You are on page 1of 13

RESEARCH REPORT doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01855.

Alcohol-specific rules, personality and adolescents’


alcohol use: a longitudinal person–environment study

Haske Van Der Vorst1, Rutger C. M. E. Engels1, Maja Deković2, Wim Meeus2 & Ad A. Vermulst1
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands1 and Department of Child and Adolescent Studies, Utrecht University, the
Netherlands2

ABSTRACT

Aims To examine the bi-directional associations between providing alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol
use. Further, to explore person–environment interactions, we tested whether Big Five personality traits moderate the
assumed association between providing alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol use. Design Longitudinal data
(three waves in 2 years) from 428 families, consisting of both parents and two adolescents (aged 13–16 years) were
used for the analyses. Analyses were conducted on four samples: a group of older adolescents and a group of younger
adolescents who already consumed alcohol, and a group of older and younger adolescents who were not drinking at
baseline measurement. Findings In general, results of structural equation modelling showed that providing clear
alcohol-specific rules lowers the likelihood of drinking initiation, regardless of the age of the youngsters. Once adoles-
cents have established a drinking pattern, the impact of parental alcohol-specific rules declined or even disappeared.
Finally, the Big Five personality traits did not moderate the association between providing alcohol-specific rules and
adolescents’ alcohol involvement. Conclusions In sum, in particular during the initiation phase of drinking, parents
could prevent the drinking of their offspring, regardless of the age or personality of their youngsters, by providing clear
alcohol-specific rules.

Keywords Adolescents, alcohol-specific rules, alcohol, personality, person–environment interactions.

Correspondence to: Haske van der Vorst, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, PO Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
E-mail: h.vandervorst@bsi.ru.nl
Submitted 9 May 2006; initial review completed 19 September 2006; final version accepted 13 March 2007

INTRODUCTION older and younger adolescent children within a family


and was based on the separate perceptions of family
Studies focusing on the influence of parents in the devel- members (father, mother, adolescents) on alcohol-specific
opment of adolescents’ drinking have examined prima- rules underscoring the consistency of this finding.
rily the influence of parental alcohol use and general Further, Van Zundert et al. [7] showed that the direct
parenting practices (e.g. [1,2]). Recent research has paid association between monitoring and adolescents’ alcohol
attention to the role of alcohol-specific socialization, use disappears when taking alcohol-specific rule-setting
which refers to activities parents undertake specifically to into account. This finding suggests that alcohol-specific
manage or prevent their children’s drinking behaviour rules are embedded in the level of behavioural control in
(e.g. providing alcohol-specific rules, showing disap- a family, but also that the rules about alcohol in particu-
proval of adolescents’ drinking or talking at home about lar are related to drinking in adolescence. In addition, Yu
alcohol use) [3–5]. [5] demonstrated that parents who prohibit their adoles-
Providing alcohol-specific rules seems to be the most cents to drink reduce the likelihood of current adoles-
influential form of alcohol-specific socialization on ado- cents’ alcohol involvement. A longitudinal study by
lescents’ drinking [3,4,6]. Van der Vorst et al. [4] showed Jackson et al. [3] showed that parents who permitted their
strong negative associations between imposing alcohol- children to drink at home were more likely to have
specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol use (bs ranging alcohol-consuming children 2 years later (see also Wood
between -0.26 and -0.52). This finding was observed for et al. [6]).

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
Alcohol-specific rules and personality 1065

Alcohol-Specific Alcohol-Specific Alcohol-Specific


Rules T1 Rules T2 Rules T3

Figure 1 Conceptual model of the


drinking sample on the associations
Adolescents' Adolescents' Adolescents'
between alcohol-specific rules and ado- Alcohol Use T1 Alcohol Use T2 Alcohol Use T3
lescents’ alcohol consumption at three
waves

Almost all the studies concentrating on alcohol- With regard to drinking, we assume that adolescents
specific rules are cross-sectional, and therefore they do could react differently when parents impose alcohol-
not provide insight into the direction of the effect of specific rules simply because they differ in their personal-
imposing alcohol-specific rules: does imposing alcohol- ity. For instance, impulsive adolescents might find it more
specific rules really prevent youngsters from drinking difficult to obey the alcohol-specific rules of their parents
alcohol as most scholars indicate, based on theoretical than other adolescents, probably due also to their vulner-
assumptions concerning parent–child effects (e.g. [8]), or ability for alcohol consumption [16]. On the contrary,
are parents adjusting their rules to the drinking of their imposing strict rules about drinking might even result in
offspring? That the relationship between parenting and abstaining from alcohol for conscientious youngsters, as
adolescents’ problem behaviour is not unidirectional— they are more likely to obey their parents and are already
the direction estimated in most empirical studies—but less prone to drink alcohol [17,18].
that adolescents are also influencing their parents’ That personal characteristics and the social environ-
behaviour, so-called bi-directionality, has been demon- ment of an individual interact is often argued in theories
strated in several studies (e.g. [9,10]). With respect to of the development of problem behaviour [13,19].
alcohol, parental strict control prevents early adoles- Empirical studies, however, exploring personality–
cents’ drinking, but early adolescents’ drinking also leads environment interactions are rare with regard to problem
to a decline in the degree to which parents provide strict behaviour, and in particular with regard to the develop-
control [11]. In other words, parents are responding to ment of alcohol use (see for exceptions [13,20]). Engels
their children’s alcohol involvement by loosening their et al. [12] demonstrated that aggressive men with less
control. Thus, a longitudinal research design measuring self-control were most at risk for developing problem
the bi-directional association between providing alcohol- drinking in young adulthood if they were raised in a non-
specific rules and adolescents’ drinking will reveal insight structured environment. Further, Goodwin et al. [21]
into the direction of effects. To develop effective alcohol demonstrated that high-risk Danish boys (e.g. those with
prevention programmes focusing on parents, it is essen- a family history of alcoholism) who experienced prob-
tial to establish the direction of the association between lems with attention and impulsiveness at the age of
alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol use over 19–20 years were most likely to be diagnosed with alco-
time. holism at the age of 29–30 years. In sum, there is some
To understand the development of alcohol consump- empirical evidence that personality has an influence on
tion one should consider the complexity of factors affect- the relation between family factors and alcohol use.
ing adolescents’ behaviours. These factors reflect the The aim of the present study was to examine the
interactions between individual characteristics, such as bi-directional relationships between providing alcohol-
personality, and environmental factors, such as alcohol- specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol consumption, con-
specific socialization [12,13]. Behaviour, in this case trolling for previous behaviour (Figs 1 and 2). We used
drinking, is always an outcome of ongoing processes structural equation modelling techniques to assess these
between the person and the environment [14], and there- hypotheses. Our main interests were the so-called cross-
fore person–environment interactions could be defined as lagged paths which refer to the longitudinal associations
a set of processes that are based on a similar set of expe- between alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ drinking.
riences having different consequences, depending on the In addition, the influence of parents might depend on the
characteristics of an individual [15]. drinking experience of adolescents (e.g. [22]). Therefore,

