You are on page 1of 7

J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat.

44 (2013) 194e200

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Journal of Behavior Therapy and


Experimental Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep

Experimental analysis of the relationship between depressed mood and


compulsive buying
Michael Kyrios a, Paul McQueen b, Richard Moulding a, *
a
Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia
b
University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background and objectives: Compulsive buying is a serious but understudied problem, where individuals
Received 25 January 2012 are unable to resist or control their buying behaviour, leading to substantial social and financial prob-
Received in revised form lems. To date there has been a lack of experimental research into the disorder.
19 October 2012
Methods: The relationship between mood and compulsive buying was examined in compulsive buyers
Accepted 24 October 2012
(N ¼ 18) and non-clinical controls (N ¼ 17), using experimental information-processing paradigms. In
study 1, it was expected that, if buying behaviours function as a coping strategy for depressed mood, then
Keywords:
an induction of depressed mood would lead to an enhanced memory for appealing consumer-objects in
Compulsive buying
Oniomania
compulsive buyers, but not controls. In study 2, we examined the association between emotional
Impulse control disorders and functional constructs and consumer items. It was expected that compulsive buyers would show
Obsessive compulsive spectrum disorders stronger semantic relationships and thus better episodic memory for objecteemotion pairs, relative to
Memory objectefunction pairs, for appealing items.
Results: Unexpectedly, in study 1 the memory-facilitating effect of depressed mood was evident among
control participants and absent among compulsive buyers. In study 2, compulsive buyers showed a lesser
association of undesirable objects with positive emotional concepts than did non-clinical controls, and
compulsive buyers were found to more strongly associate all consumer items with emotional concepts
than with concepts of function.
Limitations: Key limitations were low power and possible floor effects due to error frequency data.
Conclusion: These findings provide insights into the processes underlying CB phenomena, in particular
supporting the role of mood in compulsive buying.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction buyers being female in clinical samples, although a community-


based prevalence study has suggested that compulsive buying
Compulsive buying (CB) is a serious and common psychological may be prevalent in similar rates in males and females (Koran,
problem associated with marital conflict, bankruptcy, theft, Faber, Aboujaoude, Large, & Serpe, 2006).
embezzlement and even suicide attempts (Christenson et al., 1994). Kyrios, Frost, and Steketee (2004) have developed a cognitive-
Three core features are generally agreed to characterise CB: (a) behavioural model of compulsive buying (for a compatible model,
subjectively irresistible urges to buy; (b) loss of control over buying see Kellett & Bolton, 2009). The model emphasizes domains such as
behaviour; and (c) continued compulsive buying behaviour despite maladaptive attachments to objects, specific cognitions about the
adverse consequences (Dittmar, 2005a). Estimates of the preva- uniqueness of, and responsibility for, desired objects, and self
lence of CB in the US have ranged between 1.8% and 8.1% of the construals (Frost, Kyrios, McCarthy, & Mathews, 2007). Compulsive
general population depending on the cut-off used (Faber & buyers exhibit concerns about the potential loss of opportunities
O’Guinn, 1992). Onset is typically between the ages of 18 and 30 “to get a bargain” or to purchase an object with particular desired
(Black, 1996; Dittmar, 2005a), and with around 90% of compulsive characteristics and, despite perceptions that purchased objects are
essential, they often remain unused after being purchased (Kyrios
et al., 2004). For example, Kyrios, Moulding, Arnold, and Ciorciari
* Corresponding author. Brain and Psychological Research Centre, Faculty of Life
and Social Science, Swinburne University of Technology, PO Box 218, Hawthorn VIC,
(submitted for publication) found that beliefs about the utility of
Melbourne, Australia. Tel.: þ61 3 9214 4686. items e that items are uniquely able help the individual improve
E-mail address: rmoulding@swin.edu.au (R. Moulding). their lives e such as “This item is so unique that there is nothing

0005-7916/$ e see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.10.004
M. Kyrios et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 194e200 195

