You are on page 1of 8

NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / y n i m g

Quantity without numbers and numbers without quantity in the parietal cortex
Marinella Cappelletti , Neil Muggleton, Vincent Walsh
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Dept of Psychology, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: A dominant view in numerical cognition is that processing the quantity indicated by numbers (e.g. deciding
Received 11 June 2008 the larger between two numbers such as 12.07 or 15.02) relies on the intraparietal regions (IPS) of the
Revised 1 February 2009 cerebral cortex. However, it remains unclear whether the IPS could play a more general role in numerical
Accepted 7 February 2009
cognition, for example in (1) quantity processing even with non-numerical stimuli (e.g. choosing the larger
Available online 21 February 2009
of bikini and coat); and/or (2) conceptual tasks involving numbers beyond those requiring quantity
processing (e.g. attributing a summer date to either 12.07 or 15.02).
In this study we applied fMRI-guided TMS to the left and right IPS, while independently manipulating
stimulus and task. Our results showed that IPS involvement in numerical cognition is neither stimulus-
specic nor specic for conceptual tasks. Thus, quantity judgments with numerical and non-numerical
stimuli were equally affected by IPS-TMS, as well as a number conceptual task not requiring quantity
comparisons. However, IPS-TMS showed no impairment for perceptual decisions on numbers without any
conceptual processing (i.e. colour judgment), nor for conceptual decisions that did not involve quantity or
number stimuli (e.g. summer object: bikini or coat?). These results are consistent with proposals that the
parietal areas are engaged in the conceptual representation of numbers but they challenge the most common
view that number processing is so automatic that the simple presentation of numbers activates the IPS and a
sense of magnitude. Rather, our results show that the IPS is only necessary when conceptual operations need
to be explicitly oriented to numerical concepts.
2009 Elsevier Inc. Open access under CC BY license.

Introduction performance in terms of increases in response times in number


comparison following IPS stimulation (e.g. Andres et al., 2005;
Neuroimaging studies have shown that the parietal regions, Cappelletti et al., 2007). Moreover, recent studies on developmental
especially those around the intraparietal sulcus (IPS)1 are reliably dyscalculia showed that this is associated with right IPS dysfunctions
activated when processing number quantities, for instance when (Molko et al., 2003; Price et al., 2008; Rotzer et al., 2008).
deciding on the larger of two numbers (e.g. Dehaene et al., 2003; Despite this converging evidence, it is still unknown whether the
Nieder, 2005). Some studies have further differentiated the ne- IPS is critical for: (1) quantity processing irrespective of the stimuli
grained structures within the IPS and provided evidence that the used, i.e. numerical or non-numerical; and (2) other conceptual
bilateral horizontal segment of the IPS2 plays a role in quantity processing of numbers that do not require quantity manipulation (e.g.
processing (Dehaene et al., 2003). Further evidence of the involve- attributing a summer date to either 12.07 or 15.02). A few studies
ment of the IPS regions in number processing comes from studies have examined the stimulus-specic nature of quantitative proces-
investigating how this process is affected by permanent neurological sing. These have proposed that quantitative judgments on different
damage in patients or temporary disruption following transcranial types of stimuli may either rely on distinct subregions within the
magnetic stimulation (TMS). Patients with left parietal damage, for parietal lobe, or that the parietal regions are involved in a generic
example, can be impaired at processing number quantities (e.g. process of comparison of various types of stimuli. Behavioural support
Cipolotti et al., 1991; Dehaene and Cohen, 1991; Lemer et al., 2003; for the hypothesis of a generic comparison mechanism can be found in
Polk et al., 2001), and TMS studies have reported impaired the evidence of a similar distance effect in many continua other than
numerical stimuli (e.g. Fulbright et al., 2003; Johnson, 1939; Moyer,
1973). That is, the time it takes to compare two stimuli increases as the
Corresponding author. Fax: +44 207 813 2835. distance between the two stimuli decreases (i.e. distance effect,
E-mail address: m.cappelletti@ucl.ac.uk (M. Cappelletti). Moyer and Landauer, 1967). However, the results of imaging studies
1
In the text we refer to the intraparietal sulcus in terms of the brain regions around examining this issue are mixed: while some of these studies showed
^
the sulcus, rather than the sulcus per se, although for brevity we indicate these as IPS or
IPS regions.
common activation of the parietal areas in quantitative judgments
2
The horizontal segment of the IPS is the middle part of the sulcus in the dorsal irrespective of the stimuli used (e.g. Fias et al., 2003), others
^
parietal lobe that intersects the postcentral sulcus (Ono et al., 1990). demonstrated that different stimuli are represented in distributed

1053-8119/ 2009 Elsevier Inc. Open access under CC BY license.


doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.016
M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529 523