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
1066 Haske van der Vorst et al.

Alcohol-Specific Alcohol-Specific Alcohol-Specific


Rules T1 Rules T2 Rules T3

Figure 2 Conceptual model of the


non-drinking adolescents at T1 on the
Adolescents' Adolescents' associations between alcohol-specific
Alcohol Use T2 Alcohol Use T3 rules and adolescents’ alcohol consump-
tion at three waves

we tested our hypotheses for a sample of adolescents who nal research. At the second and third waves, respectively,
were already engaged in drinking at baseline measure- 416 and 404 families participated.
ment and for a sample of adolescents who were not drink- The families were interviewed three times with an
ing at baseline. Further, we examined whether each interval of 1 year at home in the presence of a trained
personality trait of the Big Five factor model (extraver- interviewer. An extensive questionnaire had to be com-
sion, conscientiousness, agreeableness, emotional stabil- pleted by each family member individually. This took
ity, resourcefulness) [23,24] moderated the association about 2 hours. The family members were not permitted to
between providing alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ discuss the questions or answers with each other. Each
drinking. We used three-wave full-family data (father, family received €30 after the four family members had
mother and two adolescents of the same family), which completed the questionnaire. At the end of the third wave
enabled us to compare the perceptions of each family five holiday cheques of €1000 were raffled between the
member on alcohol-specific rules and to explore possible families who participated in all three waves of the study.
differences between older and younger adolescents Each family consisted of both parents and two adoles-
within a family. cent children; 95% of the participants were of Dutch
origin, and most of the remainder was born in another
METHOD West European country or in Indonesia. The mean
age of the older siblings was 15.22 years at time 1
Participants and procedure (T1; SD = 0.60; range 14–17 years), and that of the
Data for this study were collected as part of a broader younger siblings was 13.36 years at T1 (SD = 0.50;
longitudinal survey (‘Family and Health’), which exam- range 13–15 years). Fathers’ mean age was 46 years
ined different family processes in relation to various (SD = 4.00) and mothers’ was 44 years (SD = 3.57).
health behaviours in adolescence (see the cross-sectional About one-third of both siblings followed special or low
study by van der Vorst et al. [4]). A sample of Dutch education, one-third followed an intermediate general
families with at least two adolescent children aged education and the remainder followed the highest level of
13–16 years were asked (by mail) to participate in the secondary school in the Netherlands (i.e. preparatory
study. The addresses of these families were derived from college and university education). A total of 52.8% of the
the registers of 22 municipalities in the Netherlands; 885 older adolescents and 47.7% of the younger adolescents
of the families approached agreed to participate by were boys.
returning the included response form. These families
were then contacted by telephone to establish whether Measures
they fulfilled all the inclusion criteria; i.e. the parents had
Alcohol consumption
to be married or living together, and the adolescents and
their parents should be biologically related. Families with The adolescents were asked about the intensity and fre-
twins or with adolescents who had mental or physical quency of their drinking. Intensity of drinking was mea-
disabilities were excluded from the project. Because an sured by asking the number of glasses of alcohol the
equal division of education and an equal amount of adolescents had been drinking during the previous week
sibling dyads was aimed for (i.e. boy–boy, boy–girl, girl– during weekdays and at weekends into contexts at home
girl, girl–boy), a further selection was made. Finally, a and outside the home [25]. These divisions were made to
total of 428 Dutch families participated in this longitudi- allow the adolescents to recall more accurately what they

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
Alcohol-specific rules and personality 1067

