You are on page 1of 45

Handwriting evaluation for developmental dysgraphia:

Process versus product

Sara Rosenblum, Ph.D1, Patrice L. Weiss, Ph.D1 and Shula Parush, Ph.D2

1. Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Social Welfare & Health Studies,


University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

2. School of Occupational Therapy, The Hebrew University Faculty of Medicine, Jerusalem,


Israel

Reading and Writing, 2004, 17(5), 433-458.


Abstract

The act of writing presents difficulties for 10-30% of elementary school children. This

study’s objectives were to compare the abilities of digitizer-based evaluation of the

handwriting process and conventional evaluation of the handwriting product to discriminate

between children with proficient and dysgraphic handwriting. Copied and dictated writing

samples were collected from 3rd grade students, 50 with proficient and 50 with dysgraphic

handwriting. Results indicated that both digitizer-based and conventional evaluations

differentiated between children with proficient and dysgraphic handwriting, and that together

they provided an improved understanding of writing difficulties. Moreover, copying and

dictated writing task results significantly differed. The results demonstrate the advantages of

combining both handwriting process and product testing, and utilizing both copying and

dictation tasks, in order to achieve a more comprehensive understanding and superior

evaluation of developmental dysgraphia.

Key words: digitizer, dysgraphia, evaluation, handwriting, school-aged children

1
Introduction

Handwriting is an important means of communication that enables the expression, recording,

and transmission of ideas of students throughout their educational careers (Dennis & Swinth,

2001; Hamstra-Bletz & Blote, 1993; Tseng & Cermak, 1993). Elementary school children

typically spend up to 50% of the school day engaged in writing tasks, some of which are

performed under time constraints (Amundson & Weil, 1996; McHale & Cermak , 1992;

Tseng & Chow, 2000). A child’s ability to write legibly, as well as quickly and efficiently,

enables him or her to achieve both functional written communication and academic

advancement (Amundson & Weil, 1996; Phelps, Stempel & Speck, 1985; Tseng & Cermak,

1993; Tseng & Hsueh, 1997).

Handwriting difficulty or dysgraphia was defined by Hamstra-Bletz & Blote

(1993) as a disturbance or difficulty in the production of written language that is related to the

mechanics of writing. It has also been referred to as a specific learning disability (Brown,

1981). The problem is manifested in the inadequate performance of handwriting among

children who are of at least average intelligence and who have not been identified as having

any obvious neurological problems. Teachers have estimated that 11-12% of female students

and 21-32% of male students have handwriting difficulties (Rubin & Henderson, 1982;

Smits-Engelsman, Van Galen & Michels, 1995). Dysgraphia or poor handwriting is a

common complaint among children and adults with learning disabilities, appearing with or

without other academic difficulties (Cratty, 1994; Johnson, 1995; Waber & Bernstein, 1994),

as well as in children diagnosed with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DSM-4,

American Psychiatric Association (APA), 1994), children defined by their teachers as clumsy

(Laszlo, 1990; Laszlo, Bairstow, & Bartip, 1988) and children diagnosed with developmental

right-hemisphere syndrome (Gross-Tsur, Shalev, Manor & Amir, 1995). In fact, Cratty

(1994) found that 30-40% of the surveyed children with learning disabilities had handwriting

2
difficulties.

Several authors have suggested that difficulty in the mastery of the mechanical aspects

of handwriting may interfere with the higher order processes required for the composition of

text (Berninger & Graham, 1998). Graham (1990) found that handwriting mechanics

influence the quality and quantity of the written product. This finding is supported by

Berninger et al. (1997), who reported that handwriting performance was significantly related

to fluency and quality of composition in elementary school students. Recently, Graham,

Harris, & Fink (2000) summarized existing views on the negative consequences of

handwriting difficulties. They, along with others (Briggs, 1980; Chase, 1986; Hughes,

Keeling & Tuck, 1983), suggested that teachers tend to give higher marks for neatly written

papers than for those in which legibility is poorer. Thus, it appears that poor penmanship may

influence perceptions about children’s competence as writers. Other authors have proposed

that the act of handwriting among children with difficulties can interfere with the

simultaneous execution of composition (Graham, 1990; Scardamailia, Bereiter, & Goleman

1982). It may be that when letter production is not fully automatic, the act of handwriting

makes increased demands on memory and attentional resources, which, in turn, constrain the

higher level cognitive processes required for composition (Jones & Christensen, 1999;

Berninger & Graham, 1998). Additionally, some suggest that if handwriting is very slow,

children may forget the ideas and plans held in memory before they succeed in transferring

them to paper (Graham & Weintraub, 1996).

The most frequent complaint made regarding children with

poor/dysgraphic handwriting is in the quality of their script (Smits-Engelsman & Van Galen,

1997). Compositions and essay exams are a frequent classroom requirement and are a great

source of frustration for those struggling with the skill of handwriting. It is no wonder,

therefore, that researchers have suggested that handwriting difficulties may have serious

3
consequences for the student’s academic progress, emotional well-being and social

functioning (Cornhill & Case-Smith, 1996; Kaminsky & Powers, 1981; Martlew, 1992;

Modlinger, 1983). Hence, it reinforces the importance of identifying handwriting difficulties

as early as possible both as a preventive and as a corrective aid (Phelps & Stempel, 1988).

The process of describing the characteristics of the written output of children with

handwriting difficulties began early in the twentieth century and formed the basis for the

development of handwriting evaluation scales (Ayres, 1912; Bezzi, 1962; Feldt, 1962;

Freeman, 1959; Thorndike, 1910). The objective of these scales was to determine how best to

define the “quality”, “readability” (Ayres, 1912) or "legibility" of handwriting in specific,

measurable terms. Over the years, two main approaches were utilized for evaluating the

“readability” or "legibility" of handwriting: a global-holistic evaluation of readability (Ayres,

1912; Freeman, 1959), and an analytic evaluation of readability utilizing predetermined

criteria (Alston, 1983; Amundson, 1995; Hamstra-Bletz, De Bie & De Binker, 1987; Larsen

& Hammil, 1989; Reisman, 1993).

The global evaluation scales are based on techniques used to form an overall judgment

of a written passage, that is, how readable it is as compared to a group of standard writing

samples that have been previously graded from readable to unreadable. Dissatisfaction with

these types of scales stems from their inherent subjectivity, which places their overall

reliability into question, and from the fact that scoring the readability of a writing sample

requires an exhaustive and time consuming comparison to numerous pre-graded samples.

This approach has only limited practicality (Feldt, 1962; Freeman, 1959; Graham, 1982).

In contrast, the assumption underlying the analytic method is that a relationship exists

between the general look, the “readability” or legibility of the writing, and the quality of

specific features that characterize it. Many of these specific features are related to spatial

characteristics (e.g., the spacing of the letters and the words), while others are related to letter

4
formation (e.g., letter shape and size). The writing sample is judged by first grading each

feature individually for the entire passage and then calculating an overall score (Alston,1983;

Amundson, 1995; Erez & Parush, 1999; Hamstra-Bletz, De Bie and De Binker, 1987; Larsen

& Hammil, 1989; Phelps et al., 1985; Reisman, 1991; Stott, Moyes & Henderson, 1984;

Ziviani & Elkins, 1984).

It had been hoped that the analytically based scales would provide a satisfactory solution

to handwriting evaluation in that they are less subjective than the global-holistic scales.

However, the analytic scales are also limited in ways that hinder the development of

optimally effective handwriting assessment. Analytic scales require prolonged processing

time, lack the overall holistic perspective found in global scales, and are plagued by

methodological variations that prevent direct comparisons between them (Bonny, 1992;

Graham, 1986a, 1986b; Graham & Weintraub, 1996; Rosenblum, Weiss, & Parush, 2003;

Rubin & Henderson, 1982).

