You are on page 1of 13

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/281672363

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking with


Psychosocial Applications

Article in Behavioral Science September 2015


DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

CITATIONS READS

5 563

3 authors, including:

Derek Cabrera Laura Cabrera


Cornell University Universidad Mariana
12 PUBLICATIONS 263 CITATIONS 10 PUBLICATIONS 32 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Derek Cabrera
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 27 October 2016
Systems Research and Behavioral Science
Syst. Res. (2015)
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Research Paper

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking


with Psychosocial Applications
Derek Cabrera1,2*, Laura Cabrera1,2 and Erin Powers2
1
College of Human Ecology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
2
Cabrera Research Lab, Ithaca, NY, USA

Systems thinking is a eld characterized by a bafing array of methods and approaches.


We posit that underlying all, however, are four universal rules called DSRP (distinctions,
systems, relationships, and perspectives; each containing two co-implying elements). We
make distinctions between and among things and ideas, each implying the existence of
an other. We identify systems, which are composed of parts and wholes. We recognize
relationships composed of actions and reactions. We take perspectives consisting of a
point (from which we see) and a view (that which is seen). We argue that DSRP offers a
unifying and organizing principle for the eld of systems thinking and an indispensable
analytical tool for solving complex problems. At the same time, the metacognitive practice
of applying these rules has signicant psychosocial implications, such as increasing
self-awareness, empathy, and a sense of belonging, while decreasing stereotyping and
self-harming and other-harming orientations. As such, DSRP has the potential to create
advanced analytical thinkers with prosocial orientations. Copyright 2015 John Wiley
& Sons, Ltd.
Keywords critical systems thinking; ethics; methodological pluralism; universality; complex
adaptive system

The eld of systems thinking contains an im- What is universal to systems thinking? Is there
mense diversity of methods, approaches, and a common metric by which we can measure the
specialties (Schwarz, 1996; Midgley, 2003; disparate contributions in this area? How can
Franois, 2004). Any thorough exploration of this we speak across increasingly reied boundaries
pluralism can be as discouraging as it is impres- of theory and practice to cumulate knowledge
sive for the newcomer to the eld. This extreme in and advance our eld? How can we evolve
internal differentiation poses challenges not only existing methods and practices to be more robust,
for the newcomer but for the eld as a whole. grounded, and systemic?
The answers to these questions lie in meeting
complexity with simplicity: we posit that there
* Correspondence to: Derek Cabrera, Cornell University, College of
Human Ecology, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA.
are four universal patterns or rules that undergird
E-mail: dac66@cornell.edu systems thinking of all kinds (Cabrera, 2006).

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


RESEARCH PAPER Syst. Res.

These rules, which go by the acronym DSRP, do pattern of consistency across a broad swath of
more than describe or categorize the practices disciplines.1
and frameworks within the eld. We argue that
they provide not only a unifying theory but also
an organizing principle for systems thinking as
a whole. DISTINCTIONS, SYSTEMS, RELATIONSHIPS,
There are many advocates of methodological AND PERSPECTIVES (DSRP)
pluralism within the eld [see, for example,
Jackson and Keys (1984) and Flood and Jackson We hold that DSRP is the essence of systems
(1991a,1991b) for some early, inuential works], thinking: four cognitive patterns that are univer-
and this pluralism is quite popular [widely cited sal to our various subelds and methods but
authors include Mingers and Gill (1997), Jackson more generally to human thought. DSRP in-
(2000), and Midgley (2000)]. Indeed, pluralism volves distinctions, systems, relationships, and
(the process of diversication, specialization, perspectives. Distinctions can be made between
and differentiation) is a natural outgrowth of and among things and ideas; things and ideas
scientic innovation over time. Furthermore, can be organized into systems, in which both the
some have offered classications of systems parts and the wholes can be identied; relation-
thinking methodologies (e.g., Jackson and Keys, ships can be made between and among things
1984; Jackson, 1987; Flood and Jackson, 1991a; and ideas; and lastly, things and ideas can be
Gregory and Jackson, 1992a,1992b), while others viewed from the perspectives of other people,
have opposed this kind of rationalization of the things, and ideas.
eld (e.g., Mingers, 1992, 1993; Tsoukas, 1992; It is useful to consider each rule in greater
Gregory, 1996a,1996b; Midgley, 2000; Boyd depth. Take distinctions (see Young, 2005; Peter-
et al., 2007; Zhu, 2011). Whichever approach is son and Sko-Grant, 2003). Sometimes we dene
taken, however, the fact that the diversity of a thing or idea with conscious reference to what
methodologies goes hand in hand with in- it is not. For example, systems thinking is not re-
creasingly diverse interpretations of the term ductionism, not bivalent logic, and the like
systems thinking challenges us as a transdisci- (Fuenmayor, 1991). However, when identifying
plinary practice if we lack underlying and a thing, the other is not always consciously con-
shared principles (Cabrera 2006, p. 7). What we sidered. The practice of identifying the other
are proposing with DSRP, then, is a vision of increases our awareness, enlarging our thinking
the eld of systems thinking that embraces the (Fuenmayor, 1991; Midgley and Ochoa-Arias,
plurality of methods but, critically, espouses a 2001). While making distinctions is innate to our
unifying underlying structure to all those thinking processes, the distinctions we make are
methods (Cabrera, 2014). DSRP enables univer- often not without practical and moral implica-
sality and pluralism to coexist. tions (e.g., an us presupposes a them). We will
There is extant work on distinctions, sys- discuss this further later; for now, let us remember
tems, relationships, and perspectives in systems that making distinctions is an exercise in dening
thinkingsome of which we cite later when con- boundaries, demarcating what something is from
sidering each rule in turnalthough none have what it is not (Ulrich, 1983). And we all know de-
offered an all-encompassing, complete theory ning the problem and the question, including
that explicates the structure, dynamics, and func- what is not included in it, is a critical part of
tion of systems thinking. Beyond this, however,
there is an impressive amount of scholarship in 1
We have collected thousands of cross-disciplinary examples from the
other disciplines showing the theoretical and last few decades. For instance, on distinctions, see Davies (1982), Clark
(1994), Durand and Calori (2006), and Ferry et al. (2015). On systems,
practical universality of these rules. In isolation, see Wertheimer (1923), Ackoff (1971), Tversky and Kahneman (1981),
these cross-disciplinary examples show very and Anderson (1991). On relationships, see Piaget (1974), Cook and
Campbell (1979), Schulz and Gopnik (2004), and Greene (2010). Lastly,
little other than consistency with the theory, but on perspectives, see Marvin et al. (1976), Neale and Bazerman (1983),
in the aggregate, they provide a convincing Schober (1993), and Batson et al. (1997).