and overlapping neural populations within the parietal areas (Fulb- Material and methods
right et al., 2003; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2005; Pinel et al., 2004). These
discordant results may depend on the tasks used: for instance in Stimuli and tasks
tasks such as number comparison, quantity information, has to be
extracted from the stimuli, whereas in the comparison of variables The experiment was controlled using the Cogent Graphics toolbox
such as luminance and physical size (e.g. Pinel et al., 2004) or angles (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/Cogent/) and Matlab7 software. The
and lines (e.g. Fias et al., 2003), quantity information can be obtained viewing distance was approximately 0.5 m. A total of 144 Arabic
by visual inspection of the stimuli. Thus, there is an important numbers and 144 object names were generated. Arabic numbers were
distinction between quantity processing based on extracting prior presented as pairs of 3 and 4-digits separated by a dot, e.g. 12.07. They
knowledge associated with the stimuli, and quantity processing that referred to a linear dimension of quantity, to dates (e.g. 12th July) or
can be based on the physical properties of the stimulus. At present times (e.g. seven minutes past twelve in the morning). Numerals
there is still a need to know whether the IPS regions are involved in indicating quantities ranged from 1 to 31 for the rst half of the
quantity processing of non-numerical stimuli with similar task numerical expression and from 01 to 59 for the second part (i.e. from
demands. 1.01 to 23.59). Numerals indicating dates were chosen to represent
Although the most distinctive meaning of numbers is to express either summer or winter days in the Northern hemisphere; therefore
numerical quantity or cardinality (e.g. 5 items) this is not the only summer dates included the months of June, July, and August, winter
information that numbers convey (Wiese, 2003). They carry other dates included the months of December, January and February. Dates
types of information that do not imply quantity manipulations, for were expressed in terms of day and month separated by a dot (e.g.
instance hours (e.g. 7.15 a.m.), dates (e.g. 2006) and mathematical 12th July = 12.07). Numerals referring to a date ranged from 01 to 31
constants (e.g. 3.14). Although these meanings are clearly different for the rst half of each numerical expression and from 01 to 12 for the
from quantity information, this distinction has not yet been fully second part (i.e. from 01.01 to 31.12). Each date indicating a summer
explored. Suggestive evidence of this dissociation comes from lesion (or winter) month was presented with either a winter (or summer)
studies showing dissociations between quantity manipulation and month, e.g. 12.07 and 15.02 or with a neutral month, i.e. 12.07 and
processing of other non-quantity meanings of numbers (e.g. Cappel- 24.03. Numerals indicating times were chosen to refer to either a
letti et al., 2008). Moreover, a recent fMRI study reported a distinction sleeping or a working time approximately in terms of 8am to 6pm
between processing numbers in arithmetical and encyclopaedic working day. Therefore, working times were chosen between 8am and
contexts, which are thought to rely on parietal and left temporal 6pm, and sleeping times between 10pm and 7am. Times were
regions respectively (Lochy and Van Turennout, 2005). Some recent expressed in terms of 24-hour clock with the rst pair of digits
studies have focused on another type of non-quantity information referring to the hour and the other two digits, separated by a dot,
conveyed by numbers that is order. This refers to the use of numbers to referring to the minutes past the hour (e.g. 16.30 is half past four in the
indicate the position of an item or event in an ordered sequence (e.g. afternoon). Numbers referring to a time ranged from 00 to 23 for the
the 5th items, Butterworth, 1999; Dehaene, 1997; Jacob and Nieder, rst half of each numerical expression and from 01 to 59 for the
2007). A few recent studies have shown that ordinal and quantity second part (i.e. from 00.01 to 23.59). Object names referred to
information dissociate both behaviourally (e.g. Gevers et al., 2003; concrete, countable objects whose size could be unambiguously
Turconi et al., 2006; Zamarian et al., 2007) and temporally: namely identied and that could be used in both the quantity (e.g. larger
they take place in overlapping brain regions of the parietal cortex on object: bikini or coat?) and the non-quantity tasks (e.g. summer
different time scales (e.g. Turconi et al., 2004). All this evidence object: bikini or coat?). In the latter task, each object name in a pair
suggests a distinction between quantitative and non-quantitative consisted of a stimulus unambiguously related to a season (e.g. bikini
meanings of numbers including ordinal information. However, it is for summer) and presented with another object name either
still unknown to what extent the IPS regions are critical for these non- belonging to another season (e.g. coat for winter) or with a neutral
quantity conceptual processes of numbers, and whether the IPS is object, i.e. not related to any season (e.g. car). All the experimental
equally critical for number processes that do and do not involve stimuli had been previously piloted in a study with different
quantity information. participants.

The present study Procedure

In this study, we applied fMRI-guided TMS and factorially The 3 different tasks (quantity, non-quantity categorical and
manipulated stimulus (numerical and non-numerical) and task perceptual-decision) with 2 stimuli (numbers and object names)
(quantity and non-quantity) independently. We aimed to test the were blocked (6 stimuli per block) and presented in pseudo-random
role of left and right IPS in: (1) quantity processing with numerical or order. Participants viewed pairs of stimuli presented one above the
non-numerical stimuli in terms of object names; and in (2) conceptual other with a xation cross in the middle of the computer screen.
processing of numbers requiring or not requiring the manipulation of For each pair, they were instructed to indicate with a button press
quantity information. which stimulus was the correct response to a question consisting of
To address these issues, subjects were engaged in three different two key words presented above the upper stimulus before and during
categorization tasks. Two focused on the conceptual attributes of a block of 6 trials (see Fig. 1). On every trial, participants were
either quantity (e.g. choosing the larger of 12.07 and 15.02, or the instructed to press the upper or the lower-arrow keys on the keyboard
larger of bikini and coat) or category (e.g. attributing a summer date for the upper or the lower stimulus respectively.3 Trials where the
to either 12.07 and 15.02 or choosing a summer object between correct answer was the upper or the lower stimulus were presented in
bikini and coat). The third was a perceptual task that focused the equal proportion.
subjects' attention on the colour of the stimulus (e.g. choosing a In each pair, each stimulus was presented in one of four possible
number or object name drawn in red) and therefore involved both colours (red, yellow, blue, green). In the quantity and in the non-
attention to the stimulus and response selection as in the other tasks
but without requiring a conceptual decision. We chose a non-quantity 3
Participants' ngers rested on the two designated keys of the keyboard for the
conceptual task (e.g. attributing a summer date to either 12.07 or whole duration of the experiment following a procedure common in two-choice
15.02) that required categorical processing of numerical information response experiments. This is unlikely to result in response priming as correct
but it was unlikely to rely on quantitative manipulations. responses were randomized.
524 M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529

