You are on page 1of 7

Canadian Psychology © 2010 Canadian Psychological Association

2010, Vol. 51, No. 2, 133–139 0708-5591/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018236

Clinical Psychology Graduate Students’ Perceptions of Their Scientific


and Practical Training: A Canadian Perspective

Daniel L. Peluso, R. Nicholas Carleton, and Gordon J. G. Asmundson


University of Regina

The scientist-practitioner model is the most commonly used training modality in Canadian clinical
psychology graduate programmes. Despite pervasive endorsement throughout Canadian psychology
programmes, there is a paucity of data available on Canadian student opinions of the model’s imple-
mentation. The current study assessed 134 students from 9 provinces with a 38-item questionnaire
developed by the Council of University Directors of Clinical Psychology for assessing students’
perceptions about the quantity, quality, and breadth of science training in their clinical psychology
doctoral programmes. Most students described their programs as providing a mix of research and clinical
focus, with slightly more weight given to research. Science training was reported as very important to
students, with indications they receive a good amount of high-quality training in science. Moreover, there
was a high level of agreement between desired levels of science training and the science training
received. Implications for future research and training are discussed.

Keywords: clinical psychology, clinical psychology graduate training, clinical psychology graduate
students, scientist-practitioner model

The primary goal of clinical psychology graduate programmes documentation regarding the early history of applied psychology is
in Canada, as expressed in many departmental mission statements, scant; however, it is widely held that training in professional and
is to train students according to the scientist-practitioner or “Boul- clinical psychology was prompted by the ending of the Second
der model” of training (Raimy, 1950). This model is geared toward World War (C. R. Myers, 1970). At that time, the utility of applied
training students to adequately develop both scientific research and psychology had been demonstrated through successful selec-
clinical practise skills (D. Myers, 2007). Most programmes ac- tion, training, and rehabilitation of military personnel (Vipond &
claim the model; however, each programme differs in the extent to Richert, 1977; Wright, 1974). Consequently, there was increasing
which the scientific or practical aspects of training are emphasised. demand for applied psychology in education, government, and
Moreover, incorporating a balance between research and clinical health sectors. Clinical training in Canada was not geared toward
training is indicated throughout the current Canadian Psychology professional specialisation; that is, training was encompassing
Association (CPA) guidelines for accreditation and ethical conduct and broad, offering students training in educational, industrial, and
(CPA, 2002). The question of what proportion of focus should be counselling sectors (Conway, 1984).
given to each aspect of training has been a subject of debate for The status of applied psychology amongst other academic dis-
over 50 years (Aspenson et al., 1993; D. Myers, 2007). Some ciplines was considered suspect, largely because the psychologists
Canadian researchers have argued that the scientist-practitioner of the day paid little attention to scientific research (Wright, 1969).
model is illusory and that each programme ultimately focuses on In an attempt to secure status and legitimize itself as a profession
whichever side—science or practise—the programme sees fit to in universities, psychology programmes began adopting stronger
emphasise (Conway, 1984). scientific foci. The shift was so profound that professional and
To understand the status of the scientist-practitioner model in a applied areas were eventually relegated to an inferior status in
Canadian context, it is necessary to understand the developmental favour of rigorous science and research (Conway, 1984).
history of clinical psychology training in Canada. The existing Growing concern over professionalization and the development
of professional training in psychology prompted the CPA to stage
the Opinicon conference in 1960. Despite efforts to the contrary,
Daniel L. Peluso, R. Nicholas Carleton, and Gordon J. G. Asmundson, the conference failed to address issues of professionalism and
Anxiety and Illness Behaviours Laboratory, University of Regina. training, focusing instead on developing the academic side of
Gordon J. G. Asmundson is supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health psychology (Gibson, 1974). It was during the Opinicon conference
Research (CIHR) Investigator Award; R. Nicholas Carleton is supported by that psychology was defined primarily as a science and secondarily
a CIHR Canada Graduate Scholarship Doctoral Research Award; and as a profession. This stringent operational definition served to
Daniel L. Peluso is supported by a SSHRC Canada Graduate Scholarship
widen the increasing gulf between scientists and professionals
Masters Award.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gordon (Conway, 1984).
J. G. Asmundson, Anxiety and Illness Behaviours Laboratory, University In May 1965, the CPA sponsored the Couchiching Conference
of Regina, Regina, Saskatchewan, S4S 0A2. E-mail: gordon on Professional Psychology. The primary purpose of this gathering
.asmundson@uregina.ca was to reformulate an operational definition of professional psy-