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Derek Cabrera et al.


Syst. Res. RESEARCH PAPER

addressing any practical issue or research ques- through taking micro-level and macro-level per-
tion (Churchman, 1970; Rochefort and Cobb, spectives can we understand complex phenomena.
1994). No matter what the object of our analysis is, we
There is considerable scholarship within the can protably consider its constituent parts and
systems thinking tradition on this (e.g., Goguen the larger whole of which it is part (and in a
and Varella, 1979; Ulrich, 1983; Glanville, 1990; fractal manner, the parts of parts and the progres-
Midgley et al., 1998; Midgley, 2000; Franois, sively larger wholes that constitute every system).
2004). But distinguishing boundaries is more Finally, remember that a thing or idea is almost in-
often than not construed as dening the bound- variably simultaneously a part and a whole: your
aries of the system under consideration or morning lecture is a whole composed of your
identifying what stakeholders think ought to be scholarly observations, perhaps some Socratic
done (e.g., Ulrich, 1983). The distinction rule questioning, possibly a quiz. Yet, it is one part of
explicates that every thing or idea represents a a class on systems thinking. Obviously, the more
boundary decision and is therefore fractal in its complex the phenomenon under consideration,
useoccurring at every level of scale. Therefore, the more enriching it is to identify the parts and
the distinctions rule applies to the systems, rela- the whole that constitute a system (that is likely
tionships, and perspectives rules as well. Demar- composed of multiple systems itself).
cating boundaries, distinguishing ideas and Making partwhole identications, when com-
things from the other, occurs when we recognize bined with seeing perspectives, has the potential
and identify systems, relationships, and perspec- to undermine the undue inuence that categoriza-
tives both internal and external to the system tion (and derivative structures such as taxonomies,
under consideration. classications, and hierarchies) has generally in
Now let us consider the partwhole structure of cognitive science and specically in many sub-
all systems. The systems rule incorporates the elds of disciplines such as biology, psychology,
best practices of the proverbial two types of scien- physiology, and education. For example, respec-
tists: the splitters and the lumpers.2 At the same tively, the species concept, Maslows hierarchy of
time, it offers a way out of the tired practice of needs, the food pyramid, and Blooms taxonomy
pitting holism against reductionism (also see are all inuential categorical concepts that have
Bunge, 1977). In reality, a part without a whole proven to be invalid yet have inuenced many in
is an absurdism, but a whole that has been the public sphere and have led us astray scienti-
stripped of its internal differentiation is equally cally (Cabrera and Cabrera, 2015).
impossible. So the systems rule makes the co- Moving on to relationships, we can see that
implicative relationship between part and whole these can take innumerable forms, such as feed-
explicit; one cannot consider a part without consid- back loops, correlations, and causalities. Thinking
ering its whole, and yet, one cannot consider the in terms of relationships is foundational to both
whole that encompasses every system ad innitum. cybernetics (e.g., Wiener, 1948; Bateson, 1970)
Thus, distinctions are inevitably drawn (Ulrich, and system dynamics (e.g., Forrester, 1971; Maani
1983). Recognizing systems involves breaking and Cavana, 2000). Considering relationships is a
things down into their constituent parts and also natural extension of examining systems, as the
grouping parts into larger wholes (e.g., Angyal, parts of a whole can be connected in myriad
1941; Bertalanffy, 1956, 1968; Hall and Fagen, ways, be they conceptual or more tangible
1956; Marchal, 1975; Kosko, 1993; Latimer and (including perhaps physical) or somewhere
Stevens, 1997, 1998, 1999; Hoffman, 1998; between the two. We are all accustomed to
Mortensen, 1998; Rakover, 1998; Opie, 1999). Only drawing lines to denote connections (relationships)
between various things. Of course, we can do this
with varying degrees of specicity. We can simply
2
The differentiation lumpers and splitters is rst attributed to Dar- note A and B as connected: we can say that A
win. The termseparating those who make many distinctions and
see greater diversity from those who make fewer distinctions and see
causes B, or we can specify that A and B are
more similarityrst appeared in print in Simpson (1945). negatively correlated. Beyond this, we might

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking


RESEARCH PAPER Syst. Res.