Fig. 1. Experimental design. In each trial, participants viewed pairs of stimuli presented one above the other with a xation cross in the middle of the computer screen. Stimuli could
be either two Arabic numbers (left column) or two object names (right column) presented in one of four possible colours (red, yellow, blue, green). Participants were instructed to
indicate with a button press which of these stimuli was the correct response to a question consisting of two key words presented above the upper stimulus before and during the
stimulus display. There were three types of tasks: for quantity tasks (A), there were four possible types of questions: larger?, smaller?, more?, less?. For non-quantity categorical
tasks (B), there were four different types of questions: summer month/object?, winter month/object?, sleeping time/object?, working time/object? (either month or object
was displayed depending on the stimulus condition). For the stimulus-colour decision (C) the questions were: blue/red/yellow/green number or object? The 3 different tasks
(quantity, non-quantity categorical and colour-decision) with both stimuli (numbers and object names) were blocked (6 stimuli per block) and presented in pseudo-random. Each
condition (e.g. quantity) was rst presented with numerical stimuli, e.g. larger number? (or object names, e.g. larger object?) and followed by another block with object names (or
numerical stimuli) in a counterbalanced order. Presentation of blocks of the same task with both stimuli was followed by about 16-second rest period where subjects were asked to
maintain xation on a cross in the middle of the computer screen. Trials where the correct answer was the upper or the lower stimulus were presented in equal proportion. (For
interpretation of the references to colour in this gure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

quantity categorical tasks, subjects were instructed to ignore the and that they should choose the larger (or smaller) number in each
colour of the stimuli, and to focus on the question presented above the pair irrespective of the wording of the question (i.e. larger or
stimuli. In the perceptual task (colour-decision) they were asked to more). For summer/winter questions, participants were told that
choose the stimulus whose colour corresponded to the colour each number indicated either a summer or a winter month in the
indicated by the question above the stimuli. Subjects were instructed Northern hemisphere (all participants were British and raised in the
to select the stimulus according to the colour of the ink and not UK). They were told that summer months were June, July, and
according to the colour of the object (e.g. they should not select red August and winter moths were December, January and February
just because the object name was for example strawberries or and that these months followed a day (131) separated with a dot
tomatoes). (12.07) rather than the more familiar slash (12/07). They were
Participants were told that the number stimuli could indicate instructed to select either the summer or the winter month in each
either: i) quantities, ii) dates, or iii) times. The instructions for the pair of stimuli depending on the question. For the working/sleeping
number stimuli were as follows: For the larger/smaller and more/less questions, participants were told that working or sleeping times were
questions, participants were told that numbers referred to an amount in terms of a 24-hour clock; and that working times were between
M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529 525

8am and 6pm, and sleeping times were between 10pm and 7am. subject's head, Paus, 1999). The individual Brainsight localizations can
Participants were discouraged from considering jobs that include be seen in Supplementary Material.
night shifts. Stimulation locations were calculated for each subject individually
For object names, the instructions were the same as those for the using their structural scan and the coordinates obtained from the fMRI
numbers except that the processing required for more/less ques- analysis, similar to other TMS studies (e.g. Kalla et al., 2008; Sack et al.,
tions was not the same as that required for larger/smaller questions. 2009). Briey, this involved normalisation of the structural scan
Instead, during the more/less questions participants were instructed against a standard template (using the FSL software package, FMRIB,
to select the stimulus that was more (or less) numerous than the Oxford, UK) which produced both a normalised structural scan and a
other, for example socks vs thermos, stars vs moon, bed vs blanket, mathematical description of the applied transformation. This trans-
deck chair vs swimming pool, snowakes vs snowman or cherries formation was then reverse applied to the coordinates to be targeted,
vs melon. Prior to the experiment, participants underwent a practice giving their location in untransformed space. These were marked on
session in order to familiarize themselves with the task procedure. the scan in Brainsight, and the scalp locations overlying each served as
the targets for TMS. The precise localization of the target regions of a
TMS design representative participant as well as the results of the fMRI group
analysis is shown in Fig. 2.
TMS was delivered over two brain sites and sham stimulation was For each area stimulated there were nine blocks, three for each
also given over the same regions. TMS was applied using a Magstim experimental condition (TMS, sham and no-stimulation respectively).
Rapid Rate stimulator (Magstim Company Ltd., UK) and a focal 8- Each block consisted of 18 trials for each task (quantity, non-quantity
shaped coil with each wing measuring 70 mm in diameter. TMS was and colour decision) for each stimulus (numbers or object names);
applied at 60% of the maximum output of the stimulator machine at therefore each subject performed a total of 54 trials per task per
10 Hz frequency for 500 ms at the stimulus onset. A xed level of stimulus in each experimental condition for a total of 324 trials for
stimulation was used as it has been shown that there is little each experimental condition. The order of the experimental condi-
correlation of TMS stimulation thresholds between different brain tions and of the block pairs (i.e. each task with both stimuli) was
areas (Stewart et al., 2001). Sham stimulation was produced by presented in pseudo-random order across subjects to avoid learning
orienting the coil sideways, so that the sound and feeling of physical and practice effects.
contact with the head were similar to those produced by real
stimulation. For TMS, the stimulation coil rested tangentially on the Participants
subject's scalp and the handle pointing posteriorly parallel to the
subject's midsagittal plane as calculated by frameless stereotaxy (Paus Six neurologically normal and native-English participants gave
and Wolforth, 1998). Target regions were identied by a previous fMRI their informed consent to participate in the TMS study (3 males, mean
study run with the identical experimental design on different age 22.2, range 2123). The study had been approved by the Ethics
subjects4 (Cappelletti et al., in press) and consisted of two regions Committee of the Institute of Neurology in London and was performed
on the left and right IPS (MNI coordinates: 42, 40, 42 and 38, in adherence with TMS safety guidelines (Wasserman, 1998).
44, 40 respectively). These areas corresponded to the brain regions
commonly activated by numbers and object names and identied by Analysis of TMS data
the main effect of conceptual tasks (over quantity and non-quantity
for numbers and object names) relative to xation. To ensure that The proportion of errors and the mean reaction times (RTs) were
these effects were not driven by one condition only, we used the calculated for each subject in each condition. Response times below
inclusive masking option in SPM to identify the main effect of 200 ms (i.e. anticipatory responses) and above 2 standard deviations
conceptual tasks relative to xation in areas that were activated by of the overall mean of each individual (i.e. delayed responses) were
both (i) conceptual tasks on numbers and (ii) conceptual tasks on excluded from the data set following a procedure which is common
object names at p b 0.01. To control for any correlation between practice in data analysis and in previous TMS studies (e.g. Martin et al.,
conditions, a correction was made for non-sphericity using standard 2004; Naeser et al., 2005). Furthermore, using this approach, we
SPM5 procedures. decreased the likelihood that our results were driven by outliers as we
In order to locate the site of stimulation accurately, we used a used tests that assume normal data distribution. By using the above
frameless stereotactic system (Brainsight software, Rogue Research, 2SD criterion, 3.8% of the total number of responses were outliers and
Montreal, Canada). This system allows the precise localization of were therefore excluded from the analysis. Bartlett's test was used to
anatomical areas using the MRI images of the participants (e.g. check for homogeneity of variance and no data transformation was
Dorward et al., 1999), and has been successfully used in previous necessary.
fMRI-guided TMS studies (e.g. Devlin et al., 2003). Prior to the A t-test compared the sham and the non-stimulation conditions
experiment, a high-resolution T1-weighted MRI scan of the brain of across tasks to test whether there was any difference between them.
each TMS participant was obtained in order to locate the two target As no difference emerged (see analysis below), we compared the TMS
regions relative to external landmarks on the head. These landmarks condition to sham rather than no-stimulation as sham is the most
consisted of the bridge and tip of the nose and the tragus of the ears similar to TMS in terms of the sound produced and physical contact
that are visible on both the subject's MRI scan and on his/her head. with the head.
The 3D location of these landmarks was registered using an optical A 4 2 3 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with condition [left and
tracking system. This system uses an infrared camera that can detect right TMS-IPS and left and right sham-IPS], stimulus-type [numbers
reectors attached to the objects of interest (i.e. the coil and the and object names] and task [quantity, non-quantity categorical and
colour-decision] as within-subject factors was conducted on RTs of
correct answers. The analysis aimed at testing for stimulation effects
4
Functional image analysis was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping and at verifying whether these effects interacted with task and/or
software (SPM5 software, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London; http// stimulus. In addition, two-way ANOVAs were performed on RTs of
www.l.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Scans were realigned, unwarped and spatially normalized correct answers for each task with condition [left and right TMS-IPS
(Friston et al., 1995) to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space.
Functional images were then smoothed in the spatial domain with a Gaussian kernel of
and sham] and stimulus-type [numbers and object names] as factors.
6 mm FWHM to improve the signal to noise ratio. A high pass lter was used with a We also tested whether TMS differentially affected numbers that were
cutoff period of 128s. close vs far apart, i.e. distance effect. In the quantity tasks with
526 M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529