133
134 PELUSO, CARLETON, AND ASMUNDSON

chology. The vast majority of psychologists at this conference dian regions each met their own individual needs by altering the
advocated for a more balanced approach to training that would model to fit their requirements.
emphasise both academic and applied facets of training. Despite The debate regarding Canadian training models continues today.
substantial agreement, many vocal professionals remained polar- Despite the disagreements, empirical research on the scientist-
ized on these issues. For example, some clinicians argued for a practitioner model in Canada is scant. Indeed, implementation of
professional training model resembling the PsyD programmes science training in clinical psychology programmes has not been
found in the United States (Ewing, 1963; Sutherland, 1964), given systematically studied (Merlo, Collins, & Bernstein, 2008). The
the low research productivity amongst professional psychology paucity of empirical research is made worse because student
graduates (Kelly & Fiske, 1950; Levy, 1962). By contrast, there opinions on the matter have largely been unsolicited. Preliminary
were ardent academics who argued that professional psychologists evidence suggests that American student opinions regarding the
were merely “technicians”, and advocated for a focus on rigorous scientist-practitioner model vary widely, with some being staunch
science (Conway, 1984). Despite such disagreements a consensus advocates and others describing it as a training anachronism
was reached and the scientist-practitioner model leading to the (Aspenson et al., 1993). More recently, the Council of University
PhD was ultimately adopted (Webster, 1967). The clinician was Directors of Clinical Psychology (CUDCP)—a nonprofit organi-
reconceptualized as a scientist-professional who should have the sation whose purpose is to further graduate education in clinical
ability to produce useful research. Training in clinical skills was psychology programmes espousing the scientist-practitioner
seen as important, but secondary to training in the theory under- model—invited American and Canadian doctoral students to par-
lying those skills. This second hierarchical consensus was likely ticipate in a survey on their experiences. The researchers admin-
the result of therapeutic techniques regularly being invalidated istered a rigorously developed CUDCP questionnaire to 611 clin-
(Conway, 1984). ical psychology students, assessing their scientific and practical
From 1965 to 1980, psychology programmes in Canada expe- training experiences in doctoral programmes; however, the authors
rienced an unprecedented growth. In a veritable explosion of provided no indication of the percentage of Canadian respondents,
enrolment, psychology departments were rapidly becoming the if any. Results of that study suggest that students report a fairly
largest departments in many universities. Indeed, the number of balanced emphasis on science and clinical work in their pro-
grammes (Merlo et al., 2008). These two studies represent the
departments offering graduate programmes in psychology doubled
extent of available data on scientist-practitioner models from a
(Adair, 1981). Clinical psychology, as we know it today, experi-
student perspective; consequently, current understanding of stu-
enced a rebirth of sorts as a result of psychology’s rapidly bour-
dent perceptions of their graduate programmes in clinical psychol-
geoning popularity. In 1969, of the 29 professional graduate psy-
ogy remains limited.
chology programmes, 17 were in clinical psychology (Arthur,
Given the paucity of empirically driven research in this domain,
1971), though these programmes were not accredited by the CPA,
the purpose of the current study was to administer the CUDCP
and were substantially smaller in size than those that exist today;
questionnaire to a large sample of Canadian students and therein
currently, there are 23 CPA-accredited clinical programmes in
assess their experiences with the scientist-practitioner model. The
Canada.
current study will address three specific purposes. First, the results
The rapid expansion of clinical psychology resulted in regional
will broaden our understanding of Canadian clinical psychology
diversity regarding the adoption of the scientist-practitioner model. student’s training experiences. This is crucial given that the data
For instance, French-speaking programmes in Quebec had larger available in this domain from the United States is difficult to
enrolments and were generally more practise-oriented than other generalise to a Canadian sample because of differences which exist
Canadian programmes (Conway, 1984). Furthermore, doctoral amongst clinical and counselling programmes (and the unspecified
programmes differed in the length of required internships, with number of Canadian participants in the Merlo et al., 2008 study).
some institutions exclusively offering doctoral degrees (Conway, For instance, PsyD and counselling programmes do not figure
1984). Reflecting this diversity in training, the relevance of the prominently in Canada, as there is only one PsyD programme
scientist-practitioner model continued to be contested amongst accredited by the CPA, and only four counselling programmes
professionals and was not supported amongst nonacademic clinical are accredited by the CPA. Second, the information will provide
psychologists. The Boulder model was considered by some to be students with a summation of first hand experiences with scientist-
culturally irrelevant to the Canadian context (Gibson, 1974), given practitioner models. Currently, students may unknowingly choose
that the model was conceived in the United States, and would programmes that are incongruent with their goals, thereby restrict-
therefore not reflect Canadian needs and culture (e.g., greater ing their career paths because the relative emphasis on scientific or
contact between psychology and the community). As such, there practical aspects in graduate programmes is not explicitly stated
was a demand by some for a professional model that was tailored prior to enrolment (D. Myers, 2007). Third, the results will inform
to Canadian values and institutions. Davidson (1971) proposed the existing Canadian clinical psychology programmes in Canada
“researcher-consultant” model that was designed to be more ger- about their own student perceptions.
mane to Canadian psychology. This model advocated for a com-
munity psychology focus in which the psychologist would assess
Method
the community’s needs, and subsequently develop and evaluate
programmes that were identified by the community (Davidson, Procedure
1981). Despite these efforts, a definition of a Canadian training
model was never fully developed; consequently, the scientist- Participants (N ⫽ 136) were recruited from the CPA student
practitioner model became the dominant training modality. Cana- listserve via email. The distribution of sex was highly skewed,
TRAINING OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY STUDENTS 135