had consumed that week. The scores on these four ques- a = 0.74), and ‘resourcefulness’ with items such as cre-
tions were summed to obtain an indication of the total ative, artistic and versatile (O: a = 0.68; Y: a = 0.63) (see
amount of alcohol consumption the adolescents drank [23] for more information about the psychometric proper-
during a week. Frequency of drinking was assessed by ties of the short version of the Big Five questionnaire).
asking the adolescents how many times they drank
alcohol during the past 4 weeks. They had to respond on
Analyses strategy
a six-point scale: (1) ‘have not been drinking’; (2)
‘1–3 days a month’; (3) ‘1–2 days a week’; (4), ‘3–4 days We selected a sample including adolescents who partici-
a week’; (5) ‘5–6 days a week’; and (6) ‘every day’ [26]. pated at all three waves (n = 401) for both adolescents.
Thus, all missing data on the basis of non-participation
Rules about alcohol were omitted from the analyses. Next, we divided both
samples of the younger and older adolescents into two
We developed a 10-item scale to measure the degree to
groups: adolescents who were not drinking alcohol at
which parents permit their children to consume alcohol
baseline measurement (nolder = 111; nyounger = 229) and
in various situations, such as ‘in the absence of parents at
adolescents who reported that they had been drinking at
home’ or ‘at a friends’ party’ [4]. Thus, we asked each
baseline (nolder = 290; nyounger = 172). We conducted all
family member what rules the parents had or what they
analyses on these four samples.
prohibited concerning alcohol. Respondents had to
First, descriptive analyses were conducted to calculate
answer to what degree these rules were applicable at their
the means and standard deviations of adolescents’ inten-
home. Response categories ranged from (1) ‘completely
sity and frequency of drinking in each group. Secondly,
applicable’ to (5) ‘not applicable at all’. Higher scores indi-
paired t-tests were used to compare the responses of the
cate having stricter rules about alcohol consumption. We
four family members on the scale on alcohol-specific rules
have some preliminary psychometric features of the rules
at all three waves in each group. This will gain insight
concerning alcohol scale. The scale seems to have a good
into possible differences in alcohol-specific rules towards
content validity, as explorative factor analysis revealed
older and younger adolescents in a family, into possible
one factor (items loaded between 0.56 and 0.93), but also
differences in experiences of parents and adolescents, but
the reliability was high with Cronbach’s as between 0.80
also into the stability of alcohol-specific rules over time.
and 0.93 across reporters and waves. Further, divergent
Thirdly, to test our longitudinal models, we applied struc-
validity seem to be satisfactory. Alcohol-specific rules was
tural equation modelling using version 4.2 of the Mplus
namely moderately correlated (varying between 0.34
statistical package [27]. We tested three models for the
and 0.50 depending on the perspective of the respon-
older adolescents (mother, father and adolescent reports
dents) with alcohol-specific monitoring (parents knowing
on alcohol-specific rules) as well as for the younger ado-
when their adolescent child is drinking, where and with
lescents in each group (thus a total of 12 models). [The
whom). Finally, predictive validity is shown by the results
correlations between alcohol-specific rules based on the
of this study.
reports of the mothers and alcohol-specific rules based on
the reports of the fathers were not high, varying between
Personality
0.33 and 0.55. This indicates that they are separate con-
We used the short version of the Big Five questionnaire structs. In line with this, we have tried to make a latent
[24]. Five personality dimensions were assessed on the ‘parental rules’ construct of the fathers’ and mothers’
basis of the five-factor personality model [23]. For 30 reports. However, the variables do not seem to load
traits, the adolescents were asked to what degree they together. The factor loadings of the fathers are too low
possessed each trait. We used the items of the short version (around 0.40), which is another indication that fathers’
of the Big Five questionnaire, assessed at T1. Response and mothers’ perspectives should be measured sepa-
categories ranged from (1) ‘absolutely disagree’ to (7) rately.] In each model, assessments of alcohol use were
‘absolutely agree’. The dimension ‘extraversion’ was mea- based on self-reports of the particular adolescents (older
sured with (recodes of) items such as quiet, withdrawn or younger adolescents). The distributions of the inten-
and shy (older adolescents (O): a = 0.86; younger adoles- sity of drinking variables were very skewed. Therefore,
cents (Y): a = 0.79). The dimension ‘agreeableness’ was the number of glasses of alcohol was divided into six
assessed with items such as kind, likeable and cooperative classes for the structural equation models: 1 (no glasses),
(O: a = 0.77; Y: a = 0.78). The dimension ‘conscientious- 2 (one to five glasses), 3 (six to 10 glasses), 4 (11–20
ness’ was measured with items such as organized, orderly glasses), 5 (21–30 glasses) and 6 (> 30 glasses a week).
and efficient (O: a = 0.85; Y: a = 0.84). The dimension In the first model parental rules about alcohol use
‘emotional stability’ was assessed with (recodes of) items were based on the reports of the adolescents, in the
such as nervous, fearful and sensitive (O: a = 0.73; Y: second model parental rules about alcohol use were based

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
1068 Haske van der Vorst et al.

on the reports of the mothers and in the third model on effects, see van der Vorst et al. [11]). Because the number
the reports of the fathers (see Fig. 1 for the conceptual of respondents was reduced as a consequence of the
model for adolescents who reported to consume alcohol at median split method, our aim was to be very economical
T1). The models of the non-drinking groups were the with the free parameters of the baseline model. The
same as the models of the drinking groups, except that the structural parameters were kept free (10 bs and three
alcohol use variable of the adolescents at T1 was omitted disturbance correlations for the model shown in Fig. 1
due to lack of variance (all adolescents were non-drinkers and seven bs with two disturbance correlations for the
at T1; Fig. 2). [Correlation tables of the model variables model shown in Fig. 2). All the other parameters were
can be obtained from the first author of this paper.] fixed at the values resulting from the 12 model estimates.
The variables in the models as depicted in Figs 1 and A baseline c2 was computed with no equality constraints
2 are treated as latent variables. The alcohol-specific between the structural parameters of the two groups
rules were measured by 10 items. To overcome the (unconstrained model). Next, all the bs were constrained
problem of estimating too many parameters in relation to be equal for both groups. The c2 of this constrained
to the sample size, the 10 items were split up into two model was compared with the c2 of the unconstrained
equivalent parts (parcels) according to the recommen- model. If c2 increases significantly one or more bs would
dations of Bandalos & Finney [28] and Nasser & Wisen- be significantly different across groups. To observe which
baker [29]. The indicators for adolescents’ alcohol use bs were different between two groups, additional differ-
were the two alcohol variables intensity and frequency, ence tests for each individual b were conducted. These
both measured with six ordered categories. The factor analyses were performed for each of the Big Five person-
loadings of the 12 models were mainly above 0.80. The ality traits for all 12 groups. The fit of the models was
lowest loading was 0.65. The conclusion is that the assessed by the following global fit indexes: c2, compara-
parcels/indicators represent adequately the underlying tive fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approxima-
latent variables. tion (RMSEA) and standardized root mean square
Because the alcohol indicators are non-normal and residual (SRMR) [34].
more ordered categorical (ordinal) than metric, the most
suited estimator available in Mplus is the weighted least- RESULTS
square estimator with mean- and variance-adjusted c2
Descriptives
statistic (WLSMV-estimator). However, if the data are
ordinal with at least five categories and moderately non- The older adolescents within the subsample of drinkers
normal (skewness < 2 and kurtosis < 7) an adequate consumed on average 5.4 glasses of alcohol a week at T1
alternative is the maximum likelihood (ML) estimator (Table 1). The amount of alcohol consumed increased
[30]. The skewness and kurtosis was < 2 for all alcohol significantly to 8.1 glasses a week per year later, and to
variables, while there were six categories. For these 10.1 glasses a week 2 years later. The older adolescents in
reasons and because multiple group testing (see below) the non-drinking sample at T1 consistently drank less
is somewhat complicated using the WLSMV-estimator, than the older adolescents of the drinking sample,
we decided to use the ML-estimator for all our analyses. namely an average 2.2 glasses of alcohol a week at T2
Moreover, we tested the 12 models with both estimators and 4.9 glasses a week at T3. The increase of intensity of
resulting in identical results. The low percentage of drinking for the non-drinking older adolescents was also
missing data (< 2%) was treated with the help of the full- significant. The younger adolescents in the drinking
information maximum likelihood (FIML) approach: esti- sample consumed 2.0 glasses of alcohol a week at T1, 3.8
mation of parameters using all the available information at T2 and 6.9 at T3. The younger adolescents in the non-
in the data case by case, maximizing the likelihood of the drinking sample also drank less than the adolescents of
observed data [31] with the help of the expectation the younger drinking sample. They drank on average 1.3
maximization (EM) algorithm via maximum likelihood glasses of alcohol a week at T2 and 3.2 glasses 1 year
[32]. later. The increase in intensity of alcohol use for both
To assess the moderating effects of the Big Five per- groups of the younger adolescents was significant The
sonality traits on the associations between providing older adolescents in both groups increased their fre-
alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol consump- quency of drinking significantly from T2 to T3. We also
tion, we tested all models in relation to each personality found an increase in frequency of drinking for the
trait under two conditions: low and high on the particu- younger adolescents in the non-drinking group,
lar personality trait [33]. The median split method was although they generally drank less often than the older
used to divide the sample, for instance, into a high extra- adolescents. However, the younger adolescents for the
version group and a low extraversion group (for addi- drinking group did not increase significantly the fre-
tional details on the procedures to test moderating quency of their drinking.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
Alcohol-specific rules and personality 1069

