You are on page 1of 14

Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 2003, 38(2), 131–144

© Division on Developmental Disabilities

Adult Outcomes for Students with Cognitive Disabilities


Three-Years After High School:
The Impact of Self-Determination
Michael L. Wehmeyer and Susan B. Palmer
University of Kansas

Abstract: This article reports a follow-up study of school leavers with mental retardation or learning disabilities
who were surveyed 1- and 3-years after they left school to determine what they were doing in major life areas
(employment, independent living or community integration). Students were divided into two groups based on
self-determination scores collected during their final year at high school. Comparisons between these groups on
outcomes at 1 and 3 years post-graduation indicate that students who were more self-determined fared better
across multiple life categories, including employment and access to health and other benefits, financial
independence, and independent living.

Over the last decade there has been consider- grams (Ward & Kohler, 1996), position papers
able focus in special education literature on (Field, Martin, Miller, Ward, & Wehmeyer,
the importance of self-determination in the 1998b), and student-directed planning pro-
education of students with disabilities. Due grams (Halpern, Herr, Wolf, Lawson, Doren,
largely to the federal emphasis on and fund- & Johnson, 1995; Martin & Marshall, 1995;
ing for promoting self-determination as a Powers, Sowers, Turner, Nesbitt, Knowles, &
component of transition services for youth Ellison, 1996; Wehmeyer & Sands, 1998). The
with disabilities, numerous resources are now process of promoting self-determination has
available to support instruction to achieve this been explored across age ranges, from early
outcome. Such resources range from curricu- childhood (Erwin & Brown, 2000; Wehmeyer
lar materials and guides to instructional strat- & Palmer, 2000) to secondary education
egies and methods (Field & Hoffman, 1996a; (Field & Hoffman, 1996b; Powers et al., 1996)
Field, Martin, Miller, Ward, & Wehmeyer, and across disability categories, including
1998a; Test, Karvonen, Wood, Browder, & Al- learning disabilities (Field, 1996), mental re-
gozzine, 2000; Wehmeyer, Agran, & Hughes, tardation and multiple disabilities (Gast et al.,
1998), assessment tools (Abery, Stancliffe, 2000; Wehmeyer, 1998, 2001), and autism
Smith, McGrew, & Eggebeen, 1995a, b; Wol- (Fullerton, 1998).
man, Campeau, Dubois, Mithaug, & Stolarski, In addition to this proliferation of instruc-
1994), teaching models (Wehmeyer, Palmer, tional supports and materials, an emerging
Agran, Mithaug, & Martin, 2000), model pro- international knowledge base documents that
people with disabilities experience limited
self-determination (Stancliffe, Abery, &
Correspondence concerning this article should Smith, 2000; Wehmeyer, Kelchner, & Rich-
be addressed to Michael L. Wehmeyer, University of ards, 1995; Wehmeyer & Metzler, 1995) and
Kansas, Beach Center on Disability, Haworth Hall, limited opportunities to express preferences
1200 Sunnyside Avenue, Lawrence, KS 66045-7534. and make choices (Stancliffe, 1997; Stancliffe
This research was supported by U.S. Department of
& Abery, 1997; Stancliffe & Wehmeyer, 1995),
Education Grant #HO23C40126 awarded to The
Arc of the United States. The opinions expressed
as well as evidence of the impact of environ-
herein do not necessarily reflect the position or ments on self-determination (Stancliffe et al.,
policy of the U.S. Department of Education and no 2000; Wehmeyer & Bolding, 1999, 2001).
official endorsement by the Department should be We present this extensive (though still only
inferred. partial) citation of contributions to the litera-

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 131


ture base on self-determination to illustrate gration of teaching decision-making skills to
that promoting and enhancing the self-deter- people with mental retardation. However, few
mination of individuals with disabilities has studies have looked at the impact of self-de-
become both an expectation of federal disabil- termination status itself, as opposed to a single
ity policy and a significant focus in the educa- component skill of self-determined behavior,
tion of students with disabilities. However, on adult outcomes. Sowers and Powers (1995)
while other segments of the self-determina- showed that instruction on multiple compo-
tion literature base have expanded, few stud- nents related to self-determination increased
ies have specifically examined the link be- the participation and independence of stu-
tween enhanced self-determination and dents with severe disabilities in performing
positive outcomes in lives of people with dis- community activities. Wehmeyer and Schwartz
abilities. There are several reasons for this (1998) found that the self-determination sta-
circumstance, not the least of which is that tus of adults with mental retardation living in
ethical reasons compel us to promote self- group homes predicted high quality of life
determination even in the absence of such status.
evidence. The self-determination construct is Wehmeyer and Schwartz (1997) conducted
used to reflect both a personal sense of the a study to examine outcomes of students with
term (e.g., someone who is self-determined mental retardation or learning disabilities
because they are causal agents in their lives), who were graduating from or leaving high
the most common use in education, and in a school as a function of their self-determina-
broader political or corporate sense, that of tion status. Self-determination of 80 students
self-determination as a right of nations or peo- with cognitive disabilities was measured and
ples to self-governance. In regard to the latter one year after their departure from secondary
meaning of the term and in relation to people education these young people answered ques-
with disabilities, self-determination is widely tions on a survey concerning their lives and
viewed as a fundamental human right, to gov- activities at that time. Wehmeyer and Schwartz
ern or direct one’s own life without unneces- found that students who were more self-deter-
sary interference from others, and the focus mined (controlling for intelligence level)
on promoting self-determination in education were more independent, overall, and were sig-
has certainly been influenced by this empow- nificantly more likely to be working for pay at
erment focus. In addition to acknowledging higher hourly wages. The present article re-
importance of promoting self-determination ports the outcome of a continuation and ex-
for ethical reasons, however, there is benefit tension of that study in which data were col-
to documenting the impact of self-determina- lected for students with mental retardation or
tion on lives of individuals with disabilities, learning disabilities at both one and three
both as a further reason to focus resources on years post-graduation. The purpose of the
this effort and to better understand how much study was to provide more information about
promoting self-determination contributes to impact of self-determination on the lives of
education’s goal to increase self-sufficiency, young people with disabilities and to extend
autonomy, and valued adult outcomes like the database concerning the relationship be-
employment, community integration, or inde- tween enhanced self-determination and posi-
pendent living. tive adult outcomes.
Most evidence regarding the impact of be-
ing self-determined on the lives of young peo-
Method
ple with disabilities has been either anecdotal
or extrapolated from research examining
Sample
such impact from component elements of self-
determined behavior, like goal-setting, deci- Data collection activities, as described in the
sion-making, problem-solving and self-man- Procedures section, were completed (e.g., self-
agement skills (see Wehmeyer et al., 1998, for determination measure administered at grad-
overview). For example, Hickson and Khemka uation, responses to questions about 1 and 3
(1999) and Khemka (2000) provided evi- year post-graduation outcomes collected by
dence of the importance to community inte- research staff) with 94 students served under