else like it in the world”, “If I buy this, people will find me more identify which had been shown previously. For the compulsive
attractive”, and “This item will make me feel better” e were buyers but not the control group, highly-ranked items, which
strongly related to buying symptoms in a student and community should be more associated with mood-relieving effects, were ex-
sample (N ¼ 240) over-and-above mood and other variables pected to be attended to more readily in the memory task when
implicated in social models of buying (e.g., materialistic values). these associations are primed by mood state (i.e., in the depressed
CB operates in a negative cycle with depression, with buying mood group). Consequently, it was hypothesised that CB partici-
episodes operating to relieve negative mood in the short-term, but pants but not non-clinical controls would make fewer errors when
in the long term the lack of satisfaction from consumer objects and asked to recognise high-ranked items in the depressed mood state
negative consequences of buying episodes serve to perpetuate relative to the neutral mood state, after the general effect of mood
depression. Kyrios et al. (2004) found moderate correlations was accounted for. These effects were expected for miss errors and
between CB and depression symptoms, consistent with the beliefs not false positives, due to the effects of facilitation being limited to
identified in their model that buying will serve to alleviate negative the association between aroused emotion and the co-occurrence of
mood or improve self-worth. They also found that compulsive an event (Kazui et al., 2000).
buyers believed that objects could compensate for negative
emotions, even when controlling for depressed mood per se. Faber 2.1. Method
and Christenson (1996) found approximately 25% of normal buyers
indicated that buying alters their mood, compared to 96% of 2.1.1. Participants
compulsive buyers. More directly, Miltenberger et al. (2003) found Participants undertook both study 1 and 2, and were recruited
that inflated pre-buying self-ratings of depression dropped signif- via advertisements placed around universities, public libraries and
icantly during buying episodes but increased again post-purchase. notice boards, and through responses to media publicity including
Therefore, some findings suggest that individuals with CB may newspaper articles and television and radio interviews. Interested
deliberately use buying episodes as a dysfunctional method of participants completed an initial screening questionnaire to
regulating negative emotions. However, it has not yet been shown determine suitability (approximately 200 screening questionnaires
that experimentally manipulating mood state has an impact on were distributed, N ¼ 85 returned). Additional measures were also
buying behaviour and associated information processing. One given that are detailed in a larger questionnaire-based study
difficulty in such experimental research is that it is difficult to detailed elsewhere (Kyrios, Moulding, & Zabel, submitted for
establish a controlled experimental environment in which to publication), before participants were invited to participate in the
examine the influence of modulating negative affect on CB. An experimental studies. Participants received AUD$20 reimburse-
alternative approach is to establish the relationship between mood ment of travel costs. To qualify for the CB group, individuals had to
and CB by examining information processing. If the “compensation obtain scores on the Compulsive Buying Scale below a threshold
hypothesis” is correct, then compulsive buyers should associate of 1.34, identified by Faber and O’Guinn (1992) as the cut-off for
consumer objects with emotional concepts. That is, the expectation compulsive buying; and they had to endorse at least one of two
that purchasing consumer items will serve to compensate for screening questions, “I am a compulsive shopper (or ‘addicted’ to
unpleasant emotions should lead to individuals with CB having shopping)” or “Currently experiencing problems due to excessive
a stronger semantic relationship between emotional concepts and buying” (6 participants excluded for meeting only one of two
consumer items. Experimentally demonstrating the existence of criteria). Individuals who did not meet either of these criteria were
such associations among individuals with CB, which significantly invited to participate in the control group. In addition, individuals
differ from the associations of non-buyers, would be strong classified as compulsive buyers were required to be below the
evidence in support of the compensation hypothesis. This threshold for mania and borderline personality disorder and to be
hypothesis was examined in two studies using participants with CB below the extremely high-range for depression on the measures
and non-clinical control participants. described below (overall 17 participants met one or more of these
exclusion criteria). No participants included in the study reported
2. Study one significant psychiatric history besides prior treatment for depres-
sion or anxiety disorders. Overall, 35 participants were invited to
In study one, we aimed to investigate the link between negative participate in the CB group and 27 in the NC group. Of these, a final
mood and buying through examining the effect of a negative or sample of 35 participants (28 female) participated in the experi-
neutral mood induction on recognition memory for idiosyncrati- mental tasks, with eighteen in the control group and 17 in the
cally desired vs. non-desired items. Items primed in working compulsive buyer group. One participant in the CB group was
memory can lead to a “pop-out” effect in visual search tasks for excluded from Study 2 due to a computer malfunction. The groups
stimuli that match some or all elements of the memory prime (Soto, did not differ on gender, being single, age, or education (see
Humphries & Heinke, 2006). Therefore, if depressed mood is Table 1).
associated with CB, it will trigger buying-related mental processes
and activate these in working memory, effectively “priming” the 2.1.2. Questionnaires
participant to attend to buying-related stimuli. An induced 2.1.2.1. Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale (AMS; Altman, Hedeker,
depressed mood should lead to direct attention to items perceived Peterson, & Davis, 1997). A 5-item self-rating scale of the severity
to be able to compensate for depressed mood, but not to items with of manic symptoms. The scale has satisfactory reliability and
no perceived compensatory role. This, in turn, would lead to validity.
superior engagement and, therefore, recognition of these items by
people who meet CB criteria, of the CB-related items (items 2.1.2.2. Borderline Screening Measure (BSM; Hyler et al., 1989).
subjectively perceived as appealing) presented in the memory task. A 9-item scale measuring borderline personality tendencies. The
In this study, individuals with compulsive buying and non- PDQ-4 is a widely used reliable and valid measure.
clinical (NC) controls were exposed to a depressed or neutral
mood induction, prior to being asked to memorise a sequence of 2.1.2.3. Compulsive Buying Scale (CBS; Faber & O’Guinn, 1989).
subjectively attractive and unattractive consumer items. They were A well-validated 7-item screening measure for CB that has
then shown the items along with distractor items, and asked to demonstrated good reliability and validity (Faber & O’Guinn, 1989,
196 M. Kyrios et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 194e200