with 120 (88%) participants reporting being women (Mage ⫽ 26.9; extremely, or excellent), depending on the particular question. To
SD ⫽ 4.0) and only 16 (12%) reporting being men (Mage ⫽ 30.4; tailor the questionnaire to Canadian students, the term “qualifying
SD ⫽ 6.3). This disproportion in gender is reflective of the overall exams” was changed to “comprehensive exams”, given that the latter
composition of graduate students, according to Canadian pro- term is more frequently used in Canadian programmes. Sample items
gramme statistics as posted on programme websites. Students corresponding to each response choice include, “How much is science
interested in participating were directed to an online survey. Par- emphasised/integrated in your programme as a whole?”; “How effec-
ticipation was restricted to clinical psychology students who were tive is the science training you receive for your comprehensive ex-
currently enrolled in a clinical psychology programme in Canada. ams?”; and “Overall, how would you rate the quality of the science
Participants were informed that the study received ethical approval training you receive in your graduate programme?”
from the University of Regina Research Ethics Board. Further-
more, participants were assured that all responses would be kept Results
confidential, and no identifying information was obtained from the
students. Education levels ranged from students in their first year Emphasis on Science Training: Overall Programme
of graduate school through students in their eighth year or higher. and Specific Skills
Program year distribution was as follows: 5% first year students,
25% second year, 17% third year, 15% fourth year, 18% fifth year, The majority of students described their programs as providing
10% sixth year, 7% seventh year, and 2% eighth year or above. a mix of research and clinical focus. Scores ranged from 1 ( pri-
Approximately 9% of students did not disclose their year of study. marily clinical focus) to 5 ( primarily research focus). Approxi-
In terms of provincial representation, 52 (38%) of respondents mately 47% of students reported that their program’s training
were enrolled in Ontario, 29 (29%) in Quebec, 17 (13%) in emphasised clinical and research training equally, with a mean
Saskatchewan, 8 (6%) in Nova Scotia, 7 (5%) in British Columbia score of 3.2. No students reported that their program had a strictly
and Manitoba, respectively, and 2 (1.5%) in each of Alberta, clinical focus, whereas 5% reported an entirely research focus.
Newfoundland, and New Brunswick. The sample was primarily Students also described how much emphasis was placed on
White (90%), with a smaller proportion of Asian (4%), South seven different skill areas of traditional science training in their
Asian (1%), and Hispanic (2%) participants. Two (2%) partici- programs. Results are shown in Table 1. Students typically re-
pants endorsed the “other” category, and 13 (9%) chose not to ported that they received a good amount of training in these
respond to the question. specific science skills; however, more than a third (37%) of stu-
dents indicated that they received a minimal amount of training in
grant writing, and almost a third (29%) reported a minimal amount
Measures
of training in research projects being conducted by faculty mem-
Science training questionnaire. The current study employed bers. There were also statistically significant differences between
a 38-item questionnaire developed by the CUDCP Board of Di- the years of education and the perceived focus on grant writing by
rectors to assess current students’ perceptions about the quantity, the programme, F(7, 119) ⫽ 3.58, p ⬍ .01, ␩2 ⫽ .17. Based on
quality, and breadth of science training in their clinical psychology Tukey’s post hoc comparisons, the first year students report re-
doctoral programmes (Merlo et al., 2008). The authors of this ceiving significantly more emphasis on grant writing than the
questionnaire all serve as training directors of primarily scientist- fourth, t(119) ⫽ 3.75, p ⬍ .01, r2 ⫽ .11, fifth, t(119) ⫽ 4.22, p ⬍
practitioner programmes. The authors developed a detailed expla- .01, r2 ⫽ .13, and sixth, t(119) ⫽ 3.48, p ⬍ .05, r2 ⫽ .09, year
nation of science training to be utilised in completing the ques- students. There were no other statistically significant differences
tionnaire. Given that several competing perspectives on what between the groups and there were no other differences were found
consists of “science” training—as defined by each individual pro- based on students’ year of study in their program (all ps ⬎ .05).
gramme— exist in the literature, the authors decided to employ a
“more conventional definition of science.” (Merlo et al., 2008). Areas of Traditional Science Training
Prior to questionnaire development, science was defined as:
Students were also asked to report how much science is empha-
All research-related instruction and activities. Some examples are: (1) sised in six areas of their clinical psychology training, and how
courses in research design, methodology and statistics, and philoso- much they would like science to be integrated into these areas.
phy of science, (2) required participation in research projects (e.g., a These results are provided in Table 2. The emphasis on scientific
formal, empirical Master’s Thesis or other research projects), (3) a
training that students reported receiving differed significantly from
focus on using empirically supported treatments/assessment tech-
niques in clinical work, (4) participation in research projects being
their desired emphasis in two areas, including clinical placement
conducted by faculty members, (5) opportunities to become involved and practicum, wherein students wanted more emphasis, t(125) ⫽
in conference presentations and manuscript preparation, (6) grant- ⫺4.94, p ⬍ .001, r2 ⫽ .16, and research, wherein students wanted
writing or other related experience, and (7) a focus on critical analysis less emphasis, t(125) ⫽ 2.58, p ⬍ .011, r2 ⫽ .05.
and review of the literature.
Student Opinions of Traditional Science Training
These seven skills were seen to comprise well-rounded science
training in clinical psychology. Overall, the majority of students indicated that science training
Participants responded to the questionnaire using a 5-point was either fairly (44%) or very (19%) important to them. Only one
Likert-type scale with three different response anchors ranging respondent reported that science training was not at all important,
from 1 (none, not at all, or very poor) to 5 (a great amount, whereas a substantial minority (19%) answered extremely impor-
136 PELUSO, CARLETON, AND ASMUNDSON