Hence, clinicians, educators, and scale development researchers continue to have

reservations regarding each of these approaches to handwriting assessment. Moreover,

neither the global nor the analytic evaluation scales are able to go beyond an analysis of the

written product in order to provide substantive information about the writing process. This

poses a significant limitation since it is believed that only a comprehensive description of the

real-time, dynamic characteristics of a child’s handwriting can provide insight into the motor

control mechanisms of normal handwriting and an understanding of the underlying pathology

of handwriting difficulties (Dobbie & Askov, 1995; Graham & Weintraub, 1996; Longstaff &

Heath, 1997; Sovik, Arntzen & Thygesen, 1987a, 1987b).

In recent years, computer, digitizer-based technology has led to the development of an

innovative approach to handwriting evaluation that is capable of analyzing the writing

process during the actual performance of a written task. Digitizer-based technology enables

5
not only the attainment of measures of the handwriting process, but also provides access to

information that the human eye is incapable of discerning. Initial results indicate that the use

of this technology reveals the presence of objective, measurable features that distinguish

between the writing of children who have and those who do not have handwriting difficulties

(Rosenblum, Parush, & Weiss, 2001; Smits-Engelsman, 1995; Smits-Engelsman ,Van Galen

& Portier, 1994a, 1994b; Smits-Engelsman, Van Galen & Shoemaker, 1998; Sovik et al.,

1987a, 1987b). The key instrument in this technology is a digitizer, an electronic surface

capable of recording x and y coordinates when used in tandem with a wireless pen and

personal computer. The digitizer/computer system is able to record kinematic and kinetic

variables as the writer engages in various writing tasks. The result is a comprehensive

collection of dynamic features that generates a more objective and precise set of handwriting

outcome measures than had been previously attainable.

Recently, several research groups have examined the writing performance of children

with handwriting difficulties via computerized systems (e.g., Rosenblum et al., 2001; Smits-

Engelsman & Van Galen, 1997; Sovik et al., 1987b). Their results have shown that poor

writers are characterized by having greater variability in temporal and spatial variables, lack

writing continuity and fluency, and pause for longer periods between strokes, letters, and

words than their peers (Sovik et al., 1987b; Wann & Jones, 1986; Wann & Kardirkamanathn,

1991; Shoemaker, Shellekens, Kalverboer, & Kooistra, 1994; Shoemaker & Smits-

Engelsman, 1997; Wann, 1987; Smits-Engelsman & Van Galen, 1997). It was also found

that children with poor/dysgraphic handwriting achieved higher absolute scores of “neuro-

motor noise” (variability of the motor output system) in comparison with more proficient

writers, and demonstrated less ability to regulate muscle force (Van Galen, Portier, Smits-

Engelsman, & Shoemaker, 1993).

Smits-Engelsman (1995) ascribed their deficits to a poor muscular initiation process

6
(Smits-Engelsman, 1995; Smits-Engelsman, Van Galen, & Shoemaker, 1998). Mojet (1991)

and Wann & Kardirkamanathan (1991) found that the writing of poor handwriters tends to

lack continuity within letter sequences and shows a wide variability in the orientation of

individually written letter segments. Further, the results of a study carried out by Smits-

Engelsman et al. (1994a) indicated that children with poor handwriting made more spatial

errors than proficient writers, leading them to conclude that poor handwriting may stem from

a problem in spatial control. Longitudinal research performed on children with handwriting

difficulties supports the view that children with dysgraphic handwriting fail to obey spatial

constraints, and that their handwriting lacks consistency. For example, the size of their letters

was inconsistent and they lacked adequate curvature in their strokes (Smits-Engelsman, &

Van Galen, 1997).

In previous research (Rosenblum, Parush & Weiss, 2003b), we used a digitizer to

compare the writing process of children with poor and proficient handwriting, through the

analysis of writing tasks that replicated the types of assignments commonly required in the

classroom (i.e., letters, words, sentences and paragraphs in both copying and dictated writing

modes). These studies introduced variables designed to illuminate both the temporal and

spatial characteristics of the handwriting process. Both the time and path lengths during

which the subjects’ pens were in contact with the writing surface (On Paper variables), as

well as the time and path lengths in which their pens were not in contact with the writing

surface (In Air variables) were recorded. Results of these studies indicated that when writing

sentences and paragraphs, the In Air time of children with poor handwriting was significantly

longer than that of proficient handwriters, demonstrating that they maintain contact with the

writing surface for less time than do their peers. Further analysis revealed that all the subjects

tended to keep their pens in motion during the In Air time, performing what was described as

a “motion tour”. Moreover, the motion tour produced by the children with poor handwriting

7
was longer and more intricate than that of those with proficient handwriting, reflected by the

significantly longer In Air time and path lengths that were measured for this group.

Thus, it can be seen that research in the 20th century has led to the advancement of

knowledge about the nature of deficient handwriting in several important areas. There now

exist a number of evaluation scales that can be used to manually assess the legibility of

various handwriting products through global-holistic and analytic means. Moreover,

technology has given us the means to perform computerized online monitoring of the

handwriting process. However, when each of these methodologies is considered by itself,

specific limitations become apparent. Although the use of conventional writing assessments

by educators and clinicians have enabled the evaluation of handwriting legibility, the

reliability and validity of such tools are, to some extent, limited due to their dependence on

subjective judgment for scoring and interpretation. On the other hand, although the

development of computerized analysis has advanced the understanding of the spatial and

temporal characteristics of children with poor and proficient handwriting, further study is still

needed to determine how these data are related to the actual functional deficits in

handwriting.

The purpose of this study was to compare the relative abilities of digitizer-based

evaluation and conventional evaluation to discriminate between the writing of children with

proficient and dysgraphic handwriting. A further purpose was to determine whether the

digitizer, through its unique ability to measure spatio-temporal handwriting variables that

cannot be discerned with the human eye, provides additional information important to the

characterization of handwriting difficulties.

Finally, a third objective was to address an issue that has, to date, received insufficient

attention, namely the effect of different writing modes (copying versus dictation). In most

studies, students’ handwriting has been evaluated via copying assignments (Ziviani & Elkins,

8
1984; Cornhill & Case-Smith, 1996), although in some cases students were required to

perform free style writing (Alston, 1983) or to write a sentence from memory (Sovik,

Arntzen, Samuelstuen, & Heggberget, 1994). This poses a problem, since children in their

every day life are confronted with the need to write under a variety of modes, including

copying, writing in response to dictation, free style writing, or writing from memory (such as

while taking notes). This becomes more significant if one considers the fact that each of these

modes relies on different underlying sensory modalities. For example, copying requires a

student to rely on visual stimuli, while dictation requires a reliance on auditory stimuli

(Fryburg, 1997). Moreover, it was found that students in grades 1 through 6 wrote more

legibly while copying than while writing free style (Graham, Berninger, & Weintraub, 1998).

Further, studies concerned with agraphia among patients with brain damage emphasized the

importance of utilizing different modes of functional writing (i.e., copying and dictation) for

a better understanding of the phenomenon (Hua, Chen, & Chu, 2001).

Method

Participants

Two groups of handwriters (proficient and dysgraphic), each consisting of 50 third grade

pupils, aged 8 and 9 years old, were recruited from eight regular public schools located in

four different types of municipalities in northern Israel (large town, small town, kibbutz and

community settlement). All participants were Caucasian Jews from either Ashkenazi or

Sephardic origin, who were born in Israel, used the Hebrew language as their primary means

of verbal and written communication, and were right hand dominant. The parents of all the

children who participated in the study gave their informed consent.

Third grade pupils were selected as the target population since the handwriting

development literature has indicated that by the time a child reaches third grade, his or her

9
handwriting has become more automatic, organized, and readily available as a tool to

facilitate the development of ideas (Berninger, Mizokawa, & Bragg, 1991; Levine, 1987;

O’Hare, 1999; Ziviani & Elkins, 1984). Thus, a deficiency in these qualities at this age may

be a sign of a problem.