Table 1
Demographic characteristics of sample.

Variable Value Control Compulsive buying Test

n % n %
Gender Female 16 88.9 12 70.6 c2(1) ¼ 0.87, p ¼ 0.35
Relationship Single 11 64.7 7 58.6 c2(1) ¼ 0.01, p ¼ 0.93
Married/de Facto 4 23.5 4 33.3
Separated/divorced 2 11.8 1 8.3

Mean (SD) Mean (SD)


Age 37.1 13.7 38.9 15.7 F(1,26) ¼ 0.12, p ¼ 0.73
Education 15.6 4.1 14.9 3.8 F(1,26) ¼ 0.16, p ¼ 0.69

1992). Two of the seven items pertain to emotional reactions to items were placed in a distractor set, while evenly ranked items
shopping, while the remaining five relate to financial aspects of were placed in a target set. Following the mood induction, partic-
buying. ipants were shown the 28-items from the target set in a random
order, with each shown for two seconds, and they were instructed
2.1.2.4. Compulsive Acquisition Scale (CAS; Frost et al., 1998). that they would be asked to subsequently recall which items they
An 18-item scale that measures the extent to which individuals had seen. Following this, participants were immediately presented
acquire and feel compelled to acquire possessions. The CAS-Buy with each of the original 56 items, in random order, and asked to
subscale is a broad measure of CB behaviour, which has demon- identify the target items. Note that while the memory task was
strated satisfactory reliability in previous research (Frost, Steketee, novel to this experiment, as it is specific to memory for compulsive
& Williams, 2002; Kyrios et al., 2004). buying-related items, it is based on a standard 2-alternative forced-
choice recognition memory task (e.g., Tulving, 1981). Following
2.1.2.5. Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS; Lovibond & Lovibond, this, participants completed the three VAS scales again, and were
1995). A 42-item scale measuring depression, anxiety and stress debriefed on the mood-induction.
over the previous week. Excellent internal consistency, 2-week
test-retest reliability for a clinical sample, as well as high discrim- 2.2. Results
inant validity between the three subscales have been reported.
2.2.1. Statistical methods
2.1.2.6. Visual-analogue Scales (VAS) of happiness, anxiety and All analyses were performed using R (R Development Core
depression. The validity of the mood induction was examined using Team, 2005), with the NLME package (Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, &
three VAS scales. The scales were anchored at 0, 50 and 100, with Sarkar, 2005). Most analyses were ANOVAs to test significance of
written descriptors (e.g., “Not at all happy”, “Extremely happy”). terms in linear mixed-effects (LME) models built using a hierar-
chical stepwise reduction approach. Linear mixed-effects models
2.1.3. Procedure are an extension of the standard linear model, which are robust for
Participants completed the questions and demographic infor- unbalanced data, as was the case in this study (Pinheiro & Bates,
mation, then studies 1 and 2, before completing a final computer 2000). For each analysis, a full-factorial model was first con-
task described elsewhere (Kyrios, Moulding, & McQueen, in structed including all first-order factors, covariates and interaction
preparation). In the first task, participants were shown a series of terms adjusted for any violations of assumptions. In order to
pairs of photographs of consumer items, and asked to select the maximize power, models were then subjected to stepwise removal
item in each pair that they would be more likely to buy. In total, 56 of terms based on the summary table of the marginal significance of
items were ranked from each of eight categories (body care items, terms (based on Doig et al., 1998), until each term in the model
accessories, sports equipment, kitchen items, clothes, jewellery, exhibited significance, or model fit was found to deteriorate based
footware, electronic leisure items; derived from Dittmar, Beattie, & on when the AIC value increased and ANOVA tests of likelihood
Friese, 1996). The initial order of presentation of item pairs was ratios revealed significant changes to the model.
randomised, and an iterative implementation of a merge-sort
algorithm was used to rank order the items by preference based 2.2.2. Validity of groups
on participants’ selections from, on average, 225 pairs of items. Supporting the validity of the groups, the compulsive buying
Subsequently, participants from each group were randomised to group reported significantly more CB symptoms on the compulsive
a neutral or depressed mood induction. They completed the three buying measures (CBS, M ¼ 2.75, M ¼ 1.23 respectively;
VAS scales for mood, and were given written instructions for the F(1,31) ¼ 70.55, p < 0.0001; CAS-buy, M ¼ 61.12, M ¼ 31.75,
mood-induction procedure. The induction involved listening to F(1,29) ¼ 43.52, p < 0.0001). The CB group also demonstrated
a 10-min piece of music while recalling a memory that made them greater levels of depression, F(1,31) ¼ 7.83, p ¼ 0.009 (M ¼ 11.88,
feel either sad or neutral (Badcock & Allen, 2003; Kulbartz-Klatt, M ¼ 3.72); anxiety, F(1,31) ¼ 7.48, p ¼ 0.010 (M ¼ 8.88, M ¼ 2.39);
Florin, & Pook, 1999). Following the mood induction, participants and stress, F(1,31) ¼ 9.17, p ¼ 0.005 (M ¼ 17.24, M ¼ 7.78).
again completed the VAS scales. The following musical pieces were
selected e “Russia Under the Mongolian Yoke” by Prokofiev played 2.2.3. Manipulation check of mood induction
at half speed (induction of depressed-mood e ID group); and To check the mood induction was effective, a 2 (group e CB vs
“Waltzes numbers. 11 and 12” by Chopin played at normal speed control)  2 (mood e induced depression vs neutral)  3 (time e
(induction of neutral-mood e IN group; Badcock & Allen, 2003; pre, induced mood; post-experiment) model was constructed.
Wood, Saltzberg, & Goldsamt, 1990). Overall the groups did not differ, F(2,31) ¼ 0.34, p ¼ 0.563. However,
For the memory task, the 56 consumer items from the ranking the mood induction was effective, with main effects of mood e
task were split into two 28-item sets by ranked pairs. Oddly ranked F(1,31) ¼ 8.50, p ¼ 0.007, qualified by a significant interaction
M. Kyrios et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 194e200 197

