You are on page 1of 8

Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The relationship between procrastination and academic performance:


A meta-analysis
Kyung Ryung Kim a,1, Eun Hee Seo b,⇑
a
Department of Teacher Education, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodeamun-gu, Seoul 120-749, South Korea
b
Department of General Education, Seoul Women’s University, #920 Humanities & Social Science Hall, 621 Hwarangro, Nowon-Gu, Seoul 139-774, South Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Previous findings on the relationship between procrastination and academic performance are inconsis-
Received 6 February 2015 tent. We conducted a meta-analysis of 33 relevant studies involving a total of 38,529 participants to
Received in revised form 25 February 2015 synthesize these findings. This analysis revealed that procrastination was negatively correlated with
Accepted 26 February 2015
academic performance; this relationship was influenced by the choice of measures or indicators. The
Available online 18 March 2015
use of self-report scales interfered with detection of a significant relationship between procrastination
and academic performance. The demographic characteristics of participants in individual studies also
Keywords:
affected the observed relationship. Implications of this meta-analysis are discussed.
Procrastination
Academic performance
Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Self-report
Measure
Meta-analysis

1. Introduction Rothblum, 1984) or even reported that procrastination had a posi-


tive effect on academic achievement (e.g. Brinthaupt & Shin, 2001;
Procrastination, in the shape of delaying completion of an Schraw & Wadkins, 2007). It has been suggested that students of
assignment or putting off studying for an examination, is quite greater ability procrastinate more than those with lower ability
common among the worldwide student population. Estimates (Ferrari, 1991). Ferrari concluded that procrastination tended to
indicate that 80-95% of college students (O’Brien, 2002) or at least increase during the course of a student’s academic career, as learn-
half of all students (Ozer, Demir, & Ferrari, 2009; Solomon & ing became more self-regulated.
Rothblum, 1984) engage in procrastination and the prevalence of The nature of the relationship between procrastination and aca-
the phenomenon appears to be growing (Steel, 2007). demic performance remains ambiguous as the data do not con-
Procrastination and its causes and effects, are therefore an interest- verge. The inconsistent results may be due to the use of small
ing research subject. samples; if this is the case a meta-analysis which integrates the
There is a considerable body of empirical research on the results of multiple studies statistically might determine the nature
relationship between procrastination and performance, particu- and magnitude of any association between procrastination and
larly academic performance. The results have, however been academic performance.
inconsistent. Researchers have reported negative effects of pro- The conflicting results of previous studies are also likely to be
crastination on learning and achievement, such as lower grades due to the influence of factors such as use of different measures,
and course withdrawals (e.g. Aremu, Williams, & Adesina, 2011; use of contaminated self-report data and differences in the demo-
Balkis, 2013). The time pressure resulting from procrastination graphic characteristics of samples. Van Eerde (2003) insisted that
can reduce accuracy and punctuality, and on this basis it can be although many of the effect size categories were heterogeneous
argued that procrastination will negatively influence performance among studies about procrastination, indicating that moderators
(Van Eerde, 2003). may play a role, the majority of studies did not account for
Other studies have failed to detect an association between pro- moderators. We therefore hypothesized the relationship between
crastination and academic performance (e.g. Seo, 2011; Solomon & procrastination and academic performance would be subject to
influence by one or more variables. More specifically we predicted
that the observed association would be influenced by (a) the choice
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +82 10 2641 5952.
of procrastination measure; (b) the choice of performance indica-
E-mail addresses: k.ryung@yonsei.ac.kr, topfam@hanmail.net (K.R. Kim),
tor; (c) use of self-report data and (d) the demographic profile of
chrieve@swu.ac.kr, chrieve@hanmail.net (E.H. Seo).
1
Tel.: +82 10 7476 0314. the sample.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.038
0191-8869/Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33 27