The 100 participants were identified as having proficient or dysgraphic handwriting with

the aid of the standardized and validated Teachers’ Questionnaire for Handwriting

Proficiency (Rosenblum, Jessel, Adi-Japha, Parush & Weiss, 1997), completed by their

classroom teachers. The questionnaire was constructed from criteria selected from the

literature and handwriting assessments, including handwriting legibility, speed, fatigue, and

complaints of pain or discomfort while writing (Alston, 1983; Cornhill & Case-Smith, 1996;

Rubin & Henderson, 1982). Children with documented developmental delay, neurological

deficits, or physical impairment were excluded from the study. After the children were

classified into groups of proficient versus poor handwriters according to the Teachers’

Questionnaire for Handwriting Proficiency, one of the authors (SR) administered the Hebrew

Handwriting Evaluation (HHE) (Erez & Parush, 1999), a standardized, reliable and valid

handwriting assessment for the Hebrew language, to all of the children. One hundred percent

agreement between these two measures was found (i.e., the same 50 children who were

categorized as poor, and the fifty categorized as proficient writers according to the Teachers’

Questionnaire for Handwriting Proficiency were also categorized as poor - or proficient with

the HHE). Seventy percent of these children were boys, a gender bias that concurs with

clinical reports and with the literature (Rubin and Henderson, 1982; Smits -Engelsman,

Schomaker, Van Galen, & Michels, 1996).

The children with proficient handwriting were matched to the participants in the

dysgraphic handwriting group, on the basis of gender, age, school, and grade. For each child

in the dysgraphic handwriting group, a matched control participant was chosen from his or

10
her classroom peers, and was taught by the same classroom teacher. Thus, there were no

differences between the two groups with respect to their age (8.68 ± 0.27 years for the

children with proficient handwriting and 8.61 ± 0.35 years for the children with dysgraphic

handwriting) and gender ratio (30% girls versus 70% boys in both groups).

Instruments

Digitizing Tablet and On-line Data Collection and Analysis Software

A unique data collection and analysis software program, developed specifically for the

current study, including a suite of on-line, computerized tasks programmed in C++ and

implemented via Matlab software toolkits, was used to administer the stimuli and to collect

and analyze the data. The writing tasks were performed on A4 size lined paper affixed to the

surface of a WACOM (407 X 417 X 36.3 mm) x-y digitizing tablet using a wireless

electronic pen with a pressure sensitive tip (model Up 401). Pen size and weight were similar

to those of normal pens (length=150mm, circumference=35mm, weight=11gm). The paper

was lined (spacing=0.5 cm). Displacement, pressure, and pen tip angle were sampled at 150

Hz via a 90 MHz Pentium laptop computer.

The Hebrew Handwriting Evaluation (HHE) (Erez & Parush, 1999)

The HHE was used to examine the handwriting product, assessing legibility through both

global and analytic measures. It contains one text for assessing writing performed through a

copying mode and a separate text for assessing writing performed through dictation. Both

texts are of the same level of difficulty and appropriate for third graders. In addition, each

text contains all the letters in the Hebrew alphabet, and included the same number of words

(30) and letters (107) (Erez & Parush, 1999). The inter-rater reliability of the HHE is r = .75-

.79; p < .001. Construct validity of the HHE has been established by demonstrating statistical

11
significant differences (t = -2.34; p = .027) between the performance of children with

proficient and poor handwriting (Dvash, Levi, Traub, & Shapiro, 1995). The standardized

texts from the Hebrew Handwriting Evaluation (Erez & Parush, 1999) were also chosen in

the current study as the paragraph writing tasks to be analyzed by the digitizer, to enhance the

fidelity between conventional (HHE) and the digitizer-based approaches.

All the 100 handwriting products were analyzed by the same evaluator .The evaluator

was blind as to the group membership of each child. The evaluator was certified in HHE

administration having completed a course for this purpose that was conducted by the tool

developers. As part of the certification process, inter-rater reliability of the course participants

was demonstrated (r = .75 –.79, p < .01).

Outcome Measures

The primary outcome measures included temporal and spatial measures of handwriting

kinematics. The temporal measures included the total time taken to complete the writing

tasks, On Paper time, and In Air time (i.e., the time during the writing of the paragraph that

the pen was not in contact with the writing surface). The spatial measures included the total

path length of all the characters written in the paragraph, the On Paper path length, and the In

Air path length (i.e., the excursion of the pen when it was not in contact with the writing

surface while writing the paragraph).

The outcome measures of the HHE assessment of the written product included global

legibility (scored on a 4 point Likert scale, from the most legible [1] to the least legible [4]),

which refers to the overall clarity of the handwriting. The analytic measurement of legibility

used in the HHE examined the following three variables:

1. Letters erased and/or overwritten – the number of letters that were erased and/or written

over.

12
2. Unrecognizable letters – the total number of letters that could not be recognized due to the

quality of letter closure, rounding of letters, or letter reversals.

3. Spatial arrangement of the written text, as determined according to detailed and precise

criteria, utilizing a caliper that is calibrated to the millimeter. Specifically, these criteria

included vertical alignment of letters (including the extensions of letters above and below the

lines), the spacing of words and letters (whether too wide or overlapping), and letter size. The

minimum score for spatial arrangement is 9, and the maximum is 24. For all of the four

outcome measures of the HHE, a low score indicates good performance and a high score

indicates poor performance.

Procedure

All participants were tested individually under similar environmental conditions. Specifically,

testing occurred in a quiet classroom in their school during the morning hours. In addition, in

an effort to achieve writing samples that would resemble those typically produced by the

subjects, all environmental factors were kept as similar as possible to writing conditions that

the child would normally experience. Thus, the participant was seated on a standard school

chair and in front of a school desk, appropriate to his or her height.

Paragraph copying and dictation tasks were selected to represent handwriting tasks in

which a child would typically engage. The copying task was presented visually on the screen

in Hebrew font type Gutman Yad-Brush size 20 point (shown in Figure 1). The content of

the dictation task was read to the child by the test administrator, who paced her reading in

accordance with the participant's writing speed. As shown in Table 2, the mean dictation time

for the proficient writers was 124.45 ± 29.49 s and for poor writers was 208.95 ± 78.35 s.

The tasks were written on normal writing paper with printed lineature, which was affixed to

the digitizing tablet. Each subject was instructed in the same fashion about what he or she

would be required to do. The testing for this part of the study took approximately 15

13
minutes; the subjects had previously engaged in other handwriting tasks for an additional 25

minutes (see Rosenblum et al. (2001) for a full description). The same tester carried out all

computerized data collection sessions.

With respect to the data analysis, MANOVA procedures were used to examine group

differences across the dependent conventional variables of the handwriting product (e.g.,

subjective legibility, unrecognizable letters), and the digitizer-based variables of the

handwriting process (e.g., total time/length, In Air time/length), for each writing task (e.g.,

paragraph copying versus dictation). To examine the source of significance, the data from

each task were subjected to univariate ANOVAs. Discriminant analysis was conducted in

order to determine which of both the conventional and digitizer-based variables were the best

predictors of group membership (i.e., proficient and dysgraphic) in each of the two kinds of

tasks (copying or dictation). Pearson correlations were calculated in order to check for

associations between the conventional and digitizer-based measures and between copying and

dictation performance of all the 100 children. An effect size (Eta squared) was provided for

the cases where p-values were reported.

Results

Prior to a detailed presentation of the statistical analysis, we have included some samples of

paragraphs written by typical participants from each group. In Figure 1, two representative

samples of the copying paragraph task are shown. Figure 1A shows how this paragraph

appeared on the computer screen to participants. The lower two panels of the figure show

two samples of the same paragraph as it was written by a child with proficient (1B) and a

child with dysgraphic (1C) handwriting. Heavy lines represent the actual trajectory of the

child’s pen when in contact with the writing surface, and thin lines show the In Air trajectory,

i.e., when the pen was above the writing surface. A visual examination of the extent of these

14
thin line trajectories (indicating the In Air path length) reveals that the trajectory, and hence

the time spent In Air by the child with dysgraphic handwriting, was considerably greater than

that of the child with proficient handwriting.