Fig. 2. Location of the TMS target areas based on fMRI coordinates. The TMS target areas in the (A) left and (B) right IPS on the sagittal, coronal and axial view of the brain of a
representative participant (anti-clock wise from top right). Target regions were identied by a previous fMRI study run with the identical experimental design on different subjects
(Cappelletti et al., under review) and consisted of two regions on the left and right IPS (bottom right of A and B). These areas corresponded to the brain regions commonly activated
by numbers and object names and identied by the main effect of conceptual tasks (over quantity and non-quantity categorical for numbers and object names) relative to xation.
Precise location of TMS target regions was obtained using a frameless stereotactic system (Brainsight software, Rogue Research, Montreal, Canada).

numbers, the distance between 2 numbers was calculated by names), [F(2,10) = 9.31, p b 0.006]. Specically, performance in quan-
subtracting one number from the other. In contrast, in the non- tity tasks with numbers (e.g. which is larger: 12.07 or 15.02?) was
quantity tasks with numbers, rather than the internal distance signicantly impaired (slower performance) by stimulation over the
between the 2 numbers, we calculated the distance between each IPS relative to sham stimulation of the same areas, with longer RTs
numerical stimulus in the pair and the next standard and we then following rTMS to either left [871.63 ms, 34.5 ms difference with
averaged these values for the numerical stimuli in each pair. Reported sham, t(5) = 9.56, p b 0.001] or right IPS [855.56 ms, 17.8 ms difference
results were corrected for non-sphericity using GreenhouseGeisser with sham, t(5) = 4.91, p = 0.004]. Left and right IPS stimulation
correction. differed signicantly, left IPS-TMS inducing a larger impairment than
When appropriate, we performed post-hoc paired comparisons right IPS-TMS [t(5) = 3.25, p b 0.02] (See Fig. 3). Moreover, left and
using student t-test and used Fisher LSD correction for multiple right IPS-TMS impairment was greater for comparisons of close
comparisons. Statistical signicance refers to a two-tailed p value b0.05. numbers relative to numbers far apart, i.e. distance effect [left IPS: t

Results

TMS did not affect the participants' accuracy in any of the tasks
[overall 4.6% of errors, no signicant difference between TMS and sham
across tasks and stimuli, p N 0.1]. This is consistent with similar
paradigms or tasks used in posterior parietal cortex TMS showing that
tasks performed at high levels of accuracy are likely to result in RT decit
rather than increase in error rate following TMS (Alexander et al., 2005;
Ashbridge et al., 1997; Cappelletti et al., 2007).
There was no signicant difference between sham and no-
stimulation across any of the tasks or stimuli used [t(5) = 0.45,
p = 0.67, n.s.].
The three-way ANOVA revealed a signicant main effect of task
[F(2,10) = 41.74, p b 0.001]. Moreover, the interaction of stimulus-
type and task [F(2,10) = 9.36, p b 0.005], and the three-way interac-
tion of condition, stimulus-type and task [F(6,30) = 2.36, p b 0.05]
were all signicant. Fig. 3. Performance impairments following TMS. Bars indicate the mean difference in
response time in ms in the left (black bars) and right IPS (grey bars) after TMS relative
Quantity task to sham stimulation to the same areas, for quantity, non-quantity categorical and colour
decisions with numbers and object names respectively. TMS conditions in which
response times signicantly differed from sham stimulation or between conditions are
A two-way ANOVA revealed a signicant interaction of condition indicated by an asterisk above the bars. Numbers below bars indicate the mean
(left and right TMS and sham) and stimulus-type (numbers and object differences in error rates between TMS and Sham in each experimental condition.
M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529 527