132 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003


the category of either learning disability or was for all students in the sample who held a
mental retardation through public schools in job at the time of the first-year follow-up (n ⫽
seven states (Alabama, California, Connecti- 65). The entire group of 94 was ranked ac-
cut, Kansas, North Carolina, Texas and Vir- cording to total self-determination score
ginia). Survey return rates and indicators ex- (within each disability category) and then a
amining non-response bias are included in median split made, resulting in 47 students in
the Procedures section. the high self-determination group and 47 in
For these 94 students we collected both self- the low group. There were 28 students in the
determination scores at graduation and (com- low self-determination group who were em-
pleted) 1-year and 3-year follow-up surveys. ployed (20 males, 8 females, mean age ⫽
Mean age of this sample at the time we mea- 19.45, SD ⫽ 1.45) and 37 who were employed
sured self-determination (e.g., during the last in the high self-determination group (17
semester of each student’s final year of high males, 20 females, mean age ⫽ 19.23, SD ⫽
school) was 19.25 years (range ⫽ 17–22 years, 1.32). This group was used to examine
SD ⫽ 1.56). The mean IQ for the group was changes in job status and access to important
82.91 (SD ⫽ 21.71). There were 49 males in job benefits, including insurance, health care,
the sample and 45 females. Sixty students and sick or vacation leave.
(64% of the sample) were identified as having
a learning disability (mean IQ ⫽ 96.2, SD ⫽
Procedure
13.41), while 34 students were labeled with
mental retardation (mean IQ ⫽ 59.47, SD ⫽ Participants were recruited in a variety of ways.
10.92). All students were recruited by project Project personnel sought and received per-
personnel through public schools in the seven mission from school districts to identify grad-
states listed. uating seniors with educational labels of learn-
To examine differences between students ing disability or mental retardation. These
based on self-determination status, the sample students’ teachers sent consent forms home to
was divided into a high self-determination and parents of graduating seniors, and parents
a low self-determination group based on their and students signed and returned these
self-determination score. The high self-deter- forms. An honorarium of $10 was offered for
mination group was identified as those stu- participation in initial testing during high
dents whose self-determination score was 1 school, and a continuing $10 incentive in mer-
standard deviation or more above the mean chandise coupons or check was sent as an
(n ⫽ 26, mean self-determination score ⫽ honorarium for completed surveys at each
123.19, SD ⫽ 8.50), while the low self-determi- measurement interval. We obtained informed
nation group consisted of students whose self- consent for 103 students in this manner, but
determination score fell 1 standard deviation of that total, only 77 responded to both first-
or more below the mean for the group (n ⫽ and third-year follow-up questionnaires.
22, mean self-determination score ⫽ 74.72, In addition to these 77 students, we con-
SD ⫽ 18.25). We opted for students 1 standard tacted 52 students from Connecticut, Ala-
deviation or more above and below the mean bama, and Virginia who had been involved in
to ensure these groups were actually different the initial 1-year follow-up study (Wehmeyer &
in their self-determination status and were Schwartz, 1997) described earlier, to deter-
genuinely ‘high’ and ‘low’ self-determination mine their willingness to respond to questions
groups. To ensure that the low self-determina- about their post-secondary outcomes 3-years
tion group did not disproportionately contain post-graduation. Seventeen of these students
students with mental retardation or the high responded to the questions and were paid the
group students with learning disabilities, we honoraria as well. This resulted in the total
identified members of the high and low sample of 94 students. We did this because we
groups for each disability category (based on were conscious of the difficulty in getting data
mean scores only for students in that disability three years after graduation and we felt that
category) and then combined the high and the two sub-samples were comparable. The
low groups from both disability categories. Wehmeyer and Schwartz sample graduated
A final grouping used to conduct analyses from high school in 1994, while students not