1.1. Procrastination measures Procrastination Inventory (API), investigate behavior that is often
or usually displayed in various situations (Schouwenburg, 2004).
The choice of procrastination measure is affected by one’s theo- In summary, various measures of procrastination based on dif-
retical perspective on procrastination, broadly whether it is viewed ferent theoretical perspectives have been used to examine the
as (a) a functional or dysfunctional behavior and (b) a behavior or a relationship between procrastination and academic performance.
trait. Because they are based on differing, sometimes conflicting def-
Researchers have traditionally regarded procrastination as a initions of procrastination it is plausible to assume that they would
maladaptive or dysfunctional strategy used in an attempt to cope produce different results. We hypothesized that the choice of pro-
with conflict or choices (Mann, 1982). Lay and Schouwenburg crastination measure would influence the observed relationship
(1993) and Solomon and Rothblum (1984) argued that because between procrastination and academic performance.
definitions of procrastination refer to both behavioral delay and
psychological distress one should consider the magnitude of 1.2. Indices of academic performance
procrastination in conjunction with the magnitude of its
negative psychological consequences; assumed to be emotional Various indices of academic performance including self-re-
discomfort, including guilt, depression, anxiety or stress. From this ported GPA, examination grades, assignment grades etc. have been
perspective procrastination is a wholly dysfunctional behavior. The used to examine the relationship between procrastination and aca-
Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students (PASS; Solomon & demic performance. Some researchers have reported that the
Rothblum, 1984), the most widely used scale for measuring relationship between procrastination and academic performance
procrastination in an academic context, is a representative depends on the choice of performance indicator, for example Tice
procrastination inventory based on the assumption that and Roy (1998) found that the correlation between procrastination
procrastination is dysfunctional. It consists of items asking stu- and academic performance varied from .26 to approximately
dents to report the frequency with which they procrastinate, the .66 depending on whether academic performance was indexed
extent to which procrastination causes them a problem and their using various examination or assignment grades. Jackson, Weiss,
desire to stop procrastination in six specified academic domains; Lundquist, and Hooper (2003) found that procrastination, mea-
it also includes items designed to elicit reasons for procrastination. sured using Tuckman’s scale, was negatively correlated with
Decisional Procrastination Scale (DP; Mann, 1982) is based on the cumulative grade point average (GPA) but was not associated with
conflict theory of decision making (Janis & Mann, 1977), according American College Test score (ACT). We hypothesized that the
to which procrastination is a maladaptive coping behavior (Ferrari, choice of academic performance indicator would affect the
Johnson, & McCown, 1995). The Tuckman Procrastination Scale observed relationship between procrastination and academic
(TPS; Tuckman, 1991) assesses academic procrastination resulting performance.
from inability to self-regulate or control task schedules (Ferrari
et al., 1995) is another inventory designed to measure procrastina- 1.3. Differences between self-report and external data
tion as a maladaptive behavior (Hensley, 2014).
Recently, several researchers have described procrastination as Some researchers have suggested that the lack of consistency in
a functional delay (e.g. Alexander & Onwuegbuzie, 2007; Choi & research on the relationship between procrastination and perfor-
Moran, 2009; Chu & Choi, 2005; Howell & Watson, 2007). The word mance is probably the result of using contaminated self-report
procrastination originated from the Latin verb procrastinare, mean- data (e.g. Rotenstein, Davis, & Tatum, 2009; Steel, Brothen, &
ing ‘putting forward until tomorrow’, which does not have negative Wambach, 2001). Previous studies have relied on self-report mea-
connotations. Procrastination acquired negative connotations dur- sures of procrastination, which are only weakly related to external
ing the Industrial Revolution (Ferrari et al., 1995); until then pro- indicators of procrastination (Rotenstein et al., 2009). One study
crastination was viewed neutrally and could be interpreted as a (Steel et al., 2001) reported that the correlation between observed
wise course of (in)action (Steel, 2007). Procrastination can be or externally assessed procrastination and self-reported pro-
viewed as process that is regulated by internal, individual-level crastination was 0.35 while the correlation between observed pro-
norms for delay; it may be intentional and it may also be a wise crastination and course grade was 0.87; the correlation between
strategy (Van Eerde, 2003). Working within this framework Choi self-reported procrastination and course grade was only 0.36.
and Moran (2009) developed and validated an Active Self-report performance data, especially GPAs, are often used in
Procrastination Scale which consisted of items assessing outcome research because they are easy to obtain; however there is ongoing
satisfaction, preference for pressure, intentional decisions to pro- concern about their reliability. Disappointingly, in their meta-
crastinate and ability to meet deadlines. analysis of the validity of self-reported GPAs, class ranks and test
Early research on procrastination focused exclusively on the scores Kuncel, Crede, and Thomas (2005) showed that self-re-
behavioral aspects of procrastination, conceiving procrastination ported grades were less valid than many researchers believe.
as a task-specific avoidance behavior (Schouwenburg, 2004) i.e. More than twice as many students as under-reported their grade
as situationally determined and relatively unstable across time over-reported it (Bahrick, Hall, & Berger, 1996) in another study
and contexts (Saddler & Buley, 1999; Wolters, 2003). In this para- the ratio of over-reporting to under-reporting was even higher, at
digm the causes of procrastination are task or context variables 48–1 (Zimmerman, Caldwell, & Bernat, 2002). These results indi-
that increase aversion for the task or fear of failure, rather than cate that the using self-report data may bias the results of
individual-level variables. Behavior-oriented measurement scales investigations into the association between procrastination and
such as the Academic Procrastination State Inventory (APSI; academic performance.
Schouwenburg, 1995) only assess behavior during the preceding
week. 1.4. Demographic variables
If dilatory behavior becomes chronic and habitual it can be con-
sidered a typical response, or as a habit or trait (Schouwenburg, Unfortunately there has been little reported research on pro-
2004). Nowadays, most researchers regard procrastination as a crastination among younger students, for instance elementary
personality trait which is stable across time and contexts. and secondary students. Most studies of procrastination have used
Measurement scales for trait procrastination, including Lay’s samples of college students or adults. Steel (2007) and Van Eerde
(1986) General Procrastination (GP) Scale and Aitken’s (1982) (2003) found that younger people procrastinate more than older
28 K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33