(5) = 2.593, p b 0.04 right IPS t(5) = 11.14, p b 0.001]. A distance effect affecting the most difcult conditions, i.e. close vs distant numbers,
was also observed in both the sham [t(5) = 2.76, p b 0.03] and the non- rather than interfering with numerical processing per se. We note,
TMS conditions [t(5) = 10.99, p b 0.001]. however, that if this were the case TMS-induced distance effects
Similarly, performance in quantity tasks with object names (e.g. should have been observed in all numerical tasks. In contrast, we
larger object: bikini or coat?) was signicantly delayed after showed that in the non-quantity categorical tasks with numbers
stimulation over the left [1083.2 ms, 27.03 ms difference with (summer/winter month; working/sleeping time), the distance effect
sham, t(5) = 8.38, p b 0.001] or right IPS [1069.86 ms, 13.3 ms was affected by TMS in one of these tasks (working/sleeping time) but
difference with sham, t(5) = 9.56, p = 0.001] compared to sham not the other (summer/winter month), although they were the same
stimulation over the same areas. Left and right IPS stimulation in terms of difculty at baseline. Some previous studies have already
differed signicantly, left IPS-TMS again inducing a larger impairment shown that IPS-TMS may induce different degrees of interference on
than the right IPS-TMS [t(5) = 3.49, p b 0.017]. the distance effect in numerical task of the same difculty level at
baseline (e.g. Cappelletti et al., 2007).
Non-quantity categorical task These ndings challenge past results by showing that the role of
IPS in quantity processing is stimulus-independent and its role in
A two-way ANOVA revealed a signicant interaction of condition and numerical cognition is not specic for the conceptual task performed
stimulus-type [F(2,10)= 7.28, p b 0.01]. In particular, performance in the with numbers. Two further critical conditions revealed the true
non-quantity categorical task with numerical stimuli (e.g. summer date: specicity of the IPS contribution to numerical processing: IPS-TMS
23.07 or 15.02?) was signicantly delayed following stimulation to the showed no impairment for perceptual decisions on numerical stimuli
left [1118.57 ms, 24.9 ms difference with sham, t(5) = 6.42, p b 0.001] or in the absence of any conceptual task (i.e. colour judgment), nor for
right IPS [1113.44 ms, 19.7 ms difference with sham, t(5) = 4.5, conceptual decisions that did not involve quantity or number stimuli
p = 0.006] compared to sham stimulation of the same areas. Unlike (e.g. choosing a summer object between bikini and coat). From this,
the quantity task, there was no signicant difference between left and we suggest that the simple presence of numbers is not sufcient for
right IPS stimulation [t(5) = 0.76, p = 0.5]. the IPS to be critically involved, but conceptual-level operations are
Left and right IPS-TMS impairment was greater for comparisons of also necessary. However, conceptual operations on their own are also
close numbers when they referred to times, e.g. working time: 23.07 or not sufcient to engage the IPS, but must be specically oriented to
15.02? [left IPS: t(5) = 16.7, p b 0.001; right IPS t(5) = 6.5, p b 0.001], numerical concepts.
but not when they referred to dates, e.g. summer month: 23.07 or
15.02? [left IPS: t(5) = 0.61, p = 0.56, n.s.; right IPS t(5) = 2.12, One or more quantity mechanisms?
p = 0.87, n.s.]. Similarly, a distance effect was also observed in both the
sham [t(5) = 5.7, p b 0.002] and the non-TMS conditions [t(5) = 11.94, The issue of whether the quantity expressed by different stimuli is
p b 0.001] for numbers referring to time but not to dates [t(5) = 0.8, processed in the same way or via distinct mechanisms is a long
p = 0.4 and t(5) = 0.38, p = 0.72 for sham and non-TMS respectively]. standing issue. The seminal work of Moyer and Landauer (1967)
Relative to sham, performance following IPS-TMS in the non- suggesting that the magnitude of all stimuli is processed in the same
quantity categorical task with object names (e.g. summer object: way has received support at the theoretical level (e.g. Campbell, 1994;
bikini or coat?) remained unchanged following stimulation to the Dehaene, 1997; McCloskey, 1992; Noel and Seron, 1993), and more
left [926.5 ms, 7.37 ms difference with sham, t(5) = 2.53, p = 0.06] recently has motivated research into the neuronal correlates of
and right IPS [929.4 ms, 4.4 ms difference with sham, t(5) = 8.3, magnitude processing. These have been identied mainly in the
p = 0.45]. There was no signicant difference between left and right parietal regions (Cappelletti et al., 2007, in press; Cohen Kadosh et al.,
IPS-TMS stimulation [t(5) = 0.07, p b 0.95]. 2005, 2007a,b; Dehaene et al., 2003; Pinel et al., 2001, 2004; Piazza et
al., 2004, 2007), and a recent theoretical proposal has suggested these
Perceptual (colour-decision) task areas as the locus of stimulus-independent magnitude processing
according to a common metric (Walsh, 2003). However, dissociations
A two-way ANOVA showed a non-signicant interaction of between quantity processing of Arabic numbers and of other
condition and stimulus-type [F(2,10) = 3.53, p b 0.07]. magnitude dimensions such as the size of physical stimuli have
been reported in neuropsychological patients (e.g. Cipolotti et al.,
Discussion 1991; Lemer et al., 2003) and not all functional imaging studies have
shown consistent results as to whether the bilateral IPS is involved in
This study investigated the role of the left and right IPS regions in stimulus-independent magnitude processing (e.g. Cohen Kadosh et al.
processing quantity with numerical and non-numerical stimuli, and in 2007a,b; Pinel et al., 2004) or whether the left or the right IPS is
conceptual tasks with numbers requiring or not quantity manipula- differentially involved (e.g. Castelli et al., 2006; Cohen Kadosh et al.,
tion. We showed that quantity judgments with numerical and non- 2007a,b; Pinel et al., 2004 for right IPS activation; Fias et al., 2003;
numerical stimuli (e.g. choosing the larger of 12.07 and 15.02 or the Cohen Kadosh et al., 2005 for left IPS activation). One factor that may
larger in size of bikini and coat) were equally affected following IPS- account for these discrepancies is the way in which magnitudes
TMS. Moreover, number conceptual processing either involving or not change luminance is continuous, for example, while number
involving quantity manipulation (e.g. choosing the larger of 12.07 changes are discrete (e.g. Cohen Kadosh et al., 2007a,b; Pinel et al.,
and 15.02, or attributing a summer date to either 12.07 or 15.02) 2004) irrespective of the underlying representation. Another factor, as
was also equally impaired following IPS-TMS. Our results also showed formalised by Stevens (1975), is the way in which magnitude changes
a distance effect in both quantity and non-quantity categorical are experienced weight differences are experienced as more than
judgments. This is consistent with previous results indicating that or less than another weight, while frequency and saturation changes
distance effect can be observed not just with any continuous quantity modify the categorical perception of pitch and colour respectively. A
dimension such as size of symbols (e.g. Cohen Kadosh et al., 2005; third reason for the discrepancy of previous results is the possible
Kaufmann et al., 2005; Pinel et al., 2004; Tang et al., 2006) and confound between numerical processing and response selection, both
luminance (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2005; Pinel et al., 2004), but also with engaging the parietal lobes (e.g. Cappelletti et al., in press; Goebel et
non-quantity dimensions such as letters of the alphabet (e.g. Fulbright al., 2005). However, it was beyond the purpose of this study to address
et al., 2003) or social status (Chiao et al., 2004). It may be suggested this issue. A fourth difference is that numerical magnitude judgments
that the distance effect induced by IPS-TMS is simply due to TMS (i.e. is 65 bigger than 55?) may necessitate the retrieval of learnt
528 M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529