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 133


involved in that study graduated in 1995 or 135) ⫽ 1.40, ns, IQ, F(1, 135) ⫽ 1.13, ns, or
1996. There were no significant differences on total self-determination, F(1, 135) ⫽ .156, ns.
IQ scores between students with either learn- For the 103 students recruited exclusively
ing disabilities or mental retardation involved for this study, 18 (11%) did not respond to
in the Wehmeyer and Schwartz study and the year 1 surveys. The reasons given for non-
sample recruited specifically for this study. response were that one former student was in
The project director trained research per- jail, 11 had moved with no forwarding address
sonnel at sites in all states to work with stu- or available telephone number, and 6 de-
dents in administering the self-determination clined to participate. For the third-year post-
assessment (described subsequently) and to graduation survey, 43 participants for whom
conduct telephone interviews for students we had first-year follow-up data did not com-
who did not respond to mailed question- plete third-year data (see details above). The
naires. Two mailings of the survey for each of bulk of this group (n ⫽ 35) was from the
the two years (1- and 3-years post-high school) original study, and we were never able to con-
were conducted from the project central loca- tact them with regard to a third-year follow-up.
tion. In many cases, addresses had changed, In addition, from the remaining 8 students
and research personnel had to contact a close (who did not respond to third-year surveys but
friend or relative who was given as a contact who had responded to first-year survey), one
during initial assessment while in high school. student had died, two had been jailed, and
For students not involved in the Wehmeyer three did not return the survey or declined
and Schwartz (1997) study, the return rate was to participate. We used several methods to
82.5% for the first-year follow-up question- find students who had moved, including
naire (85 returned surveys from total of 103 conducting an Internet “People Search” on
who had provided informed consent). Of Yahoo.com within the states of residence and
those 85 students who were mailed a third- surrounding states and contacting former
year survey, 77 responded, or a response rate teachers and members of the community.
of 90.5%. Overall, from the original sample of Overall, 76% of the first-year follow-up sur-
103 students for whom we had informed con- veys were returned by mail, with 24% accom-
sent, 77 responded to both first- and third-year plished via telephone interview. In year 3,
surveys, for an overall response rate of just 54% of the surveys were returned by mail and
under 75%. From the 52 students involved in 46% were conducted by phone. There were
the Wehmeyer and Schwartz study who we no statistically significant differences between
contacted to obtain 3rd year outcome informa- mail or telephone respondents for age, F(1,
tion, almost 33% (n ⫽ 17) returned a com- 68) ⫽ .001, ns, or IQ, F(1, 68) ⫽ .439, ns, for
pleted third-year survey. (The lower return year 1 respondents or, similarly, for age, F(1,
rate on this group was attributed to the fact 91) ⫽ .221, ns, or IQ, F(1, 91) ⫽ 2.315, ns, for
that at the onset of the Wehmeyer and year 3 respondents. Similarly, chi-square anal-
Schwartz study we had indicated we were to yses of responses to survey questions in Table
conduct only a one-year follow-up and did not 1 indicated no significant differences in what
put into place any mechanism to track stu- would be expected from mail or telephone
dents after that, whereas for the second group respondents for both year 1 and year 3 survey
we kept track of student location throughout results. Although most participants (73% year
the time period). Thus, there were 43 students 1 and 68% in year 3) completed their own
for whom we had first-year data but for whom surveys, recipients were instructed to get what-
we were unable to obtain third-year follow-up ever assistance they needed to answer the
data who were excluded from the final analy- questions, and this support included assis-
sis. Mean age of those 43 students was 19.58 tance from parents, other family members,
(SD ⫽ 1.34); 25 were male and 18 were fe- and, in a few cases even staff members and a
male. There were no significant differences teacher. In all cases, the young adult was to
between the groups of students (n ⫽ 43) for participate in the process. In one case, a Span-
whom we collected only first-year follow-up ish language translator gathered information
information and those for whom we com- from a participant who spoke limited English
pleted all data collection (n ⫽ 94) for age, F(1, for both survey years. Research staff was avail-

134 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003


TABLE 1 analysis. Respondents indicated their current
living arrangements and that information was
Questions from Outcome Survey
collapsed into a dichotomous variable indicat-
ing independent, community-based living ver-
Where do you currently live?
Is this the same place you lived when in high sus congregate living arrangements.
school? Student self-determination was measured
Did you choose the place you live now? using The Arc’s Self-Determination Scale (Weh-
Do you pay your own rent? Utilities? Phone bill? meyer & Kelchner, 1995) a student self-report
Do you shop for your own groceries? measure of global self-determination. The
Do you have your own bank account? Arc’s Self-Determination Scale is a 72-item self-
Have you had a job since high school? report scale that provides data on overall self-
Are you now or have you been in job training? determination by measuring individual per-
Do you have a job at the present time? Full time?
formance in the four essential characteristics
Part time?
of self-determination identified by Wehmeyer,
Does this job have any benefits? Paid Vacation?
Sick Leave? Health Insurance? Kelchner, and Richards (1996). Section 1
measures autonomy, including the individu-
al’s independence and the degree to which he
or she acts on the basis of personal beliefs,
able to clarify any information regarding the values, interests and abilities. The second sec-
questionnaires and participants were encour- tion measures self-regulation and consists of
aged to call a toll-free phone number for in- two subdomains; interpersonal cognitive
quiries. Training for telephone interviews was problem solving, and goal-setting and task
provided to all staff prior to collecting infor- performance. The third section is an indicator
mation. of psychological empowerment. Psychological
Demographic data, including age and date empowerment consists of various dimensions
of birth, intelligence score, ethnicity, verifica- of perceived control. People who are self-de-
tion of high school exit, and special education termined take action based on the beliefs that
eligibility were collected from student records (a) they have the capacity to perform behav-
of all participants while still in high school. iors needed to influence outcomes in their
environment and (b) if they perform such
behaviors, anticipated outcomes will result.
Instrumentation
Respondents choose from items measuring
Measuring adult outcomes. Adult outcomes psychological empowerment using a forced-
were determined using a survey-type question- choice method. High scores reflect positive
naire adapted from Wehmeyer and Schwartz perceptions of control and efficacy. The final
(1997) and designed to evaluate outcomes in section measures self-realization. Self-deter-
major life domains. Project personnel re- mined people are self-realizing in that they
viewed follow-up and follow-along studies to use a comprehensive, and reasonably accu-
identify instruments to collect data regarding rate, knowledge of themselves and their
adult outcomes, and identified 24 unique strengths and limitations to act in such a man-
studies conducted since 1984. From this set, ner as to capitalize on this knowledge in a
we collected all instruments available, either beneficial way. Self-knowledge forms through
through published report or from authors. experience with and interpretation of one’s
After an examination of these survey instru- environment and is influenced by evaluations
ments, we selected and adapted questions of others, reinforcements, and attributions of
from the National Consumer Survey (Jas- one’s own behavior. Respondents reply to a
kulski, Metzler, & Zierman, 1990) and the series of statements reflecting low or high self-
National Longitudinal Survey (Wagner, D’Amico, realization by indicating that they agree or
Marder, Newman, & Blackorby, 1992). Table disagree with items. High scores reflect high
1 lists the questions from the survey. All ques- levels of self-realization.
tions, with the exception of determining On the scale, 148 points are obtainable, and
where the ex-student lived, were in yes/no higher scores reflect higher self-determina-
format creating a dichotomous variable for tion. The Arc’s Self-Determination Scale was