(i.e., pre-post mood induction), F(4,67) ¼ 3.68, p ¼ 0.009; post-hoc When the neutral mood groups of NCs and CBs were compared, the
tests indicating that the pre-time point showed lower depression performance was largely indistinguishable. Unexpectedly, what
than the post-time points, but only for the induced depression distinguished the groups was that CBs in the depressed condition
group. While there was a significant group by group  mood made significantly more miss errors than NCs in the depressed
interaction, F(1,31) ¼ 11.69, p ¼ 0.002, the group  mood  time condition.
term did not contribute to the model variance, suggesting that the A possible explanation for this result is that CBs may have low
CB group demonstrated the same general response to the mood tolerance for depression and automatically engage in compensa-
inductions as the NC group. Equivalent results were found for tory processes. As discussed, a memory, rather than shopping, task
happiness, with a significant main effect of mood induction, was chosen to avoid interference of the task content on the induced
F(1,33) ¼ 18.44, p < 0.001, and induction by time interaction, mood state. It is possible, however, that viewing consumer images
F(4,66) ¼ 4.98, p ¼ 0.014. Analyses for anxiety did not show any during the memory task “mimicked” a shopping trip enough to
significant effects, indicating that the manipulation was specific for compensate for CBs’ depressed mood. Anecdotal reports from many
depression/happiness. of the compulsive buyers were that the task was “a bit like going
shopping”, and was an element of the study that they tended to
2.2.4. Experimental findings for errors enjoy.
Experimental results were analysed by group (control vs. CB), We speculate that CB’s attention in the depressed but not the
mood (induced depression vs neutral), object attractiveness rank neutral condition was directed to mood compensation processes,
(low, middle, high e based on respective thirds of the participant’s distracting from the memory task. This explanation could account
preference for the object), and error type (false positive, or miss). for the paradoxical, though non-significant, increase in miss errors
After reduction, the final model found no differences for group, in the CB group. In terms of the proposed model, this could be taken
F(1,31) ¼ 2.01, p ¼ 0.166, group  mood, F(2,31) ¼ 1.97, p ¼ 0.157, or to uphold the hypothesis that depressed mood is a trigger for
mood  error type, F(2,171) ¼ 0.27, p ¼ 0.763; item-ranking was not buying urges, as induced depressed mood was the sole factor that
included after reduction of the model. There was a significant third- revealed differences in memory performance between the groups.
order interaction of group  mood  error type, F(2,171) ¼ 5.35, For depressed CBs, attention may have been directed to mood-
p ¼ 0.006. Therefore, fewer miss errors were made by control but compensation rather than task performance, leading to poorer
not experimental participants in the induced depression vs. neutral memory for presented items relative to the control group. Future
group. Planned post-hoc analyses indicated that NCs made signif- research is needed to investigate this possibility. In addition, it
icantly fewer miss errors in the ID group than in the IN group, should be noted that the CB group generally reported worsened
t(31) ¼ 2.43, p ¼ 0.021. No significant difference emerged in the rate mood relative to the NC group. While the mood-induction was
of miss errors for CBs, and their trend was to make more miss errors found to affect both groups similarly, future studies with larger
in the ID group than in the IN group, t(31) ¼ 1.08, p ¼ 0.288 (see sample sizes should control for overall differences in mood
Fig. 1). This contradicted the hypothesis, which predicted that there between group as differences in general mood may affect the
would be a third-order interaction effect of group  mood  rank, results found (rather than differences being due to CB per se).
with CBs making fewer miss errors for highly-ranked items when
depressed than when in a neutral mood.
3. Study two