people. O’Donoghue and Rabin (1999) interpreted this finding as true heterogeneity. Huedo-Medina, Sanchez-Meca, Martin-
an indication that as individuals age, they develop strategies for Martinez, and Botella (2006) defined low heterogeneity as
overcoming or avoiding procrastination. Steel (2007) also argued I2  25%, medium heterogeneity as I2  50% and high heterogeneity
that as individuals age they would learn to avoid putting off tasks. as I2  75%.
There is little evidence on cultural variations in the prevalence We calculated weighted correlation coefficients (weighted
of procrastination. One study (Ferrari, O’Callaghan, & Newbegin, average r) using the sample sizes for individual studies and we also
2005) found that the reported prevalence of avoidant and arousal computed average z and 95% confidence intervals.
procrastination was higher in English citizens than U.S. or We hypothesized that the choice of measures, type of data (self-
Australian citizens. They concluded that chronic procrastination report or external observation) and demographic profile of the
was common among westernized, individualistic, Anglophone sample might affect the observed relationship between pro-
countries. crastination and performance. Additional analyses were carried
out to evaluate the influence of these variables. First, we divided
1.5. The purpose of this study the studies into 27 subgroups; eight subgroups for procrastination
measure, two subgroups for type of procrastination data (self-re-
The purpose of this study was to synthesize the results of pre- port; external observation), seven subgroups for performance indi-
vious studies of the relationship between procrastination and aca- cator, two subgroups for type of performance data (self-report;
demic performance and to identify factors which influence the external observation), three subgroups for age, and five subgroups
relationship. This study had two specific objectives, (a) to calculate for geographic region. Next we assessed heterogeneity using Q and
an overall effect size for the association between procrastination the I2 index for selecting fixed or random effects models and com-
and academic performance and (b) to explore whether the associa- puted weighted correlation coefficients, average z and 95% confi-
tion between procrastination and academic performance was dence intervals for each subgroup.
influenced by the choice of measures or indictors, use of self-report Finally, we applied the fail-safe N and tolerance level tests to
data or demographic variables. determine whether we could safely ignore potential publication
These objectives were addressed using meta-analysis, which bias i.e. a systematic difference in the strength or direction of
helps to identify source of inter-study variability and can uncover associations in unpublished or unretrieved studies compared with
interesting associations between studies. studies in our sample. According to Rosenthal (1979), researchers
should calculate the number of studies needed to convert the
results. If the number of additional studies needed to overturn
2. Methods the conclusion is larger than the tolerance level, the finding is
robust. That is, file drawer problem is improbable.
2.1. Sample All analyses were carried out using the MIX program (Meta-
analysis with Interactive Explanations).
We searched the ERIC, Web of Science, Science Direct and
Proquest databases for relevant studies, primarily using the key-
words procrastination, delay, performance, academic performance, 3. Results
achievement and academic achievement. We restricted the search
to articles published between 1984 and 2014 because the most Table 2 shows the results of meta-analysis of the correlations
widely used measure of procrastination, the PASS, was developed between procrastination and academic performance; it reports
in 1984. the number of studies (k), total sample size across studies (n),
Retrieved studies were only included in the analysis if they met weighted average effect size (r), average Fisher’s (z), 95% confi-
the following three criteria, (1) written in English; (2) reported dence intervals, the heterogeneity statistics Q and I2 (%) which
correlation coefficients and (3) reported measures on both pro- were used to assess the percentage of total variance which was
crastination and academic performance. Using these inclusion cri- attributable to between-study variance, fail-safe N, and tolerance
teria we selected a sample of 33 studies, based on a total of 38,529 level. Before examining the effect size on each of variance, we com-
participants. Table 1 summarizes the studies included in the meta- puted the significant probabilities of heterogeneity. The value of Q
analysis. was significant (Q = 2247.29, p < .01, I2 = 97.37%), indicating that
the null hypothesis (homogeneity in the effect sizes for the correla-
2.2. Meta-analytic procedures tion between procrastination and academic performance) should
be rejected, so we used a random effects model. The weighted
First, we coded the sample size of each study, the correlation mean correlation between procrastination and academic perfor-
coefficient (r) for procrastination and academic performance, the mance was significant (r = .13, p < .01). The value of Rosenthal’s
procrastination index used (PASS; API; TPS; APSI, GP, Choi and fail-safe N indicated that 4012 additional opposite results would
Moran’s (2009) scale; measure developed by researcher or the be needed to overturn the finding; this implies that the finding is
others), whether procrastination data were self-reported or based robust as the fail-safe N is larger than the relevant tolerance level.
on external assessment, the performance index used (GPA; mid- When the analyses indicated significant sample heterogeneity,
term or final examination score; assignment grade; quiz score; variances were calculated using a random effects model; a fixed
course grade; rating of homework or ACT), whether performance effects model was used for homogeneous variables. The weighted
data were self-reported or based on external assessment, age (sec- mean correlation between academic performance and procrastina-
ondary school student; college student; adult) and geographical tion was significant when procrastination was measured using the
region (Asia; America; Europe; Africa or Oceania). When the pub- API (r = .20, p < .01), TPS (r = .18, p < .01), Lay scale (r = .33,
lication did not provide the data we required we asked the authors p < .01), Choi and Moran scale (r = .25, p < .01) or a scale developed
to provide this information by email; some did not respond and by the researcher (r = .28, p < .01); the association was not signifi-
this resulted in missing data. cant when procrastination was measured using PASS (r = .05,
Next, we assessed heterogeneity using the homogeneity statistic p > .05). The results related to PASS and the others were not robust
Q for selecting fixed or random effects models. We also calculated because both of them had fail-safe Ns which were less than the
the I2 index using the chi-square test to calculate the percentage relevant tolerance level. Procrastination measured using Choi and
K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33 29

Table 1
Studies included in the meta-analysis.