Table 1 Means and standard deviations of frequency and intensity of drinking.

Drinking adolescents at T1 Non-drinking adolescents at T1

Older Younger Older Younger

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Frequency T1 2.6a 0.70 2.3a 0.59


Frequency T2 2.6a 0.84 2.3a 0.80 1.8a 0.83 1.5a 0.66
Frequency T3 2.7b 0.85 2.4a 0.83 2.1b 0.89 1.8b 0.81
Intensity T1 5.4a 5.43 2.0a 2.08
Intensity T2 8.1b 7.93 3.8b 4.35 2.2a 4.35 1.3a 2.60
Intensity T3 10.1c 10.19 6.9c 7.05 4.9b 8.59 3.2b 5.41

Comparisons are made over time for both adolescents for both groups. The results should be read vertically in a column. a,b,cMeans that do not share
superscripts are significantly different (P < 0.05).

Table 2 Means and standard deviations of alcohol-specific rules.

O MO FO Y MY FY

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Drinking adolescents at T1
Rules T1 2.97a 0.80 3.98a 0.65 3.99b 0.68 3.69a 0.76 4.51b 0.50 4.50b 0.60
Rules T2 2.52a 0.72 3.52b 0.70 3.50b 0.75 3.23a 0.76 4.12b 0.69 4.13b 0.68
Rules T3 2.28a 0.67 3.13b 0.72 3.11b 0.76 2.82a 0.76 3.71b 0.73 3.73b 0.77
Non-drinking adolescents at T1
Rules T1 4.03a 0.85 4.62b 0.51 4.50b 0.66 4.33a 0.71 4.79b 0.40 4.75b 0.40
Rules T2 3.28a 1.00 4.19b 0.68 4.05b 0.87 3.80a 0.85 4.45b 0.63 4.40b 0.68
Rules T3 2.88a 1.00 3.60b 0.89 3.60b 0.86 3.34a 0.93 4.04b 0.78 4.03b 0.82

O = Older adolescents, MO = mothers’ perspectives about the older adolescents, FO = fathers’ perspectives about the older adolescents, Y = younger
adolescents, MY = mothers’ perspectives about the younger adolescents, FY = fathers’ perspective about the younger adolescents. Comparisons are made
between reports of older adolescents, fathers and mothers at each wave. The same holds for the younger adolescents. The results should be read
horizontally. a,bMeans that do not share superscripts are significantly different (P < 0.05).

Perceptions of alcohol-specific rules drinking as well as of non-drinking adolescents


appeared to be less strict towards the 15-year-old
Comparisons of responses of the adolescents and
younger adolescents than towards the older adolescents
parents of the drinking samples as well as the non-
at that age.
drinking samples showed a consistent pattern. We
found for each sample strong significant differences in
Structural equation modelling
perceptions of providing alcohol-specific rules at all
three measurements (Table 2). Both parents reported Drinking samples
that they imposed stricter rules than both youngsters
The fit of the three models of the older adolescents and of
reported them to do at all three measurements.
the three models of the younger adolescents was satisfac-
However, the rules on alcohol imposed by parents
tory (Table 3).
declined significantly during the 2 years according to
the four family members. Further, although the parents
Models of the older adolescents
treated their offspring differently (they were stricter with
the younger adolescents than with the older ones at all All cross-sectional associations between alcohol-specific
three waves) fathers and mothers were, on average, rules and older adolescents’ alcohol use were significant
similar in their rule-setting to their older adolescent and for the three models (Table 3), with the exception of cor-
also to their younger adolescent. Finally, when the relation of the model of the mothers at T2 and of the
younger adolescents reached the age of the older ado- model of the adolescents at T3. It is noteworthy that cor-
lescents at the first measurement (15 years), parents of relations between cross-sectional latent variables are

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
1070 Haske van der Vorst et al.

Table 3 Standardized estimates and fit measures for models of drinking adolescents at T1 tested for reports by adolescents, fathers and
mothers.

