nelsons framework to include credence
1975) attribute this behavior to the younger consumers impa- goods, suggesting that credence attributes cannot be judged con-
tience, while Lucas (1972) argues that the younger generations fidently by the consumer even after purchase and consumption.
are greater risk takers than the older generations. Therefore, we We present two alternate hypotheses based on risk and cost
offer a competing hypothesis: theory, as both points of view have been espoused in extant
literature.
H11b. For the moderator age, the effect size for information
According to risk theory, consumers are expected to under-
search and each of the antecedents is high for consumers >30
take high information search for credence and experience
years and low for consumers <30 years.
products, and low information search for search products (e.g.
Respondent type. Extant studies in the marketing literature Mitra et al. 1999). We provide a sample of products that we cat-
widely use student subjects. Some critics suggest that students egorize as search, experience, and credence in our analysis, in
as participants are different from adults in the general population line with extant literature (e.g. Nelson 1970; Mitra, Reiss, and
(e.g. James and Sonner 2001). Some observe that students have Capella 1999): (a) Search: computer, checking account, grocery
superior skills at many of the exercises required in these studies, items, household appliances; (b) Experience: television, auto-
may be more compliant with authority, may try harder to com- mobile, apparel, movie; (c) Credence: life insurance, exercise
plete the experimental tasks and may be more conscious of doing club, medicine supplements, car repair.
a good job (e.g. Sears 1986). This practice of using compara- We argue that there is an element of customization that may be
tively homogenous student samples in relational research leads associated with experience and credence goods (Guiltinan 1987;
to restricted ranges of effect size (Peterson 2001). This limitation Zeithaml 1981). The variability associated with these products
results in a bias for stronger effects than will be found among contributes to the uncertainty of product performance. There-
non-student samples. The heterogeneity of the non-student sam- fore, in accordance with risk theory, the risk associated with
ple, in contrast, increases error variance, which attenuates the these products is high, explaining the need for extensive infor-
magnitude of effect and possibly underestimates the effect size mation search. Similarly, experience products include those that
(Fern and Monroe 1996). Therefore, this moderator variable is cannot be evaluated before consumption, and consumers are
divided into the following two groups: (a) Students and (b) likely to undertake extensive information search for these kinds
Non-Students, and we predict that: of products/services as well. Experience products include those
that have to be purchased in order to be consumed (e.g. movie), as
H12. The relationship between information search and each
well as those that offer trial prior to purchase (e.g. automobiles,
of the antecedents is stronger for student sample, and is weaker
apparel, shoes). For the latter type of product it is commonly
for non-student sample.
observed that consumers do undertake extensive search for infor-
Study design. An experimental design usually generates mation through examination of alternatives that are available in
less error variance in the denominator of the effect size and the market. As search goods and services can be evaluated before
produces larger effect sizes (Hunter and Schmidt 1990). In addi- consumption (Anderson and Sullivan 1993), the consumer per-
tion, excluding potential confounds generally leads to stronger ceives a low risk for search products as compared to that for
relationships in an experimental setting compared with a non- experience and credence products, and the information search
experimental one. Fern and Monroe (1996) expect that effect for search products is expected to be the lowest. Hence, we
sizes will be different for studies employing fixed-effects model present the following hypothesis:
versus those using survey methods. Therefore, the two groups
H14a. For the moderator product type, as per risk theory, the
created for this moderator variable are: (a) Experiment and
effect size for information search and each of the antecedents is
(b) Non-Experiment. Hence, the following hypothesis:
high for credence and experience products, and low for search
H13. The relationship between information search and each products.
of the antecedents is stronger for experiments, and is weaker for
Conversely, as per cost theory consumers are less likely to
non-experiments.
undertake extensive information search for credence and experi-
Product type. Consumer decision-making is influenced by ence products due to high search costs (Nelson 1970, 1974), low
product type, which is classified as: credence versus experi- perceived benefit (since there is little or no performance infor-
ence versus search goods (Darby and Karni 1973; Mitra, Reiss, mation) and greater shopping effort. In contrast, consumers will
and Capella 1999), convenience goods versus shopping goods typically undertake more information search for search products
versus specialty goods (Hassanein and Head 2006), infrequently because of lower associated search costs. The amount of knowl-
purchased and frequently purchased goods (e.g. Putrevu and edge available to the consumer, prior to purchase, varies with
Ratchford 1997), and hedonic and utilitarian products (Laurent the lowest for credence products and the highest for search prod-
and Kapferer 1985), among others. This review adopts the search ucts. Therefore, consumers will be most sceptical of claims for
versus experience versus credence product categorization for experience and credence products because these claims cannot
product type. Nelson (1970) distinguishes between products on be verified even after purchase and immediate consumption of
search versus experience characteristics. Search characteristics goods (Nelson 1974). Hence, we present the following alternate
can be evaluated prior to purchase. Experience attributes can hypothesis on product type:
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 245