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 135


normed with 500 students with and without process of deriving one or more mathematical
cognitive disabilities in rural, urban and sub- equations for the purpose of assigning individ-
urban school districts in five states. The Scale’s uals to groups. We were interested only in the
concurrent criterion-related validity was estab- first application of discriminant function anal-
lished by showing relationships between The ysis, that of data interpretation and identifying
Arc’s Self-Determination Scale and conceptually how groups vary according to a set of indepen-
related measures. The scale had adequate dent or predictor variables (IQ, self-determi-
construct validity, including factorial validity nation).
established by repeated factor analyses, and
discriminative validity and internal consis-
Results
tency (Chronbach alpha ⫽ .90; Wehmeyer,
1996). The scale has been used in several Univariate analysis of variance examining dif-
research efforts with individuals with cognitive ferences between high and low self-determina-
disabilities (Cross, Cooke, Wood, & Test, tion groups indicated no statistically signifi-
1999; Sands, Spencer, Gliner, & Swaim, 1999; cant difference between these groups on
Wehmeyer, Agran, & Hughes, 2000; Weh- either age F(1,46) ⫽ .425, ns, or IQ scores
meyer & Schwartz, 1997, 1998; Zhang, 1998). F(1,46) ⫽ 1.04, ns. The mean age for the low
self-determination group was 19.68 years
(SD ⫽ 1.881), while the mean age for the high
Analyses
self-determination group was 19.37 years
Comparisons of dichotomous indicators of (SD ⫽ 1.48). The mean IQ score for the low
major adult outcomes by self-determination self-determination group was 78.86 (SD ⫽
group were conducted using chi-square anal- 23.34), while for the high self-determination
yses. We also examined the significance of group it was 85.27 (SD ⫽ 27.07). Table 2
changes between the first-year follow-up and provides results from chi-square analyses as-
the third-year follow-up for students in both sessing the relation between self-determina-
the high self-determination and low self-deter- tion status and major life outcome areas (in-
mination group using the McNemar test for dependent living, financial independence,
the significance of changes (Siegel, 1956). and employment).
Prior to analyzing data using chi-square and Figure 1 illustrates outcomes in each fol-
McNemar analyses, we examined differences low-up year by self-determination group on
between high and low self-determination major areas of financial independence. Chi-
groups on intelligence test scores and age us- square analyses reveal significant relations be-
ing univariate analysis of variance. Given the tween self-determination status on year 1 fol-
results of this analysis, we opted to conduct a low-up for maintaining a banking account,
discriminant function analysis to examine the and the McNemar test indicates significant
degree to which self-determination score and changes from year 1 to 3 only for students in
intelligence test scores predicted outcomes on the high self-determination group on paying
questions identified by chi-square analysis. for groceries.
The discriminant function analysis was con- In addition, there were significant differ-
ducted with the specific question responses as ences on the McNemar test for significance of
the grouping variable (always posed as a di- changes only for the high self-determination
chotomous variable) and IQ score and self- group on changes in overall benefits (p ⫽
determination score entered as independent .021), vacation (p ⫽ .002), and sick leave (p ⫽
or predictor variables. Discriminant function .008). That is, only persons in the high self-
analysis has two primary purposes, interpreta- determination group made significant im-
tion of data and classification of data. Klecka provements in access to those benefits from
(1980) suggested, “a researcher is engaged in year 1 to 3. In addition, there were consis-
interpretation when studying the ways in tently fewer people in the high self-determi-
which groups differ–that is, is one able to nation group who lost benefits and more first
discriminate between groups on the basis of time receivers of benefits. Figure 2 compares
some set of characteristics?” (p. 9). The sec- total percentage of people in each group with
ond application, classification, involves the benefits at the third-year measurement, calcu-

136 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003


TABLE 2

Results from chi square analyses comparing differences in low and high self-determination groups in adult
outcome areas (“yes” responses only)

Low SD High SD

Actual Expected Actual Expected p-value

Area Year 1 3 1 3 1 3 1 3 1 3

Living Independently 1 2 2.7 5.2 5 10 3.3 6.8 .15 .03


Living other than HS home 17 5 13.3 8.7 12 14 15.7 10.3 .03 .028
Maintain bank account 11 11 13 16 24 19 19 16 .001 .123
Received job training 11 7 11 10.3 13 16 13 12.7 .61 .05
Held a job since high school 13 15 14.6 17.9 24 25 22.4 22.1 .15 .02
Holds job currently 10 13 13.9 13.4 21 17 17.1 16.6 .02 .52
Work full time 8 11 5.1 10.3 6 12 8.9 12.7 .04 .45
Work part time 3 3 6.1 3.6 13 5 9.9 4.4 .04 .48

SD ⫽ Self-Determination

lated by subtracting the total lost from the 3 provides group statistics from the discrimi-
initial percentage of members having the ben- nant function analysis, including means and
efit and then adding in the percent who did standard deviations for IQ and self-determina-
not have the benefit in the first-year but who tion scores. Table 4 provides data on tests of
did by the third-year. equality of group means, canonical discrimi-
Because there were substantial, if not statis- nant functions identified via the analysis, the
tically significant, differences in IQ between function structure matrix, and classification
the low and high self-determination groups, results. Tests of Equality of Group Means, re-
we conducted the discriminant function anal- porting the outcomes of analyses of variance
ysis, as described previously, for each variable computed for each variable, indicated signifi-
identified through chi-square analyses. Table cant differences in self-determination scores

Figure 1. Indicators of financial independence for low and high self-determination groups (Split 1 SD above
and below mean self-determination score. * Significant for high self-determination group only at p >
.05 using McNemar test for significance of changes. ** Significant differences in year 1 between
both groups on chi-square test).