Adolescents Mothers Fathers

Older Younger Older Younger Older Younger

n 290 172 290 172 290 172


Cross-sectional
Rules T1–Alcohol T1 -0.41*** -0.43*** -0.34*** -0.34** -0.26*** -0.17
Rules T2–Alcohol T2 -0.17** -0.17** -0.09 -0.09 -0.17** -0.06
Rules T3–Alcohol T3 -0.07 -0.10 -0.10** -0.05 -0.04 -0.01
Stability paths
Rules T1–Rules T2 0.69*** 0.64*** 0.70*** 0.68*** 0.65*** 0.67***
Rules T2–Rules T3 0.53*** 0.57*** 0.57*** 0.47*** 0.59*** 0.52***
Rules T1–Rules T3 0.16* 0.12 0.18* 0.33*** 0.16* 0.22***
Alcohol T1–Alcohol T2 0.65*** 0.76*** 0.65*** 0.76*** 0.62*** 0.79***
Alcohol T2–Alcohol T3 0.63*** 0.78*** 0.60*** 0.87*** 0.59*** 0.66***
Alcohol T1–Alcohol T3 0.16 -0.19 0.16 -0.30 0.17 -0.11
Cross-lagged paths
Rules T1–Alcohol T2 0.08 -0.11 0.10 -0.18* -0.01 -0.00
Rules T2–Alcohol T3 0.07 0.06 -0.09 0.05 -0.09 0.09
Alcohol T1–Rules T2 0.07 -0.10 -0.02 0.01 0.09 -0.04
Alcohol T2–Rules T3 0.09 -0.03 0.04 -0.06 0.01 -0.05
Fit measures
d.f. 38 38 38 38 38 38
c2 60.248 41.305 55.967 48.241 53.412 44.995
P 0.016 0.328 0.030 0.123 0.050 0.202
CFI 0.987 0.997 0.990 0.991 0.992 0.994
RMSEA 0.043 0.022 0.040 0.040 0.037 0.033
SRMR 0.029 0.031 0.035 0.032 0.024 0.043

*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. CFI: comparative fit index, RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation, SRMR: standardised root mean square
residual.

decreasing over time, as these correlations are controlled the cross-lagged paths from adolescents’ drinking to pro-
for previous influences and must be interpreted as partial viding alcohol-specific rules.
correlations. Further, rules about alcohol and adoles-
cents’ alcohol use showed strong stability over time in all Non-drinking sample
models. More importantly, all cross-lagged associations
The fit of the six models was satisfactory (Table 4).
were not significant, which implies that providing
alcohol-specific rules does not prevent older adolescents’ Models of the older adolescents
drinking and that parents do not become more permissive
as their offspring consume more alcohol. According to the older adolescents themselves, as well as
the mothers and fathers, alcohol-specific rules (T1) were
Models of the younger adolescents strongly negatively related to older adolescents’ drinking
(T2; Table 4). This implies that imposing strict rules about
Alcohol-specific rules was cross-sectionally significantly
alcohol prevents 15-year-olds who do not yet drink
related only to younger adolescents’ alcohol use in the
alcohol to start consuming alcohol intensively and fre-
model of the younger adolescents themselves (T1, T2)
quently. This longitudinal effect was also found a year
and in the model of the mothers at T1. Alcohol-specific
later (from T2 to T3) in the model with adolescent reports.
rules predict rules over time, and previous drinking pre-
Alcohol use of the older adolescents (T2) was not associ-
dicts drinking later on. According to mothers, rules about
ated significantly with alcohol-specific rules over time
alcohol at T1 were related negatively to adolescents’
(T3).
alcohol use at T2. This indicates that rules about alcohol
prevent younger adolescents’ alcohol involvement later
Models of the younger adolescents
on. The other cross-lagged paths from alcohol specific
rules to alcohol use of younger adolescents in the three The preventive effect of providing alcohol-specific rules
models we tested were not significant. The same holds for (T1) on adolescents’ drinking behaviour (T2) appeared to

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
Alcohol-specific rules and personality 1071

Table 4 Standardized estimates and fit measures for models of non-drinking adolescents at T1 tested for reports by adolescents,
fathers and mothers.

Adolescents Mothers Fathers

Older Younger Older Younger Older Younger

n 111 229 111 229 111 229


Cross-sectional
Rules T2–Alcohol T2 -0.24** -0.41*** -0.21*** -0.26*** -0.29*** -0.15**
Rules T3–Alcohol T3 -0.10 -0.16*** -0.17** -0.11** -0.09 -0.10*
Stability paths
Rules T1–Rules T2 0.61*** 0.62*** 0.80*** 0.61*** 0.51*** 0.59***
Rules T2–Rules T3 0.57*** 0.64*** 0.74*** 0.60*** 0.79*** 0.46***
Rules T1–Rules T3 0.22** 0.13 -0.01 0.27*** 0.16* 0.10
Alcohol T2–Alcohol T3 0.64*** 0.58*** 0.68*** 0.58*** 0.73*** 0.61***
Cross-lagged paths
Rules T1–AlcoholT2 -0.34*** -0.13 -0.54*** -0.02 -0.28*** -0.21*
Rules T2–Alcohol T3 -0.30*** -0.10 -0.11 -0.21** -0.07 -0.08
Alcohol T2–Rules T3 -0.04 0.00 -0.02 -0.05 0.14 -0.00
Fit measures
d.f. 23 23 23 23 23 23
c2 39.031 31.583 50.275 37.235 35.904 23.732
P 0.020 0.109 0.001 0.031 0.042 0.419
CFI 0.979 0.995 0.965 0.991 0.984 1.000
RMSEA 0.079 0.040 0.103 0.052 0.071 0.012
SRMR 0.042 0.024 0.048 0.031 0.043 0.022

*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. CFI: comparative fit index, RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation, SRMR: standardised root mean square
residual.