H14b. For the moderator product type, as per cost theory, the support for six; five hypotheses could not be tested due to
effect size for information search and each of the antecedents is insufficient data. Overall, we find strong support for H12 . The
high for search products, and low for credence and experience findings on costHigh/Low (rStudent = 0.15; rNon-Student = 0.07),
products. price dispersionHigh/Low (rStudent = 0.14; rNon-Student = 0.07),
time pressure (rStudent = 0.30; rNon-Student = 0.07),
Due to space limitations predictions and findings on the knowledgeHigh/Low (rStudent = 0.14; rNon-Student = 0.12), prior
moderators gender, education and income, are reported in Web experienceHigh/Low (rStudent = 0.18; rNon-Student = 0.12),
Appendix E. and involvement (rStudent = 0.56; rNon-Student = 0.24) are
as per expectations: effect sizes strengthen for student
Methodology and ndings: Moderator effects samples.
The findings on prior experienceMedium (rStudent = 0.16;
Effect sizes (Table 4) are partitioned into subgroups based rNon-Student = 0.25) are contrary to the hypothesized directions.
on the moderator variables (Geyskens, Steenkamp, and Kumar One possible reason maybe that students rely less on personal
1998; Viswesvaran and Sanchez 1998) (Table 5). In order to experience compared to non-students. Research shows that stu-
test the hypotheses, post hoc t tests of difference in mean effect dents are less willing to share personal experiences compared
sizes between subcategories of the same variable (e.g. exper- to non-students (e.g. Gough 2011). The findings on perceived
iment vs. non experiment) are computed with the Bonferonni risk (rStudent = 0.15; rNon-Student = 0.14), though in the expected
correction for simultaneous tests. The hypothesized relation- direction, are not significant.
ships, the underlying theories, and the kind of support obtained Study design (H13 ). Of the 13 hypotheses for the moderator
for the hypotheses, are presented in Table 5. study design, we test eight hypotheses, and find support for
Age (H11a /H11b ). Of the 13 hypotheses for the moderator age, seven; five hypotheses could not be tested due to insufficient
we test eight, and find support for all eight; five hypotheses could data. Overall, we find strong support for H13 . The findings on
not be tested due to insufficient data. Overall, we find strong sup- costHigh/Low (rExperiment = 0.20; rNon-Experiment = 0.06), num-
port for H11a and H11b . The increased cognitive capacity of the ber of alternatives (rExperiment = 0.33; rNon-Experiment = 0.19),
younger consumer probably provides an explanation for the find- time pressure (rExperiment = 0.38; rNon-Experiment = 0.09),
ings for costHigh/Low (rage<30 = 0.12; rage>30 = 0.16), and on perceived risk (rExperiment = 0.18; rNon-Experiment = 0.14), nan-
price dispersionHigh/Low (rage<30 = 0.10; rage>30 = 0.003), as the cial constraint (rExperiment = 0.22; rNon-Experiment = 0.03),
cost associated with searching for information is high, and older prior experienceHigh/Low (rExperiment = 0.42;
consumers search for less information. A possible reason for the rNon-Experiment = 0.12), and involvement (rExperiment = 0.31;
findings on uncertainty (rage<30 = 0.32; rage>30 = 0.15) could be rNon-Experiment = 0.25), are as per expectations, where the
that in the context of uncertainty while all consumers undertake findings from experiments display a strengthened effect size as
increased information search, younger consumers are able to compared to those from non-experiments.
process more information. Another possible explanation could The findings on price dispersionHigh/Low (rExperiment = 0.02;
be that younger consumers find an uncertain situation more rNon-Experiment = 0.09) are not as per the hypothesis. Note that the
challenging than older consumers. A similar argument explains number of observations in the first cell (i.e. experiment) is one,
the observation on involvement (rage<30 = 0.42; rage>30 = 0.30). and therefore, sample size could be a possible reason for this
Findings on these four antecedents provide support for H11a . finding.
The findings for time pressure (rage<30 = 0.29; Product type (H14a /H14b ). Of the 13 hypotheses for
rage>30 = 0.11) is in line with Freuds (1911/1959) devel- the moderator product type (search/experience/credence),
opmental theories and the work of Green, Fry, and Myerson we test nine hypotheses, and find support for six; four
(1994) on intertemporal choice that suggest that the level hypotheses could not be tested due to insufficient data.
of patience changes over the lifespan of an individual, and The findings for price dispersionHigh/Low (rSearch = 0.03;
that young people are more impatient than older people, and rExperience = 0.19; rCredence = 0.21), nancial constraint
therefore, search for less information. The findings on perceived (rSearch = 0.01; rExperience = 0.25; rCredence = 0.09) and involve-
risk (rage<30 = 0.12; rage>30 = 0.22) agree with the reasoning ment (rSearch = 0.23; rExperience = 0.35; rCredence = 0.32), provide
that younger consumers are more impatient, and higher risk support for risk theory, with consumers undertaking greater
takers. The findings on knowledgeHigh/Low (rage<30 = 0.14; information search for experience and credence products.
rage>30 = 0.06) and prior experienceHigh/Low (rage<30 = 0.24; The findings for number of alternatives (rSearch = 0.26;
rage>30 = 0.11) also agree with the reasoning that younger rExperience = 0.15; rCredence = 0.18), knowledgeHigh/Low
consumers are greater risk takers and so in the context of high (rSearch = 0.12; rExperience = 0.14; rCredence = 0.16) and
or low prior experience/knowledge they are not motivated to prior experienceHigh/Low (rSearch = 0.11; rExperience = 0.20;
look for more information and mitigate the risk associated with rCredence = 0.19), provide support for cost theory, with
making a choice. These findings provide support for H11b . The consumers undertaking greater information search for search
analysis for the moderator age allows us to identify boundary products. These findings provide support for our supposition
conditions under which specific theories hold. that the same product categories have different impact on
Respondent type (H12 ). Of the 13 hypotheses for the consumer information search based on the situation consumers
moderator respondent type, we test eight hypotheses, and find find themselves in.
246 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

Table 5
Moderator analysis: hypotheses and findings.

*p < 0.05 (t-test); n.c.: not calculated. Multiple regression did not enter this variable in the regression model; n.a.: not available, since
enough information is not present due to non reporting; K in parentheses.
a Hypothesized direction of effect size for each moderator group; = High; = Low.
b Support obtained (Y)/not obtained (N).

CC Cognitive Competence.
c Read: cognitive competence provides an explanation for this relationship.
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 247