No Author (year) Sample N Procrastination scale Performance measurement


1 Akinsola, Tella, and Tella (2007) Africa, college students 150 Tuckman Course grade
2 Aremu et al. (2011) Africa, secondary 200 Lay GPA
students
3 Babadogan (2010) Europe, college 77 Cakici GPA
students
4 Balkis, Duru, and Bulus (2013) Europe, college 281 API GPA
students
5 Balkis (2013) Europe, college 290 API GPA
students
6 Beck, Koons, and Milgrim (2000) Europe, college 411 PASS Examination score
students
7 Burnam, Komarraju, Hamel, and Nadler (2014) America, college 393 PASS GPA
students
8 Choi and Moran (2009) America, college 185 By researcher, Chu and GPA
students Choi
9 Chu and Choi (2005) America, college 230 By researcher, Choi and GPA
students Moran
10 Corkin et al. (2011) Europe, college 206 PASS Course grade
students
11 Demeter and Davis (2013) America, college 123 PASS Quiz
students
12 Duru and Balkis (2014) Europe, college 261 Aitken GPA, ACT
students
13 Feizche, Young, and Hickson (2003) America, college 206 PASS GPA; course grade; assignment grade
students
14 Hensley (2014) America, college 320 Choi and Moran, Mid-term and final examination scores; course
students Tuckman grade
15 Howell and Watson (2007) America, college 170 PASS, Tuckman Course grade
students
16 Howell, Watson, Powell, and Buro (2006) America, secondary 95 PASS, Tuckman, By Course grade
students researcher
17 Jackson et al. (2003) America, college 219 Tuckman GPA; ACT score
students
18 Kennedy and Tuckman (2013) America, college 671 Tuckman Course grade
students
19 Kim and Seo (2013) Asia, college students 278 Choi and Moran GPA
20 Klassen, Krawchuk, and Rajani (2008) America, college 261 Tuckman GPA
students
21 Klingsieck, Fries, Horz, and Hofer (2012) Europe, Adults 425 Lay GPA
22 Lowinger, He, Lin, and Chang (2014) Asia, college students 264 PASS Course grade
23 Lubbers, Margaretha, Werf, Kuyper, and Europe, secondary 9,812 By researcher Course grade
Hendriks (2010) students
24 Michinov, Brunot, Le Bohec, Juhel, and Delaval Europe, Adults 40 Tuckman Assignment grade
(2011)
25 Moon and Illingworth (2005) America, college 349 API Mid-term and final examination scores
students
26 Orpen (1998) Oceania, secondary 102 Schouwenburg Final examination score
students
27 Owen and Newbegin (1997) Oceania, secondary 418 PASS, Tuckman Course grade(English, math)
students
28 Rotenstein et al. (2009) Europe, college 297 By researcher Homework, course grade
students
29 Seo (2011) Asia, college students 172 By researcher, PASS Mid-term examination score
30 Seo (2012) Asia, college students 172 Choi and Moran Mid-term examination score
31 Solomon and Rothblum (1984) Europe, college 342 PASS Quiz
students
32 Steel et al. (2001) America, college 152 By researcher Quiz; mid-term and final examination scores;
students course grade
33 Tice and Baumeister (1997) America, college 44 Lay Examination scores; assignment grade; term
students grade

Moran’s scale was positively correlated with performance whereas association between performance and procrastination were not
procrastination measured using other scales was negatively corre- robust when performance was indexed using mid-term or final
lated with performance. examination score or homework rating.
The weighted mean correlation between procrastination and The weighted mean correlation between procrastination and
performance was significant when performance was indexed by performance was highest for the secondary school student sub-
GPA (r = .12, p <.01), assignment grade (r = .64, p < .01), quiz group (r = .32, p < .01). Procrastination was negatively associated
score (r = .29, p < .01) or course grade (r = .24, p < .01), but not with performance in the college student subgroup (r = .16,
when performance was indexed using mid-term or final examina- p < .01). The weighted mean correlation between procrastination
tion score (r = .11, p > .05). The correlation was highest when per- and performance was significant in the Oceania (r = .46, p < .01),
formance was indexed using assignment grade. The findings on the Europe (r = .19, p < .05) and America (r = .18, p < .01) subgroups
30 K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33

Table 2
The correlation between procrastination and academic performance.

k Sample total n Weighted Average Confidence interval Q I2 (%) Fail safe N Tolerance
r z (95%) level
Total
Procrastination–performance 82 38,529 .13 3.19** [ .21, .05] 2247.29** 97.37 4012 420
Procrastination indicators
PASS 17 4809 .05 1.15 [ .15, .04] 169.30** 91.14 61 95
API 8 2577 .20 4.11** [ .29, .10] 44.60** 84.31 213 50
TPS 12 3301 .18 2.26* [ .45, .07] 824.17** 98.67 548 70
GP 7 1661 .33 4.02** [ .49, .17] 51.75** 88.40 180 45
Choi & Moran 6 1505 .25 10.21** [.20, .30] 7.21 30.69 147 40
Measures developed by the 29 24,267 .28 2.66** [ .40, .01] 2062.59** 98.64 1251 155
researchers
APSI 1 102 – – – – – – –
The others 2 307 .17 3.07** [ .28, 06] .69 0 4 20
Self-report data
Yes 68 35,878 .18 4.76** [ .18, .07] 1550.41** 95.74 789 350
No 14 2651 .16 1.76 [ .18, .05] 158.55** 93.06 669 80
Performance indicators
GPA 17 5390 .12 2.82** [ .20, .04] 152.03** 89.48 325 95
Mid-term or final examination scores 22 4752 .11 1.46 [ 25, .03] 582.20** 96.56 91 120
Assignment grade 4 350 .64 3.95** [ .42, .01] 9.81* 69.42 41 30
Quiz 8 1377 .29 2.90** [ .74, .05] 620.36** 98.87 319 50
Course grade 28 25,847 .24 4.52** [ .34, .14] 1567.61** 98.28 1203 150
Homework 2 594 .15 3.68** [.07, .23] .06 0 4 20
ACT 1 219 – – – – – – –
Self-report data
Yes 20 6503 .08 1.78 [ .16, .01] 238.49** 92.03 244 110
No 62 32,026 .21 4.87** [ .29, .12] 3271.08** 98.17 1673 320
Demographic characteristics
Age
Secondary school 8 21,598 .32 4.25** [ .46, .17] 65.38** 89.29 1153 50
College 71 15,638 .16 3.36** [ .25, .07] 2874.03** 97.60 10,411 365
Adult 3 1293 .15 2.08* [ .30, .01] 1.36 78.47 16 25
Geographical region
Asia 7 1332 .00 .05 [ .16, .15] 50.40** 90.08 0 45
America 60 13,349 .18 4.64** [ .28, .09] 2247.29** 97.37 28,541 310
Europe 9 21,826 .19 5.44* [ .26, .12] 108.47** 92.62 420 55
Africa 2 350 .25 .44 [ .87, 1.37] 194.35** 99.49 40 20
Oceania 4 1672 .46 3.84** [ .70, .23] 101.35** 97.04 585 30
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