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 137


Figure 2. Percent total with benefits at year 3 by self-determination status (median split). * Significant
differences for high self-determination group only on McNemar test of changes.

across all variables except living outside the variables, job training since high school, hold-
family home 1-year post high school and work- ing a job currently, and working part-time in
ing full-time 1-year post graduation. There year 1. Chi-square analyses conducted as part
were significant differences for IQ on all but 3 of the canonical discriminant function analy-

TABLE 3

Discriminant Function Analysis: Group Statistics

Mean Std Deviation

Variable Level IQ SD IQ SD

Live Year 3 Family 78.68 99.45 21.74 21.45


Independently 94.04 111.15 17.14 16.78
Live Other HS, Year 1 Yes 84.56 103.81 21.74 18.72
No 88.69 106.04 22.24 20.47
Live Other HS, Year 3 Yes 77.84 98.89 21.45 21.02
No 90.39 108.60 20.09 18.20
Bank acct Yr 1 Yes 86.14 106.42 20.24 17.54
No 74.46 93.38 23.51 24.39
Job Training since HS, Yr 3 Yes 84.62 108.75 23.94 14.71
No 80.51 96.54 17.47 24.37
Held Job since HS, Yr 3 Yes 84.58 105.40 20.17 18.44
No 65.45 84.72 20.61 26.15
Holds job currently, Yr 1 Yes 85.44 106.49 19.21 18.69
No 77.24 94.58 25.95 21.94
Work full time, Yr 1 Yes 91.50 101.43 19.84 19.53
No 79.93 104.56 21.51 21.34
Work part time, Yr 1 Yes 81.97 110.40 19.79 17.08
No 84.92 99.47 23.00 21.03

SD ⫽ Self-Determination

138 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003


TABLE 4

Discriminant Function Analysis: Tests of Equality of Group Means, Summary of Canonical Discriminant
Functions, and Function Structure Matrix

Function
Test of Equality Canonical Discriminant Structure
of Group Means Functions Matrix

Wilk’s
Variable IQ SD Lambda Chi-square IQ SD

Live: Yr 3 F(1, 86) ⫽ 10.54, F(1, 86) ⫽ 6.29, .858 13.06, p ⫽ .001 .859 .664
p ⫽ .002 p ⫽ .014
Live Other HS, Yr 1 F(1, 74) ⫽ .571, F(1, 74) ⫽ .215, .991 .690, p ⫽ .708 .901 .554
p ⫽ .002 p ⫽ .644
Live Other HS, Yr 3 F(1, 92) ⫽ 8.15, F(1, 92) ⫽ 5.37, .890 10.626, p ⫽ .005 .846 .687
p ⫽ .005 p ⫽ .023
Bank acct Yr 1 F(1, 92) ⫽ 5.72, F(1, 92) ⫽ 8.29, .886 10.99, p ⫽ .004 .696 .838
p ⫽ .019 p ⫽ .005
Job Training HS Yr 3 F(1, 89) ⫽ .859, F(1, 89) ⫽ 8.57, .912 10.99, p ⫽ .004 .316 .998
p ⫽ .357 p ⫽ .004
Held Job HS, Yr 3 F(1, 91) ⫽ 8.68, F(1, 91) ⫽ 10.97, .847 14.95, p ⫽ .001 .588 .701
p ⫽ .004 p ⫽ .001
Current job, Yr 1 F(1, 92) ⫽ 2.94, F(1, 92) ⫽ 7.29, .914 8.158, p ⫽ .017 .581 .918
p ⫽ .091 p ⫽ .008
Work full time, Yr 1 F(1, 87) ⫽ 6.052, F(1, 87) ⫽ .451, .917 7.424, p ⫽ .024 .878 ⫺.240
p ⫽ .016 p ⫽ .504
Work part time, Yr 1 F(1, 86) ⫽ .395, F(1, 86) ⫽ 6.7653, .908 8.25, p ⫽ .016 ⫺.212 .878
p ⫽ .531 p ⫽ .011

SD ⫽ Self-Determination

sis replicated findings for the sample as a dents with disabilities. As reflected in Table 2,
whole from the chi-square analyses for the by one-year after high school, students in the
high and low groups configured based on the high self-determination group were dispro-
standard deviation split (e.g., Table 2) with portionately likely to have moved from where
the exception of one variable, living some- they were living during high school, and by
where other than the student’s high school the third-year they were still disproportion-
home. Finally, the function structure matrix, ately likely to live somewhere other then their
providing within groups correlations of each high school home and were significantly more
predictor variable with the canonical variable likely to live independently. As depicted in
and indicating which variable has the largest Figure 1 and Table 1, there were several indi-
correlation with the canonical variable score, cators of financial independence, with stu-
indicated that in 5 of the 9 variables, the self- dents in the high self-determination group
determination score was the more useful vari- more likely to maintain a bank account by
able in the discriminant function. year 1. There were also significant changes in
paying for their own groceries by year 3, as
well as a trend toward greater financial inde-
Discussion
pendence across multiple indicators (Figure
These results replicate findings from our ear- 1). This trend in financial independence is
lier study examining adult outcomes one-year likely due to differences in employment status
after high school (Wehmeyer & Schwartz, and training. Students in the high self-deter-
1997) and provide additional validation of im- mination group were disproportionately likely
portance of self-determination in lives of stu- to hold a job by the first-year follow-up, work-