The findings on costHigh/Low (rSearch = 0.09; hypotheses, and find support for seven. Overall, we find
rExperience = 0.19; rCredence = 0.01) and costMedium some support for HP . The finding for price dispersionHigh/Low
(rSearch = 0.30; rExperience = 0.19; rCredence = 0.32) are not (rInfrequently purchased = 0.05; rFreq Purch = 0.11) is unexpected.
as per expectations. Note that the effect sizes for the two levels This finding may be because in the context of high disper-
of the antecedent cost display similar patterns: effect sizes sion, consumers find it costly to undertake extensive search,
for experience products are the lowest. Experience products while in the context of low perceived price dispersion they
are usually hedonic (e.g. meal at a restaurant), while search do not expect any substantial gains by undertaking addi-
and experience products are often utilitarian (e.g. opening a tional information search for infrequently purchased products.
bank account, buying insurance) in nature. Information on Another possible explanation could be attributed to the presence
product attributes is processed by consumers when undertaking of individuals who have high search costs for homogeneous
information search. We argue that search and credence prod- products (e.g. Varian 1980). The finding for time pressure
ucts can be described in terms of attributes that are usually (rInfreq Purch = 0.38; rFreq Purch = 0.10) probably indicates that
utilitarian (and not hedonic) in nature. However, attributes especially for infrequently purchased products (i.e. more com-
describing experience products are usually more hedonic (and plex decision-making situations), consumers find it difficult to
less utilitarian). These differences in the nature of attributes process information in situations where time is a constraint, and
increase the cost of processing this information associated undertakes lower information search. The finding for nancial
with experience products. The cognitive processes involved constraint (rInfreq Purch = 0.03; rFreq Purch = 0.16) is contrary to
in processing information on attributes for the different types expectations. One possible reason for such behavior may be
of products, therefore, probably provides an explanation for that in situations of financial constraint, consumers have access
the findings for the two levels of cost of information search. to less number of choices when choosing infrequently pur-
The findings on perceived risk (rSearch = 0.16; rExperience = 0.13; chased products, and access to a greater number of choices
rCredence = 0.13) are not significant. Overall, we find good when choosing frequently purchased products. Consumers
support for H14a/b . The analysis for the moderator product type appear to display a greater involvement (rInfreq Purch = 0.22;
also allows us to identify boundary conditions under which rFreq Purch = 0.26) with frequently purchased products. A pos-
specific theories hold. sible reason may be that frequently purchased products provide
Additional analysis. We undertake additional analysis on a consumers with an opportunity for seeking variety than does the
different type of product type, subcategorizing the effect sizes infrequently purchased product category. The findings for per-
by infrequently purchased/frequently purchased products (HP , ceived risk (rInfreq Purch = 0.14; rFreq Purch = 0.14) and costMedium
Table 5). The reason for undertaking this additional analysis is (rInfreq Purch = 0.30; rFreq Purch = 0.28) are not significant.
to test an alternative product type categorization that is widely
used and extensively adopted in extant literature, and is a means Multivariate analysis
for validating the findings obtained for the other moderators.
Infrequently purchased products are those that are not bought In this section we present and test a holistic model on
often, and therefore, consumers are expected to undertake more consumer information search that includes several inverted-U
extensive information search on these products than on the ones shaped variables as uncovered in the analysis of direct effects.
that are frequently purchased. Habit has been shown to exert a Such a model of offline information search should ideally
strong influence in the case of frequently purchased products and include all the ten significant variables identified in this analysis
services. Also, infrequently purchased products usually call for as antecedents to information search, and the intercorrelations
a higher monetary investment, which further explains the need among these ten antecedents (e.g. involvement-cost; dispersion-
for undertaking more information search. The marginal benefit number of alternatives) from past studies need to be identified for
associated with information search for an infrequently purchased testing such a model. A search for these intercorrelations among
product is more than that associated with that of a frequently the antecedents, however, yields intercorrelations for only four
purchased product. We expect that most of the hypothesized of those variables (prior experience, knowledge, perceived risk,
relationships are dictated by perceived risk. However, we note and cost), and benet. We include the variable benet in this
an exception. We expect that when consumers have low product validation model. (The rationale for including this variable is to
knowledge/prior experience they will undertake less informa- ensure the representation of benet in the costbenefit paradigm.
tion search for infrequently purchased products due to the cost Note that we formulate PAc specifically on the variable benet,
associated with searching for information. but are not able to include it in the meta-analysis as the number
Based on the above arguments, we make the following pre- of effect sizes uncovered are less than ten (see Web Appendix
dictions: F).) A correlation matrix containing all these variables needs to
be constructed in order to test the holistic model.
HP . For the moderator product type (infrequently pur-
In our review, we find that the relationships between infor-
chased/frequently purchased), the effect size for information
mation search and the antecedent variables knowledge, prior
search and each of the antecedents is high for infrequently
experience, and cost are inverted-U, where the first variable has
purchased products, and low for frequently purchased products.
three distinct effect sizes, while the later two variables have
Of the 13 hypotheses for the moderator product type (infre- two distinct effect sizes each. Therefore, the correlation matrix
quently purchased/frequently purchased product), we test 13 should contain the intercorrelations among all the following
248 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

variables: information search, benet, knowledge1, knowledge2, the importance of considering the different levels of knowledge,
knowledge3, prior experience1, prior experience2, perceived prior experience and cost separately.
risk, cost1, and cost2. In extant literature, however, the relation-
ships between the different levels of each of the three antecedents General discussion
that have inverted-U relationships with information search (e.g.
knowledge3, prior experience1), and the other antecedents are In conducting the meta-analysis, we make several contribu-
not reported. Therefore, we construct a correlation matrix using tions to the marketing literature. First, we present an overarching
correlations reported in extant literature, and impute reason- framework for consumer information search, and test it. The
able values specifically for relationships that have not been framework that we propose is based on the EOI theory and
investigated in extant literature (see Web Appendix G). Such presents a parsimonious categorization of antecedents to con-
an approach is often followed in meta-analytic reviews, where sumer information search. Second, this study demonstrates that
the findings obtained on direct effects are further validated the relationships of the variables cost, price dispersion, knowl-
through multivariate approaches like multiple regression proce- edge and prior experience with information search are inverted-
dure, or structural equation modeling (Szymanski and Henard U shaped. Note that the inverted-U shape of the relationships for
2001). Note that the variables knowledge1, knowledge2, and knowledge and prior experience are well-established in extant
knowledge3 are the different levels representing the variable literature. Our findings on moderator analysis demonstrate that
knowledge. Similarly, prior experience (prior experience1 and effect sizes are stronger for students (vs. non-students) and for
prior experience2), and cost (cost1 and cost2) are conceptualized experiments (vs. surveys) results that are also well-established
as having two levels each. in the marketing literature. As many of our findings agree with
A baseline model is formulated where the relationships those established in extant literature, they provide us with con-
between the antecedent variables (benet, knowledge1, knowl- fidence that our findings on the direct effects and moderators
edge2, knowledge3, prior experience1, prior experience2, are robust. Therefore, this study reconciles inconsistent findings,
perceived risk, cost1, and cost2, as discussed above) and infor- and urges researchers to re-examine their understanding of effect
mation search are considered (Model 1a) (Fig. 1 a) (see Grace sizes (and sometimes the directions) of specific antecedents to
and Keeley 2006) for a discussion on nonlinear modeling). The offline information search, and offers an improved understand-
results of a structural equations model (using AMOS 18.0) ing of the general strength and variability of the relationships
yield a significant chi-square (2 = 264.17, df = 31; p = 0.000) between several antecedent variables and information search.
for Model 1a, suggesting that the model be rejected. To assess The inverted-U shaped relationships, especially for cost and
the overall fit of the model, we reviewed a number of goodness- price dispersion, present a new, different and updated view on
of-fit indices, including RMSEA (.073), CFI (.843), TLI (.773), how these factors affect consumer information search.
indicating that the model does not have an acceptable fit. Modi- Third, this study conducts moderator analyses to examine
fication indices indicate several antecedents relationships with whether study conditions further explain the direct effects. Our
benet. Model 1b (Fig. 1a) is tested, and the fit indices sug- analyses identify boundary conditions under which specific the-
gest that the model has an acceptable fit (2 = 93.00, df = 25, ories hold. Note that of the 65 hypotheses for the moderators, we
p = 0.000; RMSEA = 0.044; CFI = 0.954; TLI = 0.918) (Web are able to test 46, and find support for 36 (we do not find support
Appendix H). Two alternate models, Model 2a and Model 2b for ten hypotheses, and we cannot test nineteen hypotheses due
(Fig. 1b), are tested with knowledge, prior experience and cost to insufficient data). A support for the majority of the hypotheses
as composite variables (Grace and Keeley 2006) (Web Appendix that we were able to test further enhances our confidence in our
I). Neither of these models provides an acceptable fit. Model 1b findings and makes a significant contribution to the literature.
displays the best fit.
In Model 1b, all estimated coefficients of the antecedents to Implications for retailing and e-tailing
information search are significant, except knowledge2 (Fig. 1a).
The coefficients associated with knowledge1 ( = 0.225) and Our findings have strong implications for retailing as well
knowledge3 ( = 0.482) indicate that they are both significant, as for e-tailing. We find that information search undertaken by
and that the directions of the coefficients are opposite to each younger consumers is high under conditions of high uncertainty.
other, supporting the hypothesized nonlinear relationship with In order to reduce uncertainty, marketers may need to understand
information search. Similar findings are reported for prior the nature of uncertainty associated with the purchase process
experience1 ( = 0.113), prior experience2 ( = 0.241), cost1 and provide information accordingly. As Cox and Rich (1964)
( = 0.189) and cost2 ( = 0.195). These findings validate our suggest, the uncertainty experienced by a consumer may be asso-
hypotheses that the variables knowledge, prior experience and ciated with the product, the brand, and the place of purchase,
cost have nonlinear relationship with information search. Note among others, and that the consumer may abandon the purchase
that similar results regarding nonlinearity of the three variables decision-making process if the level of uncertainty continues
knowledge, prior experience and cost hold across all the four to remain high. This finding is pertinent in the context of e-
tested models. In order to further test the importance of knowl- commerce with younger consumers increasingly making most
edge1, knowledge3, prior experience1, prior experience2, cost1 of their purchase decisions online.
and cost2, each of these paths are deleted and Model 1b is tested The findings on the moderating effect of age on involve-
again. In each case, the chi-square value increased, indicating ment, suggests that the younger consumers probably search for
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 249