describe the relationship between A and B as thinking does not imply only the expansion of
characterized by an entire system and identify its boundaries through perspective taking]. Perspec-
parts, thereby utilizing the distinctions and sys- tives pervade all forms of thinking, even if they
tems rule to further explicate the relationships rule. are sometimes less obvious than distinctions, sys-
There are also important problems caused by tems, and relationships. Thus the age-old adage:
our bias toward identifying structural parts and when we change the way we look at things, the
ignoring dynamical, interacting ones (Forrester, things we look at change. Indeed, Churchman
1971). By all accounts, it is the relationships be- (1968) offers the famous aphorism that the systems
tween and among things that lead to complexity. approach begins when rst you see the world
Yet, when we deconstruct systems, we often do through the eyes of another (p. 231). Perspective
so by isolating structural parts but not dynamical taking is central to a number of soft systems meth-
ones. In neglecting these dynamical parts, we odologies that have been built on Churchmans in-
search for explanations without them and end sight (e.g., Mason and Mitroff, 1981; Ackoff et al.,
up with misguided concepts of emergence that 2006; Checkland and Poulter, 2006; Christakis and
border on the magical.3 Emergence does not Bausch, 2006).
mean that the sum is greater than the parts unless Perspectives are often embedded in some of the
we have ruled out relationships as being part of the most wicked problems we face as a society (Rittel
whole. If we were to account for all the structural and Webber, 1973), creating the need for systems
and dynamical parts, the whole would equal pre- thinking based on simple rules. A perspective can
cisely the sum of the parts in interaction. be seen as a lens through which we view the
The DSRP algorithm holds that relationships world, its objects, and ideas. True awareness of
have their own identities and are interdependent. our perspectives is akin to unmasking our mental
This is in contrast to the vast majority of modern models of reality, which ideally enables us to better
network theory, where the relationship is identi- approximate that reality (and this view holds that
ed only by the nodes that it relates. Instead, the product of systems thinking, our mental
DSRP compels us to identify these relationships models, is always tightly coupled with, and insep-
(for example, the relationship between Carlos arable from, our actions/behaviors). Our wicked
and Jack may be Alissa) and indicate when rela- problems result from the mismatch between
tionships act as whole systems inclusive of sub- reality and our perceptions of reality (Bateson,
parts and perspective. 1972, 1979), so perspectives are truly critical to sys-
Finally, we have more to say about perspectives, tems thinking and problem-solving of all kinds.
which consist of a point (the vantage point or the
looker) and a view (that which is seen or looked
at). Once perspective taking is introduced into the
equation, it is possible to say that parts and wholes D, S, R, and P Contain Two Co-Implied
may have different meanings from different points Elements
of view (e.g., Churchman, 1968; Checkland, 1981;
Ulrich, 1983; Checkland and Scholes, 1990; An important feature of DSRP is that each rule
Checkland and Poulter, 2006) and therefore that co-implies two elements. For example, with re-
human cognition is germane to the analysis of sys- spect to distinctions, the existence of a thing or
tems. Perspectives can be used to expand our idea4 automatically implies the existence of an
thinking and include more options or to restrict other and vice versa. Table 1 illustrates the co-
our thinking and cause greater focus when neces- implication involved in the systems, relation-
sary [here we support Ulrich (1983) and Midgley ships, and perspectives rules as well. As you
and Ochoa-Arias (2004) in their view that systems can see, part implies the existence of a whole, an

3 4
Cabrera (2006) isolated three different uses of the term emergence In previous work (Cabrera and Colosi, 2008), we labeled the refer-
and posited that two were invalid and magical-leaning and the third ence thing or idea as the identity. Whichever word is used, the salient
was semantic around the mathematical denition of the term sum. feature is the co-implication of thing/idea (or identity) and other.

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Derek Cabrera et al.


Syst. Res. RESEARCH PAPER

Table 1 Four DSRP rules, each with two co-implied operation of these rules serves to demystify the
elements idea that systems thinking is an emergent pro-
Simple rule Element 1 Element 2 perty. It is well understood that complexity
emerges from the collective action of agents
Distinction (D) Thing/idea Other following simple rules (Couzin et al., 2002;
System (S) Part Whole
Relationship (R) Action Reaction Gell-Mann, 1996). DSRP explicates the underlying
Perspective (P) Point View formalism for these simple rules and interactions,
as seen in this equation:

action implies a reaction, and a point implies a ST n : Dio Sw Rar Pv : j
_n
agentsj<
view. We argue that the four DSRP rules and their
co-implying elements are the essentials of sys- The equation explains that autonomous agents
tems thinking. While the underlying rules are (information, ideas, or things) following simple
simple, their combination and repetition can pro- rules (D, S, R, and P) with their elemental pairs
duce outcomes of near innite complexity. (i-o, p-w, a-r, and -v) in nonlinear order (:) and
with various co-implications of the rules (),
the collective dynamics of which over a time se-
THE SIMPLICITY UNDERLYING ries j to n lead to the emergence of what we might
COMPLEXITY refer to as systems thinking (ST).
What emerges from the use of DSRP is an
While we enumerate D, S, R, and P separately, the adaptive, evolving mindsetsomething that
reality is that they co-occur. For example, while closely resembles complex and adaptive systems
distinguishing a thing from an other, we can also thinking. If accepted, these rules would serve to
note that it exists as part of a system and in rela- make the eld more unied and broadly accessi-
tionship with other parts, which can all be seen ble and as such would enable better carpentry or
from a particular perspective. Let us take an exam- parenting as much as more robust network and
ple. Perhaps we are considering an assistant system dynamics modeling. In keeping with this
professor and making that distinction, which goal, our research lab has developed systems
implies the existence of other(s), or not-assistant thinking and modeling software that enables
professors. We can simultaneously see the assistant everyone to visually map their ideas in a way
professor as part of a system, say an academic de- that makes them better systems thinkers and
partment. Within this department, we can identify learners. We have taught systems thinking to
relationships between the assistant professor and groups of students from PreK to graduate school
other parts, such as graduate and undergraduate and to professionals in and outside academia
students, senior colleagues, department chair, (Cabrera and Cabrera, 2012b). This is because
post-docs, and ofce and research staff. Note that DSRP is content agnostic, so it can be applied to
we can specify these relationships (e.g., mentor/ any topical domain or existing methodology.
mentee or boss/subordinate). Finally, we can take The simplicity of the four rules and their broad
a perspective on the assistant professor, for exam- application makes DSRP inherently democratic.
ple, viewing her or him from the point of view of We are constantly increasing our evidence base
students or staff or even more abstract perspec- with case studies and statistics showing that sys-
tives such as salary or publication record. tems thinking serves to boost learning across a
Although we are fond of calling DSRP systems host of disciplines and activities.
thinking made simple (Cabrera and Cabrera, We think systems thinking made simple is ex-
2015), we do not mean to suggest that we are tremely promising in a world that is increasingly
simplifying the eld of systems thinking. Rather, complex, changing, and in which we are ever
we consider systems thinking as a complex, adap- more rapidly inundated with information. For
tive, and emergent phenomenon that has four un- example, our lab is working with the University
derlying, universal, and simple rules. Indeed, the of WisconsinExtension on a program called