< Fig. 1 here>

The first group of variables to be analyzed were the conventional measures obtained

via the Hebrew Handwriting Evaluation (HHE) (see Table 1). The MANOVA applied to

these four variables (global legibility, letters erased and/or overwritten, unrecognizable letters

and spatial arrangement) yielded statistically significant differences between the children

with proficient and the dysgraphic handwriting for both the copying task (F(4, 95) = 13.57, p

< .001) and for the dictation task (F(4, 95) = 57.67 p < .001). Univariate ANOVAs, used to

examine the source of the significance, showed that children with proficient handwriting

received significantly lower scores (i.e. performed better) in all the four variables than did the

children with dysgraphic handwriting, for both the copying and dictation tasks.

< Table 1 here>

The second group of measures included the spatial and temporal variables obtained from the

computerized digitizer system (see table 2). As shown previously, the six variables obtained

via the digitizer (total length, On Paper length, In Air length, total time, On Paper time, and

In Air time) yielded statistically significant differences between the children with proficient

and the dysgraphic handwriting for both the copying (F(5, 91) = 9.82, p <.0001) (Rsoenblum,

Parush, & Weiss, 2003a, 2003b). These same variables also differentiated between the two

groups for the dictation tasks (F(5, 93) = 11.96 p < .0001). When the data from each variable

were subjected to univariate ANOVAs, results showed that children with proficient

handwriting required significantly less total time, On Paper time, and In Air time for both the

copying and dictation tasks, than did the children with dysgraphic handwriting. Moreover,

they had shorter total, On Paper and In Air path lengths during both the copying and dictation

15
tasks than did the children with poor handwriting.

< Table 2 here>

Discriminant analysis was conducted to determine whether the eight variables (four

conventional and four digitizer based measures) could predict group membership (i.e.,

children with proficient versus children with dysgraphic handwriting). Results indicated that

the discriminant function was found to be significant for both the copying (F(8, 87) = 9.95, p

< .0001) and the dictation tasks (F(8, 89) = 25.96, p = .0001) (see Table 3). Amongst the

conventional variables, the highest predictor was the subjective legibility variable during the

dictation task. Amongst the digitizer-based measures, the highest predictor was In Air time

during the copying task. In contrast, On Paper length was the lowest predictor.

< Table 3 here >

When the data from all the participants were analyzed, significant correlations, ranging from

.31 to .65, were found between all four conventional measures and three of the digitizer based

measures (In Air time, In Air length and On Paper time), for both copying and dictation tasks

(see Table 4.1). However, when correlations between the conventional and digitizer-based

measures were calculated separately for each study group (Table 4.2 versus Table 4.3),

results indicated that the majority of significant correlations were found to exist amongst the

dysgraphic writers.

< Table 4 here >

Significant and high correlations between copying and dictation writing tasks, ranging from

.54 to .86, were found for all of the digitizer-based measures (see Table 4). In general, the

values of the correlations were higher for the digitizer-based measures than for the

conventional measures.

< Table 5 here >

The results of copying and dictation performance of the proficient and dysgraphic

16
handwriters were previously presented in tables 1 and 2. As a second phase, a

MANOVA was used to compare the results of the conventional and digitizer-based measures

between the copying and dictation for each of the study groups. Results indicated that no

significance differences (F(4, 96) = 0.54, p < .71) were found in the conventional measures

between copying and dictation tasks within the proficient group, but significant differences

were found in the dysgraphic group (F(4, 96) = 24.61 p < .0001). To examine the source of

these significant differences in the dysgraphic group, the data from each variable were

subjected to univariate ANOVAs. The results indicated that the children with dysgraphic

handwriting performed significantly better (i.e., achieved lower scores) for subjective

legibility and spatial arrangement during the copying task (p < .01, partial eta squared is .37

for subjective legibility and .58 for spatial arrangement). In contrast, both children with

proficient and dysgraphic handwriting performed significantly differently when copying and

when writing from dictation, for all the temporal and spatial digitizer based measures (p <

.01, Partial eta squared ranging between .29 - .63)

Discussion

The current study represents one of the first attempts to utilize digitizer-based data as an

adjunct to conventional handwriting assessment in order to examine the contribution of each

method to the identification and characterization of dysgraphic handwriting. This study also

examines the importance of evaluating handwriting in both copying and dictation writing

modes. In order to address these goals we began by examining whether both conventional

and digitizer-based handwriting measures used in this study differentiated significantly

between dysgraphic and proficient handwriting, and to what extent they complement each

other in the evaluation process.

The fact that the conventional measures used in the current study differentiated

significantly between dysgraphic and proficient handwriting is not trivial. Researchers over

17
the years have not been satisfied that the criteria that have been relied on thus far truly

represent the critical components of handwriting legibility nor comprise the optimal way to

measure them (Bonny, 1992; Daniel & Froude, 1998; Graham, 1986b; Phelps et al., 1985;

Reisman, 1993; Rubin & Henderson, 1982). This sentiment is echoed by Dennis & Swinth’s

(2001) recent statement that although legibility is certainly a crucial component of written

communication it remains quite difficult to define. In addition, a review of handwriting

evaluation scales (Rosenblum et al., 2003) reveals that although many handwriting evaluation

developers have attempted to define the characteristics of handwriting that determine

legibility, they have not yet been shown to discriminate sufficiently well between dysgraphic

and proficient handwriting. For example, the BHK scale (Hamstra Bletz, De Bie, & De

Binker, 1987) is an evaluation tool that has been found to discriminate between children with

dysgraphic and non-dysgraphic handwriting. Yet, further research indicated that 25% of the

items used in the original BHK to measure different aspects of handwriting legibility were

eliminated in a revised edition, since they were found not to discriminate sufficiently well

between dysgraphic and non-dysgraphic handwriting (Hamstra-Belt and Blote, 1993). The

items that were found to discriminate well between these groups were those that examined

various aspects of letter formation (poor letter alignment, acute turns in connecting joins to

letters, irregularities in joins, absence of joins, collisions of letters and inconsistent letter

size), word alignment and word spacing.

Our results show that the conventional criteria used in this study (i.e., global

legibility, letter erased or overwritten, unrecognizable letters, and spatial arrangement) did

successfully discriminate between dysgraphic and proficient handwriting. These criteria are

congruent with those used in previous studies. In the Hebrew Handwriting Evaluation, Erez

and Parush (1999) demonstrated significant differences between children with poor and

proficient handwriting using these same criteria (Sary, Parush, & Jarus, 1996; Lifshietz &

18
Parush,1993; Pindack & Parush, 1996). Graham, Boyer-Schick, & Tippets, (1989) found that

almost two-thirds of the legibility scores of students with writing difficulties on the Test of

Legible Handwriting criteria (TOLH; Larsen & Hammil, 1989), were due to problems in

letter shaping, spacing and organization. According to the BHK researchers (Hamstra-Belt

and Blote, 1993), dysgraphic writing can be characterized by a consistently lower quality of

individual spatial writing features. These include inconsistent letter size, acute turns in the

letters and joints, relatively uneven and unsteady writing, and sudden changes in size and

direction of letter writing. Graham, Weintraub, & Berninger (2001) found that letter

legibility made a significant contribution to the prediction of text legibility.

Having confirmed that all of the conventional and the digitizer based measures were

significantly different for dysgraphic versus proficient handwriting, discriminant analysis was

then used to determine to what extent the conventional and digitizer based variables

discriminated between them. The results indicated that global legibility discriminated best.

The fact that conventional measures discriminated so well between proficient and dysgraphic

handwriting supports the importance of continuing to include conventional handwriting

assessments in the process of evaluating children’s handwriting. Furthermore, it is interesting

to observe that among the conventional measures, including analytical handwriting legibility

criteria, global legibility discriminated best. The fact that some handwriting researchers over

the past five years have returned to collecting data on handwriting quality through the use of

global legibility ratings by classroom teachers seems to reinforce the importance of our

finding that conventional measures of global legibility are highly effective. Dennis and

Swinthe's (2001) suggestion that it is easier to achieve agreement between testers when using

overall legibility scores than when measuring handwriting quality through analytic legibility

criteria provides further support for our results.