(a)
Cost1
(0.137***) -0.189***
Cost2 (-0.192***)
(0.128***)
Benefit 0.195***
(R2=0.108) 0.354*** (0.198***)
(0.361***)
(0.099***)

Knowledge1 -0.225***
(0.084***) (-0.229***)
Information Search
Knowledge2 0.024
R2=0.567
(0.025)
(0.197***) (0.091***) (R2=0.552)
0.482***
Knowledge3
(0.491***)

0.086***
(0.087***)
Perceived Risk
-0.113***
(-0.114***)
Prior Exp1
0.241***
(0.245***)
Prior Exp2

(b)
Cost1 -0.738***
(-0.735***)

0.757a
Cost2 Cost
(0.790a)

Benefit (0.000***) 0.256***


(R2=0.044) (0.261***)
0.352***
(0.074) (0.361***)
Knowledge1 -0.419
(-0.438)
0.087*** Information Search
(0.196***) Knowledge2 0.061a Knowledge
(0.087***) R2=0.569
(0.047a) (R2=0.552)
Knowledge3 0.942
(0.938)
0.236***
(0.269***)
0.087***
(0.087***) Prior Exp
Perceived Risk -0.429***
(-0.426***)

Prior Exp1
0.912a
(0.913a)
Prior Exp2

1. Solid lines represent relationships between antecedents and information search, as proposed by Models 1a and 2a. Results of Models 1a
and 2a are in parentheses. Dotted lines represent additional relationships suggested by modification indices, as suggested by Models 1b and
2b. Results of Models 1b and 2b are not in parentheses.
2. *** indicates significance at p < .01 level; ** indicates significance at p < .05 level; * indicates significance at p < .1 level

Fig. 1. (a) Model 1a and Model 1b. (b) Model 2a and Model 2b.
250 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