be significant in the model of the fathers. In the model related negatively significantly to adolescents’ drinking,
based on information of the mothers, this significant and for adolescents scoring low on agreeableness,
association was observed a year later (T2 to T3). Thus, alcohol-specific rules were not related to alcohol use
parents who provided strict rules on alcohol consumption (T2–T3; Dc2 (10) = 21.129, P < 0.01; bhigh = -0.23,
are less likely to have a drinking younger adolescent later P < 0.01; blow = -0.06, P > 0.05). In all other models
on than permissive parents. no differences between the group scoring low on a trait
and the group scoring high on a specific personality
Multi-group analyses trait were observed. This indicates that personality does
not moderate the association between providing
To establish whether adolescents scoring low or high on
alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol use
each of the five personality traits (extraversion, conscien-
in the case that adolescents already drink alcohol at
tiousness, agreeableness, emotional stability, resourceful-
T1.
ness) differ in their reaction on alcohol-specific rules
setting, we conducted multi-group analyses in every Non-drinking adolescents
model based on drinking adolescents and non-drinking
All cross-lagged associations were not significantly differ-
adolescents (five multi-group analyses per model for a
ent between the group scoring low on a trait and the
total of 12 models). Here, only differences between cross-
group scoring high on a trait for the non-drinking
lagged effects are discussed, because these effects are our
samples.
main interest.
Further, it should be stressed that despite the large
number of multi-group analyses (60 models were tested
Drinking adolescents
in total), we found a difference in paths for only one multi-
We found one significant different cross-lagged path group analysis in the drinking group of older adolescents.
for the drinking groups, namely in the model of older
DISCUSSION
adolescents (paternal reports) concerning the personal-
ity trait ‘agreeableness’. For older adolescents scoring The aim of the present study was to determine the
high on agreeableness, alcohol-specific rules were bi-directional associations between providing alcohol-

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
1072 Haske van der Vorst et al.

specific rules and adolescents’ drinking behaviour using Thus, parents do not seem to become more permissive
longitudinal data from parents and their children. In because they notice that their youngsters are drinking.
addition, we assessed whether each trait of the Big It is noteworthy that we do not know whether parents
Five personality model moderates the association are consistent in providing alcohol-specific rules in the
between alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ alcohol short term. Do they impose the same rules each day, or
consumption. are they more permissive in some occasions than others?
First of all, providing alcohol-specific rules was related When parents fluctuate strongly in their alcohol-specific
cross-sectionally strongly negatively to the alcohol con- rule-setting in daily life, it is perhaps not a clear signal
sumption of both older and younger adolescents who towards adolescents, leading to a higher likelihood of
were already drinking and who still were in the initiation adolescent drinking [35]. Thus, it might be that it is not
phase of drinking. These findings correspond with those the mean level of alcohol-specific rules that is important,
of Jackson et al. [3] and Yu [5]. More importantly, provid- but the flexibility (or rigidity) of the rule-setting. On the
ing alcohol-specific rules prevented older and younger other hand, rigidity of parents might be perceived as an
adolescents’ alcohol use from starting to consume indication of inflexibility to deal with youngsters’ needs
alcohol. The validity of this finding is illustrated by the (see [36]), and lead subsequently to more adolescents’
consistent results across the different family members, alcohol involvement. Nevertheless, it would be important
and across the two children in a family, with the excep- to investigate the flexibility of parents in their alcohol-
tion of the younger adolescents with their perspective. On specific rule-setting by, for instance, observing how
the other hand, imposing alcohol-specific rules did not parents and adolescents communicate about alcohol-
prevent the alcohol use of adolescents who had already specific socialization.
started to drink. Thus, if an adolescent has established a
drinking pattern, the impact of parents declines or even
Person–environment interactions
disappears, at least with respect to alcohol-specific rules.
However, in the case that an adolescent has not started to We argued that the complexity of factors affecting ado-
drink regularly, no matter whether he/she is 13 or 15 lescents should be considered fully in order to understand
years old, parents seem to be able to control their off- the development of drinking, for instance by testing
spring drinking by providing alcohol-specific rules. This person–environment interactions [12,13,20]. Person–
might be an interesting implication for health organiza- environment issues have been a topic for debate for years
tions that develop alcohol prevention programmes in the field of developmental psychopathology, but have
focusing on parental involvement. Alcohol prevention generally been neglected in the research on alcohol use
programmes should make parents aware that they play a [13,19,20]. We hypothesized that the impact of pro-
role in preventing youth drinking by setting rules before viding alcohol-specific rules on adolescents’ drinking
their children have established a drinking pattern. depends on adolescents’ characteristics, e.g. the Big Five
Further, all family members reported that parents personality traits of adolescents. However, no consistent
were stricter towards the younger adolescents than significant findings were observed that could confirm our
towards the older ones at all three measurements, hypothesis. Again, the advantages of using multi-
proving clearly that parents are treating their youngsters informant data and data on younger as well as older ado-
differently with concern to alcohol use. Parents seem to lescents, in relation to the large numbers of analyses
become more permissive towards adolescents’ alcohol use conducted, underline the robustness of our findings.
over the years. On the basis of the mean scores on Therefore, we think this finding is a meaningful contribu-
alcohol-specific rules, it is quite clear that parents become tion to the scientific debate, showing that personality and
less strict over time. Relatively, however, the results show the environment are not necessarily related (as scholars
that parents are fairly stable in imposing alcohol-specific have been suggesting), at least not with regard to paren-
rules. Moreover, it is interesting that when the younger tal socialization and adolescents’ alcohol use. Neverthe-
adolescents reached the age of the older adolescents at less, this might seem striking considering that the older
the first measurement (15 years), parents appeared to be non-drinking group of adolescents is a somewhat special
less strict towards the 15-year-old younger adolescents group in the Netherlands; the majority of Dutch youth
than towards the older adolescents at that age. This have already been drinking alcohol at the age of 15 [37].
finding on birth order indicates that parents are influ- In this light one might assume that personality charac-
enced, at least partly, by the behaviour of their offspring. teristics would be a relevant explanation for the atypical
Nevertheless, this pattern was not supported by the behaviours of these adolescents. However, in the current
bi-directional results of our longitudinal models. For both study we showed that personality is not playing a role
drinking and abstaining adolescents at the first wave, here. On the basis of this we might conclude that im-
alcohol use did not affect alcohol-specific rule-setting. posing alcohol-specific rules affect only the drinking of