but not in the Africa (r = .25, p > .05) or Asia (r = .00, p > .05) sub- The meta-analysis revealed that procrastination is negatively
groups. The correlation between procrastination and performance correlated with academic performance. It is possible that low
was not robust in the adult subgroup or the Asia subgroup. achievement is an inevitable corollary of procrastination; it seems
Self-reported procrastination was negatively associated with inevitable that if one were to delay the writing of a assignment and
performance (r = .18, p < .01) whereas the weighted mean consequently submit it late, or if one were to put off studying for
correlation between externally assessed procrastination and per- an examination and consequently fail to cover all the relevant
formance was not significant (r = .16, p > .05). Procrastination material this would be reflected in poor grades.
was negatively correlated with externally assessed performance The extant body of evidence on the relationship between pro-
(r = .21, p < .01) whereas the weighted mean correlation between crastination and performance is inconsistent. We hypothesized
procrastination and self-reported performance was not significant that this inconsistency occurred because the observed association
(r = .08, p > .05). was influenced by a number of variables, and this was confirmed
Table 3 shows associations between self-reported or externally by our meta-analysis. Our results also corroborated Van Eerde’s
assessed procrastination and self-reported or externally assessed (2003) report that many of the effect size categories were hetero-
performance. Self-reported procrastination was negatively corre- geneous among studies about procrastination, indicating that
lated with externally assessed performance (r = .15, p < .01) but moderators may play a role. This study showed that the pro-
was not associated with self-reported performance (r = .08, crastination measures developed by Aitken (1982), Lay (1986)
p > .05). The weighted mean correlation between externally and Tuckman (1991) and scales developed by researchers for use
assessed procrastination and externally assessed performance in a specific study were negatively correlated with academic per-
was significant (r = .39, p < .05). formance; however when procrastination was measured using
Choi and Moran’s (2009) scale the opposite association was
4. Discussion observed, as we expected. This indicates that the observed associa-
tion between procrastination and academic performance is influ-
This study synthesized the results of previous studies of the enced by the choice of procrastination measure. It also implies
relationship between procrastination and academic performance that Choi and Moran’s (2009) scale measures different characteris-
and investigated potential factors of the relationship. tics from other scales of procrastination. As mentioned in
K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33 31

Table 3
The association between self-reported or actual procrastination and self-reported or actual performance.

Self-report k Sample total n Weighted r Average z 95% CI Q I2 (%) Fail safe N Tolerance level
Procrastination Performance
Yes Yes 20 6503 .08 1.78 [ .16, .01] 238.49** 92.03 244 110
No 48 29,375 .15 4.30** [ .22, .08] 1311.64** 96.49 3313 250
No Yes 0 0 – – – – – – –
No 14 2651 .39 2.98* [ .65, .13] 1065.16 ** 98.78 306 80
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