19
Results of the analysis of the digitizer based data in the current study revealed that the

child’s pencil ‘traveled’ above the writing surface between the writing of successive character

segments, letters and words (the In Air phenomena), for a significantly longer period of time

and trajectory among the non-proficient handwriters than among the proficient writers

(Rosenblum et al., 2003b). These results may provide clues as to the underlying difficulties

that limit the performance of children with dysgraphic handwriting during the process of

writing and that are manifested in the quality of their written products. This contention is

supported by the fact that all four conventional measures were signifcantly correlated with

the digitizer based spatio-temporal (In Air length and In Air time) measures, mainly among

the children with dysgraphic handwriting. That is, the poorer the score a child received for

global legibility, the longer the child’s measured In Air time and length were found to be.

This relationship was considerably less among the children with proficient handwriting.

Therefore, one may speculate that these phenomena are an expression of the reduced

continuity, consistency, and efficiency of writing movements that characterize the writing

performance of children with dysgraphia, as described in earlier clinical (Ziviani & Elkins,

1984) and digitizer studies (Mojet, 1991; Smits-Engelsman & Van Galen, 1997; Van Galen et

al.,1993; Wann, 1987; Wann & Kardirkamanathan, 1991). These results are quite intriguing

since they demonstrate that digitizer technology has the unique capability to reveal some of

the perceptual-motor processes underlying handwriting that would not be apparent from

classical handwriting evaluations.

The literature provides support for our suggestion that the In Air phenomenon may be

an expression of the underlying processing difficulties experienced by children with

dysgraphic handwriting. Stott, Henderson & Moyes (1987) wrote that the graphic production

of a letter involves two processes. The first process is the formation of a cognitive (or visual)

schema of the letter, including knowledge of its defining features and recognition of its

20
variant forms. The second process involves the development of a motor schema of the letter

by which the visual picture is translated into a sequence of movements. The failure to form

letters may result from a deficiency in either of these processes. A different explanation

offered by Eidlitz & Simner (1999), is that the form errors characterizing the writing of

children with dysgraphic handwriting might result from a poorly developed or unstable

memory image which gradually fades and then disappears as children print.

Our data, provided by the digitizer, is not sufficient to determine which of the above

explanations is correct; rather it allowed us to document the In Air phenomena occurring

between letters and letter strokes, and revealed its consistent appearance in both copying and

dictation modes of handwriting. Clearly, further research is needed to determine the actual

relationship between the In Air phenomena and the underlying mechanisms that contribute to

dysgraphia. Indeed such research may contribute to the formulation of new and improved

educational strategies to help dysgraphic children, as well as determine the effectiveness of

such strategies. For example, consider a child whose performance on a conventional

assessment indicates a tendency to have difficulty producing certain letterforms, through

repeated erasures or overwriting of the letters. If conventional evaluation was followed-up by

a digitizer-based evaluation that revealed prolonged ‘motion tours’, similar to those observed

in this study, it would provide the clinician with clues as to the nature of the child's hesitation,

and lack of sufficient internalization of these letterforms. Such detailed data, provided by the

digitizer, would demonstrate clearly the child's need to spend extra time, energy, and

attentional reserves in contemplating how to write and erase that which has not been formed

correctly. This type of pictorial representation of the child's difficulties in writing could do

more than help focus remediation efforts; it could also be used to illustrate the existing

problem to the child, his parents and teachers, as well as provide effective documentation of

improvement in handwriting.

21
The process of using digitizer-based data to discover clues regarding the underlying

mechanisms responsible for clinically manifested behaviors has begun to be appreciated in

other areas of research as well. For example, in the field of gait analysis, the physical

therapist typically uses conventional visual analysis to assess the quality of gait performance

in clinical practice (Halligan & Wade, 1998). In contrast, computerized gait analysis is

occasionally available to provide clinicians with a quantitative analysis of temporal-spatial

parameters (e.g. gait kinematics and kinetics). Such computerized analysis systems enable the

objective measurement of phenomena that the human eye cannot discern, but is less suitable

for providing an overall judgment of the quality of gait (Halligan & Wade, 1998). As in the

current study, researchers have discovered correlations between the objective computerized

and the conventional visual gait analysis measures (Damiano & Abel, 1996; O’Sullivan, Said,

Dillon, Hofman, & Hughes, 1998; Schwartz, Novacheck & Trost, 2000). The computerized

assessment procedures aim to provide a deeper understanding of the basic causes and

mechanics of abnormal gait, whereas the clinically oriented assessments are more suited to

the resolution of issues concerned with the global characteristics of gait. Such considerations

prompted Mulder, Nienhuis & Pauwels (1998) to argue that both conventional observation as

well as computerized gait analysis are necessary for a comprehensive characterization and

understanding of human gait. Thus, based on findings that seem to parallel those derived

from our study, the position of gait analysis scientists reinforces the conclusions we have

stated in terms of the evaluation of handwriting. That is, the use of a combination of

conventional and computerized objective methods may lead to a better understanding of the

handwriting process and product of children with proficient and dysgraphic handwriting.

Our study also served the purpose of examining the importance of including both

copying and dictation tasks in the evaluation of handwriting. This information would be

quite useful to educators since most handwriting tests targeting children with developmental

22
dysgraphia evaluate writing in only one mode or the other, despite the fact that school

children are required to perform handwriting tasks in both modes.

This question was examined in two ways. First, a correlational matrix was used to assess the

extent of convergence between the variables tested. This was done to determine the degree to

which the same measure, under two different conditions (copying and dictation) were

correlated. As was observed, the digitizer based measures in the copying mode correlated

with high magnitude with the corresponding measures in the dictation mode. This appears to

indicate that quantitative handwriting measures used in this study test the construct no matter

which mode is used, suggesting that testing in one mode would be sufficient. However, the

correlations between the corresponding conventional measures in copying versus dictation

modes were only moderate, suggesting that the variables being measured are related but are

expressions of different underlying mechanisms depending on the mode being used. The

conclusion that ensues from this analysis supports the importance of looking at both modes of

writing.

The second way in which the need for testing in both writing modes was investigated

was through the use of MANOVA and Univariate analyses. Significant differences were

found between copying and dictation performance for two of the conventional measures in

the dysgraphic group and for all of the digitizer based measures in both groups. No

significant differences were found between the conventional measures of the proficient

writer’s group. These results clearly support the fact that handwriting should be tested in both

modes, especially when using quantitative measures.

One can speculate that the statistical differences in results between copying and

dictation tasks resulted from the fact that each writing mode may entail the use of different

underlying perceptual-motor processes. Melvin and Levine (1993) provided support for this

contention when they stated that in a copying task, the child only needs the ability to imitate,

23
while in a dictation task the child needs the ability to access the graphic representation of

letters and words. This may provide an explanation as to why the scores achieved on the

conventional measures in the current study were better in the dictation task, and why the

digitizer based In Air time and length measurements were longer in the copying task.

Although significant differences were found between writing modes for the children in the

dysgraphic writer’s group, it was not found for the conventional measures among the children

in the proficient writer’s group, suggesting that proficient writers do not need to be examined

in both modes. In light of the results of our study, further research is recommended to expand

on the applications and possibilities offered by using both dictation and copying modes in

handwriting evaluation and remediation.

Conclusions

The findings of this study serve to expand on the information provided by previous

digitizer handwriting research and indicate that a relationship exists between the conventional

appearance of the written product of children with dysgraphia and quantitative digitizer

measures (In Air time and length). Pooled results from both conventional and digitizer based

methods may provide information related to the problematic underlying processing

mechanisms of these children. In addition, our findings provide concrete data indicating the

importance of utilizing both copying and dictation tasks in the evaluation of children’s

handwriting. These results have broad implications for professionals in the educational and

clinical fields who are responsible for the identification and remediation of handwriting

problems in children. More research is needed aimed at the development of techniques

incorporating the use of computerized data in order to establish a comprehensive and

systematic evaluation system for children with dysgraphic handwriting of all ages, based on a

combination process of examining both handwriting process and product.