Table 1
Students’ Perceptions of the Amount of Training in Seven Science Skills

Minimum–
maximum 1 (none) 2 (minimal 3 (fair 4 (good 5 (great
Skill M (SD) scores Mode ␹2(df) % amount) % amount) % amount) % amount) %

Required participation in
research projects 4.35 (0.84) 2–5 5 (3) ⫽ 76.31ⴱ 0 4 12 30 54
Involvement in conference
presentations and/or
manuscripts 3.67 (0.92) 2–5 4 (3) ⫽ 24.21ⴱ 0 12 28 41 19
Focus on critical
analysis/review of the
literature 3.84 (0.87) 2–5 4 (3) ⫽ 32.34ⴱ 0 6 28 42 24
Coursework in research
methodology, philosophy of
science, statistics, and so
forth 3.65 (0.86) 2–5 4 (3) ⫽ 38.76ⴱ 0 1% 29 46 15
Focus on using empirically
supported treatments in
clinical work 3.96 (0.96) 1–5 4 (4) ⫽ 64.69ⴱ 1 7 21 37 34
Participation in faculty research 3.09 (1.20) 1–5 2 (4) ⫽ 17.92ⴱ 8 28 25 24 15
Grant writing or other related
experience 2.91 (1.10) 1–5 2 (4) ⫽ 45.56ⴱ 6 39 21 26 8

Note. N ⫽ 134. These results reflect responses to the Science Training Questionnaire (Merlo, Collins, & Bernstein, 2008).

