You are on page 1of 4

Table 1.

Amino acid composition of heme- dues providing peptide bonds known to Thus while the results of tryptic hy-
rythrin.
Residespr 13500
be susceptible to cleavage by this drolyses indicate the presence of two
Amino acid
Residues per 13,500 g enzyme. All attempts to separate the kinds of subunits, this conclusion is not
of protein presumed different subunits have failed. unequivocal since the observations from
Lysine 10.5 These attempts included (i) chroma- chymotryptic hydrolyses could also be
Histidine 6.5 tography on DEAE-Sephadex, CM- rationalized in terms of a single chain.
Arginine 3.03 Sephadex, and Amberlite IRC-50 under In either event it is clear that variations
Aspartic acid 16.2 a variety of conditions, with and without in peptide constitution of the subunits
Threonine 4.35 do occur from one individual to an-
3.38
urea, and (ii) electrophoresis in starch
Serine other (8).
Glutamic acid 9.5 gel under different conditions and with
a variety of dissociating reagents. Some W. R. GROSKOPF
Proline 4.03
Glycine 6.2 of these efforts have produced resolv- J. W. HOLLEMAN
Alamine 5.3 able peaks but these peaks have shown I. M. KLOTZ
1/2 Cystine 0.96 peptide maps identical with those from Department of Chemistry, Northwestern
Valine 3.72 the original protein. An alternative in- University, Evanston, Illinois
Methionine 0.85 terpretation must therefore be borne in S. KERESZTES-NAGY
Isoleucine 8.34 mind, namely, that the subunits are E. MARGOLIASH
Leucine 7.36 basically identical (4) but that the link- Abbott Laboratories, Chicago, Illinois
Tyrosine 4.8
8.5 ages susceptible to attack by trypsin References and Notes
Phenylalanine
Ammonia 12.2 are so grouped that more peptides are
1. I. M. Klotz and S. Keresztes-Nagy, Nature
produced than would be expected

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on November 15, 2018


195, 900 (1962).
from the number of susceptible bonds, 2. , Biol. Bull. 123, 484 (1962).
3. The prefix mero, indicating "part of," has been
because of occasional partial hydrol- used [Biochemistry 2, 445 (1963)] following
alike one would expect 14 to 15 pep- ysis. similar connotations in meromysin, suggested
by A. G. Szent-Gyorgi, Arch. Biochem. Bio-
tides in a peptide map of a tryptic A further complication in the struc- phys. 42, 305 (1953).
4. C. Manwell, Science 139, 755 (1963).
hydrolysate, whereas the number actu- ture of hemerythrin arises from obser- 5. 1. M. Klotz, T. A. Klotz, H. A. Fiess, Arch.
ally found is 28 to 30. The same num- vations of differences in peptide maps Biochem. Biophys. 68, 284 (1957).
6. Tryspin was treated with acid to denature any
ber of tryptic peptides has been re- of enzymic digests of the protein of chymotrypsin which might have been present
ported by Manwell (4). individual animals. One such difference as a contaminant.
7. E. Margoliash and E. L. Smith, J. Biol. Chem.
The most obvious interpretation of has been reported by Manwell (4). We 237, 2151 (1962).
these results is that the subunits of have found in the chymotryptic digests, 8. Supported in part by grant GM-09280 from the
National Institutes of Health. We thank Dr.
hemerythrin are of two distinct types differences indicative of substitutions of F. C. Neuhaus for advice and assistance in the
amino acids in at least one, and possi- use of the amino acid analyzer.
with major differences in the respective
sequences of amino acids. On the other bly more, positions in the chain. Fig- 23 May 1963
hand, peptide maps of chymotryptic ure 1 shows the peptide map of the
hydrolysates of either pooled hemeryth- chymotryptic digest of pooled heme-
rin or protein from individual worms rythrin. When similar chymotryptic
do not show substantially more peptide digests of the hemerythrin of individual Adaptation to Chromatic Aberration
spots than can be accounted for by the worms are examined, a class of worms by the Human Visual System
sum of the number of amino acid resi- may be distinguished, in which the pep-
tides marked 1 3B (basic, tryptophan- Abstract. Prolonged exposure to the
containing) and 3A (acidic, tyrosine- chromatic dispersion produced by
NEUTRAL + containing) are present, and another prism spectacles leads to a perceptual
PEPTIDES ACIDIC PEPTIDES in which they are absent. These two
BASIC PEPTIDES
adaptation. The adaptation develops
68
ORIGIN peptides are invariably either present or rapidly in the first two days of the
IB INT~N absent together. This suggests the sub- wearing of the spectacles, and seems to
3B