24
Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Esther Adi-Japha for her creative work in designing and

programming the data collection and analysis program. We also thank Yaron Segal for

development of the Matlab analysis modules, Gil Rosenzweig for assistance with the research

design and statistical analysis, and Sarina Goldstand for editorial assistance. Financial

support from the Israeli Ministry of Education is gratefully acknowledged

25
References

Alston, J. (1983). A legibility index: Can handwriting be measured? Educational Review

35, 237-242.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (4th ed.) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Amundson, S. J. (1995). Evaluation Tool of Children's Handwriting: ETCH, Examiner's

Manual. Homer: O.T. Kids.

Amundson, S.J., & Weil, M. (1996). Prewriting and handwriting skills. In J. Case-Smith,

A.S. Allen, & P.N. Pratt (Eds.)Occupational therapy for children (pp. 524-541).

St, Louis, MI; Mosby-year book.

Ayres, L. (1912). A Scale for Measuring the Quality of Handwriting of School Children.

Russel Sage Foundation, New York

Berninger,V., & Graham, S. (1998). Language by hand: a synthesis of a decade of

research on handwriting. Handwriting Review 12, 11-25.

Berninger, V., Mizokawa, D., & Bragg, R. (1991). Theory-based diagnosis and

remediation of writing. Journal of School Psychology 29, 57-59.

Berninger, V.W., Vaughan, K.B., Abbott, R.D., Abbott, S.P., Woodruff Rogan, Brooks,

A., Reed, E., & Graham,S. (1997) Treatment of handwriting problems in

beginning writers:Transfer from handwriting to composition. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 89, 652- 666.

Bezzi, R. (1962). A standarized manuscript scale for grades 1, 2, and 3. Journal of

Educational Research 55, 339-340.

Bonny, A. M. (1992). Understanding and assessing handwriting difficulties: Perspective

from the literature. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal 39, 7-15.

Briggs, D. (1980). A study of the influence of handwriting upon grades using

26
examination scripts. Educational Review, 32, 185-193.

Brown, J.K. (1981).Learning Disabilities: A pediatric neurologist’s point of view,

Transactions of the College of Medicine of South Africa. December, 49-104.

Chase, C. (1986). Essay test scoring: Interaction of relevant variables. Journal of

Educational Measurement 23, 33-41.

Cornhill, H., & Case-Smith, J. (1996). Factors that relate to good and poor Handwriting.

American Journal of Occupational Therapy 50, 732-739.

Cratty, B.J. (1994). Clumsy child syndromes, descriptions, evaluation and remediation.

(pp. 143-144). USA: Harwood academic.

Damiano, D. L., & Abel, M. F. (1996). Relation of gait analysis to gross motor function

in cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine and child Neurology 38, 389-396.

Daniel, M. E., & Froude, E. (1998). Reliability of occupational therapist and

teacher evaluations of handwriting quality of grade 5 and 6 primary school

children. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal 45, 48-58.

Dennis, J. L., & Swinth, Y. (2001). Pencil grasp and children’s handwriting

Legibility during different length writing tasks. The American Journal of

Occupational Therapy 55, 171- 183.

Dobbie, L., & Askov, E. N. (1995). Progress of handwriting research in the 1980s

and future prospects. The Journal of Educational Research 88, 339-351.

Dvash, L. E., Levi, M., Traub, R., & Shapiro, M. (1995). Reliability and validity of

the Hebrew handwriting Evaluation. Israel, Jerusalem: School of Occupational

Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, Hebrew University (Unpublished Thesis).

27
Eidlitz, M. R., & Simner, M. (1999). The nature of dysgraphic handwriting in grade-1

children. In G. L. Leedham, V. Sagar, & X. Xuhong ( Eds.) Proceedings of the

ninth biennial conference of the International Graphonomics Society (pp. 171-

175).The netherlands: Manigary technological University.

Erez, N., & Parush, S. (1999). The Hebrew Handwriting Evaluation (2nd ed.). Israel,

Jerusalem: School of Occupational Therapy. Faculty of Medicine. Hebrew

University of Jerusalem.

Feldt, L. S. (1962). The reliability of measures of handwriting quality. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 53, 288-299.

Freeman, F. N. (1959). A new handwriting scale. Elementary School Journal 59,

218-221.

Fryburg, E. L. (1997). Reading and learning disability. (p. 197). Springfield,

Illiniois: Charles Thomas.

Graham, S. (1982). Measurement of handwriting skills: A critical review.

Diagnostique, 8, 32-42.

Graham, S. (1986a). A review of handwriting scales and factors that contribute to

variability in handwriting scores. Journal of School Psychology 24, 63-72.

Graham, S. (1986b). The reliability, validity, and utility of three handwriting

measurement procedures. Journal of Educational Research 79: 373-380.

Graham,S. (1990). The role of production factors in learning disabled students’ compositions.

Journal of Educational Psychology 82, 781-791.

Graham, S., Berninger, V.W., & Weintraub, N. (1998). The relationship between

Handwriting style and speed and legibility. The Journal of Educational Research

5, 290-296.

28
Graham, S., Boyer-Schick, K., & Tippets, E. (1989). The validity of handwriting scale from

the Test of Written Language. Journal of Educational Research 82, 166-171.

Graham, S., Harris, K.R., & Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting causally related to learning to

write? Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers. Journal of

Educational Psychology 92, 620-633.

Graham, S., & Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and

prospects from 1980-1994. Educational Psychology Review 8, 7-86

Graham, S., Weintraub, N., & Berninger, V. (2001). Which manuscript letters do

Primary grade children write legibly.Journal of Educational Psychology 3, 488-

497.

Gross-Tsur,V.,Shalev,R.S., Manor,O. & Amir,N.(1995). Developmental right-

hemisphere syndrom;Clinical spectrum of the nonverbal learning disability.

Journal of Learning Disability 28, 80-86.

Halligan, P.W., & Wade, D.T. (1998). Visual gait analysis: The development of a

Clinical Assessment and scale. Clinical Rehabilitation 12, 107-119.

Hamstra- Bletz, L., DeBie, J., & Den Brinker, B. (1987). Concise Evaluation Scale for

children’s handwriting. Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger.

Hamstra-Bletz, L. & Blote, A. (1993). A longitudinal study on dysgraphic handwriting in

primary school. Journal of Learning Disability 26, 689-699.

Hua, M.S., Chen S.T., & Chu, Y.C. (2001). Chinese writing function in patients with left

Versus right putaminal hemorrhage. Journal of clinical and experimental

neuropsychology 23, 372-385.

Hughes, D.C., Keeling, B., & Tuck, B. F. (1983). Effects of achievement expectations

and handwriting quality on scoring essays. Journal of Educational Measurement

20, 65-70.

29
Johnson, D.J. (1995). An overview of learning disabilities: Psychoeducational

perspectives. Journal of Child Neurology 10, S2-S8.

Jones, D., & Christensen, C.A. (1999). Relationship between automaticity in handwriting

and student’s ability to generate written text. Journal of Educational Psychology

91, 44-49.

Kaminsky, S., & Powers, R. (1981). Remediation of handwriting difficulties, a

practical approach. Academic Therapy 17, 19-25.

Larsen, S. C., & Hammill, D. P. (1989). Test of legible handwriting. Pro-Ed., Austin, TX

Laszlo, J. I. (1990). Child perceptuo-motor development; Normal and abnormal

development of skilled behaviour. In C. A. Hauert (Ed.) Developmental

Psychology: Cognitive, perceptuo-motor and neurophysiological perspective.

Advances in psychology (pp. 273-308). Amsterdam: North Holland.

Laszlo,J.I., Bairstow, P.J., & Bartip, J.(1988). A new approach to treatment of

perceptuo-motor dysfunction: previously called clumsiness. Support for

Learning 3, 35-40.

Levine, M. (1987). Developmental variation and learning disorders. Cambridge, MA:

Educators Publishing.

Lifshietz, N., & Parush, S. (1996) Screening Assessment for Children with Handwriting

Deficits. Unpublished Thesis, Jerusalem: The Hebrew university

Longstaff, M.G., & Heath, RA.(1997) Space-time invariance in adult handwriting.

Acta Psychologica 97, 201-214.