2.3. Discussion of study one Study two aimed to extend the examination of the relationship
between mood and objects using a semantic-memory task. Dittmar
The compensation hypothesis for CB suggested there should be (2005a) found that psychologically motivated purchases were
a significantly lower proportion of miss errors than false positives associated with an expectation that the object would improve self-
in the CB group, especially in the depressed mood condition for image in about 17% of cases, and that in approximately 48% of cases
appealing items. The differences in performance on the memory positive mood change was experienced immediately following the
task between the NC and CB groups that emerged were indepen- purchase. This powerful association between objects, self-image
dent of the appeal of items but dependent on the mood condition. and mood should produce semantic networks which over time

Fig. 1. Interaction plots of significant interaction effects accounting for errors in study one.
198 M. Kyrios et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 194e200

directly link certain consumer objects with emotional concepts. effects, group was not significant, F(1,32) ¼ 2.78, p ¼ 0.105; and
Given that prior semantic relationships between pairs of items can neither was group by rank, F(2,160) ¼ 0.06, p ¼ 0.942. However, the
facilitate the episodic memory of those items being paired together third order interaction, group  rank  category was significant,
(Silberman, Miikkulainen, & Bentin, 2001), it was hypothesised that F(8,160) ¼ 2.23, p ¼ 0.028 (Fig. 2).
the semantic relationship between an object and an emotion could
be demonstrated by measuring individuals’ memories for pairs of 3.2.2. Experimental results
images of objects along with emotion words, compared to pairs of The key difference between the NC and CB groups was the rate
object images with function words. of errors made on low-ranked items paired with positive emotional
Participants were shown consumer items paired with words words, which were significantly lower in the NC group, t(32) ¼ 2.10,
from four categories (positive emotional, negative emotional, p ¼ 0.044 (Fig. 2). The lowest rate of errors for CBs was for low
functional and neutral), and had to subsequently identify these ranked items paired with negative emotional words, but this did
pairings in a recognition memory task. As all groups were expected not differ significantly between the NCs and CBs, t(32) ¼ 0.43,
to possess semantic associations between objects and functions, p ¼ 0.668. The CBs made more errors than NCs when items were
but not between objects and unrelated words, fewer errors were paired with functional words, regardless of rank, although this
predicted for objects paired with functional words rather than with difference only approached significance, t(64) ¼ 1.98, p ¼ 0.052.
neutral words across all participants and groups. As control While the patterns of errors differed according to rank in both the
participants do not engage in psychologically-motivated buying as NC and CB groups, a post-hoc ANOVA on the model for only the
regularly as compulsive buyers, the semantic relationship between control participants indicated no significant differences in error
emotions and objects was expected to be very distal (weak) for rates by item rank, F(1,85) ¼ 0.00, p ¼ 1.000, or word categories by
controls, regardless of the subjective appeal (rank) of the item, rank, F(4,85) ¼ 0.38, p ¼ 0.821. Thus, functional and emotion-pairs
whereas associations with the utility, or function, of objects should had equivalent error rates in the NC group, contrary to predictions.
be more proximal. For the control group it was therefore expected CBs’ rate of errors depended on both item rank and item pairs.
that episodic memory for objectefunction pairs would be better For high-ranked (HR) items, negative or positive emotion pairs
than the memory for objecteemotion pairs, regardless of item rank. were associated with less errors than functional word pairs,
In contrast, CBs were expected to exhibit better memory for consistent with the hypothesis, t(160) ¼ 1.63, p ¼ 0.003. For low-
objecteemotion pairs than objectefunction pairs for highly ranked (LR) items, functional and emotional pairs had equivalent
appealing items. For non-appealing items, which are not typically rates of errors, t(160) ¼ 0.22, p ¼ 0.828, as in the NC group. It was
purchased during compulsive buying episodes, the same pattern as predicted that the fewer errors among CBs would be made on HR
controls was predicted. Since buying episodes involve both nega- items paired with positive emotional words vs. negative emotional
tive emotions before, and positive mood change during and after words, but this was not borne out, t(160) ¼ 0.60, p ¼ 0.545. Positive
buying (Dittmar, 2005b; Miltenberger et al., 2003), both negative word pairs were however associated with less errors in the CB
and positive emotions were expected to be associated with highly- group for HR vs. LR data, t(160) ¼ 2.83, p ¼ 0.017.
desired items for compulsive buyers, with positive emotions being
more strongly associated. 3.3. Discussion of study two