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 139


ing either full- or part-time and to have held a self-determined, our own work and that of
job or received job training by year 3. Finally, others suggests that intelligence level, in and
for students across the complete sample who of itself, cannot account for differences in
were employed, those scoring higher in self- self-determination. Results from Wehmeyer
determination made statistically significant and Schwartz (1997) were consistent with
advances in obtaining job benefits, including those in this study and there were no signifi-
vacation and sick leave and health insurance, cant differences on IQ scores between low and
an outcome not shared by their peers in the high self-determination groups in that study.
low self-determination group. Overall, there Wehmeyer and Bolding (1999) conducted a
was not a single question on which the low matched-samples study to examine the role of
self-determination group fared more posi- environmental settings on the self-determina-
tively than the high self-determination group. tion of people with mental retardation inde-
Although there were no statistically signifi- pendent of the contribution of level of intelli-
cant differences between the low and high gence. Two-hundred and seventy-three adults
self-determination groups on IQ scores, there with mental retardation were recruited based
was a difference of over 6 points between on whether they worked or lived in one of
means of the groups that should be consid- three environments hypothesized to limit or
ered when interpreting these outcomes. To promote self-determination; (1) community-
ensure that differences in outcomes between based (e.g., independent living or competitive
the groups were not just a function of cogni- employment), (2) community-based congre-
tive ability, we conducted discriminant func- gate (e.g., group home or sheltered employ-
tion analysis, as summarized in Tables 3 and 4 ment), and (3) non-community based congre-
and in the Results section. These findings il- gate (e.g., institution, work activity program).
lustrate the complexity of untangling the im- Participants in each environmental group
pact of self-determination from other vari- were matched with one other person in each
ables, particularly intelligence, but also other group based on IQ score (within 5
support the fact that when considering how to points), and, when possible, by age and gen-
promote positive outcomes, educators and der. This resulted in 91 matched triplets, in
others are well served to consider factors like which individuals differed only by the environ-
self-determination in addition to intelligence ment in which they lived or worked. Data
testing information. The impact of intelli- analysis indicated significant differences in
gence on positive adult outcomes is, obviously, level of self-determination, autonomy, life sat-
not orthogonal to the impact of self-determi- isfaction, and opportunities to make choices
nation, as intellectual capacity also contrib- based on environment, with persons who lived
utes to one’s capacity to become self-deter- or worked in non-congregate community-
mined. However, our research suggests that based settings having more adaptive levels on
students’ opportunities to learn skills related each measure (despite the fact that the groups
to self-determination and to practice such were similar in mean age and mean IQ
skills are often limited by teacher beliefs about scores). Wehmeyer and Bolding (2001) also
student capacity to become self-determined as conducted a within-person study (thus con-
a function of the severity of the student’s in- trolling for IQ) of individuals with mental re-
tellectual capacity. Wehmeyer, Agran, and tardation who were moving from a more re-
Hughes (2000) found that teachers working strictive setting (group home, sheltered
with students with severe cognitive disabilities workshop) to a less restrictive setting (sup-
were significantly less likely to use student- ported living or work). The self-determination
directed learning strategies, which teach stu- of each person was measured 6 months (on
dents to self-regulate learning and behavior, the average) prior to and after his or her move
than teachers working with students with mild and changes in self-determination scores as a
disabilities, primarily because they did not be- function of the change in environments were
lieve that these students were capable of be- looked at. There were significant increases in
coming more self-determined. self-determination scores as a function of
While there is no doubt that intellectual movement to environments that, presumably,
ability contributes to one’s capacity to become supported more choice and autonomy.

140 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003


Recently Stancliffe, Abery, and Smith in the same classrooms. In fact, the impact of
(2000) reported findings from a study that collapsing the outcomes for young adults with
examined the personal control exercised by learning disabilities with those for young
74 adults with mental retardation who lived in adults with mental retardation would, hypo-
community-based settings. Using multiple thetically, be to constrain the overall findings.
measures of adaptive and challenging behav- That is, for a variety of reasons, it may be more
ior, self-determination knowledge and compe- likely that students with mental retardation
tencies, and environmental variables, these would have less positive adult outcomes than
authors found that personal characteristics their peers with learning disabilities, and thus
(intelligence, adaptive behavior, challenging collapsing outcomes limits the potential for
behaviors), self-determination skills, and envi- differences based on self-determination status.
ronmental factors (residential size, type of While we recognize this, we believe it is impor-
funding stream, community living situation) tant to look at outcomes related to self-deter-
all contributed to personal control. These and mination across all students and in this case
other findings suggest that while IQ is related we believe that the experiences of students
to self-determination, other factors contribute with learning disabilities and of students with
significantly to this outcome. In all, these find- mental retardation, particularly students with
ings call for the need to look at multiple vari- limited support needs, are similar enough to
ables that go beyond intelligence test scores to warrant examining those outcomes together.
examine successful outcomes for youth, in- Nevertheless, there is a need to examine such
cluding self-determination. outcomes for each disability category and to
Another competing explanation for differ- control more closely for differences that
ences in adult outcomes was the school expe- might have been introduced by using differ-
riences of students. That is, it is possible that ent sub-samples (primarily differential school
students in the high self-determination group and learning experiences and employment
received qualitatively different (i.e., better) opportunities) in subsequent research.
educational services and supports than their There are other limitations inherent in this
peers in the low self-determination group and, study, which are pertinent to most longitudi-
as such, differences in adult outcomes could nal investigations. All former students did not
be attributed to educational experiences answer their own questions (Peraino, 1990),
rather than self-determination. We find this and the data-collection methods for follow-up
explanation unlikely, given that data were col- surveys included both mail and telephone for-
lected from public school districts in seven mats, rather than a consistent method for ev-
states and that students from the same school ery survey. Halpern (1990) recommended
districts were represented in both the high personal and telephone interviews over mail
and low self-determination groups. In addi- surveys, but expenses and proximity of partic-
tion, our sample included both students with ipants to research sites precluded obtaining
mental retardation and students with learning the information exclusively through inter-
disabilities and students from two sub-sam- views. These limitations should be considered
ples. We did such to ensure that we could along with benefits of collecting longitudinal
recruit a large enough sample to conduct re- data.
search activities. We examined differences in Certainly self-determination status did not
students in the original (Wehmeyer & contribute to differences in all areas or on all
Schwartz, 1997) sample and students re- items. However, in addition to statistically sig-
cruited strictly for this study, and there were nificant differences between groups and
no observable differences in variables, includ- changes from first to third-year measurement
ing IQ scores. As to including students from periods, a general trend showed that students
two disability categories, there was no theoret- in the high self-determination group were
ical reason that students in either disability achieving more successful outcomes. As men-
category would not show benefits in adult out- tioned previously, there was no question or
comes from greater self-determination. In item on which students in the low self-deter-
many cases these students were served in sim- mination group fared better then the high
ilar educational programs and, in some cases, group. In all, we believe that findings from