_Q28)YB 4~B 78O


Q IA stitution of a residue by either tyrosine be a special reaction to the relative
or tryptophan, thus affording a new change in illuminance at intensity
SBO l~B
IOB c.12B
15B
9 0 3A point of chymotryptic attack, or the gradients on the retina.
deletion of a portion of the peptide
\J
( 6AgA chain. Perceptual adaptation to changes of
(90 0 the eyes' normal optical linkage to the
138 16B 2A
The variation in peptides 1 3B and
U V4A
n 3A is the most obvious because they environment may be experienced by
( 6N reveal amino acids which are easily anyone who, wearing a new pair of
7N detected by specific color reagents. It glasses, is at first disturbed by the dis-
ONA e9N
is probably not the only one that exists tortional side-effects. Such adaptation
in hemerythrins from the species Gol- suggests the existence of regulating
17B0 X S0A fingia gouldii. Manwell (4) has found mechanisms underlying normal vision,
variations in electrophoretic behavior and provides a novel perspective on the
in the protein from individual worms dependence of perception on past ex-
012 N of the same species which he suggests perience. Among the most interesting
may be due to a single substitution of these adaptation phenomena are
Fig. 1. Peptide map of a chymotryptic of an acidic residue for an uncharged those reported by Gibson (1) and
hydrolysate of hemerythrin. one. Kohler (2) for the image aberrations
12 JULY 1963 167
6 TARGET LUMINANCE = 0.4 MLAM. TARGET LUMINANCE gradient. Our subjects, who had good
= 40.0 MLAM.
uncorrected vision, wore 20-prism
RELATIVE LUMINANCE = 1.25 RELATIVE LUMINANCE = 1.25 diopter spectacles continuously for 10
C)
cr 4
0
0 0
0
days, removing them only for the daily
LL
battery of tests that lasted an hour.
0 The adaptation response was meas-
21 * 0
ured by an indirect technique sug-
z
0 gested by Kohler; its power to com-
m
0 .0 pensate for prismatic dispersion was
determined. With the prism spectacles
z
0 *- removed, the subject viewed an achro-
matic test pattern of vertical stripes
w
a-
Ul) 61 RELATIVE LUMINANCE = 14.0 RELATIVE LUMINANCE= 14.0
through a Hughes variable prism. His
0
task was to eliminate any apparent
color fringes by adjusting the power
4 of the variable prism. These "null-
adjustments" should be close to zero-
prism diopters before wearing prism
2 spectacles, evincing the subject's sensi-
* 0
tivity to prismatic dispersion. With the
development of an adaptation response
22