p ⬍ .01.

tant. In addition, most students indicated that they identify them- after, elective coursework and clinical work were both rated as
selves as scientists fairly strongly (35%), very strongly (30%), or equally effective. Regarding the quality of science training that
extremely strongly (14%). they receive, the mean score was 3.9; scores ranged from 2
( poor) to 5 (excellent). Ratings for quantity of science training
Effectiveness of Traditional Science Training were also assessed, with a mean score of 3.9; scores ranged
Most students reported that their science training is very from 2 ( poor) to 5 (excellent). Finally, students rated the
effective as indicated by chi-square analysis (see Table 3). breadth of science training that they receive, with a mean score
Specifically, areas of science training that were rated as being of 3.6, and a range of 2 ( poor) to 5 (excellent).
the most effective were, in order, research, followed by pro- There was no significant correlation between ratings of the
gram as a whole, coursework, and comprehensive exams; there- quality of science training and number of years of study (r ⬍ .10,

Table 2
Discrepancies Between Received Versus Desired Emphasis on Science in Various Areas of Training

Minimum–
maximum 1 (none) 2 (minimal 3 (fair 4 (good 5 (great
Area M (SD) scores t(df) % amount) % amount) % amount) % amount) %

Received programme as a whole 4.12 (0.79) 2–5 t(126) ⫽ 1.75, r2 ⫽ .02 0 4 14 48 34


Desired programme as a whole 4.00 (0.75) 2–5 0 3 18 54 24

Received required coursework 3.95 (0.76) 2–5 t(126) ⬍ 0.01, r2 ⬍ .01 0 5 17 56 22


Desired required coursework 3.95 (0.69) 2–5 0 2 19 60 19

Received elective coursework 3.57 (0.93) 1–5 t(123) ⫽ –1.95, r2 ⫽ .03 2 11 30 43 15


Desired elective coursework 3.73 (0.84) 1–5 1 8 23 54 14

Received clinical work 3.38 (0.85) 1–5 t(125) ⫽ –4.94, r2 ⫽ .16ⴱ 1 12 45 33 10


Desired clinical work 3.76 (0.78) 2–5 0 6 26 52 16

Received research 4.51 (0.71) 2–5 t(125) ⫽ 2.58, r2 ⫽ .05ⴱ 0 2 8 29 62


Desired research 4.33 (0.72) 2–5 0 2 9 43 46

Received comprehensive exams 3.79 (1.08) 1–5 t(118) ⫽ 0.87, r2 ⫽ .01 5 7 20 40 28


Desired comprehensive exams 3.73 (0.90) 1–5 4 2 24 54 16

Note. N ⫽ 134. These results reflect responses to the Science Training Questionnaire (Merlo, Collins, & Bernstein, 2008).

Significantly different after Bonferroni adjustment (Westfall & Wolfinger, 1997).
TRAINING OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY STUDENTS 137

Table 3
Perceived Effectiveness of Traditional Science Training

Minimum–
maximum 1 (none) 2 (minimal 3 (fair 4 (good 5 (great
Area M SD scores Mode ␹2(df) % amount) % amount) % amount) % amount) %

Programme as a whole 3.59 .78 1–5 4 (4) ⫽ 107.45ⴱ 1 6 35 48 9


Required coursework 3.49 .77 1–5 4 (4) ⫽ 109.10ⴱ 1 7 42 43 7
Elective coursework 3.28 .83 1–5 3 (4) ⫽ 94.87ⴱ 2 13 48 31 6
Clinical work 3.28 .90 1–5 3 (4) ⫽ 70.03ⴱ 2 16 41 34 7
Research 3.98 .76 2–5 4 (3) ⫽ 51.71ⴱ 0 2 25 47 26
Comprehensive exams 3.37 .93 1–5 4 (4) ⫽ 74.63ⴱ 5 9 38 41 8

Note. N ⫽ 134. These results reflect responses to the Science Training Questionnaire (Merlo, Collins, & Bernstein, 2008).

p ⬍ .01.