information while physical size and luminance decisions may be all activations reported in fMRI studies are indicative of an area being
based on visual stimulus properties rather than memory-related necessary for normal performance of that task. There are two reasons
strategies. Our study avoided this confound by using magnitude for this: rst there is some 'redundancy' in perceptual and cognitive
dimensions with similar processing requirements: choosing the larger systems (e.g. Price and Friston, 2002); second, fMRI activations are the
of two Arabic numbers (e.g. 12.07 vs 15.02) or of two objects (e.g. result of a particular subtraction and thus indicate activity elicited by
bikini vs coat) relies in both cases on the retrieval of associated one kind of stimulus/task relative to another stimulus/task. Necessity
information which is not contained in the stimuli. By using this novel of an area to those stimuli/tasks can only be ascertained by
experimental manipulation we have provided new evidence suggest- interference. Another reason may lie in the type of tasks used or the
ing that the bilateral IPS is necessary for processing magnitude response selection demands of the task. For instance, tasks such as
expressed by different stimuli when these are based on similar stimulus detection (Eger et al., 2003) activate the parietal lobes
cognitive resources. because they require conceptual processing in the form of identity
recognition (i.e. when distinguishing a number from another
Quantity and non-quantity conceptual number processing in the IPS stimulus). Moreover, the perceptual tasks used in some of these
studies (e.g. Gbel et al., 2005; Tang et al., 2006; Thioux et al., 2005)
We have also shown that the IPS regions are equally involved in involved visual search processes that have been shown to activate the
quantity processing and in other conceptual tasks with numbers that superior parietal lobes (e.g. Coull et al., 2003; Pollmann et al., 2003)
do not require quantity manipulation, i.e. categorical tasks. The idea whereas a colour detection task placed minimal demands on visuo-
that number semantics can be extended to include non-quantity spatial mechanisms because it could be based on any unit of the string.
conceptual operations on numbers has been proposed by some Our ndings that the left and right IPS are critically involved in
previous theoretical accounts (Butterworth, 1999; Dehaene, 1997). conceptual but not perceptual processing of numbers is also in
However, this proposal has not yet been systematically investigated as keeping with some previous studies based on fMRI adaptation
most of the previous studies focused on quantity processing. By paradigms (fMRIA, e.g. Cohen Kadosh et al., 2007a,b; Cantlon et al.,
showing that the parietal regions are involved not only in processing 2006; Piazza et al., 2007). These studies showed that IPS adaptation
the quantitative features of numbers but also in other non- (i.e. in the absence of any explicit task) is modulated by numerical
quantitative conceptual manipulations our results support the idea magnitude but not by changes in the colour of numerical stimuli. Our
that number semantics in the IPS is not limited to the processing of results further expand this evidence by showing that the IPS plays a
magnitude but can be extended to include other numerical conceptual critical role in number processing and that this is specic for
processing (Butterworth, 1999; Dehaene, 1997). conceptual (quantity and non-quantity) but not perceptual number
Our ndings show that the involvement of the IPS regions in processing.
number processing is modulated by the nature of the number task In conclusion, we have shown that the role of the IPS in numerical
performed: responses were delayed only when subjects were cognition is dependent on a combination of stimuli and task
performing quantitative and non-quantitative conceptual judgments operations. Specically, the left and right IPS are critical for: (1)
but not perceptual judgments with numerical stimuli. These results are performing quantity and non-quantity conceptual operations with
consistent with proposals that the parietal areas are engaged in the numbers; and for (2) quantity processing irrespective of the stimuli
conceptual representation of numbers (Dehaene, 1997; Dehaene and used, i.e. numbers or object names. However, this study shows that
Cohen, 1995), and with a recent fMRI study using the same the IPS is not critically involved in perceptual decisions based on
experimental design showing the involvement of the IPS only in numbers or in conceptual tasks that do not involve quantity or
conceptual (quantity and non-quantity) but not in perceptual number numerical stimuli.
tasks (Cappelletti et al., in press). How do these results combine with
the most common view that number processing is so automatic that Acknowledgments
the simple presentation of numbers activates a sense of magnitude
(e.g. Ansari et al., 2006; Dehaene and Akhavein, 1995; Eger et al., 2003; This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal
Thioux et al., 2005)? One possibility is that a magnitude representation Society (MC), and by the MRC (VW and NM).
might have been automatically activated in the colour task but TMS
had no effect on colour processing, therefore no disruption by TMS was Appendix A. Supplementary data
seen in that task. Another possibility is that different types of
numerical representations may be triggered by different types of Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in
task requirements (Fischer and Rottmann, 2005; Shaki and Petrusic, the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.016.
2005). In our study these numerical representations may differ in the
conceptual (quantity and non-quantity) relative to perceptual tasks. References
Moreover, these representations may rely on different brain regions or
Alexander, I., Cowey, A., Walsh, V., 2005. The right parietal cortex and time perception:
may pose different task requirements on the same regions, therefore back to Critchley and the Zeitraffer phenomenon. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22,
resulting in different TMS effects. As such, our results suggest a 1030610315.
renement of the idea of task-independent automatic magnitude Andres, M., Seron, X., Olivier, E., 2005. Hemispheric lateralization of number
comparison. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 283290.
activation and are in keeping with proposals suggesting that Ansari, D., Dhital, B., Siong, S.C., 2006. Parametric effects of numerical distance on the
magnitude representations may interact with task requirements. intraparietal sulcus during passive viewing of rapid numerosity changes. Brain Res.
It still remains to be explained why some previous imaging studies 1067, 181188.
Ashbridge, E., Walsh, V., Cowey, A., 1997. Temporal aspects of visual search studies by
showed similar parietal activation in numerical tasks whether they transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia 35, 11211131.
required quantity processing or not (e.g. Gbel et al., 2005; Eger et al., Butterworth, B., 1999. The Mathematical Brain, McMillan.
2003; Shuman and Kanwisher, 2004; Tang et al., 2006; Thioux et al., Campbell, J.I.D., 1994. Architectures for numerical cognition. Cognition 53, 144.
Cantlon, J.F., Brannon, E.M., Carter, E.J., Pelphrey, K.A., 2006. Functional imaging of
2005). For instance, Thioux et al. (2005) found no difference in IPS
numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biol. 4, e125.
activation when contrasting number comparison to a task where Cappelletti, M., Barth, H., Fregni, F., Pascual Leone, A., Spelke, E., 2007. rTMS over the left
subjects have to decide if numbers were written in plain characters or and the right intraparietal sulcus disrupts discrete and continuous quantity
not. Similarly, Eger et al. (2003) reported IPS activation in a simple processing. Exp. Brain Res. 179, 631642.
Cappelletti, M., Jansari, A., Butterworth, B., Kopelman, M.D., 2008. A case of selective
number detection task (i.e. indicate when a stimulus is a number) not impairment of encyclopedic numerical knowledge or when December 25th is no
requiring quantity manipulation. The simplest possibility is that not longer Christmas day, but 20 + 5 is still 25. Cortex 44, 325336.
M. Cappelletti et al. / NeuroImage 46 (2009) 522529 529