3.1. Method Study two was designed to demonstrate that the regular linking
of consumer objects to both negative and positive emotional states
3.1.1. Procedure during psychologically-motivated buying episodes would lead to
Participants were shown two sequences of 12 images of the formation of stronger semantic associations for CBs. It was
consumer items, paired with words. The images were of the six hypothesised that, among the control participants, errors would be
items most highly ranked by the participant and the six lowest- lower for items paired with functional words than items paired
ranked items, with each item displayed twice as a target, each with emotional words. In practice, for the NCs, the rate of errors
time paired with a different target word. Across the two sequences, was generally invariant across all the categories of words, sug-
six words from each of four word categories (positive emotional gesting that emotional and functional words have equidistant
[þE], negative emotional [E], functional [FC] and neutral [NU]) semantic associations to consumer items among the general pop-
were paired with the items, balanced for rank. Functional words ulation. This suggests that some emotional impact is given to
were words that could generically refer to items as having a func- consumer objects even in a normal group, consistent with dimen-
tion, being effective, useful or practical. Neutral words were words sional views of buying that suggest that emotional buying occurs
that could refer to an object, but with neither emotional nor even in non-clinical individuals, but these associations did not
functional significance. Word-image pairs were displayed for four predominate over associations with the functionality of items.
seconds, followed by a one second blank square. Following each 12- Examination of the post-hoc tests indicated that, relative to the
image sequence, participants were shown 24 wordeimage pairs, “baseline” performance of the NC group (where the category of
half from the previous sequence and half of which included the words and rankings had no bearing on the rate of errors), emotional
same words and images in random pairings. Participants indicated and functional words did show differential associations with
whether they had seen each pairing. consumer items among the CB group. Compared to NCs, CBs had
a trend towards making more errors for all items paired with
3.2. Results functional words (p ¼ 0.052), made more errors for low-ranked
items paired with positive emotional words, and essentially the
3.2.1. Model building same rate of errors for all items paired with negative emotional
Data was analysed using the same procedure as study 1, based words and highly-ranked items paired with positive emotional
on the three variables of group (CB or control), word type (þE, E, words. Thus, these patterns suggest some differences between
FC), and rank (low, high). Inclusion of “neutral” words led to groups according to proximal associations with semantic descrip-
substantial deterioration of the models and the category was tors. In particular, CBs seem less likely to associate objects with
therefore excluded from analyses. The final models included the functional concepts, consistent with descriptions of compulsive
terms group, group  rank and group  rank  category. Of these buying as often not emphasizing the normal function of objects, but
M. Kyrios et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 194e200 199

Fig. 2. Plots of significant interaction effects in study two.