p ⬎ .05). This suggests student perception of the quality of their though students indicated that a fair amount of science training
education remains fairly constant throughout their training. As was emphasised in clinical work (i.e., clinical placements and
might be expected, there was a small but statistically significant practicum), they reported that this training was not very effective;
negative correlation between the quantity or amount of science indeed, as 52% of students indicated that they would like a good
training and years of study (r ⫽ ⫺.18, p ⬍ .05). In contrast, there amount of science emphasis placed on their clinical work it ap-
was no significant correlation between ratings of the breadth of pears that this is a training area in need of some modification.
science training and years of study (r ⬍ .10, p ⬎ .05). The Clinical work, according to this study, was defined as clinical
previous two results are somewhat counterintuitive, suggesting the placements or practicum. It is difficult to determine why students
possibility that students may not perceive additional time spent perceive their science training to be less effective in clinical
with scientific training as providing additional breadth; neverthe- work. It may be that practicum sites are not adequately integra-
less, the quality, quantity, and breadth of training are all positively ting science into the training programmes, according to students.
correlated (all rs ⬎ .60, all ps ⬍.01), suggesting that despite Given that practising clinicians rarely conduct research (Haynes,
student perceptions across time, students recognise the confluent, Lemsky, & Sexton-Radek, 1987), it may be the case that science
supporting relationship of the three variables— quality, quantity, is not seen as a priority. Conversely, it is possible that science is
and breadth of science training. not being effectively integrated into clinical work at the university
level, because some programmes offer practicum at their institu-
Discussion tion. In any case, clinical programmes may find it particularly
worthwhile to review these issues internally, including their own
Science has formed the foundation of training and has retained
graduate students and their experiences.
a position of paramount importance since the early years of clinical
psychology; it continues to be inextricably linked to the training Regarding the breadth of science training, students were gener-
provided in doctoral clinical psychology programmes. The vast ally satisfied with the emphasis received in their programmes.
majority of clinical psychology programmes in Canada espouse There was, however, an exception; nearly half of the students
the scientist-practitioner model. Even amongst programmes that do reported receiving no training or minimal training in grant writing
not explicitly endorse such a model, science training is deemed to or other related experience. It is reasonable to speculate that
be an essential part of graduate training. Despite pervasive en- student responses to this question included all funding applications
dorsement of the scientist-practitioner model in Canada, there are (e.g., grants, scholarships, bursaries); however, that speculation
little data available on Canadian student opinions of the imple- necessarily overlooks the distinction between grant applications
mentation of the scientist-practitioner model. The results of the which are typically more complex and scholarship applications
present study suggest that, in general, clinical psychology students which are often relatively less complex. This finding was surpris-
enrolled in Canadian programmes report feeling satisfied with the ing, given the implicit importance of writing funding applications
level of science training they receive. in academia for the future success of both students and profession-
Consistent with previous research using a U.S. student sample als. For example, students with funding place less strain on the
(Merlo et al., 2008), Canadian students reported that their training supervisor’s budget, leaving those resources to be allocated to
was slightly more weighted toward research than clinical practise. other areas such as research, conferences, and attracting future
The overwhelming majority of Canadian students indicated that students. Grants also provide funding for research, thereby increas-
they preferred a good amount of science training in their pro- ing the likelihood of sustained funding, and ultimately, increasing
grammes, suggesting that most perceive science training to be an the probability of promotion in the academic field (Merlo et al.,
integral aspect of their overall academic training. In addition, 2008). Thusly, grant writing, and by extension funding, is impor-
Canadian students reported, with some exceptions, that both the tant in generating and perpetuating research.
quality and quantity of the science training received was highly The difference may be the result of faculty assuming senior
congruous with their desired levels; thusly, it appears that training students possess the analytical and writing skills necessary to
programmes have been successful at integrating science into train- formulate a successful grant proposal and thusly are given less
ing to a degree that is consistent with student expectations. Al- instruction than more junior students, although this point is purely
138 PELUSO, CARLETON, AND ASMUNDSON