Cappelletti, M., Lee, H.L., Freeman, E.D., Price, C.J., in press. The role of the right and left Moyer, R.S., Landauer, T.K., 1967. The time required for judgements of numerical
parietal lobes in the conceptual processing of numbers. inequality. Nature 215, 15191520.
Castelli, F., Glaser, D.E., Butterworth, B., 2006. Discrete and analogue quantity processing Molko, N., Cachia, A., Riviere, D., Mangin, J.F., Bruandet, M., Le Bihan, D., et al., 2003.
in the parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, 46934698. Functional and structural alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental
Cipolotti, L., Butterworth, B., Denes, G., 1991. A specic decit for numbers in a case of dyscalculia of genetic origin. Neuron 40 (4), 847858.
dense acalculia. Brain 114, 26192637. Naeser, M.A., Martin, P.I., Nicholas, M., Baker, E.H., Seekins, H., Helm-Estabrooks, N.,
Chiao, J.Y., Bordeaux, A.R., Ambady, N., 2004. Mental representations of social status. Cayer-Meade, C., Kobayashi, M., Theoret, H., Fregni, F., Tormos, J.M., Kurland, J.,
Cognition 93, B49B57. Doron, K.W., Pascual-Leone, A., 2005. Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia
Cohen Kadosh, R., Henik, A., Rubinsten, O., Mohr, H., Dori, H., van de Ven, V., et al., 2005. after TMS to part of right Broca's area: an open-protocol study. Brain Lang. 93,
Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by 95105.
fMRI. Neuropsychologia 43, 12381248. Nieder, A., 2005. Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical competence. Nat.
Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., Schuhmann, T., Kaas, A., Goebel, R., Henik, A., Sack, Neurosci. Rev. 6, 177190.
A.T., 2007a. Virtual dyscalculia induced by parietal-lobe TMS impairs automatic Noel, M.P., Seron, X., 1993. Arabic number reading decit: a single-case study or when
magnitude processing. Curr. Biol. 17, 15. 236 is read (2306) and judged superior to 1258. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 10, 317339.
Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., Kaas, A., Henik, A., Goebel, R., 2007b. Notation- Ono, M., Kubik, S., Abernathey, C.D., 1990. Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci. Thieme.
dependent and independent representations of numbers in the parietal lobes. Paus, T., 1999. Imaging the brain before, during, and after transcranial magnetic
Neuron 53, 307314. stimulation. Neuropsychologia 37, 219224.
Coull, J.T., Walsh, V., Frith, C.D., Nobre, A.C., 2003. Distinct neural substrates for visual Paus, T., Wolforth, M., 1998. Brainsight, Rogue Research, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
search amongst spatial versus temporal distracters. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 368379. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, 399402.
Dehaene, S., 1997. The Number Sense. Oxford University Press, Penguin press, New York, Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S., 2004. Tuning curves for
Cambridge (UK). approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44, 547555.
Dehaene, S., Akhavein, R., 1995. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S., 2007. A magnitude code common to
number processing. J. Exper. Psychol., Learn., Mem., Cogn. 21, 314326. numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 53,
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., 1991. Two mental calculation systems: a case study of severe 293305.
acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsychologia 29, 10451074. Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere, D., LeBihan, D., 2001. Modulation of parietal activation by
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., 1995. Towards an anatomical and functional model of number semantic distance in a number comparison task. NeuroImage 14, 10131026.
processing. Math. Cogn. 1, 83120. Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S., 2004. Distributed and overlapping cerebral
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Cohen, L., 2003. Three parietal circuits for number representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments.
processing. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 20, 487506. Neuron 41, 983993.
Devlin, J.T., Matthews, P.M., Rushworth, M.S.F., 2003. Semantic processing in the left Polk, T.A., Reed, C.L., Keenan, J.M., Hogarth, P., Anderson, C.A., 2001. A dissociation
inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and between symbolic number knowledge and analogue magnitude information. Brain
transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 7184. Cogn. 47, 545563.
Dorward, N.L., Alberti, O., Palmer, J.D., Kitchen, N.D., Thomas, D.G.T., 1999. Accuracy of Pollmann, S., Weidner, R., Humphreys, G.W., Olivers, C.N., Muller, K., Lohmann, G.,
true frameless stereotaxy: in vivo measurement and laboratory phantom studies. Wiggins, C.J., Watson, D.G., 2003. Separating distracter rejection and target
J. Neurosurg. 90, 160168. detection in posterior parietal cortexan event-related fMRI study of visual
Eger, E., Sterzer, P., Russ, M.O., Giraud, A.L., Kleinschmidt, A., 2003. A supramodal marking. NeuroImage 18, 310323.