more information because of a higher level of involvement com- largely focus on studying the effects of a handful of antecedent
pared to older consumers. This finding suggests that in providing variables on the central construct. For example, the relationship
information, retailers may use affect-based appeals for younger between cost and information search has been extensively mod-
consumers and cognition-based appeals for older consumers eled. However, the relationship between benet and information
online as well as offline. The finding on the moderating effect of search (weighted r = 0.40, Web Appendix F), despite display-
age on time pressure, suggests that promotion and product cues ing a strong effect size, remains under-investigated. This finding
that lay an increased emphasis on aspects that shorten the infor- is further corroborated through the validation of the overall
mation search stage and help consumers make choices quickly, model (Model 1b). Future studies should explicitly include the
may be appropriate for younger consumers. variable benet in the context of studying consumer information
Even though consumers generally undertake a greater amount search.
of information search for infrequently purchased products, under Second, our findings provide evidence that the relation-
the conditions of price dispersion (high or low), and time pres- ships between several antecedents and information search are
sure they are likely to undertake a limited search for infrequently inverted-U shaped. Specifically, for the antecedent cost, we
purchased products compared to frequently purchased products. uncover both positive and negative relationships with consumer
This observation suggests that the increased cost of information information search. Our findings call attention for the need to
search is a likely reason. An implication for retailers is that they explicitly model the different levels of cost of search in future
may provide price-related information for some of the top com- studies.
peting brands in the product category, thereby encouraging the Third, we also find an inverted-U shaped relationship for
consumer to buy from that particular retailer, as well as increas- price dispersion, suggesting that even though the overall effect
ing consumer trust in the retailer. It is especially convenient to size between this antecedent and information search is positive,
provide this information on retailers websites. there may be variations in the degree of the effect depending on
Consumer interactions with stores and brands to obtain the underlying reason for the perceived dispersion. For example,
information prior to purchasing a product/service are increas- when price dispersion is high, consumers search for less infor-
ingly taking place on the Internet, often through a variety of mation. One reason could be that consumers may learn where to
devices (accessed through desktop computers, laptops or mobile look for low prices as they learn that market prices are dispersed
devices). However, this assertion may not apply equally to all (Urbany 1986). Another reason could be that there is less benefit
product categories. For example, consumers information search that is expected from this search. Studies included in the meta-
and purchase of frequently purchased products (e.g. grocery analysis do not measure price dispersion after controlling for
items a product category that is strongly represented in the the differences in attributes of products. Since price dispersion
studies included in the meta-analysis) from online stores are lim- has traditionally been used as an indicator of search benefit in
ited. Also, consumers reach out across various channels to shop. information economics (Tellis and Wernerfelt 1987), there is a
Retailers, therefore, embrace a multichannel context, leveraging need for accounting for the different degrees of benefit that may
traditional as well as new and evolving channels to reach out to be obtained through search.
consumers. In sum, the traditional (i.e. offline) channel contin- Fourth, our findings establish that cost and price disper-
ues to be an important component in a retailers channel mix, sion have an inverted-U shaped relationship with information
and consumer search behavior in this channel is an activity that search. Economics literature suggests that search cost and price
retailers may ignore at their own peril. dispersion are related (e.g. Carlson and McAfee 1983; Stahl
We expect that the categories of variables affecting consumer 1989). Consumers undertake limited information search in a
information search investigated in this meta-analysis, are likely marketplace characterized by high price dispersion (when search
to influence search undertaken across new and evolving chan- costs are high). Conversely, when the market is competitive
nels as well. For example, Korgaonkar, Silverblatt, and Girard (i.e. limited price dispersion), consumers also undertake limited
(2006) suggest that in the online context, perceived risk is greater information search. Similarly, if all consumers have the same
for experience goods than for search goods. Zauberman (2003) positive search costs, there will also be no price dispersion and
demonstrate that in the context of online consumer informa- no information search (the Diamond paradox).
tion search, cost of search is a key determinant of the amount Fifth, for knowledge and prior experience, we are able to
of search undertaken. Therefore, the categories of antecedent demonstrate an inverted-U shaped relationship with information
variables that we investigate in our meta-analysis (e.g. cost, per- search, where at medium level of the antecedent variables, the
ceived risk, knowledge) as well as the moderating variables (e.g. amount of information search undertaken is the highest, which
product type search, experience, credence), in the context of agrees with the findings reported by Moorthy, Ratchford, and
offline information search, are likely to affect consumer infor- Talukdar (1997). Sixth, the review also provides evidence that
mation search in the online context as well (e.g. Konus, Verhoef, the effect sizes between information search and the antecedents
and Neslin 2008). including perceived risk, nancial constraint, uncertainty and
involvement are positive, and that between time pressure and the
Implications for theory and research central construct is negative, as per expectations.
Contingency effects. Moderating effects of several modera-
Main effects. First, the review makes it apparent that empir- tors including, study conditions (respondent type, study design),
ical investigations in the area of consumer information search product categories (search, experience, credence/infrequently
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 251

purchased, frequently purchased), and demographic character- be embraced, which explicitly incorporates and manipulates the
istics (age) are tested on the direct effects. Overall, we find different levels of the antecedent variables an approach that
strong support for all the moderators. Through the moderator is currently missing in literature. More experimental studies are
analysis we are able to identify specific conditions under which also called for as our meta-analysis finds that experiments dis-
certain theories hold (Table 5). For the moderator age, we offer play a strengthened effect size as compared to non-experiments.
competing hypotheses based on the theory of cognitive compe- Also, a number of other variables that are not relevant to
tence and that of risk theory. Our findings suggest that theory of consumer information search in the offline context may impact
cognitive competence explains the moderating effect of age for information search in the new and evolving channels. Many
the variables costHigh/Low , price dispersionHigh/Low , uncertainty of the new and emerging channels (for undertaking shopping)
and involvement. Risk theory provides an explanation for the have become a reality because of developments in technology.
moderating effect on the variables time pressure, perceived risk, Concomitantly, users of these channels are expected to interact
knowledgeHigh/Low and prior experienceHigh/Low. with technology for fulfilling information and shopping needs.
Our findings on the moderator product type (search, expe- A set of factors that probably impact search in these channels
rience, credence), identifies conditions in which cost theory or include variables that capture the users interaction with tech-
risk theory provide explanation for the observed effect sizes. The nology, such as self efcacy, afnity to technology, ease of use,
moderating effect of search/experience/credence product type usefulness, and use of decision aids, to name a few. Also, the
on consumer information search under the conditions of price phenomenon of word-of-mouth (WOM) is expected to have
dispersionHigh/Low , nancial constraint and involvement may be an increased significance in the context of online information
explained by risk theory. Similarly, the tenets of cost theory prob- search as compared to that of offline context. The enthusiastic
ably provide an explanation for the moderating effect on number adoption of social media (including social networking websites,
of alternatives, knowledgeHigh/Low and prior experienceHigh/Low. review posting sites, product sites that include reviews of prod-
Similarly, for another product type categorization (infre- ucts), and the ensuing electronic (e-WOM) have implications for
quently purchased/frequently purchased), we find that it is the amount of information search undertaken by a consumer.
risk theory that provides explanation for the moderating effect A separate meta-analysis needs to be conducted to investigate
on most of the antecedent variables. Our moderator analysis the effects of antecedents on information search in online set-
also indicates that there are several relationships (e.g. price tings. All studies included in this meta-analysis are set in offline
dispersion search/experience/credence product type; prior traditional retailing settings, and the choice to focus the investi-
experience search/experience/credence product type) that have gation to this specific format is conscious and logical. This article
not received adequate attention in the context of offline consumer hopes to provoke future research, especially collating consumer
information search. The information search literature will ben- information search behavior from newer retail formats, which
efit from undertaking studies on these relationships that have so can then be compared to the findings presented here. Such com-
far received limited attention. parisons will expand the understanding of information search
behavior of consumers across various retailing formats.
Limitations and directions for future research