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on November 15, 2018


0 42 during spectacle wearing, the null-
adjustments should change in the direc-
2 4 6 8
ADAPTATION
10 12 14
PERIODj
16 18 2 4 6 8 O 12 14 16 18 tion of the spectacle prism power. The
amount of prismatic dispersion present
D ADAPTATION PERIOD
when the subject sees no color fringes
TESTING DAYS is our measure of the dispersion-com-
Fig. 1. Development of compensation for chromatic dispersion during prism spectacle pensation power of the adaptation
wearing. The ordinate values are the mean amounts of prismatic dispersion present when response.
the subjects reported no visual color fringes on the target. The data are shown for four This test was carried out under two
conditions of absolute and relative target luminance. kinds of illumination: (i) narrow-band
illumination with a peak at 642 mpt
and a half-width of 15 m,u; and (ii) full-
produced by prism spectacles. In con- matic fringes in remaining tied to the spectrum illumination produced by a
trast to the rapid correction of prism- edges of objects during eye movement. tungsten lamp with a color temperature
induced visual-motor errors (3), these In this they were different from or- of 32000K.
slower developing corrections of the dinary negative after-images of color, With the narrow-band illumination,
visual appearance of the environment which move with the eyes. (ii) They the strong saturation of the illuminant
seem to involve alterations of basic appeared both in polychromatic and hue seemed to obscure color fringes.
sensory functions. monochromatic illumination, but only However, three of our subjects con-
We have investigated adaptation to if the object differed in brightness from sistently reported fringes of blue and
the chromatic dispersion of the retinal its background. In this they were un- an intense red after ten days of spectacle
image produced by prism spectacles, like the prismatic fringes. (iii) They wearing. Confirmatory evidence that
extending Kohler's self-observations of were more definite in weak illumina- these color fringes were in fact pro-
this phenomenon. When prisms are tion than in strong, again unlike the duced within the visual system, and not
mounted with identical orientations prismatic ones. by some residual dispersion of the
before the two eyes, the dispersion they Observations (i) and (ii) suggest retinal image, was provided by the fact
produce manifests itself visually in the that the adaptation to prismatic disper- that adjusting the variable prism had
appearance of color fringes wherever sion consists of a special reaction of the no effect on them.
the short- and long-wavelength com- visual system to intensity gradients in With full-spectrum illumination, sub-
ponents of the retinal image fail to the retinal image, gradients of a kind jects could always find a setting of the
overlap. With base-left prisms, for ex- related to those which ordinarily elicit variable prism which eliminated color
ample, bluish fringes are seen on the simple edge perception. This adaptation fringes. On either side of this setting,
right of light regions, and reddish response manifests itself in an attenua- fringes of opposite hue would be pro-
fringes on the left. After prolonged tion of prism-produced color fringes duced. At this "visual null point" the
wearing, the strength of the fringes may and, in the absence of prismatic dis- adaptation response must have exactly
noticeably diminish. More striking, an persion, in the appearance of reversed compensated for the image dispersion
opposition illusion takes place after the color fringes. Observation (iii) seems present.
spectacles are removed: blue fringes to be a clue to just what properties of Figure 1 illustrates the development
on the left of light regions, and red on an intensity gradient evoke the adap- of this compensation for prismatic dis-
the right. tation response. persion, evincing the growth of an
Kohler made three important obser- We have measured the development adaptation response. The subjects were
vations on these after-effect color of this adaptation response in six sub- tested for 4 days prior to putting on
fringes as he experienced them: (i) jects, and tested its dependence on the spectacles; then on alternate days
They behaved like the original pris- various parameters of image intensity during the wearing period. Three of
168 SCIENCE, VOL. 141
the subjects were tested for 4 days which we measured to determine com- compensation for prismatic dispersion
after removal; the others continued pensation power, produces a relative consists of an alteration of some normal
wearing the spectacles, in connection displacement of the long- and short- psychophysical function of relative
with a study of adaptation to spatial wavelength components of the target target luminance, such as contour for-
distortion. Prismatic dispersion is indi- image. Thus the long-wavelength image mation or color contrast.
cated in terms of a standard measure, of the target is partially superimposed The possibility of similar adaptation
namely, the relative angular displace- on the short-wavelength image of the to the chromatic aberration produced
ment of the Fraunhofer C and F lines, background, and vice versa. The color by spherical spectacle lenses, or to the
which bracket the region of the spec- fringes which a prism initially pro- intrinsic chromatic aberration of the
trum to which the eyes are most sensi- duces are due, then, to the difference eye (4), is not established. The disper-
tive. In terms of this measure, the between the luminance of the target and sion produced by these differs in certain
spectacles worn by the subjects had a that of the background. This luminance important respects from that of the
dispersion of 10.2 minutes of arc. The difference will be increased both by prism spectacles. As a fundamental test,
data points show the mean compensa- increasing absolute target luminance however, it may be suggested that
tion power of the subjects' eyes, that is, alone (by increasing target and back- wherever adaptation such as that we
the amount of prismatic dispersion in ground luminance in proportion), and have studied takes place, it should mani-
the variable prism which was just bal- by increasing relative target luminance fest itself in ineradicable color fringes
anced by the adaptation response. The alone (by decreasing background lumi- along high contrast borders in mono-
solid lines describe the data in terms of nance). In accordance with this, the chromatic illumination.
a steady state before spectacle-wearing prism-produced color fringes are more JOHN C. HAY
and an exponential growth process dur- vivid in high illumination than in low, Department of Psychology, Smith
ing wearing. and along high-contrast borders than College, Northampton, Massachusetts