introduction, Choi and Moran’s (2009) scale is based on a model of the externally assessed performance is lower than self-reported
procrastination which recognizes that it can be adaptive and performance, assuming that the method of measuring procrastina-
includes items intended to assess ability to meet deadlines and tion is similar in both cases. This finding is consistent with research
outcome satisfaction, which might not lead to poor achievement. suggesting that students tend to over-report their grades (Bahrick
These findings raise doubts about whether or not we should call et al., 1996; Zimmerman et al., 2002). This finding suggests that
people who are able to meet deadlines and are satisfied with their use of self-report performance data is likely to reduce the observed
results procrastinators. Some researchers have argued that ‘active correlation between procrastination and academic performance.
procrastination’ is not procrastination at all, but rather a form of To enable us to draw more accurate conclusions about the
purposeful delay (Corkin, Yu, & Lindt, 2011; Ferrari, 2010; Pychyl, relationship between procrastination and academic performance
2009); it should be noted that Corkin et al. (2011) used the term we conducted a four-way comparison of performance and pro-
‘active delay’ rather than active procrastination. This meta-analysis crastination indicators, comparing studies which had used self-re-
also provides support for the argument that active procrastination port data for both procrastination and performance; self-report
should be distinguished from other forms of procrastination. procrastination data and external assessment of performance;
We also hypothesized that choice of performance indicator externally assessed procrastination and self-report performance
would affect the observed association. Meta-analysis showed that data or externally assessment of both procrastination and perfor-
GPA, assignment grade, quiz score and course grade were nega- mance. We found that externally assessed procrastination was
tively associated with procrastination. Interestingly, there was negatively correlated with externally assessed performance. When
considerable variation in the magnitude of the association across both variables were indexed using self-report instruments the
the different performance indices we analyzed, with the correla- weighted mean correlation was not significant, perhaps because
tion between procrastination and performance ranging from .12 for both variables self-report data tend to be overestimates. These
to .64 depending on the performance indicator. These results sug- findings suggest that in some previous studies the negative relation-
gest that the magnitude of the observed association between pro- ship between procrastination and academic performance was
crastination and academic performance is influenced by the nature masked by reliance on self-report data. Our findings accord closely
of the performance indicator used. The largest average correlation with previous research (Rotenstein et al., 2009; Steel et al., 2001)
was found when performance was indexed by grade for an assign- concluding that self-report data are inaccurate and contaminated.
ment. This may be because one of the criteria for evaluating assign- The other finding from this meta-analysis was that the observed
ments is punctual submission, which procrastinators often fail; it relationship between procrastination and performance is influ-
may also be because assignment grades were externally deter- enced by the demographic profile of the study sample.
mined in all the studies we analyzed. This finding is consistent Procrastination was most strongly correlated with academic per-
with the finding that GPA, which was usually self-reported, was formance among secondary school students. This is related to ear-
comparatively weakly correlated with procrastination. lier studies which reported that younger people procrastinate
Procrastination was more negatively correlated with externally more than older people (Steel, 2007; Van Eerde, 2003) and implies
assessed performance than self-reported performance. that procrastination has more negative consequences for young
An important finding from this meta-analysis was that the people than adults. There is therefore an argument for teaching
observed association between procrastination and academic per- young people strategies for avoiding procrastination.
formance was influenced by whether performance was indexed An interesting finding was that procrastination was negatively
using self-report data or external observations. Interestingly correlated with performance in westernized and individualistic
although self-reported procrastination was negatively linked to regions such as America, Europe and Australia. This finding is con-
performance, there was no significant association between perfor- sistent with the argument (Ferrari et al., 1995) that procrastination
mance and externally assessed procrastination. This implies that acquired negative connotations at the time of the Industrial
self-reported procrastination tends to be higher than externally Revolution and a report that chronic procrastination is common
assessed procrastination, assuming that the method of measuring in westernized, individualistic, Anglophone countries (Ferrari,
performance is similar in both cases; in other words people seem O’Callaghan, & Newbegin, 2005). It appears that delaying a task is
to exaggerate the extent to which they delay carrying out academic likely to be interpreted more negatively in westernized, individu-
work. Few people claim never to put off an assignment. Estimates alistic societies.
that 80–95% of college students (O’Brien, 2002) or at least half of all Overall our meta-analysis confirmed that procrastination is
students (Ozer et al., 2009; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984) engage in negatively correlated with academic performance. The mixed
procrastination suggest that we all tend to believe that we are pro- results of research on this relationship may be attributed to several
crastinators and would do better if we were to kick this bad habit. factors, including variability in the measures of procrastination and
Observed performance was negatively correlated with pro- performance used, particularly the use of self-report instruments,
crastination, whereas there was no association between self-re- and variability in the demographic profile of samples.
ported performance and procrastination, indicating that if This study was subject to some limitations. First, although we
performance is indexed using external observations one is likely used several search engines to try to ensure that we retrieved all
to find a strong association with procrastination. This implies that of the relevant studies it is possible that we failed to include
32 K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33

relevant unpublished studies or that our search strategy failed to for exams, and academic achievement: A structural model. European Journal of
Psychology of Education, 28, 825–839. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-
retrieve all the relevant studies from the databases we searched.
0142-5.
Strangely, we could find only a few studies published in 1980s or ⁄
Beck, B. L., Koons, S. R., & Milgrim, D. L. (2000). Correlates and consequences of
1990s. We were also unable to analyze some variables because behavioral procrastination: The effects of academic procrastination, self-
the number of studies which had used them was too small. consciousness, self-esteem and self-handicapping. Journal of Social Behavior
and Personality, 15(5), 3–13.
Further research using a variety of indices would be valuable. Brinthaupt, T. M., & Shin, C. M. (2001). The relationship of cramming to academic
This meta-analysis focused on the effects of choice of measure or flow. College Student Journal, 35, 457–472.

methodology; is also possible that the association between pro- Burnam, A., Komarraju, M., Hamel, R., & Nadler, D. R. (2014). Do adaptive
perfectionism and self-determined motivation reduce academic
crastination and academic performance is moderated by ability procrastination? Learning and Individual Differences, 36, 165–172. http://
and task variables (Van Eerde, 2003). Future studies should inves- dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.10.009.

tigate the role of other potential factors (e.g. cognitive ability and Choi, J. N., & Moran, S. V. (2009). Why not procrastinate? Development and
validation of a new active procrastination scale. Journal of Social Psychology, 149,
motivation). 195–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.149.2.195-212.
Despite its limitations this study has several implications for ⁄
Chu, A. H. C., & Choi, J. N. (2005). Rethinking procrastination: Positive effects of
research and practice. First, delaying may be a harmful habit which ‘active’ procrastination behavior on attitude and performance. Journal of Social
Psychology, 145, 245–264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.017.
negatively affects student achievement. It still seems valid to inter- ⁄
Corkin, D. M., Yu, S. L., & Lindt, S. F. (2011). Comparing active delay and
pret procrastination in an unfavorable light, in spite of the revival of procrastination from a self-regulated learning perspective. Learning and
interest in the older, more positive view of procrastination and ⁄
Individual Differences, 21, 602–606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.005.
Demeter, D. V., & Davis, S. E. (2013). Procrastination as a Tool: Expecting
arguments that it can sometimes be an adaptive strategy. We
unconventional components of academic success. Creative Education, 4(7A2),
should remain concerned about procrastination among the student 144–149. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ce.2013.47A2018.