The theoretical, substantive and managerial contributions Acknowledgements


outlined earlier, notwithstanding, the study does have limita-
tions. This study uses the results of bivariate findings in the The authors wish to thank Professors Ruth Bolton, Shelby
meta-analysis. Only those studies that are accessible in the Hunt, Brian Ratchford, and Dr. Leslie Vincent for their com-
English-language databases are included in the study. Second, ments on an earlier version of this paper. The authors would
intercorrelations among all the ten antecedent variables cannot also like to thank the editors and the three anonymous reviewers
be located. Therefore, the holistic model including all the ten for providing a truly collegial review process.
variables cannot be tested.
Our findings open up several hitherto unrecognized avenues Appendix A. Supplementary data
for future research. It is evident from the reported findings that
several antecedents to information search are inverted-U shaped, Supplementary material related to this article can be
suggesting that it is imperative that these variables be so consid- found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
ered in future studies. Importantly, future investigations need to j.jretai.2014.03.001.
consider the different levels of the same variable. Such attempts
will help in providing the much-needed data that we were unable
to find for the partially filled cells that put a constraint on References
hypothesis testing. These future studies can generate the required
correlations to test a more comprehensive model that include Anderson, Erin, Wujin Chu and Barton A. Weitz (1987), Industrial Purchas-
ing: An Empirical Exploration of the Buyclass Framework, Journal of
many or all of the antecedents identified in this meta-analysis. Marketing, 51 (July), 7186.
We also note that studies in the information search literature Anderson, Eugene W. and Mary W. Sullivan (1993), The Antecedents and
have mostly concentrated on the survey methodology, indicat- Consequences of Customer Satisfaction for Firms, Marketing Science, 12
ing that a greater use of the experimental methodology needs to (2), 12543.
252 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

Assmus, Gert, John U. Farley and Donald R. Lehmann (1984), How Advertising Fern, Edward F. and Kent B. Monroe (1996), Effect Size Estimates: Issues
Affects Sales: Meta-Analysis of Econometric Results, Journal of Marketing and Problems in Interpretation, Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (2),
Research, 21 (1), 6574. 89105.
Avery, Rosemary J. (1996), Determinants of Search for Nondurable Goods: An Freud, Sigmund (1911/1959), Formulations Regarding the Two Principles of
Empirical Assessment of the Economics of Information Theory, Journal of Mental Functioning, New York: Basic Books.
Consumer Affairs, 30 (2), 390420. Gemunden, Hans Georg (1985), Perceived Risk and Information Search: A
Baradell, Janet G. and Kitty Klein (1993), Relationship of Life stress and Body Systematic Meta-analysis of the Empirical Evidence, International Journal
Consciousness to Hypervigilant Decision Making, Journal of Personality of Research in Marketing, 2, 79100.
and Social Psychology, 64 (2), 26773. Geyskens, Inge, Jan-Benedict E.M. Steenkamp and Nirmalya Kumar (1998),
Bauer, Raymond A. (1960), Consumer Behavior as Risk-Taking, in Dynamic Generalizations About Trust in Marketing Channel Relationships Using
Marketing for a Changing World, Hancock R. S. ed. Chicago: American Meta-Analysis, International Journal of Research in Marketing, 15,
Marketing Association, 38998. 22348.
Beatty, Sharon E. and Scott M. Smith (1987), External Search Effort: An Inves- Glass, Gene V. (1976), Primary, Secondary, and Meta-Analysis of Research,
tigation across Several Product Categories, Journal of Consumer Research, Educational Research, 5 (10), 38.
14 (June), 8395. Grace, James B. and Jon E. Keeley (2006), A Structural Equation Model
Bettman, James R. (1974), Toward a Statistics for Consumer Decision Net Analysis of Postfire Plant Diversity in California Shrublands, Ecological
Models, Journal of Consumer Research, 1 (1), 7180. Applications, 16, 50314.
(1979), An Information Processing Theory of Consumer Green, Leonard, Astrid F. Fry and Joel Myerson (1994), Discounting of Delayed
Choice, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Rewards: A Life-Span Comparison, Psychological Science, 5, 336.
Bettman, James R. and Whan C. Park (1980), Effects of Prior Knowledge Gregan-Paxton, Jennifer and Deborah Roedder-John (1995), Are Young Chil-
and Experience and Phase of the Choice Process on Consumer Decision dren Adaptive Decision Makers? A Study of Age, Journal of Consumer
Processes: A Protocol Analysis, Journal of Consumer Research, 7 (3), Research, 21 (4), 56780.
23448. Gough, Matthew (2011), Looking After Your Pearls: The Dilemmas of Mental
Bloch, Peter H., Daniel L. Sherrel and Nancy M. Ridgeway (1986), Con- Health Self-Disclosure in Higher Education Teaching, Journal of Mental
sumer Search: An Extended Framework, Journal of Consumer Research, Health Training, Education & Practice, 6 (4), 20310.
13 (June), 11926. Guiltinan, Joseph P. (1987), The Price Bundling of Services: A Normative
Bodur, H. Onur and Lissa Matyas (2008), When Do Consumers Prefer More Framework, Journal of Marketing, 51, 7485.
Choice? Moderating Effects of Regulatory Focus, Advances in Consumer Hassanein, Khaled and Milena Head (2006), The Impact of Infusing Social
Research, 35, 7878. Presence in the Web Interface: An Investigation Across Product Types,
Brucks, Merrie (1985), The Effects of Product Class Knowledge on Information International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 10 (2), 3155.
Search Behavior, Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (June), 116. Hedges, Larry V. and Ingram Olkin (1985), Statistical Methods for Meta-
Burnkrant, Robert E. (1976), A Motivational Model of Information Processing Analysis, Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Intensity, Journal of Consumer Research, 3, 2130. Hill, C. Jeanne (2001), The Nature of Problem Recognition and Search in the
Carlson, John A. and R. Preston McAfee (1983), Discrete Equilibrium Price Extended Health Care Decision, Journal of Services Marketing, 15 (6/7),
Dispersion, Journal of Political Economy, 91 (3), 48093. 45479.
Chen, Chien-Chung (2012), Detecting Moderator Effects On Construct Rela- Hockey, G. Robert J. (1970), Effect of Loud Noise on Attentional Selectivity,
tionships In Empirical Sales Research: A Meta-Analysis, Journal of Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 2836.
Management & Marketing Research, 11 (September), 118. Hunter, John E. and Frank L. Schmidt (1990), Methods of Meta-Analysis:
Cheon, Hongsik John, Chang-Hoan Cho and John Sutherland (2007), A Meta- Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings, Newbury Park: Sage Pub-
Analysis of Studies on the Determinants of Standardization and Localization lications.
of International Marketing and Advertising Strategies, Journal of Interna- Irwin, Julie R. and Gary H. McClelland (2003), Negative Consequences
tional Consumer Marketing, 19 (4), 10947. of Dichotomizing Continuous Predictor Variables, Journal of Marketing
Claxton, John, Joseph N. Fry and Bernard Portis (1974), A Taxonomy of Pre- Research, 60 (August), 36671.
purchase Information Gathering Patterns, Journal of Consumer Research, Jacoby, Jacob, Donald E. Speller and Carol K. Berning (1974), Brand Choice
1 (December), 3542. Behavior as a Function of Information Load: Replication and Extension,
Cox, Donald F. and Stuart U. Rich (1964), Perceived Risk and Consumer Journal of Consumer Research, 1, 3342.
Decision-Making: The Case of Telephone Shopping, Journal of Marketing Jacoby, Jacob, Robert A. Chestnut and William A. Fisher (1978), A Behavioral
Research, 1 (4), 329. Process Approach to Information Acquisition in Nondurable Purchasing,
Davids, Anthony and Bradley B. Falkoff (1975), Juvenile Delinquents Then and Journal of Marketing Research, 15 (November), 53244.
Now: Comparisons of Findings from 1959 and 1974, Journal of Abnormal James, William L. and Brenda S. Sonner (2001), Just Say No to Traditional Stu-
Psychology, 84 (April), 1614. dent Samples, Journal of Advertising Research, 41 (SeptemberOctober),
Darby, Michael R. and Edi Karni (1973), Free Competition and the Opti- 6371.
mal Amount of Fraud, Journal of Law and Economics, 16 (April), Johnson, Eric J. and J. Edward Russo (1984), Product Familiarity and Learning
6788. New Information, Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (June), 54250.
DeSarbo, Wayne S. and Jungwhan Choi (1999), A Latent Structure Double Kahneman, Daniel (1973), Attention and Effort, Englewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice-
Hurdle Regression Model for Exploring Heterogeneity in Consumer Search Hall.
Patterns, Journal of Econometrics, 89, 42355. Katona, George and Eva Mueller (1955), A Study of Purchase Decisions, in
Dholakia, Utpal M. (2001), A Motivational Process Model of Product Involve- Consumer Behavior: The Dynamics of Consumer Reaction, Clark L. H. ed.
ment and Consumer Risk Perception, European Journal of Marketing, 35 New York: New York University Press, 3087.
(11/12), 134060. Kirasic, Kathleen C., Gary L. Allen, Shannon H. Dobson and Katherine S.
Dowling, Grahame R. and Richard Staelin (1994), A Model of Perceived Risk Binder (1996), Aging, Cognitive Resources, and Declarative Learning,
and Intended Risk-Handling Activity, Journal of Consumer Research, 21 Psychology and Aging, 11 (4), 65870.
(June), 11934. Konus, Umut, Peter C. Verhoef and Scott A. Neslin (2008), Multichannel
Engel, James F., David T. Kollat and Roger L.D. Blackwell (1973), Consumer Shopper Segments and Their Covariates, Journal of Retailing, 84 (4),
Behavior, 21st ed. New York: Free. 398413.
Eysenck, Michael W. and Manuel G. Calvo (1992), Anxiety and Perfor- Korgaonkar, Pradeep, Ronnie Silverblatt and Tulay Girard (2006), Online
mance: The Processing Efficiency Theory, Cognition and Emotion, 6, Retailing, Product Classifications, and Consumer Preferences, Internet
40934. Research, 16 (3), 26788.
M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254 253