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 141


this study support ongoing efforts to enhance Field, S. (1996). Self-determination instructional
self-determination in relation to more positive strategies for youth with learning disabilities. Jour-
transition outcomes. The next step in evaluat- nal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 40 –52.
ing the impact of such efforts, in addition to Field, S., & Hoffman, A. (1996a). Steps to self-determi-
nation. Austin, TX: ProEd.
replication of these findings, would be to ex-
Field, S., & Hoffman, A. (1996b). Promoting self-
amine longer term outcomes for students who
determination in school reform, individualized
receive specific interventions that promote planning, and curriculum efforts. In D. J. Sands &
self-determination, compared with students M. L. Wehmeyer (Eds.), Self-determination across the
who do not receive similar learning experi- life span: Independence and choice for people with dis-
ences. Such an examination would provide abilities (pp. 197–213). Baltimore: Paul H.
the causal link between self-determination Brookes.
and positive outcomes missing from this study. Field, S., Martin, J., Miller, R., Ward, M., & Weh-
Implications for practice. Over the past de- meyer, M. (1998a). A practical guide to teaching
cade there has been increasing awareness that self-determination. Reston, VA: Council for Excep-
promoting or enhancing self-determination is tional Children.
Field, S., Martin, J. E., Miller, R., Ward, M. J, &
an important effort. This study provides fur-
Wehmeyer, M. L. (1998b). Self-determination for
ther confirmation of this direction, emphasiz- persons with disabilities: A position statement of
ing the potential benefit to students who leave the Division on Career Development and Transi-
school as self-determined young people. As tion. Career Development for Exceptional Individuals,
described in the Introduction, there are now 21, 113–128.
multiple resources to support teachers to pro- Fullterton, A. (1998). Putting feet on my dreams:
mote self-determination (Field et al., 1998a; Involving students with autism in life planning. In
Wehmeyer et al, 1998; Wehmeyer, Palmer, et M. L. Wehmeyer & D. J. Sands (Eds.), Making it
al., 2000). Moreover, it is the case that many happen: Student involvement in education planning,
state and local curricula for all students in- decision-making and instruction (pp. 279 –296). Bal-
timore: Paul H. Brookes.
clude standards and benchmarks pertaining
Gast, D. L., Jacobs, H. A., Logan, K. R., Murray,
to self-determination related skills (e.g., goal
A. S., Holloway, A., & Long, L. (2000). Pre-session
setting, decision making, problem solving, assessment of preferences for students with pro-
etc.), and instruction to promote self-determi- found multiple disabilities. Education and Training
nation can both enhance student access to the in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
general curriculum and promote student ca- 35, 393– 405.
pacity to progress in the curriculum (Weh- Halpern, A. S. (1990). A methodological review of
meyer, Lattin, & Agran, 2001). follow-up and follow-along studies tracking school
leavers from special education. Career Development
for Exceptional Individuals, 13, 13–27.
References Halpern, A. S., Herr, C. M., Wolf, N. K., Lawson,
J. D., Doren, B., & Johnson, M. D. (1995). NEXT
Abery, B. H., Stancliffe, R. J., Smith, J., McGrew, K., S.T.E.P.: Student transition and educational plan-
& Eggebeen, A. (1995a). Minnesota Opportunities ning. Teacher manual. Eugene, OR: University of
and Exercise of Self-Determination Scale–Adult Edition. Oregon.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, Institute Hickson, L., & Khemka, I. (1999). Decision-making
on Community Integration. and mental retardation. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.),
Abery, B. H., Stancliffe, R. J., Smith, J., McGrew, K., International review of research in mental retardation
& Eggebeen, A. (1995b). Minnesota Self-Determina- (Vol. 22, pp. 227–265). San Diego: Academic
tion Skills, Attitudes, and Knowledge Evaluation Press.
Scale–Adult Edition. Minneapolis: University of Jaskulski, T., Metzler, C., & Zierman, S. A. (1990).
Minnesota, Institute on Community Integration. Forging a new era: The 1990 reports on people with
Cross, T., Cooke, N. L., Wood, W. W., & Test, D. W. developmental disabilities. Washington, D. C.: Na-
(1999). Comparison of the effects of MAPS and tional Association of Developmental Disabilities
ChoiceMaker on student self-determination skills. Councils.
Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Khemka, I. (2000). Increasing independent deci-
Developmental Disabilities, 34, 499 –510. sion-making skills of women with mental retarda-
Erwin, E. J., & Brown, F. (November, 2000). Vari- tion in simulated interpersonal situations of
ables that contribute to self-determination in abuse. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105,
early childhood. TASH Newsletter, 26(3), 8 –10. 387– 401.