population and develop strategies to help students, particularly Duru, E., & Balkis, M. (2014). The role of academic procrastination tendency on the
young students, overcome or avoid procrastination. Second, so- relationships among self doubt, self esteem and academic achievement.
Education and Science, 39(173), 274–287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
called ‘active procrastination’ might not be a form of procrastina- j.lindif.2014.10.012.
tion at all; it might be more appropriate to consider it as a com- ⁄
Feizche, B. A., Young, B. R., & Hickson, K. C. (2003). Individual differences in
pletely separate construct. Use of the term ‘active procrastination’ academic procrastination tendency and writing success. Personality and
Individual Differences, 35, 1549–1557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-
leads to the misunderstanding that it is good to put off studying 8869(02)00369-0.
because such delay can be beneficial; we therefore argue that it is Ferrari, J. R. (1991). Self-handicapping by procrastinators: Protecting self-esteem,
necessary to distinguish more clearly between active procrastina- social-esteem, or both? Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 245–261. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(91)90018-L.
tion and procrastination. Third, researchers should bear in mind Ferrari, J. R. (2010). Still procrastinating? The no-regrets guide to getting it done.
that the observed association between procrastination and perfor- Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley.
mance is influenced by both the measures used and the demo- Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J., & McCown, W. G. (1995). Procrastination and task avoidance:
Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Plenum Press.
graphic profile of the sample and should take this into Ferrari, J. R., O’Callaghan, J., & Newbegin, I. (2005). Prevalence of procrastination in
consideration when designing studies and interpreting results. the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and avoidance delays
Finally, we recommend that researchers devise alternatives to ⁄
among adults. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 1–6.
Hensley, L. C. (2014). Reconsidering active procrastination: Relations to motivation
self-report instruments for indexing procrastination and other vari-
and achievement in college anatomy. Learning and Individual Differences, 36,
ables. Researchers should remember that participants cannot 157–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.10.012.

evaluate themselves accurately and may not report honestly either. Howell, A. J., & Watson, D. C. (2007). Procrastination: Associations with
achievement goal orientation and learning strategies. Personal and Individual
Differences, 43, 167–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.023.

Acknowledgements Howell, A. J., Watson, D. C., Powell, R. A., & Buro, K. (2006). Academic
procrastination: The pattern and correlates of behavioral postponement.
Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1519–1530. http://dx.doi.org/
This work was supported by a young researchers grant from 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.023.
Seoul Women’s University (2014). Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sanchez-Meca, J., Martin-Martinez, F., & Botella, J. (2006).
Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or index?. Psychological
Methods, 11, 193–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193.

References Jackson, T., Weiss, K. E., Lundquist, J. J., & Hooper, D. (2003). The impact of hope,
procrastination, and social activity on academic performance of midwestern
college students. Education, 124, 310–321.
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making: A psychological analysis of conflict,
meta-analysis. choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press.

Kennedy, G. J., & Tuckman, B. W. (2013). An exploration into the influence of
academic and social values, procrastination and perceived school belongingness
Aitken, M. (1982). A personality profile of the college student procrastinator.
on academic performance. Social Psychology of Education, 16(3), 435–470. http://
Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Pittsburgh.
⁄ dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-013-9220-z.
Akinsola, M. K., Tella, A., & Tella, A. (2007). Correlates of academic procrastination ⁄
Kim, E. K., & Seo, E. H. (2013). The relationship of flow and self-regulated learning
and mathematics achievement of university undergraduate students. Eurasia
to active procrastination. Social Behavior and Personality, 41(7), 1099–1114.
Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 3(4), 363–370.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2013.41.7.1099.
Alexander, E. S., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2007). Academic procrastination and the role ⁄
Klassen, R. M., Krawchuk, L. L., & Rajani, S. (2008). Academic procrastination of
of hope as a copying strategy. Personality and Individual Differences, 42,
undergraduates: Low self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of
1301–1310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.008.
⁄ procrastination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 915–931. http://
Aremu, A. O., Williams, T. M., & Adesina, F. T. (2011). Influence of academic
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.07.001.
procrastination and personality types on academic achievement and efficacy of ⁄
Klingsieck, K. B., Fries, S., Horz, C., & Hofer, M. (2012). Procrastination in a distance
in-school adolescents in Ibadan. IFE Psychologia, 19, 93–113. http://dx.doi.org/
university setting. Distance Education, 33(3), 295–310. http://dx.doi.org/
10.4314/ifep.v19i1.64591.
⁄ 10.1080/01587919.2012.723165.
Babadogan, C. (2010). The impact of academic procrastination behaviors of the
Kuncel, N. R., Crede, M., & Thomas, L. L. (2005). The validity of self-reported grade
students in the certificate program in English language teaching on their
point averages, class ranks, and test scores: A meta-analysis and review of the
learning modalities and academic achievements. Procedia Social and Behavior
literature. Review of Educational Research, 75, 63–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/
Science, 2, 3263–3269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.499.
00346543075001063.
Bahrick, H. P., Hall, L. K., & Berger, S. A. (1996). Accuracy and distortion in memory
Lay, C. H. (1986). At last, my research article on procrastination. Journal of Research
for high school grades. Psychological Science, 7, 265–271. http://dx.doi.org/
in Personality, 20, 474–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(86)90127-3.
10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00372.x.
⁄ Lay, C. H., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (1993). Trait procrastination, time management,
Balkis, M. (2013). Academic procrastination, academic life satisfaction and
and academic behavior. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 8, 647–662.
academic achievement: The mediation role of rational beliefs about studying.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)00176-S.
Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 13(1), 57–74. ⁄
⁄ Lowinger, R. D., He, Z., Lin, M., & Chang, M. (2014). The impact of academic self-
Balkis, M., Duru, E., & Bulus, M. (2013). Analysis of the relation between academic
efficacy, acculturation difficulties, and language abilities on procrastination
procrastination, academic rational/irrational beliefs, time preferences to study
K.R. Kim, E.H. Seo / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 26–33 33