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking


RESEARCH PAPER Syst. Res.

ThinkWater, a federally funded5 project to in- create more ethical, compassionate, self-reective
crease the application of systems thinking to and prosocial individuals is not by directly incul-
water education, extension, and research. Evalua- cating morals but by instruction in DSRP com-
tion results show that youth participating in bined with opportunities for and examples of its
ThinkWater education efforts master course con- application.
tent more completely when rst taught a DSRP What is the relevance of DSRP in domains tradi-
lesson; students also develop characteristics of sys- tionally associated with psychology and sociol-
tems thinkers. What might surprise some people, ogy? How exactly does DSRP encourage balance
however, are some of the ethical and humanistic between the needs of oneself and the other as well
implications of our work on DSRP (see chapter as between our emotional and cognitive natures?
10 in Cabrera and Cabrera, 2015). For example, How does it increase empathy and understanding
case studies (Cabrera and Cabrera, 2012a) in rural and general interpersonal awareness? How does it
Pennsylvania involving adjudicated youth in three promote introspection and prosocial behavior?
residential centers showed high levels of transfer The answer to each of these questions is through
from what students were learning in the core cur- metacognition, or awareness of ones thinking,
riculum utilizing DSRP to their own therapeutic which includes emotion-laden thoughts.
interventions. One student remarked, I now see DSRP increases awareness that our views are
the relationship between triggers, my self destruc- mental models of reality, at best good approxima-
tive behaviors, my victims, and my future tions, but frequently poor ones. It promotes
(Cabrera and Cabrera 2012a). We explore the ethi- awareness of
cal implications of DSRP in the succeeding text.
the distinctions we makewho or what is in-
cluded and who or what is not;
the perspectives we take and how they shape
PROSOCIAL IMPLICATIONS OF DSRP our understanding of events and internal
processes;
We believe that simple DSRP rules create not only the fact that there are multiple ways to com-
highly intelligent systems thinkers but also emo- bine and recombine parts (e.g., people) into
tionally intelligent, prosocial people with an ethi- wholes (e.g., groups); and
cal compass. Let us explain. Our minds are not our interrelatedness, which is conducive to
only responsible for our cognitions, they also pro- feelings of belonging and also increases cogni-
cess feelings, thoughts, emotions, and motives. zance of our effects on other people.
See Maturana and Varela (1987) on the link Each rule corresponds to an ethical stance. We
between emotions and cognition. Just as DSRP will delve into each simple rule briey to better
increases awareness of our own thinking, its illustrate the signicance of DSRP for developing
application produces awareness of, and reection metacognitive, sophisticated learners, and
upon, our internal landscape, our feelings, and prosocial human beings of the type needed to
our relationships with ourselves and others (see solve societal problems. The application of DSRP
Gregory, 2000, for a discussion of the importance to our internal lives aptly demonstrates the
of critical self-reection, among other things, to interrelation and simultaneity of the four rules
systems practice). Consequently, systems thinking (i.e., one will see how making distinctions and
develops our analytical and social/emotional having awareness of relationships, systems, and
selves in tandem. perspectives frequently co-occur).
A systems thinker is an emergent property pro-
duced by following the simple rules of DSRP.
Therefore, we suggest that the best way we can Distinctions
5
Based upon work supported by the National Institute of Food and
Agriculture, U.S. Department of Agriculture, under Agreement No.
Every time we make a distinction by discussing
2011-51130-31148. a thing or idea, we are creating an other, even

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Derek Cabrera et al.