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on November 15, 2018


Two levels each of target-stripe ab- along low-contrast borders. HERBERT L. PICK, JR.
solute luminance (L1) and luminance This means that for a fixed amount Institute of Child Development,
relative to background (L,/L2) were of angular dispersion, a constant level University of Minnesota, Minneapolis
tested, giving the four developmental of compensation would require that EDWENNA RoSSER
curves shown in Fig. 1. Changing the adaptation response strength increase Swarthmore College,
relative luminance from 1.25 to 14.0 with increases of either absolute or rela- Swarthmore, Pennsylvania
produced no important change in the tive target luminance. Since we found
eyes' compensation power. Changing that the amount of compensated dis- References and Notes
the absolute luminance produced a persion did not change significantly with
change in compensation power which is increases of relative target luminance, 1. J. J. Gibson, J. Exptl. Psychol. 16, 1 (1932).
2. I. Kohler, Oesterreichische Akademie der Wis-
statistically significant (p < .01); a it follows that the strength of the adap- senschafteni, Philosophische-historische Klasse
hundred-fold increase in absolute target tation response must have increased in 227, 1 (1951).
3. R. Held and A. V. Hein, Perceptual and
luminance reduced the compensation proportion to relative target luminance. Motor Skills 8, 87 (1958).
power by approximately one-half. 4. S. H. Bartley, in Handbook of Experimental
On the other hand, since the amount of Psychology, S. S. Stevens, Ed. (Wiley, New
The same order of inverse relation- compensated dispersion decreased with York, 1951), p. 921.
5. Supported by grants M-6427(A) and M-
ship between compensation power and increases of absolute target luminance, 6430(A) from the National Institutes of
absolute target luminance was more we infer that the adaptation response Mental Health. We thank Dr. J. J. Gibson
for use of the facilities of the Cornell Uni-
general, in a special test series on the does not change in proportion to abso- versity Psychology Department, and Dr. Ed-
tenth day, throughout a luminance lute target luminance. win Hart who performed ophthalmological
examinations on our subjects before and after
range from 10-3 to 40 mlam. At the We may hypothesize, therefore, that the experiment.
lower end of this range, the compensa- the adaptation response underlying 12 April 1963
tion power actually exceeded the dis-
persion power of the spectacles. Below
this region of ordinary color sensitivity,
no color fringes were seen. Therefore
although polychromatic light is not Behavioral Response Rates in Pigeons:
necessary to evoke the adaptation re- Effect of a-Methyl-m-tyrosine
sponse, stimulation of the receptors
participating in wavelength discrimina- Abstract. An intramuscular injection of a-methyl-m-tyrosine (100 mg/kg),
tion is necessary. which differentially depletes serotonin and norepinephrine in both brain and
The effects of relative and absolute heart, was given to two groups of pigeons trained to peck at a key for food. The
target luminance on the adaptation re- first group received an injection 12 hours before the daily session and showed
sponse itself cannot be directly inferred no behavioral effect. Response rates of birds in the second group, which were
from their effects on its power to com- injected 30 minutes after the start of the daily session, decreased and returned
pensate for prismatic dispersion. Indeed, to normal within 9 hours after injection. Preliminary data on brain serotonin of
an analysis of the joint effects of pris- pigeons indicate that the disruption of the behavior follows the same time course
matic dispersion and target luminance as the change in serotonin.
on the retinal image indicates that the
adaptation response must vary with Since 1954, data have been accumu- levels of this amine affect behavior. One
relative target luminance, and to a lesser lated which suggest that 5-hydroxytryp- way to investigate the effect of serotonin
degree-if at all-with absolute target tamine (serotonin) has an important upon the central nervous system is to
luminance. Our reasoning is as follows. function in the central nervous system correlate the abnormal levels of this
The angular dispersion of the prism, and that variations in the physiological amine with changes in behavior. In-
12 JULY 1963 169
Adaptation to Chromatic Aberration by the Human Visual System
John C. Hay, Herbert L. Pick Jr. and Edwenna Rosser

Science 141 (3576), 167-169.


DOI: 10.1126/science.141.3576.167

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on November 15, 2018


ARTICLE TOOLS http://science.sciencemag.org/content/141/3576/167

REFERENCES This article cites 2 articles, 0 of which you can access for free
http://science.sciencemag.org/content/141/3576/167#BIBL

PERMISSIONS http://www.sciencemag.org/help/reprints-and-permissions

Use of this article is subject to the Terms of Service

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published by the American Association for the
Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. 2017 © The Authors, some rights
reserved; exclusive licensee American Association for the Advancement of Science. No claim to original U.S.
Government Works. The title Science is a registered trademark of AAAS.

You might also like