rather purchasing for mood or identity-repair reasons. Further- wanted and unwanted items. Thus, associations will be formed
more, CBs made significantly more errors than the NC group in between mood and consumer items in general. Purchases, accom-
remembering non-desirable items paired with positive emotional panied by positive mood change, however, only occur for particular
words, and this rate was also higher than when desirable items items that, as per the findings of Dittmar et al. (1996), are typically
were paired with positive emotional words for the CBs. This purchased during compulsive buying episodes.
suggests that positive emotions have a semantically more distal If replicated, the results of these studies suggest that it is
association with non-desirable items than they do for desired possible to design effective therapeutic interventions that could
objects for CBs. It was hypothesised that there would be a similar occur within the controlled environment of the therapist’s
pattern of association by CBs as NCs for these non-desired items, consultation room. Study one was taken to suggest that a mood-
assuming that non-desired items do not become linked with repair process was occurring within the laboratory setting, even
consumer objects through psychologically-motivated buying though the situation was substantially different to an actual
episodes. Perhaps, though, items at the lowest end of CBs’ rankings shopping trip. Study two provides evidence for a particular area in
were not just undesired, but actively rejected. This was reflected by which cognitive-behavioural interventions may be useful. Since
some CB participants’ verbal comments during the ranking task, emotional concepts were found to be strongly associated with
such as, “That is so ugly!” consumer items, treatment involving exposure to consumer items
Where CBs were presented with highly desirable items, pairings coupled with both cognitive and behavioural techniques intended
with either positive or negative emotional words were remem- to extinguish maladaptive associations between objects and
bered more readily than pairings with functional words. The emotions could prove effective. Mitchell, Burgard, Faber, Crosby,
strongest effect was for desirable items paired with positive and de Zwaan (2006) have demonstrated positive results from
emotional words, although there was no significant difference a CBT treatment program that includes thought restructuring and
between the rate of errors for high-ranked items paired with exposure and response prevention as components of treatment.
positive and negative words. Overall the results support the notion Study two suggests that the thought restructuring component of
that a semantically more proximal relationship exists between such therapies should place some emphasis on extinguishing the
desirable consumer items and both positive and negative associations between consumer items and emotions. The studies
emotional words than functional words for CBs, as predicted. also reinforce the use of techniques such as the development of
strategies for dealing with high-risk times for shopping (e.g.,
4. Overall discussion alternative pleasurable activities for when emotions are negative;
e.g., see Mitchell, 2011).
Together, these findings provide insights into the processes The studies presented here were the first to experimentally
underlying CB phenomena. The importance of mood in CB is sup- manipulate factors such as mood in order to study compulsive
ported by our findings; although future replication studies would buying. A key limitation of this research is the low number of
strengthen the generalizability of our research. The unexpected participants in each group, with consequentially low power for
finding from study one of greater rates of miss errors rates by statistical analyses. As much as possible, the impact of this limita-
depressed compulsive buyers compared to depressed controls was tion was minimised by utilising linear mixed-effects models that
taken to indicate that attention was being directed to recruiting the allowed the removal of terms that did not strongly contribute to the
presented consumer items into the mood-repair process, which led variance in the observed data. Such removal of terms increased the
to a negative impact on memory. The results of study two provide power of the final models, reducing the probability of type two
evidence for the role of depressed mood in producing urges to buy. errors across the remaining terms. Nevertheless, the possibility
Better memory among compulsive buyers, regardless of the appeal exists when using this approach that removed terms may have
of items, for emotions linked to consumer items, compared to shown significance with a larger sample. In addition to low
concepts of function linked to items, can be understood in terms of participant numbers, the use of frequency data in the studies
the temporal associations between specific items and moods. produced floor effects. There was sufficient variability in studies
Shopping episodes may include “browsing” behaviour, with acute one and two for statistical analysis but the frequency of errors did
urges to buy then occurring and leading to purchases. The browsing not ideally follow a normal distribution. Finally, future studies
process will bring the compulsive buyer into contact with both should also attempt to control for differences in general mood, and
200 M. Kyrios et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 194e200