Syst. Res. RESEARCH PAPER

if unconsciously. It is lack of attention to this Systems


other that plagues some forms of systems think-
ing, be it unidentied interactions, concepts, Applying DSRP shows us that categorizations
complexities, systems, perspectives, or frame- are really partwhole groupings from a perspec-
works. Being aware of distinctions that we and tive, rather than real-world distinctions. Practic-
others make is an important ethical skill (Ulrich, ing an ethic of identifying perspectives applied
1983), in that these distinctions can often be to partwhole systems could eventually lead to
oppressive to the individuals classied as some- the decline of moral systems, schema, and scien-
thing (or those not thus classied). Awareness of tic facts constructed around discrete taxonomies,
us/them distinctions, for example, is a pivotal categories, and hierarchies.
step in understanding the marginalization of Identifying different parts is an important eth-
the other. Midgley and Pinzn (2011) point out ical skill, in particular to combat the prevalence
that making the other visible is not enough to of generalizations and stereotypes about the
promote ethical action; making the other visible other. For example, being able to identify an in-
can either increase or decrease marginalization creasing number of parts, each with their own
depending on whether positive or negative attri- particular perspective, can deter us from assum-
butes are ascribed to the other. While we agree, ing there is a monolithic Muslim perspective on
it is important to further elaborate our under- terrorism or Republican view on gay marriage.
standing of distinction making and ethical An appreciation of nuance and diversity would
action, and DSRP as an algorithm helps to do go far in healing some of our fractious political
this. DSRP identies the other as a rst step in dialogue. Similarly, breaking our complex emo-
an ethical mental process. Resultant of this iden- tional reactions into discrete feelings motivated
tication of the other is the acknowledgement by discrete mental models can do much to in-
that it is a distinct entity, inclusive of unique crease emotional maturity and self-regulation
relationships, existence in unique partwhole (Kashdan et al., 2014).
conguration, and, critically, possessing a unique Finally, lumping things into wholes is also so-
perspective. It is ethical to acknowledge the cially adaptive, in that it enables us to focus on
totality of the other in this way. our similarities, what binds us together. It is quin-
Distinctions also have importance intraper- tessentially human to need and want to be part of
sonally. Distinguishing thoughts from feelings is something larger than oneself (Durkheim, 1951).
a critical step in developing emotional intelli- Feeling ones parthood or belonging to a larger
gence, as is distinguishing one feeling from an- whole is conducive to cooperation and all sorts
other. The self-awareness generated by applying of prosocial behavior and is correlated with im-
the distinction rule is a prerequisite to any sort proved physical and emotional health outcomes
of emotional control. Furthermore, distinguishing (Hagerty et al., 1996).
others thoughts and feelings from our own re-
duces the likelihood of projection [a defense
mechanism of attributing ones unacceptable Relationships
thoughts or feelings to others (Quinodoz, 2005)]
and the further defensive thoughts and actions Greater understanding of the multitude of rela-
that frequently result. Applying DSRP to ones tionship types promotes a deeper comprehension
emotions enables more adaptive responses, learn- of causality, which as systems thinkers know, of-
ing processes, and reductions in self-destructive ten operates in nonlinear ways (Newton, 1687;
behavior and associated anxiety and depression Piaget, 1974; Strogatz, 1994; Bar-Yam, 1997;
(see Kashdan et al., 2014, on making distinctions Bransford et al., 1999; Cabrera, 2006, p. 7072).
in the context of self-reection). The salutary ef- The relationships rule solves a number of pitfalls
fects of making distinctions are also related to in traditional Western thinking, including the
those of recognizing systems composed of wholes tendencies to think solely in terms of direct cau-
and parts. sality, to fail to appreciate the importance of

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking


RESEARCH PAPER Syst. Res.

context, and to overlook relationships as parts of awareness that our views are merely one of many
a system. This nuanced (i.e., nonlinear and dy- possible perspectives can inhibit prejudice and
namical) understanding of causality helps us socially harmful forms of dogmatism. Communi-
understand and analyze wicked problems in all cation with others who are unlike ourselves is
realms. Taking a complex or nonlinear view of re- greatly enhanced by the ability to take perspec-
lationships, for example, reduces the assignment tives, as long as we do not fall into the trap of
of blame or credit to individuals or groups, believing that we are able to understand others
promoting compassion in general. Indeed, when completely in their own terms, which can lead
disasters are analyzed using a systems approach, to a new dogmatism (Gregory, 1992). We can then
the blaming of individuals is often replaced with follow those perspectives through in making our
understanding how those individuals have distinctions and recognizing systems and
interacted as parts of wider pathological systems relationships.
(Fortune and Peters, 1995). Philosophically, this Furthermore, perspective taking can lead us to
has implications for the idea of unadulterated examine our own cherished beliefs, see where
free will, which undergirds much of our penal our perspectives were uncritically adopted from
system and also several cultural tropes, such as social inuences in our environment, and in-
rugged individualism and the American Dream crease our mental and emotional exibility, in-
rooted in equality of opportunity. We are not cluding self-acceptance (Churchman, 1979). We
arguing against individual agency (or will) but can evaluate our moral codes and personal
instead for a thorough-going account of the mul- values and beliefs more critically, which makes
tiple webs of causality (what might be termed us less likely to blindly impose them on others
structural constraints) in which individuals (Romm, 2001). In a similar vein, we can reevalu-
operate. ate the golden rule, which is non-perspectival.
Looking at the elements of relationships Instead of Do unto others as you would have
action and reactionenables individuals to bet- them do unto you, our maxim would be to treat
ter appreciate the consequences of their actions. others in a fashion analogous to how we want to
Combined with the systems rule, understanding be treated, given their perspectives on behavior.
relationships ideally makes us less likely to harm Making the golden rule analogous and therefore
ourselves or others based on understanding that perspectival may at rst appear as trite, but the
we are part of a larger whole. It can also reduce implications for such a long-standing moral com-
egoism (Shen and Midgley, 2007) and promote a pass are signicant.
broader denition of ones interests as incorpo-
rating the well-being of the community and its
constituent members. We come to appreciate CONCLUSION
our own embeddedness as we recognize the
complex forms of interdependence that mark We have posited that four simple rules underlie
our relationships and social structures. systems thinking of all kinds. We offer these
rulesDSRPas a unifying theory that inheres
in all of the practices, approaches, and methods
Perspectives that make up the diverse and complex eld of
systems thinking. DSRP is a form of metacogni-
Taking perspectives is integral to our thinking, tion in which we consciously make distinctions
and DSRP increases our awareness (metacogni- (thing or idea/other) and recognize systems
tion) of the perspectives we take and those we (part/whole), relationships (action/reaction),
do not take. Perspective taking builds upon dis- and perspectives (point/view). In over a decade
tinction making (recognizing the other), enabling of case study research and evaluation studies,
us to proverbially walk in the others shoes we have demonstrated that this form of systems
(Churchman, 1968). It is a prerequisite to authen- thinking can be taught at all levels and improves
tic understanding and empathy. Additionally, performance across multiple domains, effectively

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Derek Cabrera et al.