Martlew, M. (1992). Handwriting & spelling: Dyslexic children’s abilities compared

children of the same chronological age and younger children of the same spelling

level. British Journal of Educational Psychology 62, 375-390.

McHale, K., & Cermak, S. A.(1992). Fine motor activities in elementary school:

30
Preliminary findings and provisional implications for children with fine motor

problems. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 46, 898-892.

Melvin, D., & Levine, M.D. (1993). Developmental variation and learning disorders

(pp.308-345). Cambridge, MA: Educators Pub.

Modlinger, I. (1983). Disorders of Written Language: Diagnosis and Remedial Teaching.

Tel Aviv; Sifriyat Hapoalim.

Mojet, J. W. (1991). Characteristics of the developing handwriting skill in elementary

education. In J. Wann, A.M. Wing, & N. Sovik. Development of Graphic Skills.

(pp. 53-74). London: Academic Press.

Mulder, T., Nienhuis, B., & Pauwels, J. (1998). Clinical gait analysis in a rehabilitation

context: some controversial issues. Clinical Rehabilitation, 12, 99-106.

O'Hare, A. (1999). Dysgraphia and dyscalculia. In K. Whitmore., H. Hart., & G.

Willems. A neurodevelopmental approach to specific learning disabilities. (pp.

96-119).Cambridge: Mac Keith Press.

O’Sullivan, J. D., Said, C.M., Dillon, L.C., Hoffman, M., & Hughes, A.J.(1998). Gait

analysis in patients with Parkinson’s disease and motor fluctuations: influence

of levodopa and comparison with other measures of motor function. Movement

disorders 13, 900-906.

Phelps, J., Stempel, L., & Speck, G. (1985). The children’s handwriting scale: A new

diagnostic tool. Journal of Educational Research 79, 46-50.

Phelps, J., & Stempel, L. (1988). The Children’s Handwriting Evaluation Scale for

Manuscript Writing. Reading Improvement 25, 247-255.

Pindak, V., & Parush, S. (1996). The Connection between Motor Programming and

31
the ability of Finger Identify - to Handwriting Quality of Third Grade children.

Unpublished Thesis. Israel, Jerusalem: Faculty of Medicine, Hebrew University,

Jerusalem.

Reisman, J. E. (1991). Poor handwriting, who is refered?. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy 45, 849-852.

Reisman, J. E. (1993). Development and reliability of the research version of the

Minessota Handwriting Test. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics


13, 41-55.
Rosenblum, S., Jessel, A., Adi-Japha, E., Parush, S. and Weiss, P.L. (1997). Quantitative

characteristics of normal and dysgraphic children in Hebrew script. Proceedings

of the Ninth Biennial Conference of the International Graphonomics Society. (pp.

71-72) Italy, Genova.

Rosenblum, S., Parush, S., & Weiss, P., (2001). Temporal measures of poor and

proficient handwriters. In R.G.J. Meulenbroek, & B. Steenbergen (Eds.),

Proceedings of the Tenth biennial conference of the International Graphonomics

Society.( pp. 119-125) .The Netherlands, University of

Nijmegen; IGS Pub.Rosenblum,

Rosenblum, S., Parush, S. and Weiss, P.L. (2003a). Computerized temporal handwriting

characteristics of proficient and poor handwriters. The American Journal of

Occupational Therapy 57, 129-138.

Rosenblum, S., Parush, S. and Weiss, P.L. (2003b). The In Air phenomenon: temporal

and spatial correlates of the handwriting process. Perceptual and Motor skills

96, 933-954.

Rosenblum, S., Weiss, P.L. and Parush, S. (2003). Product and process evaluation of

handwriting difficulties: A review. Educational Psychology Review 15, 41-81.

Rubin, N., & Henderson, S. E. (1982). Two sides of the same coin: Variation in

32
teaching methods and failure to learn to write. Special Education: Forward Trends

9, 17-24.

Sary, T., Parush, S., & Jarus, T. (1996). Handwriting quality and kinesthetic function of

children with and without handwriting difficulty. Unpublished Thesis, School of

Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, Hebrew University, Jerusalem: Israel.

Scardamailia, M., Bereiter, C., & Goleman, H. (1982). The role of production factors

In writing ability. In M. Nystrand, (Ed’) What writers know: The language

process and structure of written Discourse. (pp. 173-210). N.Y: Academic

Press.

Schoemaker, M. M., Shellekens, J. M. H., Kalverboer, A. F, & Kooistra, L. (1994). Pattern

drawing by clumsy children: a problem of movement control. In M. L. Simner, W.

Hulstujin, & P. Girouard, (Eds.). Contemporary issues in the forensic, developmental,

and neurological aspects of handwriting. (pp 45-64) (Monograph of the Association

of Forensic Document Examiners, Vol 1). Toronto, Canada: Association Forensic

Document Examiners

Shoemaker, M. M., and Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M. (1997). Dysgraphic children with

and without a generalized motor problem: Evidence for subtypes?. In F. Colla,

Masulli, & P. Morasso, (Eds.), IGS 1997 proceedings: Eight Binnial Conference. The

International Graphonomics Society. (pp.11-12). The Netherlands Nijmegen: IGS

Schwartz, M. H., Novacheck, T. F., & Trost, J. (2000). A tool for quantifying hip flexor

function during gate. Gait posture 12, 122 –127.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M. (1995). Theory-based diagnosis of fine-motor coordination

development and deficiencies using handwriting tasks. PhD Thesis. The

Netherlands: University of Nijmegen.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Schomaker, M. M., Van Galen, G. P., & Michels, C. J. M.

33
(1996). Physiotherapy for children’s writing problems: Evaluation study. In

M.L. Simner, C.G. Leedham, & A.J.W.M Thomassen (Eds.). Handwriting and

Drawing Research: Basic and applied issues. (pp. 227-240). Amsterdam: IOS

press.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., & Van Galen, G .P. (1997). Dysgraphia in children: Lasting

psychomotor deficiency or transient developmental delay? Journal of Experimental

Child Psychology 67, 164-184.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Van Galen, G. P., & Michels, C. G. J. (1995). De leekrach

beoordeeld: Inschatting van schrijfvaardigheidsproblemen en motorisch achterstand

bij basisschool leerlingen. (prevalence of poor handwriting and the

validity of estimation of motor proficiency and handwriting performance by

teachers). Tijdschrift voor Onderwijsresearch 20, 1-15.

Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M., Van Galen, G.P., & Michels, C.G.J. (1995). Teachers

assessment of the motor proficiency level and motor writing skills of elementary

school pupils. In B.C.M. Smits-Engelsman . Theory based diagnosis of fine-

motor coordination development and deficiencies using handwriting tasks.(pp.

33-49). Unpublished PhD thesis, The Netherlands: Nijmegen.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M, Van Galen, G. P., & Portier, S. J. (1994a) Psychomotor aspects

of poor handwriting in children. In M.L. Simner, W. Hulstijn, & P.L. Girouard,

(Eds.) Contemporary issues in the forensic, developmental and nerological aspects of

handwriting. Monograph of the association of forensic document examination,

1.(pp.17-43). Toronto, Canada: Association Forensic Document Examiners.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Van Galen, G. P., & Portier, J. (1994b). Psychomotor

development of handwriting proficiency: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study on

developmental features of handwriting. In C. Faure, P. Keuss, G. Lorette, & A.

34
Vinter, (Eds.). Advances in Handwriting and Drawing, A multidisiciplinary approach.

(pp. 187-205). Paris: Europia press.

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Van Galen, G. P., & Shoemaker, M. M. (1998). Theory-

Based diagnosis and subclassification in the developmental coordination

disorder. In J. Rispens, T.A. Van Yperen, & W. Yule (Eds.) Perspective on the

classification of specific developmental disorders. (pp.245-264). London:

Academic Pub.

Sovik, N., Arnzen, O., Samuelstuen, M., & Heggberget, M. (1994). Relations between

linguistic wordgroups and writing. In Faure, C., Keuss, P., Lorette, G., & Vinter,

A. (Eds.) Advances in Handwriting and Drawing, A Multidisciplinary Approach

(pp. 231-246). Europia Press, Paris,

Sovik, N., Arntzen, O., & Thygesen, R. (1987a). Relation of spelling and writing in

learning disabilities. Perceptual and Motor Skills 64, 219-236.