Laaksonen, Pirjo (1994), Consumer Involvement: Concepts and Research, Lon- Peterson, Robert A. (2001), On the Use of College Students in Social Sci-
don, UK: Routledge. ence Research: Insights from a Second-Order Meta-Analysis, Journal of
Laroche, Michael, Gad Saad, Mark Cleveland and Elizabeth Browne (2000), Consumer Research, 28 (3), 45061.
Gender Differences in Information Search Strategies for a Christmas Gift, Petty, Richard E. and John T. Cacioppo (1981), Issue Involvement as a Mod-
Journal of Consumer Marketing, 17 (6/7), 50024. erator of the Effects on Attitude of Advertising Content and Context, In
Laroche, Michel, Mark Cleveland and Elizabeth Browne (2004), Exploring Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 8, Monroe K. B. ed. Provo, UT:
Age-Related Differences in Information Acquisition for a Gift Purchase, Association for Consumer Research, 204.
Journal of Economic Psychology, 25 (1), 6195. Punj, Girish N. and Richard Staelin (1983), A Model of Consumer Information
Laurent, Gilles and Jean-Noel Kapferer (1985), Measuring Consumer Involve- Search Behavior for New Automobiles, Journal of Consumer Research, 9
ment Profiles, Journal of Marketing Research, 49, 4153. (March), 36680.
Lucas, Robert E. (1972), Expectations and the Neutrality of Money, Journal Putrevu, Sanjay and Brian T. Ratchford (1997), A Model of Search Behavior
of Economic Theory, 4, 10324. with an Application to Grocery Shopping, Journal of Retailing, 73 (4),
Malhotra, Naresh K. (1982), Information Load and Consumer Decision Mak- 46386.
ing, Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (4), 41931. Ratchford, Brian T. and Narasimhan Srinivasan (1993), An Empirical Investi-
(1983), On Individual Differences in Search Behavior gation of Returns to Search, Marketing Science, 12 (1), 7387.
for a Nondurable, Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (June), 12531. Roedder-John, Deborah, Carol A. Scott and James R. Bettman (1986), Sam-
Mata, Rui, Lael J. Schooler and Jorg Rieskamp (2007), The Aging Decision pling Data for Covariation Assessment: The Effect of Prior Beliefs on Search
Maker: Cognitive Aging and the Adaptive Selection of Decision Strategies, Patterns, Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (1), 3847.
Psychology and Aging, 22 (4), 796810. Roedder John, Deborah and Catherine A. Cole (1986), Age Differences in
McDowd, Joan M. (1997), Inhibition in Attention and Aging, Journals of Information Processing: Understanding Deficits in Young and Elderly Con-
Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 52, sumers, Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (3), 297315.
26573. Rothschild, Michael (1974), Searching for the Lowest Price when the Dis-
Milford, James T. and Raymond P. Perry (1977), A Methodological Study of tribution of Prices is Unknown, Journal of Political Economy, 82 (4),
Overload, Journal of General Psychology, 97, 1317. 689711.
Miller, George A. (1956), The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Schaie, K. Warner and Sherry L. Willis (1993), Age Difference Patterns of
Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information, Psychological Psychometric Intelligence in Adulthood: Generalizability Within and Across
Review, 63, 8197. Domains, Psychology and Aging, 8 (1), 4455.
Mitchell, Andrew A. (1981), The Dimensions of Advertising Involvement, In Schaninger, Charles M. and Donald Sciglimpaglia (1981), The Influ-
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 8, Monroe Kent B. ed. Provo, UT: ence of Cognitive Personality Traits and Demographics on Consumer
Association for Consumer Research, 2530. Information Acquisition, Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (2),
Mitra, Kaushik, Michelle C. Reiss and Louis M. Capella (1999), An Exami- 20816.
nation of Perceived Risk, Information Search and Behavioral Intentions in Sears, David O. (1986), College Sophomores in the Laboratory: Influences of a
Search, Experience and Credence Services, Journal of Services Marketing, Narrow Data Base on Social Psychologys View of Human Nature, Journal
13 (3), 20828. of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (3), 51530.
Moore, William L. and Donald R. Lehmann (1980), Individual Differences in Simon, Herbert A. (1957), Models of Man: Social and Rational, New York: John
Search Behavior for a Nondurable, Journal of Consumer Research, 7 (3), Wiley & Sons.
296307. (1979), Rational Decision Making in Business Organi-
Moorthy, Sridhar, Brian T. Ratchford and Debabrata Talukdar (1997), Con- zations, American Economic Review, 69 (4), 493513.
sumer Information Search Revisited: Theory and Empirical Analysis, (1987), Satisficing, In The New Palgrave: A Dictionary
Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (4), 26377. of Economics, Vol. 4, Eatwell , John , Milgate M. and Newman P. eds. New
Nelson, Philip (1970), Information and Consumer Behavior, Journal of Polit- York: MacMillan, 2435.
ical Economy, 78 (MarchApril), 31129. Smith, Gerald E., Meera P. Venkataraman and Ruby R. Dholakia (1999),
(1974), Advertising as Information, Journal of Political Diagnosing the Search Cost Effect: Waiting Time and the Moderating
Economy, 82 (JulyAugust), 72954. Impact of Prior Category Knowledge, Journal of Economic Psychology, 20,
Newman, Joseph W. and Richard Staelin (1971), Multivariate Analysis of Dif- 285314.
ferences in Buyer Decision Time, Journal of Marketing Research, 8 (May), Srinivasan, Narasimhan and Brian T. Ratchford (1991), An Empirical Test of a
1928. Model of External Search for Automobiles, Journal of Consumer Research,
Newman, Joseph W. (1977), Consumer External Search: Amount and Determi- 18 (2), 23342.
nants, in Consumer and Industrial Buying Behavior, Woodside Arch, Sheth Stahl, Dale O. II (1989), Oligopolistic Pricing with Sequential Consumer
Jagdish and Bennet Peter, eds. New York: North-Holland Search, The American Economic Review, 79 (4), 70012.
Norman, Donald A. and Daniel J. Bobrow (1975), On Data-Limited and Staal, Mark A. (2004), Stress, Cognition, and Human Performance: A Liter-
Resource-Limited Processes, Cognitive Psychology, 7 (1), 4464. ature Review and Conceptual Framework [NASA Technical Memorandum
Otnes, Cele C. (1990), A Study of Consumer External Search Strategies Per- 212824], Moffett Field, CA: NASA-Ames Research Center.
taining to Christmas Shopping, (unpublished doctoral dissertation). TN: Stigler, George (1961), Economics of Information, Journal of Political Econ-
University of Tennessee. omy, 69 (3), 21325.
Pan, Xing, Brian T. Ratchford and Venkatesh Shankar (2003), The Evolu- Stone, Robert N. (1984), The Marketing Characteristics of Involvement,
tion of Price Dispersion in Internet Retail Markets, Advances in Applied Advances in Consumer Research, 11 (1), 2105.
Microeconomics, 12, 85105. Sultan, Fareena, John U. Farley and Donald R. Lehmann (1990), A Meta-
Parayitam, Satyanarayana and Robert S. Dooley (2011), Is too much Cogni- Analysis of Applications of Diffusion Models, Journal of Marketing
tive Conflict in Strategic Decision-Making Teams too Bad?, International Research, 27 (February), 707.
Journal of Conict Management, 22 (4), 34257. Szymanski, David M. and David H. Henard (2001), Customer Satisfaction:
Pauwels, Koen, Peter S.H. Leeflang, TeerlingF Marije L. and K.R. Eelko Huiz- A Meta-Analysis of the Empirical Evidence, Journal of the Academy of
ingh (2011), Does Online Information Drive Offline Revenues? Only for Marketing Science, 29 (1), 1635.
Specific Products and Consumer Segments!, Journal of Retailing, 87 (1), Tellis, Gerald J. and Birger Wernerfelt (1987), Competitive Price and Qual-
117. ity under Asymmetric Information, Marketing Science, 6 (Summer),
Payne, John W., James M. Bettman and Mary F. Luce (1996), When Time is 24053.
Money: Decision Behavior Under Opportunity-Cost Pressure, Organiza- Urbany, Joel E. (1986), An Experimental Examination of the Economics of
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66 (2), 13152. Information, Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (2), 25771.
254 M. Maity et al. / Journal of Retailing 90 (2, 2014) 233254