number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 37, 719725. Price, G.R., Friston, K.J., 2002. Degeneracy and Cognitive anatomy. Trends in Cognition
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., Orban, G.A., 2003. Parietal Science 6, 416421.
representation of symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, Price, G.R., Holloway, I., Rsnen, P., Vesterinen, M., Ansari, D., 2008. Impaired parietal
4756. magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia. Curr. Biol. 17 (24),
Fischer, M.H., Rottmann, J., 2005. Do negative numbers have a place on the mental R1042R1043.
number line? Psychol. Sci. 47, 2232. Rotzer, S., Kucian, K., Martin, E., Aster, M.v., Klaver, P., Loenneker, T., 2008. Optimized
Friston, K.J., 1995. Neuronal transients. Proc. Biol. Sci. 22, 401405. voxel-based morphometry in children with developmental dyscalculia. Neuro-
Fulbright, R.K., Manson, S.C., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C.M., Gore, J.C., 2003. Quantity Image 39, 417422.
determination and the distance effect with letters, numbers, and shapes: a Sack, A.T., Cohen Kadosh, R., Schuhmann, T., Moerel, M., Walsh, V., Goebel, R., 2009.
functional MR imaging study of number processing. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 24, 193200. Optimizing functional accuracy of TMS in cognitive studies: a comparison of
Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., Fias, W., 2003. The mental representation of ordinal sequences methods. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 207221.
is spatially organized. Cognition 87, B87B95. Shaki, S., Petrusic, W.M., 2005. On the mental representation of negative numbers:
Gbel, S.M., Johansen-Berg, H., Behrens, T.E., Rushworth, M.F.S., 2005. Response- context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments. Psychon. Bull. Rev.
selection related parietal activation during number comparison. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 931937.
16, 15361551. Shuman, M., Kanwisher, N., 2004. Numerical magnitude in the human parietal lobe:
Jacob, S.N., Nieder, A., 2007. The ABC of cardinal and ordinal number representations. tests of representational generality and domain specicity. Neuron 44, 557569 28.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 4143. Stevens, S., 1975. Psychophysics: Introduction to its Perceptual, Neural, and Social
Johnson, D.M., 1939. Condence and speed in the two-category judgment. Arch. Prospects. John Wiley & Sons, New York.
Psychol. 241, 150. Stewart, L.M., Walsh, V., Rothwell, J.C., 2001. Motor and phosphene thresholds: a
Kalla, R., Muggleton, N.G., Juan, C.H., Cowey, A., Walsh, V., 2008. The timing of the transcranial magnetic stimulation correlation study. Neuropsychologia 39,
involvement of the frontal eye elds and posterior parietal cortex in visual search. 415419.
NeuroReport 2 (19), 10671071. Tang, J., Critchley, H.D., Glaser, D.E., Dolan, R.J., Butterworth, B., 2006. Imaging
Kaufmann, L., Koppelstaetter, F., Delazer, M., Siedentopf, C., Rhomberg, P., Golaszewski, informational conict: an fMRI study of numerical stroop. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18,
S., Felber, S., Ischebeck, A., 2004. Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects 20492062.
in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 25 (3), Thioux, M., Pesenti, M., Costes, N., De Volder, A., Seron, X., 2005. Task independent
888898. semantic activation for numbers and animals. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 284290.
Lemer, C., Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Cohen, L., 2003. Approximate quantities and exact Turconi, E., Jemel, B., Rossior, B., Seron, X., 2004. Electrophysiological evidence for
number words: dissociable systems. Neuropsychologia 41, 19421958. differential processing of numerical quantity and order in humans. Cogn. Brain Res.
Lochy, A., Van Turennout, M., 2005. Semantics of numbers: a cortical shift? Poster 21, 2238.
presented at OHBM, Toronto. Turconi, E., Campbel, J.I.D., Seron, X., 2006. Numerical order and quantity processing in
Martin, P., Naeser, M.A., Theoret, H., Tormos, J.M., Nicholas, M., Kurland, J., Fregni, F., number comparison. Cognition 98 (3), 273285.
Seekins, H., Doron, K., Pascual-Leone, A., 2004. Transcranial magnetic stimulation as Walsh, V., 2003. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and
a complementary treatment for aphasia. Semin. Speech Language 25, 181191. quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 483488.
McCloskey, M., 1992. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: evidence from Wasserman, E.M., 1998. Electroencelophagr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 116.
acquired dyscalculia. Cognition 44, 107157. Wiese, H., 2003. Numbers, Language and the Human Mind. Cambridge University Press.
Moyer, R.S., 1973. Comparing objects in memory: evidence suggesting an internal Zamarian, L., Egger, C., Delazer, M., 2007. The mental representation of ordered
psychophysics. Percept. Psychophys. 7, 82138. sequences in visual neglect. Cortex 43, 542550.

You might also like