142 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003


Klecka, W. R. (1980). Discriminant analysis. Beverly autonomy: Promoting self-competence in children and
Hills, CA: Sage Publications. youth with disabilities (pp. 275- 290). Baltimore:
Martin, J. E., & Marshall, L. H. (1995). Choice- Paul H. Brookes.
Maker: A comprehensive self-determination tran- Wehmeyer, M. L. (1996). A self-report measure of
sition program. Intervention in School and Clinic, self-determination for adolescents with cognitive
30, 147–156. disabilities. Education and Training in Mental Retar-
Peraino, J. M. (1992). Post-21 follow-up studies: dation and Developmental Disabilities, 31, 282–293
How do special education graduates fare? In P. Wehmeyer, M. L. (1998). Self-determination and
Wehman (Ed.), Life beyond the classroom: Transition individuals with significant disabilities: Examin-
strategies for young people with disabilities (pp. 21– ing meanings and misinterpretations. Journal of
70). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 23,
Powers, L., Sowers, J., Turner, A., Nesbitt, M., 5–16.
Knowles, E., & Ellison, R. (1996). TAKE Wehmeyer, M. L. (2001). Self-determination and
CHARGE: A model for promoting self-determina- mental retardation. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), Inter-
tion among adolescents with challenges. In L. E. national review of research in mental retardation (Vol.
Powers, G. H. S. Singer, & J. Sowers (Eds.), Pro- 24, pp. 1– 48). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
moting self-competence in children and youth with dis- Wehmeyer, M. L., Agran, M., & Hughes, C. (1998).
abilities (pp. 291–322). Baltimore: Paul H. Teaching self-determination to students with disabili-
Brookes. ties: Basic skills for successful transition. Baltimore:
Sands, D., Spencer, K., Gliner, J., & Swaim, R. Paul H. Brookes.
(1999). Structural equation modeling of student Wehmeyer, M. L., Agran, M., & Hughes, C. (2000).
involvement in transition-related actions: The A national survey of teachers’ promotion of self-
path of least resistance. Focus on Autism and Other determination and student-directed learning.
Developmental Disabilities, 14, 17–27. Journal of Special Education, 34, 58 – 68.
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behav- Wehmeyer, M. L., & Bolding, N. (1999). Self-deter-
ioral sciences. New York: McGraw Hill. mination across living and working environ-
Sowers, J., & Powers, L. (1995). Enhancing the par- ments: A matched-samples study of adults with
ticipation and independence of students with se- mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 37, 353–
vere physical and multiple disabilities in perform- 363.
ing community activities. Mental Retardation, 33, Wehmeyer, M. L., & Bolding, N. (2001). Enhanced
209 –220. self-determination of adults with intellectual dis-
Stancliffe, R. J. (1997). Community living-unit size, ability as an outcome of moving to community-
staff presence and resident’s choice-making. Men- based work or living environments. Journal of In-
tal Retardation, 35, 1–9. tellectual Disability Research, 45, 1–13.
Stancliffe, R. J., & Abery, B. H. (1997). Longitudinal Wehmeyer, M. L., & Kelchner, K. (1995). The Arc’s
study of deinstitutionalization and the exercise of self-determination scale. Silver Springs, MD: The Arc
choice. Mental Retardation, 35, 159 –169. of the United States.
Stancliffe, R. J., Abery, B. H., & Smith, J. (2000). Wehmeyer, M. L., Kelchner, K., & Richards, S.
Personal control and the ecology of community- (1995). Individual and environmental factors re-
living settings: Beyond living-unit size and type. lated to the self-determination of adults with men-
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105, 431– tal retardation. Journal of Vocational Rehabilitation,
454. 5, 291–305.
Stancliffe, R. J., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (1995). Vari- Wehmeyer, M. L., Kelchner, K., & Richards. S.
ability in the availability of choice to adults with (1996). Essential characteristics of self-deter-
mental retardation. Journal of Vocational Rehabili- mined behaviors of adults with mental retarda-
tation, 5, 319 –328. tion and developmental disabilities. American Jour-
Test, D. W., Karvonen, M., Wood, W. M., Browder, nal on Mental Retardation, 100, 632– 642.
D., & Algozzine, B. (2000). Choosing a self-deter- Wehmeyer, M. L., Lattin, D., & Agran, M. (2001).
mination curriculum: Plan for the future. Teach- Promoting access to the general curriculum for
ing Exceptional Children, 33(2), 48 –54. students with mental retardation: A decision-mak-
Wagner, M., D’Amico, R., Marder, C., Newman, L., ing model. Education and Training in Mental Retar-
& Blackkorby, J. (1992). What happens next? Trends dation and Developmental Disabilities, 36, 329 –344.
in postschool outcomes of youth with disabilities. Menlo Wehmeyer, M. L., & Metzler, C. A. (1995). How
Park, CA: SRI International. self-determined are people with mental retarda-
Ward, M. J., & Kohler, P. D. (1996). Promoting tion? The National Consumer Survey. Mental Re-
self-determination for individuals with disabili- tardation, 33, 111–119.
ties: Content and process. In L. E. Powers, Wehmeyer, M. L., & Palmer, S. (2000). Promoting
G. H. S. Singer, & J. Sowers (Eds.), On the road to the acquisition and development of self-determi-

Self-Determination And Adult Outcomes / 143


nation in young children with disabilities. Early relationship between self-determination, qual-
Education and Development, 11, 465– 481. ity of life, and life satisfaction for adults with
Wehmeyer, M. L., Palmer, S. B., Agran, M., Mith- mental retardation. Education and Training in
aug, D. E., & Martin, J. (2000). Teaching students Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
to become causal agents in their lives: The self- 33, 3–12.
determining learning model of instruction. Excep- Wolman, J., Campeau, P., Dubois, P., Mithaug, D., &
tional Children, 66, 439 – 453. Stolarski, V. (1994). AIR self-determination scale and
Wehmeyer, M. L., & Sands, D. J. (1998). Making it user guide. Palo Alto, CA: American Institute for
happen: Student involvement in education planning, Research.
decision-making and instruction. Baltimore: Paul H. Zhang, D. (1998). The effect of self-determination in-
Brookes. struction on high school students with mild disabilities.
Wehmeyer, M. L., & Schwartz, M. (1997). Self-de- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
termination and positive adult outcomes: A fol- New Orleans.
low-up study of youth with mental retardation or
learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 63, 245– Received: 4 June 2002
255. Initial Acceptance: 15 July 2002
Wehmeyer, M. L., & Schwartz, M. (1998). The Final Acceptance: 30 October 2002

144 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-June 2003

You might also like