Urbany, Joel E., Peter A. Dickson and Rosemary Kalapurakal (1996), Price Weiss, Allen M. and Jan B. Heide (1993), The Nature of Organizational
Search in the Retail Grocery Market, Journal of Marketing, 60 (2), Search in High Technology Markets, Journal of Marketing Research, 30
91104. (2), 22033.
Urbany, Joel E., Peter R. Dickson and William L. Wilkie (1989), Buyer Uncer- Wolf, Fredric M. (1986), Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Methods for Research
tainty and Information Search, Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (2), Synthesis, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
20814. Zauberman, Gal (2003), The Intertemporal Dynamics of Consumer Lock-In,
Varian, Hal R. (1980), A Model of Sales, The American Economic Review, 70 Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (3), 40519.
(4), 6519. Zeithaml, Valarie A. (1981), How Consumer Evaluation Processes Differ
Viswesvaran, Chockalingam and Juan I. Sanchez (1998), Moderator Between Goods and Services, in Marketing of Services (unpublished doc-
Search in Meta-Analysis: A Review and Cautionary Note on Exist- toral dissertation), Donnelly James H. and George William R., eds. Chicago,
ing Approaches, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58 (1), IL: American Marketing Association, 18690.
7787.
Vriend, Nicolaas J. (1996), Rational Behavior and Economic Theory, Journal
of Economic Behavior and Organization, 29, 26385.

You might also like