speculative. Conversely, it may be the case that junior students are tion of the Boulder model via the perceptions of students. Students
more likely to apply for scholarships because they have not yet were generally satisfied with the level of science training—with
received them; in contrast, over time an increasing number of grant writing and clinical integration being the two notable excep-
students will have funding and not be continuing to apply. Lastly, tions. Whether scholarship or grant-writing education is the re-
it may be that junior students are more likely than senior students sponsibility of the department or incumbent on individual super-
to seek out and attend to grant-writing instruction because of their visors remains to be decided. Accordingly, future research should
relatively lower levels of experience. explore this issue with students, supervisors, and departments. If
departments place responsibility for scholarship or grant-writing
Limitations training on faculty members, it may be beneficial for departments
to regulate this process through expert tutorials. Such tutorials may
There are limitations within the current study that are worth foster a culture in which applying for funding is an expected part
noting. First, the study may have suffered from a sample bias, of professional psychology. The finding that students are not
given that the majority of provinces provided responses from only satisfied with the integration of science into their clinical work
two to eight students. For instance, in the case of British Columbia, presents some concerns— given that science training is a require-
only seven respondents participated, a figure which is not repre- ment for CPA accreditation in clinical psychology programmes—
sentative of the number of students actually enrolled in clinical and the Canadian Code of Ethics (CPA, 2002) stipulates that
programmes in that province. Consequently, results are difficult to practising psychologists must stay abreast with research. More-
generalise for the entire province. Second, although only Canadian over, this suggests that the scientist-practitioner model is not being
student participants were solicited, no methods were employed to implemented as effectively as may be hoped, given that clinical
confirm a participant’s nationality or student status; accordingly, it practise should be informed with relevant and up-to-date scientific
is possible, though unlikely, that some of the respondents were not research. Future research should examine which aspects of inte-
students in Canadian clinical programs. gration are lacking. For example, students may feel that there is
Limitations also exist with regard to the CUDCP questionnaire insufficient emphasis on reading literature or attending scholarly
itself. First, although rigorously developed, the questionnaire was conferences to supplement clinical training. Given the myriad of
designed for American programmes espousing the scientist- ways in which science could be integrated into clinical work,
practitioner model. Although Canadian programmes adhere to the greater specificity regarding which aspect of science integration is
same model, it is possible that the questions may have been more ineffective appears warranted.
appropriate for American programmes. Moreover, the operational Finally, the vast majority of students indicated that emphasis on
definition of science that was adopted for the current study was science in their graduate training was very important; however,
appropriated from the CUDCP. Thusly, it is possible that there is students may not always readily perceive the cumulative value of
divergence between American and Canadian programmes regard- their training over time. The present study did not assess whether
ing what elements should be subsumed under science. Second, the students intend on continuing to integrate science into their clinical
results of this study were not compared to the results of Merlo et practise after graduation. Future studies should determine how
al. (2008). The Merlo et al. study did not exclude Canadian important it is for students to incorporate science in their profes-
participants; however, the percentage of Canadian respondents is sional careers and, perhaps more important, how they intend on
unknown. As a result, we feel that a direct comparison of the doing so.
countries would be inappropriate, and would not result in mean-
ingful information. Fourth, the psychometric properties of the Conclusions
CUDCP questionnaire have not been extensively assessed. Con-
sequently, the results may not be robust. Fifth, the current data This study represents an important first step in determining the
does not provide details allowing discrimination between the ad- status of the scientist-practitioner model in Canadian clinical psy-
ditive and integrative effects of training. Future research should chology programmes. The research was exploratory and the results
attempt to explore whether these two dimensions of training can be preliminary. More information about the implementation of the
usefully delineated. Finally, it is possible that the current results scientist-practitioner model is needed, from representative samples
reflect a tautological relationship between student expectations and of students as well as from faculty and directors of clinical train-
Canadian psychology programs that acclaim the scientist- ing. Ultimately, it is our hope that these insights, once generated,
practitioner model. For example, students’ support of the Boulder will be used to improve the quality of training in Canadian clinical
model might be a reflection of the fact that they have been psychology programmes.
socialized into valuing research-based clinical training. Despite
these limitations, the results of this study warrant attention as they
Résumé
currently represent the only explicitly Canadian data available on
this issue. Le modèle de formation scientifique-praticien est le plus courant
pour les programmes d’études supérieures en psychologie clinique
Implications for Future Research au Canada. En dépit d’un puissant appui de par le pays au sein des
programmes de psychologie, il existe peu de données sur les
Pervasive endorsement of the scientist-practitioner model in opinions des étudiants sur l’application de ce modèle. Dans la
Canadian clinical psychology programmes is limited by the current présente étude, on a demandé à 134 étudiants de 9 provinces de
lack of knowledge about its status or implementation. The results répondre à un questionnaire de 38 items, élaboré par le Council of
of this study provide some insights on the Canadian implementa- University Directors of Clinical Psychology, en vue d’évaluer
TRAINING OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY STUDENTS 139