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
Alcohol-specific rules and personality 1073

adolescents who have not already established a drinking with twins. Secondly, it might be that our sample does not
pattern, despite the adolescents’ personality. reflect the Dutch situation accurately, as we do not have
Nevertheless, regardless of our conclusion concern- any information about the families who did not respond
ing personality and providing alcohol-specific rules, it is on our recruitment letter. Thirdly, one should be careful
meaningful to speculate why we found such a lack of in generalizing our results to other countries with other
support for our hypothesis. The general assumption is drinking cultures. Dutch adolescents drink more fre-
that the personality of an individual is a composite of quently than adolescents from other European countries
scores on a continuum of each of the five personality [38]. It is difficult to predict whether providing alcohol-
traits [20]. Perhaps a certain composite makes an adoles- specific rules would be more effective—in terms of con-
cent more sensitive to the impact of alcohol-specific rules tinuation of drinking—in cultures where children start
instead of just one trait (see [23]). In the current study we drinking at a later age, drink generally less often and
tested exclusively the moderating effect of each single intensively, or where the societal norms on youth alcohol
trait, while perhaps specific constellations of personality are less permissive. Therefore, it would be important to
factors are, in particular, putting youth at risk for a lack of investigate the role of alcohol-specific rules in other
effect in providing alcohol-specific rules on their drinking (drinking) cultures. Further, providing alcohol-specific
behaviour. Secondly, it could be that other individual rules might prevent heavy drinking or problem drinking
characteristics are of influence in the association in adolescence but also later in life. It would be interesting
between alcohol-specific rules and adolescents’ drinking, to find out what the long-term effects are of providing
such as self-control or aggression. For example, the risk alcohol-specific rules instead of the rather short-term
for problem drinking increases for young adult men who effects as we did. In addition, it would be important to
are aggressive and experience low levels of family func- explore how drinking peers affect the impact of alcohol-
tioning [12]. It might be that providing alcohol-specific specific socialization, as the effect of a peer-drinking
rules has less effect on especially the drinking of aggres- culture might interfere with the effect parents have on
sive male adolescents. Thirdly, the explanation is method- their offspring [39]. Finally, it is still unclear how parents
ological. We conducted a median-split method to divide sanction their child after he or she break the rules about
our sample. Although this is a commonly used method, it alcohol. Not perceiving consequences after not following
has a limitation, namely adolescents scoring in the a rule might give an adolescent the impression that the
middle of the range of each trait are also included in alcohol-specific rules are not as strict as the adolescent
the analyses. Analyses conducted on adolescents with thought in the first place. This might, in turn, affect
more extreme scores might show significant person– future drinking. Unfortunately, we have no data on
environment interactions. However, we were not able to parental sanctions.
perform this type of analysis as we would have encoun- In sum, the current study is one of the first which
tered problems with statistical power. Finally, our lack of provides insight into the impact of alcohol-specific rules
findings might be due to small samples. [We also con- on the development of adolescents’ alcohol consumption,
ducted multi-group analyses on the whole sample of as well as the role of the adolescents’ personality in this.
older and younger adolescents, so combining drinkers The findings yield substantial evidence that imposing
and non-drinkers at T1. Both these samples contained strict rules prevents the uptake of drinking, regardless of
428 adolescents. We did not find significant moderating their personality, but in the case that adolescents are
effects of each personality trait in these samples. In this already involved in drinking, the impact of parents
case a lack of power could not be the reason for not declines or even disappears.
finding significant results.] For smaller samples it is more
difficult to obtain significant effects, especially when one Acknowledgements
is testing a complex model such as ours.
Rutger Engels was supported by a fellowship of the Dutch
Organization of Scientific Research (NWO) during the
Strengths, limitations and implications for future preparation of this manuscript.
research
References
Although the current study has several strengths, such
as its longitudinal design and the multi-informant data, it 1. Barnes G. M., Farrell M. P. Parental support and control as
is limited by some factors. Although we selected families predictors of adolescent drinking, delinquency and related
problem behaviours. J Marr Fam 1992; 54: 763–76.
carefully on the basis of, for instance, educational level or
2. Reifman A., Barnes G., Dintcheff B. A., Farrell M. P., Uhteg
the proportion of sibling dyads, the findings should not be L. Parental and peer influences on the onset of heavier
generalized to single-parent families, to families with no drinking among adolescents. J Stud Alcohol 1998; 59: 311–
biological relation between parent and child or to families 17.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
1074 Haske van der Vorst et al.