Syst. Res. RESEARCH PAPER

democratizing systems thinking (Cabrera and the engagement of self-aware, conscientious,


Cabrera, 2012b). Our concern is greater than im- prosocial agents to gure out and enact solutions
proving cognition and analytical problem-solving, locally, nationally, and globally (Gregory, 2000).
although these are absolutely critical goals in an With DSRP, we can achieve both.
interdependent, rapidly evolving world. We are
also concerned with the moral and psychological
development of prosocial human beings.
Whether considering our societal, national, REFERENCES
global, or personal problems, both the problems
themselves and the outcomes we want are fre- Ackoff RL. 1971. Towards a system of systems con-
quently complex and not always susceptible to cepts. Management Science 17(11): 661671.
Ackoff RL, Magidson J, Addison HJ. 2006. Idealized
direct control. DSRP allows us to focus on simple Design: Creating an Organizations Future. Wharton
rules that, in combination and repetition, pro- School Publishing: Upper Saddle River NJ.
duce the emergent properties we seek in our eco- Anderson JR. 1991. The adaptive nature of human
nomic, social, political, and educational systems, categorization. Psychological Review 98(3): 409429.
and in ourselves. As we see it, these outcomes Angyal A. 1941. Foundations for a Science of Personality.
Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA.
are generally more robust than ones achieved Bar-Yam Y. 1997. Dynamics of Complex Systems. Studies
through direct, system-level manipulations. in Nonlinearity, Devaney RL (ed). Addison-Wesley:
DSRP enables us individually and collectively to Reading, MA.
become more adaptive, better learners, clearer Bateson G. 1970. Form, substance, and difference.
thinkers, and better humans. General Semantics Bulletin 37: 513.
Bateson G. 1972. Steps to an Ecology of Mind: A Revolu-
As systems thinking teachers, colleagues, tionary Approach to Mans Understanding of Himself.
bosses, parents, and friends, we need to endow Ballantine Books: New York.
others with a powerful model of reection that Bateson G. 1979. Mind and Nature: A Necessary Unity
will bring their meaning-making processes more (1st ed.). Dutton: New York.
fully into conscious awareness. This awareness Batson CD, Early S, Salvarani G. 1997. Perspective
taking: imagining how another feels versus imagining
will of course increase their cognitive capacities how you would feel. Personality and Social Psychology
and analytical skills, and it will also redound to Bulletin 23(7): 751758.
their psychological and social benet. In our Bertalanffy L von. 1956. General system theory. General
experience, teaching DSRP has many positive Systems 1: 110.
externalities, including equipping people to bet- Bertalanffy L von. 1968. General Systems Theory. Penguin:
London.
ter endure stressful events and situations, learn Boyd A, Geerling T, Gregory W et al. 2007. Systemic
more adaptive responses, and reduce behaviors evaluation: a participative, multi-method approach.
deleterious to the self and others. It creates more Journal of the Operational Research Society 58:
socially integrated, aware, and conscientious 13061320.
individuals. This makes for better friends, family Bransford J, Brown AL, Cocking RR. 1999. How People
Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School. National
members, colleagues, and group members. Academy Press: Washington, D.C.
DSRP is about much more than thinking; it is Bunge M. 1977. General systems and holism. General
also about action. It is both theory and practice. Systems 22: 8790.
Beset by increasingly complex political, eco- Cabrera D. 2006. Systems Thinking. (Doctoral dissertaion).
nomic, environmental, and social issues (and Cornell University: Ithaca, NY. http://www.eco-
mmons.cornell.edu/bitstream/1813/2860/1/Derek
many which span all areas), we are rst of all in CabreraDissertation.pdf, Accessed 15 July 2015.
need of a method that reveals the pitfalls in our Cabrera D. 2014. In search of universality in systems
mental models (Senge, 1990). DSRP offers this. thinking. Plenary for 58th Meeting of the International
Then it is incumbent upon us to explore the psy- Society for the Systems Sciences, Washington, D.C.
chosocial effects of this form of metacognition. https://www.crlab.us/#dr-derek-cabrera-s-plenary-
for-58-th-meeting-of-the-international-society-for-the-
Even cutting-edge analytical tools like systems systems-sciences-at-the-school-of-business-at-george-
thinking are no match for complex, multi-system, washington-university-washington-dc, Accessed 8
political, economic, and social problems without July 2015.

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking


RESEARCH PAPER Syst. Res.