perhaps in hoarding behaviour, that may co-exist with differences Faber, R. J., & O’Guinn, T. C. (1992). A clinical screener for compulsive buying. Journal
of Consumer Research, 19, 459e469.
in compulsive-buying symptoms.
Frost, R. O., Kim, H., Morris, C., Bloss, C., Murray-Close, M., & Steketee, G. (1998).
Overall, the present studies support that future research of CB Hoarding, compulsive buying and reasons for saving. Behaviour Research and
could further incorporate experimental methodologies. For instance, Therapy, 36, 657e664.
future research could examine differences in cognitive and behav- Frost, R. O., Kyrios, M., McCarthy, K., & Mathews, Y. (2007). Self-ambivalence
and attachment to possessions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 21,
ioural patterns between subtypes of buyers, for example between 236e247.
individuals who are compulsive buyers vs. those who just overspend Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., & Williams, L. F. (2002). Compulsive buying, compulsive
(e.g., perhaps due to a lack of budgeting skills). Furthermore, future hoarding and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
33, 201e214.
research could experimentally manipulate “virtual” shopping or Hyler, S. E., Rieder, R. O., Williams, J. B., Spitzer, R. L., Hendler, J., & Lyons, M.
buying trips to examine the interrelations between mood and (1989). A comparison of clinical and self-report diagnoses of DSM-III
buying urges or related phenomena in more ecologically-valid personality disorders in 552 patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30,
170e178.
contexts. Such methodological advances could help overcome Kazui, H., Mori, E., Hashimoto, M., Hirono, N., Imamaura, T., Tanimukai, S., et al.
some of the limitations of questionnaire-based research. (2000). Impact of emotion on memory: controlled study of the influence of
emotionally charged material on declaritive memory in Alzheimer’s disease.
Declaration of interest British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 343e347.
Kellett, S., & Bolton, J. V. (2009). Compulsive buying: a cognitive-behavioural model.
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 16, 83e99.
All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. Koran, L. M., Faber, R. J., Aboujaoude, E., Large, M. D., & Serpe, R. T. (2006). Estimated
prevalence of compulsive buying behaviour in the United States. American
Role of funding organizations Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1806e1812.
Kulbartz-Klatt, Y. J., Florin, I., & Pook, M. (1999). Bulimia nervosa: mood changes do
have an impact on body width estimation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology,
The study was not financially supported by a sponsor. 38, 279e287.
Kyrios, M., Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (2004). Cognitions in compulsive buying and
Acknowledgements acquisition. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 28, 241e258.
Kyrios, M., Moulding, R., Arnold, F., & Ciorciari, J. Cognitive, self, and mood factors in
compulsive buying, submitted for publication.
The authors would like to thank all the individuals who Kyrios, M., Moulding, R., & McQueen, P. M. The influence of beliefs on urge to
participated in this study, and the two anonymous reviewers for acquire in compulsive buying: an experimental analysis, in preparation.
their suggestions. Kyrios, M., Moulding, R., & Zabel, P. A comparative study of compulsive buying with
clinical and non-clinical controls, submitted for publication.
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional
References states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the
Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33,
Altman, E. G., Hedeker, D., Peterson, J. L., & Davis, J. M. (1997). Altman self-rating 335e343.
mania scale. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 948e955. Miltenberger, R. G., Redlin, J., Crosby, R., Stickney, M., Mitchell, J., Wonderlich, S.,
Badcock, P. B. T., & Allen, N. B. (2003). Adaptive social reasoning in depressed mood et al. (2003). Direct and retrospective assessment of factors contributing to
and depressive vulnerability. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 647e670. compulsive buying. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
Black, D. W. (1996). Compulsive buying: a review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 57, 34, 1e9.
50e54. Mitchell, J. E. (2011). Compulsive buying disorder group treatment manual. In
Christenson, G. A., Faber, R. J., de Zwaan, M., Raymond, N. C., Specker, S. M., A. Muller, & J. E. Mitchell (Eds.), Compulsive buying: Clinical foundations and
Ekern, M. D., et al. (1994). Compulsive buying: descriptive characteristics and treatment. New York: Routledge.
psychiatric comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 55, 5e11. Mitchell, J. E., Burgard, M., Faber, R. J., Crosby, R. D., & de Zwaan, M. (2006). Cognitive
Dittmar, H. (2005a). Compulsive buying e a growing concern? An examination of behavioral therapy for compulsive buying disorder. Behaviour Research and
gender, age, and endorsement of materialistic values as predictors. British Therapy, 44, 1859e1865.
Journal of Psychology, 96, 467e491. Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-plus. New York:
Dittmar, H. (2005b). A new look at “compulsive buying”: self-discrepancies and Springer.
materialistic values as predictors of compulsive buying tendency. Journal of Pinheiro, J. C., Bates, D. M., DebRoy, S., & Sarkar, D. (2005). nlme: Linear and nonlinear
Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 832e859. mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-66.
Dittmar, H., Beattie, J., & Friese, S. (1996). Objects, decision consideration and R Development Core Team. (2005). R: A language and environment for statistical
self-image in men’s and women’s impulse purchases. Acta Psychologica, 93, computing (v. 2.2.1). Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://
187e206. www.R-project.org.
Doig, C. J., Sutherland, L. R., Sandham, J. D., Fick, G. H., Verhoef, M., & Meddings, J. B. Silberman, Y., Miikkulainen, R., & Bentin, S. (2001). Semantic effect on episodic
(1998). Increased intestinal permeability is associated with the development of associations. In J. D. Moore, & K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-third
multiple organ dysfunction syndrome in critically ill ICU patients. American annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 934e939). Mahwah, NJ:
Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 158, 444e451. Erlbaum.
Faber, R. J., & Christenson, G. (1996). In the mood to buy: differences in the mood Soto, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Heinke, D. (2006). Working memory can guide pop-
states experienced by compulsive buyers and other consumers. Psychology & out search. Vision Research, 46, 1010e1018.
Marketing, 13, 803e819. Tulving, E. (1981). Similarity relations in recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Faber, R. J., & O’Guinn, T. C. (1989). Classifying compulsive consumers: advances Verbal Behavior, 20, 479e496.
in the development of a diagnostic tool. Advances in Consumer Research, 16, Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused
738e744. attention? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 899e908.

You might also like