3. Jackson C., Henriksen L., Dickinson D. Alcohol-specific tions of person–environment correlations and interactions
socialization, parenting behaviors and alcohol use by chil- for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol
dren. J Stud Alcohol 1999; 60: 362–7. 1997; 9: 335–64.
4. van der Vorst H., Engels R. C. M. E., Meeus W., Deković M., 20. O’Connor B. P., Dvrok T. Conditional associations between
Van Leeuwe J. The role of alcohol-specific socialization in parental behavior and adolescent problems: a search for
adolescents’ drinking behaviour. Addiction 2005; 100: personality–environment interactions. J Res Pers 2001; 35:
1464–76. 1–26.
5. Yu J. The association between parental alcohol-related 21. Goodwin D. W., Knop J., Jensen P., Gabrielle W. F., Pennick
behaviors and children’s drinking. Drug Alcohol Depend E. C. Thirty-year follow-up of men at high risk for alcohol-
2003; 69: 253–62. ism. Ann NY Acad Sci 1994; 708: 1210–13.
6. Wood M. D., Read J. P., Mitchell R. E., Brand N. H. Do 22. Spijkerman R., Van Den Eijnden R. J. J. M., Overbeek G. J.,
parents still matter? Parent and peer influences on alcohol Engels R. C. M. E. The impact of peer and parental norms
involvement among recent high school graduates. Psychol and behaviour on adolescent drinking: the mediating role of
Addict Behav 2004; 18: 19–30. drinker prototypes. Psychol Health 2007; 22: 7–29.
7. van Zundert R. M. P., van der Vorst H., Vermulst A. A., 23. Dubas J. S., Gerris J. R. M., Janssens J. M. A. M., Vermulst A.
Engels R. M. C. E. Pathways to alcohol use among Dutch A. Personality types of adolescents: concurrent correlates,
students in regular education and education for adolescents antecedents, and type–parenting interactions. J Adolesc
with behavioral problems: the role of parental alcohol use, 2002; 25: 79–92.
general parenting practices, and alcohol-specific parenting 24. Vermulst A. A., Gerris J. R. M. QBF: Quick Big Five Persoon-
practices. J Fam Psychol 2006; 20: 456–67. lijkheidstest Handleiding [Quick Big Five Personality Test
8. Darling N., Steinberg L. Parenting style as context: an inte- Manual]. Leeuwarden, the Netherlands: LDC Publications;
grative model. Psychol Bull 1993; 113: 487–96. 2005.
9. Kerr M., Stattin H. Parenting of adolescents: action or reac- 25. Engels R. C. M. E., Knibbe R. A., Drop M. J. Why do late
tion? In: Booth A., Crouter A., editors. Children’s influences adolescents drink at home? A study on psychological well-
on family dynamics: the neglected side of family relationships. being, social integration and drinking context. Addict Res
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003: 121–51. 1999; 7: 31–46.
10. Stice E., Barrera M. A longitudinal examination of the recip- 26. Engels R. C. M. E., Knibbe R. A. Alcohol use and intimate
rocal relations between perceived parenting and adoles- relationships in adolescence: when love comes to town.
cents’ substance use and externalizing behaviors. Dev Addict Behav 2000; 25: 435–9.
Psychol 1995; 31: 322–34. 27. Muthén L. K., Muthén B. O. Mplus User’s Guide, 4th edn. Los
11. van der Vorst H., Engels R. C. M. E., Meeus W., Deković M., Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén: 1998–2006.
Vermulst A. Parental attachment, parental control and 28. Bandalos D. L., Finney S. J. Item parceling issues in struc-
early development of alcohol use: a longitudinal study. tural equation modeling. In: Marcoulides G. A., Schu-
Psychol Addict Behav 2006; 20: 107–16. macker R. E., editors. New developments and techniques in
12. Engels R. M. C. E., Vermulst A. A., Dubas J. S., Bot S. M., structural equation modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Gerris J. Long-term effects of family functioning and child Erlbaum Associates; 2001, p. 269–96.
characteristics on problem drinking in young adulthood. 29. Nasser F., Wisenbaker J. A Monte Carlo study investigating
Eur Addict Res 2005; 11: 32–7. the impact of item parceling strategies on parameter esti-
13. Rose R. J. A developmental behavioural–genetic perspective mates and their standard errors in CFA. Struct Equat Model
on alcoholism risk. Alcohol Health Res World 1998; 22: 131– 2006; 13: 204–28.
43. 30. Finney S. J., DiStefano C. Non-normal and categorical data
14. Sroufe L. A., Egeland B. Illustrations of person– in structural equation modeling. In: Hancock G. R., Mueller
environment interaction from a longitudinal study. In: R. O., editors. Structural equation modeling. A second course.
Wachs T. D., Plomin R., editors. Conceptualization and mea- Greenwich: Information Age Publishing (IAP); 2006,
surement of organism–environment interaction. Washington, p. 269–314.
DC: American Psychological Association; 1991: 64–84. 31. Wothke W. Longitudinal and multigroup modeling with
15. Rutter M., Pickles A. Person–environment interactions: con- missing data. In: Little T. D., Schnabel K. U., Baumert J.,
cepts, mechanisms, and implications for data analysis. In: editors. Modeling longitudinal and multilevel data. Mahwah,
Wachs T. D., Plomin R., editors. Conceptualization and mea- NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000, p. 219–40.
surement of organism–environment interaction. Washington, 32. Kaplan D. Structural equation modeling. Foundations and
DC: American Psychological Association; 1991: 105–36. extensions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2000.
16. Walton K. E., Roberts B. W. On the relationship between 33. Jöreskog K., Sörbom D. PRELIS 2: User’s Reference Guide.
substance use and personality traits: abstainers are not mal- Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.; 1996.
adjusted. J Res Pers 2004; 38: 515–35. 34. Marsh H. W., Balla J. R., McDonald R. P. Goodness-of-fit
17. Cook M., Young A., Taylor D., Bedford A. P. Personality indexes in confirmatory factor analysis: the effect of sample
correlates of alcohol consumption. Pers Individ Diff 1998; size. Psychol Bull 1996; 103: 391–410.
24: 641–7. 35. Engels R. C. M. E., Bot S. M. Social influences on adolescent
18. Tucker J. S., Friedman H. S., Tomlinson-Keasey C., Schwartz substance use. In: De Ridder D., De Wit J., editors. New per-
J. E., Wingard D. L., Criqui M. H. et al. Childhood psychoso- spectives on health behaviour: the role of self-regulation. Chich-
cial predictors of adulthood smoking, alcohol consumption ester: John Wiley; 2006, p. 19–20.
and physical activity. J Appl Soc Psychol 1995; 25: 1884– 36. Hollenstein T., Granic I., Stoolmiller M., Snyder J. Rigidity in
99. parent–child interactions and the development of external-
19. Rutter M., Dunn J., Plomin R., Simonoff E., Pickles A., izing and internalizing behavior in early childhood.
Maughan B. et al. Integrating nature and nurture: implica- J Abnorm Child Psychol 2004; 32: 595–607.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075
Alcohol-specific rules and personality 1075

37. Poelen E. A. P., Scholte R. H. J., Engels R. C. M. E., Boomsma The Swedish Council for Information on Alcohol and
D. I., Willemsen G. Prevalence and trends of alcohol use and Other Drugs (CAN), Council of Europe, Co-operation Group
misuse among adolescents and young adults in the Nether- to Combat Drug Abuse and Illicit Trafficking in Drugs
lands from 1993 to 2000. Drug Alcohol Depend 2005; 79: (Pompidou Group); 2004.
413–21. 39. Mounts N. S. Adolescents’ perceptions of parental manage-
38. Hibell B., Andersson B., Bjarnasson T., Ahlström S., Bala- ment of peer relationships in an ethnically divers sample.
kireva O., Kokkevi A. et al. Alcohol and Other Drug Use Among J Adolesc Res 2004; 19: 446–67.
Students in 35 European Countries. The ESPAD Report 2003.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction, 102, 1064–1075

You might also like