Cabrera D, Cabrera L. 2012a. IU17 Annual Superinten- Forrester JW. 1971. Counterintuitive behavior of social
dents Conference Research Presentation. State College, systems. Theory and Decision 2: 109140.
PA. Fortune J, Peters G. 1995. Learning from Failure: The
Cabrera D, Cabrera L. 2012b. Thinking at Every Desk: Systems Approach. Wiley: Chichester.
Four Simple Skills to Transform Your Classroom. W. W. Franois C. 2004. Distinction; distinction (primary);
Norton: New York, NY. distinctions (cinematics of). In International Encyclo-
Cabrera D, Cabrera L. 2015. Systems Thinking Made pedia of Systems and Cybernetics, Franois C. (Ed.),
Simple: New Hope for Solving Wicked Problems. (2nd ed., Vol. 1): K.G. Saur: Mnchen, Germany;
Odyssean Press: Ithaca, NY. 176177.
Cabrera D, Colosi L. 2008. Distinctions, systems, rela- Fuenmayor RL. 1991. The roots of reductionism: a
tionships, and perspectives (DSRP): a theory of counter-ontoepistemology for a systems approach.
thinking and of things. Evaluation and Program Plan- Systems Practice 4: 419448.
ning 31(3): 311317. Gell-Mann M. 1996. Lets call it plectics. Complexity 1: 3.
Checkland P. 1981. Systems Thinking, Systems Practice. Glanville R. 1990. The self and the other: the purpose
Wiley: Chichester. of distinction. In Cybernetics and Systems 90 Pro-
Checkland P, Poulter J. 2006. Learning for Action: A ceedings of the European Meeting on Cybernetics and
Short Denitive Account of Soft Systems Methodology, Systems Research, Trappl R (ed). World Scientic:
and Its Use for Practitioners, Teachers and Students. Singapore.
Wiley: Chichester. Goguen JA, Varela FJ. 1979. Systems and distinctions
Checkland P, Scholes J. 1990. Soft Systems Methodology duality and complementarity. International Journal of
in Action. Wiley: Chichester. General Systems 5: 3143.
Christakis AN, Bausch K. 2006. Co-laboratories of Greene AJ. 2010. Making connections: the essence of
Democracy: How People Harness Their Collective memory is linking one thought to another. Scientic
Wisdom and Power. Information Age Publishing: American Mind. http://www.scienticamerican.com/
Charlotte. article/making-connections/, Accessed 8 July 2015.
Churchman CW. 1968. The Systems Approach. Dell: Gregory AJ, Jackson MC. 1992a. Evaluating organiza-
New York. tions: a systems and contingency approach. Systems
Churchman CW. 1970. Operations research as a profes- Practice 5: 3760.
sion. Management Science 17: B37-53. Gregory AJ, Jackson MC. 1992b. Evaluation methodol-
Churchman CW. 1979. The Systems Approach and Its ogies: a system for use. Journal of the Operational
Enemies. Basic Books: New York. Research Society 43: 1928.
Clark T. 1994. National boundaries, border zones, and Gregory WJ. 1992. Critical Systems Thinking and
marketing strategy: a conceptual framework and Pluralism: A New Constellation. Ph.D. thesis, City
theoretical model of secondary boundary effects. University, London.
Journal of Marketing 58: 6780. Gregory WJ. 1996a. Discordant pluralism: a new strat-
Cook TD, Campbell DT. 1979. Quasi-Experimentation: egy for critical systems thinking? Systems Practice 9:
Design & Analysis Issues for Field Settings. Houghton 605625.
Mifin Company: Boston. Gregory WJ. 1996b. Dealing with diversity. In Critical
Couzin ID, Krause J, James R, Ruxton GD, Franks Systems Thinking: Current Research and Practice,
NR. 2002. Collective memory and spatial sorting Flood, RL, Romm, NRA (eds). Plenum: New York.
in animal groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology 218: Gregory WJ. 2000. Transforming self and society: a
111. critical appreciation model. Systemic Practice and
Davies C. 1982. Sexual taboos and social boundaries. Action Research 13: 475501.
American Journal of Sociology 87: 10321063. Hagerty B, Williams R, Coyne J, Early M. 1996. Sense
Durand R, Calori R. 2006. Sameness, otherness? of belonging and indicators of social and psycholog-
Enriching organizational change theories with philo- ical functioning. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 10:
sophical considerations on the same and the other. 235244.
Academy of Management Review 31: 93114. Hall AD, Fagen RE. 1956. Denition of system. General
Durkheim E. 1951 1897. Suicide: A Study in Sociology. Systems 1: 1828.
Free Press: New York, NY. Hoffman WC. 1998. The topology of wholes, parts and
Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Gentner D. 2015. Prelinguistic re- their perception-cognition. Psycoloquy 9: http://
lational concepts: investigating analogical process- www.cogsci.ecs.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?
ing in infants. Child Development. DOI: 10.1111/ 9.03, Accessed 7 July 2015.
cdev.12381 Jackson MC. 1987. New directions in management
Flood R, Jackson M. 1991a. Creative Problem Solving: science. In New Directions in Management Science.
Total Systems Intervention. Wiley: Chichester. Jackson MC, Keys P. (eds). Gower: Aldershot.
Flood R, Jackson M. 1991b. Critical Systems Thinking: Jackson MC. 2000. Systems Approaches to Management.
Directed Readings. Wiley: Chichester. Kluwer/Plenum: New York.

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Derek Cabrera et al.