behavior in chinese international students. College Student Journal, 28(1), (Eds.), Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment
141–152. (pp. 71–96). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Lubbers, M. J., Margaretha, P. C., Werf, V. D., Kuyper, H., & Hendriks, A. A. (2010). Schouwenburg, H. C. (2004). Academic procrastination: Theoretical notions,
Does homework behavior mediate the relation between personality and measurement, and research. In H. C. Schouwenburg, C. H. Lay, T. A. Pychyl, &
academic performance? Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 203–208. J. R. Ferrari (Eds.), Counseling the procrastinator in academic settings (pp. 3–17).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.01.005. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Mann, L. (1982). Decision-making questionnaire. Unpublished inventory. Flinders Schraw, G., & Wadkins, T. (2007). Doing the things we do: A grounded theory of
University of South Australia, Australia. academic procrastination. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 12–25. http://

Michinov, N., Brunot, S., Le Bohec, O., Juhel, J., & Delaval, M. (2011). Procrastination, dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.12.

participation, and performance in online learning environments. Computers & Seo, E. (2011). The relationships among procrastination, flow, and academic
Education, 56, 243–252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2010.07.025. achievement. Social Behavior and Personality, 39, 209–218. http://dx.doi.org/

Moon, S. M., & Illingworth, A. J. (2005). Exploring the dynamic nature of 10.2224/sbp.2011.39.2.209.

procrastination: A latent growth curve analysis of academic procrastination. Seo, E. (2012). Cramming, active procrastination, and academic achievement. Social
Personality and Individual Difference, 38, 297–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Behavior and Personality, 40(8), 1333–1340. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/
j.paid.2004.04.009. sbp.2012.40.8.1333.

O’Brien, W. K. (2002). Applying the trans-theoretical model to academic Solomon, L. J., & Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and
procrastination. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Houston. cognitive-behavioral correlates. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 503–509.
O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (1999). Incentives for procrastinators. Quarterly Journal http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-0167.31.4.503.
of Economics, 114, 769–816. Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical

Orpen, C. (1998). The causes and consequences of academic procrastination: A review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133,
research note. Westminster Studies in Education, 21, 73–75. http://dx.doi.org/ 65–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65.

10.1080/0140672980210107. Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrastination and personality,

Owen, A. M., & Newbegin, I. (1997). Procrastination in high school achievement: A performance, and mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 95–106.
casual structure model. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12(4), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00013-1.
869–887. Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination,
Ozer, B. U., Demir, A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2009). Exploring academic procrastination performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawding.
among Turkish students: Possible gender differences in prevalence and reasons. Psychological Science, 8(6), 454–458.

Journal of Social Psychology, 149, 241–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/ Tice, D. M., & Roy, F. B. (1998). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance,
SOCP.149.2.241-257. stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8,
Pychyl, T. A. (2009). Active procrastination: Thoughts on oxymorons. Retrieved June 454–458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x.
29, 2010, from: <http://www.psychologythoday.com/blog/dont-delay/200907/ Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent validation of the
active-procrastination-thoughts-oxymorons>. procrastination scale. Educational & Psychological Measurement, 51, 473–480.
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The ‘‘file drawer problem’’ and tolerance for null results. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164491512022.
Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638. Van Eerde, W. (2003). A meta-analytically derived nomological network of

Rotenstein, A., Davis, H. Z., & Tatum, L. (2009). Early birds versus just-in-timers: procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1401–1418. http://
The effect of procrastination on academic performance of accounting students. dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00358-6.
Journal of Accounting Education, 27, 223–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Wolters, C. A. (2003). Understanding procrastination from a self-regulated learning
j.jaccedu.2010.08.001. perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 179–187. http://dx.doi.org/
Saddler, C. D., & Buley, J. (1999). Predictors of academic procrastination in college 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.179.
students. Psychological Reports, 84, 686–688. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/ Zimmerman, M. A., Caldwell, C. H., & Bernat, D. H. (2002). Discrepancy between self-
pr0.1999.84.2.686. report and school-record grade point average: Correlates with psychosocial
Schouwenburg, H. C. (1995). Academic procrastination: Theoretical notions, outcomes among African American adolescents. Journal of Applied Social
measurement, and research. In J. R. Ferrari, J. L. Johnson, & W. G. McCown Psychology, 32, 86–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01421.x.

You might also like