Sovik, N., Arntzen, O., & Thygesen, R. (1987b). Writing characteristics of “normal”,

dyslexic and dysgraphic children. Journal of Human Movement Studies 31,

171-187.

Starch, D. (1919). Educational research and statistics: A scale for measuring handwriting.

School and Society 9, 155-158.

Stott, D.H., Henderson,S.E., & Moyes, F.A. (1987). Diagnosis and remediation of

handwriting problems. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly 4, 137-147.

Stott, D. H., Moyes, F. A., & Henderson, S. E. (1984). Diagnosis and Remediation

of handwriting problems. Burlington; Ontario: Hayes pub’. Cradiff: Drake

Educaitonal.

Thorndike, E. L. (1910). Handwriting. Teacher College Record, 11, 83-175.

Trap-porter, J., Gladden, M. A., Hill, D. S. & Cooper, J. O. (1983). Space size

35
and accuracy of second and third grade students cursive handwriting. Journal of

Educational Research 76, 231-233.

Tseng, M. H., & Cermak, S. H. (1993). The influence of ergonomic factors and

perceptual - motor abilities on handwriting performance. American Journal of

Occupational Therapy 47, 919-926.

Tseng, M. H., & Chow, S. M. K. (2000). Perceptual-motor function of school-age

children with slow handwriting speed. American Journal of Occupational Therapy

54, 83-88.

Tseng, M. H., & Hsueh, I. P. (1997). Performance of school-age children on a Chinese

Handwriting Speed Test. Occupational Therapy International 4, 294 - 303.

Van Galen, G. P., Portier, S. J., Smits- Engelsman,B. C. M., & Shoemaker, L. R. B.

(1993). Neuromotor noise and poor handwriting in children. Acta Psychologica 82,

161-178.

Waber, D. P., & Bernstein, J. H. (1994). Repetative graphomotor output in L.D. and

nonlearning –disabled children. The Repeated Patterns Test. Developmental

Neuropsychology 10, 51-65.

Wann, J. P. (1987). Trends in the refinement and optimization of fine-motor trajectories:

Observations from an analysis of the handwriting of primary school children.

Journal of Motor Behavior 19, 13-37.

Wann, J. P., & Jones, J. G. (1986). Space-time invariance in handwriting. Human

Movement Science 5, 275-296.

Wann, J. P, & Kardirkmanathan, M. (1991). Variability in childrens handwriting

computer diagnosis of writing difficulties. In J.Wann, A.M. Wing, A. M &

N. Sovik, Development of Graphic Skills. (pp. 223-236). London: Academic

Press.

36
Ziviani, J., & Elkins, J. (1984). An evaluation of handwriting performance. Educational

Review 36, 249-261.

37
Figures

Figure 1: Handwriting samples of a paragraph copying of a typical proficient and a typical

dysgraphic handwriting .

(1A) Stimulus on the computer monitor:

(1B) Sample of proficient handwriting

38
(1C) Sample of dysgraphic handwriting

39
Tables
Table 1

Comparison of mean performances of participants in the proficient and the dysgraphic

groups, in conventional measures, for both copying and dictation tasks.

Conventional (HHE) measures – Copying task


Proficient Dysgraphic
Means SD Means SD Sig Partial eta squared

Global legibility 1.26 0.49 2.42 1.05 ** 0.34

Letters erased or 1.98 1.71 4.14 2.50 ** 0.21


overwritten

Unrecognizable 2.62 1.81 6.12 3.28 ** 0.31


letters

Spatial 6.58 0.95 8.28 2.58 ** 0.16


arrangement

Conventional (HHE) measures – Dictation task


Proficient Dysgraphic
Means SD Means SD Sig Partial eta squared

Global legibility 1.22 0.46 3.10 0.86 ** 0.65

Letters erased or 1.68 1.45 4.44 3.05 ** 0.25


overwritten

Unrecognizable 2.26 1.89 6.34 2.37 ** 0.48


letters

Spatial 6.50 1.31 11.08 2.73 ** 0.54


arrangement

Note: **p<.01

40
Table 2

Comparison of mean performances of participants in the proficient and the dysgraphic

groups, in digitizer-based measures for both copying and dictation tasks.

Digitizer-based measures – Copying task


Proficient Dysgraphic
Means SD Means SD Sig Partial eta squared
Total length 0.25
(mm) 3029.4 569.9 4191.9 1331.0 **

On paper length 0.04


(mm) 978.4 178.5 1079.3 292.5 **

In air length 0.26


(mm) 2051.0 467.8 3112.6 1191.6 **

Total time (s) 171.3 55.8 313.1 131.1 0.33


**
On paper time 0.20
(s) 47.5 11.6 66.9 25.1 **

In air time (s) 123. 8 847. 246.2 117.8 ** 0.32

Digitizer-based measures – Dictation task


Proficient Dysgraphic
Means SD Means SD Sig Partial eta squared
Total length 0.25
(mm) 2569.2 434.6 3516.2 1041.0 **

On paper length 391.5 0.04


(mm) 862.7 169.4 1000.9 **

In air length 862.3 0.26


(mm) 1706.5 329.2 2515.3 **

Total time (s) 124.4 29.5 208.9 78.3 0.33


**
On paper time 20.6 0.20
(s) 42.8 10.0 58.8 **

In air time (s) 81.7 24.9 150.2 68.4 ** 0.32

Note: *p< .05 **p<.01

41
Table 3

The degree of discrimination between study groups, for all the conventional measures (HHE)

and the digitizer-based measures, for copying and dictation tasks separately.

Measures Copying Dictation


All eight conventional and Digitizer-based measures F(8,87) = 9.95 F(8,89) = 25.96
P<.0001 p<.0001
Conventional Global legibility 0.81 0.95
Letters erased and/or overwritten 0.64 0.59
Unrecognizable letters 0.77 0.81
Spatial arrangement 0.57 0.86
Digitizer-based In air time 0.79 0.65
On paper time 0.63 0.52
In air length 0.71 0.62
On paper length 0.29 0.26

42
Table 4

Correlation matrix between the Hebrew Handwriting evaluation (HHE) measures and spatial

and temporal digitizer measures of paragraph copying

4.1 Entire sample On paper length In air length On paper time In air time
(mm) (mm) (s) (s)
HHE variables COPY DICT COPY DICT COPY DICT COPY DICT
Global legibility .42** .52** .31** .41** .51** .65**
Letters erased and/or overwritten .47** .50** .38** .31* .54** .54**
Unrecognizable letters .41** .52** .36** .50** .52**
Spatial arrangement .58** .54** .31** .36** .62** .57**

4.2 Children with proficient On paper length In air length On paper time In air time
handwriting (mm) (mm) (s) (s)
HHE variables COPY DICT COPY DICT COPY DICT COPY DICT
Global legibility .41**
Letters erased and/or overwritten .33*
Unrecognizable letters .31**
Spatial arrangement

4.3 Children with dysgraphic On paper length In air length On paper time In air time
handwriting (mm) (mm) (s) (s)
HHE variables COPY DICT COPY DICT COPY DICT COPY DICT
Global legibility .35** .33* .39**
Letters erased and/or overwritten .35** .24* .44** .27**
Unrecognizable letters .30* .36* .34**
Spatial arrangement .50** .31* .59** .35**

COPY = copying task DICT = Dictation task *p< .05 **p<.01

43
Table 5

Correlations between copying and dictation (HHE and Digitizer variables) of entire sample

In On Total In air On Total Global Letters Unrecognizable Spatial


air Paper time Length Paper length legibility erased letters arrangement
time time length

COPY/DICT 0.84 0.86 0.84 0.80 0.84 0.81 0.70 0.54 0.58 0.64
Correlation ** ** ** ** ** ** ** ** ** **

N=100 DICT = Dictation COP = Copying **p<.01

44

You might also like