Syst. Res. RESEARCH PAPER

Jackson MC, Keys P. 1984. Towards a system of sys- Mingers JC. 1992. What are real friends for? A reply to
tems methodologies. The Journal of the Operational Mike Jackson. Journal of the Operational Research
Research Society 35: 473486. Society 43: 732735
Kashdan T, Feldman Barrett L, McKnight P. 2014. Mingers JC. 1993. The system of systems methodologies
Unpacking emotion differentiation: transforming a reply to Schecter. Journal of the Operational Research
unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in Society 44: 206208.
negativity. Association for Psychological Science 24: Mingers J, Gill A (eds). 1997. Multimethodology: The
1016. Theory and Practice of Combining Management Science
Kosko B. 1993. Fuzzy Thinking: The New Science of Fuzzy Methodologies. Wiley: Chichester.
Logic. Hyperion: New York. Mortensen C. 1998. Perceptual cognition, parts and
Latimer C, Stevens C. 1997. Some remarks on wholes, wholes. Psycoloquy 9. http://www.cogsci.ecs.soton.
parts and their perception. Psycoloquy 8. http:// ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?9.1, Accessed 8 July 2015.
www.cogsci.ecs.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?8.13, Neale MA, Bazerman MH. 1983. The role of
Accessed 7 July 2015. perspective-taking ability in negotiating under dif-
Latimer C, Stevens C. 1998. Wholes and parts: topol- ferent forms of arbitration. Industrial and Labor Rela-
ogy, mereology and mechanism. Replies to Hoffman tions Review 36: 378388.
Mortensen and Rakover on part-whole-perception. Newton I. 1687 1999. The Principia: Mathematical Principles
Psycoloquy 9. http://www.cogsci.ecs.soton.ac.uk/ of Natural Philosophy. Cohen B, Whitman A, Budenz J.
cgi/psyc/newpsy?9.31, Accessed 7 July 2015. (trans.). University of California Press: Berkeley.
Latimer C, Stevens C. 1999. Intrinsic representations: Opie J. 1999. Gestalt theories of cognitive representation
simultaneity of local and global processing? Reply to & processing. Psycoloquy 10. http://www.cogsci.ecs.
Opie on part-whole-perception. Psycoloquy 10. http:// soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?10.021, Accessed 7
www.cogsci.ecs.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?10.041, July 2015.
Accessed 7 July 2015. Peterson MA, Skow-Grant E. 2003. Memory and learning
Maani KE, Cavana RY. 2000. Systems Thinking and in gure-ground perception. Psychology of Learning
Modelling: Understanding Change and Complexity. And Motivation: Advances In Research And Theory:
Pearson: Auckland, NZ. Cognitive Vision 42: 135.
Marchal JH. 1975. On the concept of a system. Philoso- Piaget J. 1974. Understanding Causality (Miles D, Miles
phy of Science 42: 448468. M, Trans.). The Norton Library: New York.
Marvin RS, Greenberg MT, Mossler DG. 1976. The Quinodoz JM. 2005. Reading Freud: A Chronological Explo-
early development of conceptual perspective taking: ration of Freuds Writings. Routledge: New York.
distinguishing among multiple perspectives. Child Rakover SS. 1998. Can mechanistic explanatory concepts be
Development 47: 511514. applied to part-whole? Psycoloquy 9. http://www.cogsci.
Mason RO, Mitroff II. 1981. Challenging Strategic Plan- ecs.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?9.02, Accessed 8 July
ning Assumptions. Wiley: New York. 2015.
Maturana HR, Varela FG. 1987. The Tree of Knowledge. Rittel HWJ, Webber MW. 1973. Dilemmas in a general
Shambhala: Boston, MA. theory of planning. Policy Sciences 4: 155169.
Midgley G. 2000. Systemic Intervention: Philosophy, Rochefort DA, Cobb RW. 1994. The Politics of Problem
Methodology, and Practice. Kluwer Academic Pub- Denition: Shaping the Policy Agenda. University Press
lishers: New York, NY. of Kansas: Lawrence, Kansas.
Midgley G. 2003. Systems Thinking. Sage Publications: Romm NRA. 2001. Accountability in Social Research:
Thousand Oaks, CA. Issues and Debates. Kluwer/Plenum: New York.
Midgley G, Munlo I, Brown M. 1998. The theory Schober MF. 1993. Spatial perspective-taking in con-
and practice of boundary critique: developing versation. Cognition 47: 124.
house services for older people. The Journal of the Schulz L, Gopnik A. 2004. Causal learning across do-
Operational Research Society 49: 467478. mains. Developmental Psychology 40: 162176.
Midgley G, Ochoa-Arias AE. 2001. Unfolding a theory Schwarz E. 1996. Streams of Systemic Thought. Neuchtel,
of systemic intervention. Systemic Practice and Action Switzerland. http://www.slideshare.net/Community
Research 14: 615650. ActionHero/streams-of-systemic-thought, Accessed 8
Midgley G, Ochoa-Arias AE. 2004. Introduction to July 2015.
community operational research. In Community Senge PM. 1990. The Fifth Discipline: The Art and Prac-
Operational Research: OR and Systems Thinking for tice of the Learning Organization. Century Business:
Community Development, Midgley G, Ochoa-Arias London.
AE. (eds). Kluwer: New York. Shen CY, Midgley G. 2007. Toward a Buddhist systems
Midgley G, Pinzn LA. 2011. Boundary critique and its methodology 1: comparisons between Buddhism
implications for conict prevention. Journal of the and systems theory. Systemic Practice and Action Re-
Operational Research Society 62: 15431554. search 20: 167194.

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking


RESEARCH PAPER Syst. Res.

Simpson G. 1945. The principles of classication and a Wertheimer M. 1923. Laws of organization in percep-
classication of mammals. Bulletin of the American tual forms (English translation, 1938). In Ellis WD
Museum of Natural History 85:1350. (ed), A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. Harcourt
Strogatz SH. 1994. Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos: With Brace: New York.
Applications to Physics, Biology, Chemistry, and Engi- Wiener N. 1948. Cybernetics. Scientic American 179:
neering. Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA. 1419.
Tsoukas H. 1992. Panoptic reason and the search for to- Young J. 2005. On insiders (emic) and outsiders (etic):
tality: a critical assessment of the critical systems views of self, and othering. Systemic Practice and Ac-
perspective. Human Relations 45: 637657. tion Research 18: 151162.
Tversky A, Kahneman D. 1981. The framing of decisions Zhu Z. 2011. After paradigm: why mixing-
and the psychology of choice. Science 211: 453458. methodology theorising fails and how to make it
Ulrich W. 1983. Critical Heuristics of Social Planning: A work again. Journal of the Operational Research Society
New Approach to Practical Philosophy. Paul Haupt: 62: 784798.
Bern, Switzerland.

Copyright 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Syst. Res. (2015)
DOI: 10.1002/sres.2351

Derek Cabrera et al.

You might also like