l’opinion des étudiants sur la quantité, la qualité et la portée de la Ewing, R. M. (1963). Some thoughts on the education and training of
formation en science dans le cadre de leur programme de troisième clinical psychologists. Canadian Psychologist, 4a, 55–59.
cycle en psychologie clinique. Selon la plupart des répondants, Gibson, D. (1974). Enculturation stress in Canadian psychology. Canadian
leur programme est axé tant sur la recherche que sur le travail Psychologist, 15, 145–151.
Haynes, S. N., Lemsky, C., & Sexton-Radek, K. (1987). Why clinicians
clinique, la recherche bénéficiant d’un peu plus d’importance. Les
infrequently do research. Professional Psychology: Research and Prac-
étudiants ont rapporté que la formation en science leur était très
tice, 18, 515–519.
importante et ont indiqué qu’ils obtenaient une bonne dose de Kelly, E. L., & Fiske, D. W. (1950). The prediction of success in the VA training
formation de grande qualité dans ce domaine. En outre, le niveau program in clinical psychology. American Psychologist, 5, 395–406.
désiré de formation en science équivalait, en grande partie, au Levy, L. H. (1962). The skew in clinical psychology. American Psychol-
niveau reçu. Sont examinées les réprecussions des résultats sur les ogist, 17, 244 –249.
études ultérieures ainsi que sur la formation. Merlo, L. J., Collins, A., & Bernstein, J. (2008). CUDCP-affiliated clinical
psychology student views of their science training. Training and Edu-
Mots-clés : psychologie clinique, formation des diplômés en psy- cation in Professional Psychology, 2, 58 – 65.
chologie clinique, étudiants diplômés en psychologie clinique, Myers, C. R. (1970). Whatever happened to Canadian psychology? Cana-
modèle scientifique-praticien dian Psychologist, 11, 128 –132.
Myers, D. (2007). Implication of the scientist-practitioner model in coun-
selling psychology training and practice. American Behavioral Scientist,
References 50, 789 –796.
Adair, J. G. (1981). Canadian psychology as a profession and discipline. Raimy, V. C. (1950). Training in clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs,
Canadian Psychology, 22, 163–122. NJ: Prentice Hall.
Arthur, A. Z. (1971). Applied training programmes of psychology in Sutherland, J. S. (1964). The case history of a profession. Canadian
Canada: A survey. Canadian Psychologist, 12, 46 – 65. Psychologist, 5a, 209 –224.
Aspenson, D. O., Gersh, T. L., Perot, A. R., Galassi, J. P., Schroeder, R., Vipond, D., & Richert, R. A. (1977). Contributions of Canadian psychol-
Kerick, S., . . . Brooks, L. (1993). Graduate psychology students’ per- ogists to the war effort, 1939 –1945. Canadian Psychological Review,
ceptions of the scientist-practitioner model of training. Counseling Psy- 18, 169 –174.
chology Quarterly, 6, 201–215. Webster, E. C. (1967). The Couchiching conference on professional psy-
Canadian Psychological Association. (2002). Accreditation standards and chology. Montreal, Quebec: Canadian Psychological Association.
procedures for doctoral programmes and internships in professional psy- Westfall, P. H., & Wolfinger, R. D. (1997). Multiple tests with discrete
chology (4th Rev.). Retrieved from, http://www.cpa.ca/cpasite/userfiles/ distributions. The American Statistician, 51, 3– 8.
Documents/Accreditation/Accreditation%20Manual%20Jan08.pdf Wright, M. J. (1969). Canadian psychology comes of age. Canadian
Conway, J. B. (1984). Clinical psychology training in Canada: Its devel- Psychologist, 10, 229 –253.
opment, current status, and the prospects for accreditation. Canadian Wright, M. J. (1974). CPA: The first ten years. Canadian Psychologist, 15,
Psychology, 25, 177–191. 112–131.
Davidson, P. O. (1971). Graduate training and research funding in clinical
psychology in Canada (Special Report to the Science Council of Canada,
1970). Reprinted in Canadian Psychologist, 12, 141–175. Received March 10, 2009
Davidson, P. O. (1981). Some cultural, political and professional antecedents of Revision received July 13, 2009
community psychology in Canada. Canadian Psychology, 22, 315–320. Accepted July 14, 2009 䡲

You might also like