You are on page 1of 4

Users' Guides to the Medical Literature

II. How to Use an Article About Therapy or Prevention


A. Are the Results of the Study Valid?
Gordon H. Guyatt, MD, MSc; David L. Sackett, MD, MSc; Deborah J. Cook, MD, MSc;
for the Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group

CLINICAL SCENARIO You are distressed by the rising crea- cal Subject Headings (MeSH), and you
You are working as an internal medi- tinine level and the rheumatology fel- quickly find that "lupus nephritis" is one
cine resident in a rheumatology rotation low with whom you discuss the problem such heading and "plasmapheresis" an¬
and are seeing a 19-year-old woman who suggests that you contact the hematol- other. You add a methodological term
has had systemic lupus erythematosus di- ogy service to consider a trial of plas- that will restrict your results to high-
agnosed on the basis of a characteristic mapheresis. The fellow states that plas- quality studies, "randomized controlled
skin rash, arthritis, and renal disease. A mapheresis is effective in reducing the trial (PT)" (PT stands for publication
renal biopsy has shown diffuse prolifera- level of the antibodies responsible for type). The search, which you restrict to
tive nephritis. A year ago her creatinine the nephritis and cites a number of tri- English-language articles, yields a total
level was 140 \g=m\mol/L, 6 months ago it was als that have suggested therapy is ben¬ of three articles. One is a trial of pred¬
180 \g=m\mol/L, and in a blood sample taken eficial. When you ask her if any of the nisone and cyclophosphamide1; a second
a week before this clinic visit, 220 \g=m\mol/L.
studies were randomized clinical trials, examines the effect of plasmapheresis
Over the last year she has been taking she acknowledges that she is uncertain. on risk of infection.2 The third citation,
You present the dilemma to the at¬ which describes "a controlled trial of
prednisone, and over the last 6 months, tending physician who responds with a plasmapheresis," appears most likely to
cyclophosphamide, both in appropriate suggestion that, before you make a de¬ address the issue at hand, the effective¬
doses.
cision, you review the relevant litera¬ ness of plasmapheresis in improving

From the Departments of Medicine and Clinical Epi- ture. The attending recommends that clinically important outcomes.
demiology and Biostatistics, McMaster University, you bring the patient back in 2 weeks, The relevant article is a randomized
Hamilton, Ontario. at which time you can offer her the ap¬ trial in which 46 patients received a stan¬
A complete list of the members (with affiliations) of
the Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group appears propriate therapy. dard therapeutic regimen of prednisone
in the first article of this series (JAMA. 1993;270:2093\x=req-\ and cyclophosphamide, and 40 patients
2095). The following members contributed to this THE SEARCH received standard therapy plus plasma¬
article: Gordon Guyatt (Chair), MD, MSc; Eric Bass,
MD, MPH; Patrick Brill-Edwards, MD; George Brow- You decide that the most helpful ar¬ pheresis.3 Despite the fact that antibody
man, MD, MSc; Deborah Cook, MD, MSc; Michael ticle would include patients with severe levels decreased in those undergoing
Farkouh, MD; Hertzel Gerstein, MD, MSc; Brian
Haynes, MD, MSc, PhD; Robert Hayward, MD, MPH; lupus that threatens renal function and plasmapheresis, there was a trend to¬
Anne Holbrook, MD, PharmD, MSc; Roman Jaeschke, who are already receiving immunosup- ward a greater proportion of the plas-
MD, MSc; Elizabeth Juniper, MCSP, MSc; Andreas pressive agents. Plasmapheresis must mapheresis-treated patients dying
Laupacis, MD, MSc; Hui Lee, MD, MSc; Mitchell be compared with a control management (20% vs 13%) or developing renal failure
Levine, MD, MSc; Virginia Moyer, MD, MPH; Jim Nish-
ikawa, MD; Andrew Oxman, MD, MSc, FACPM; Ameen strategy, and patients must be random¬ (25% vs 17%). This seems to settle
Patel, MD; John Philbrick, MD; W. Scott Richardson, ized to receive or not receive the plas¬ the issue of whether to offer your pa¬
MD; Stephane Sauve, MD, MSc; David Sackett, MD, mapheresis. Finally, the article must re¬ tient plasmapheresis. You wonder, how¬
MSc; Jack Sinclair, MD; K. S. Trout, FRCE; Peter Tug-
well, MD, MSc; Sean Tunis, MD, MSc; Stephen Walter, port clinically important outcomes, such ever, whether the study could have led
PhD; John Williams, Jr, MD, MHS; and Mark Wilson, asdeterioration in renal function. You to an inaccurate or biased outcome. The
MD, MPH. arefamiliar with the software program remainder of this article will provide
Reprint requests to McMaster University Health Sci-
ences Centre, 1200 Main St W, Room 2C12, Hamilton,
Grateful Med and use it for your search. you with the tools to address this ques¬
Ontario, Canada L8N 3Z5 (Dr Guyatt). The program provides a listing of Medi- tion.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a World Health Organization User on 08/01/2013


Readers' Guides for an Article About Therapy Will the Results Help Me less rigorous trials include the demonstra¬
Are the results of the valid? in Caring for My Patients? tion that steroids may increase (rather
study
Primary guides: This question has two parts. First, than reduce) mortality in patients with
Was the assignment of patients
randomized?
to treatments
are the results applicable to your pa¬ sepsis,7 that steroid injections do not ame¬
Were ail patients who entered the trial properly tient? You should hesitate to institute liorate facet-joint back pain," and that plas-
accounted for and attributed at its conclusion?
Was follow-up complete?
the treatment either if your patient is mapheresis does not benefit patients with
Were patients analyzed in the groups to which too dissimilar from those in the trial, or polymyositis.1' Such surprises may occur
they were randomized? if the outcome that has been improved when treatments are assigned by random
Secondary guides:
Were patients, health workers, and study person¬
isn't important to your patient. Second, allocation, rather than by the conscious
nel "blind" to treatment? if the results are applicable, what is the decisions of clinicians and patients. In
Were the groups similar at the start of the trial?
Aside from the experimental intervention, were the
net impact of the treatment? The im¬ short, clinical outcomes result from many
pact depends on both benefits and risks causes, and treatment is just one of them:
groups treated equally?
What were the results? (side effects and toxic effects) of treat¬ underlying severity of illness, the pres¬
ence of comorbid conditions, and a host of
How large was the treatment effect? ment and the consequences of withhold¬
How precise was the estimate of the treatment effect?
ing treatment. Thus, even an effective other prognostic factors (unknown as well
Will the results help me in caring for my patients? as known) often swamp any effect of
Can the results be applied to my patient care? therapy might be withheld when a pa¬
Were all clinically important outcomes considered? tient's prognosis is already good with¬ therapy. Because these other features also
Are the likely treatment benefits worth the potential out treatment, especially when the treat¬ influence the clinician's decision to offer
harms and costs? the treatment at issue, nonrandomized
ment is accompanied by important side
effects and toxic effects. studies of efficacy are inevitably limited
We summarize our approach to evalu¬ in their ability to distinguish useful from
useless or even harmful therapy. As con¬
INTRODUCTION ating and applying the results of articles firmation of this fact, it turns out that
In the first article4 in this series we
addressing therapeutic effectiveness in studies in which treatment is allocated by
the Table. House staff and practicing
introduced a framework for using the any method other than randomization tend
medical literature to solve patient prob¬
physicians alike need an approach that to show larger (and frequently false-posi¬
is both efficient and comprehensive. We
lems and provide better clinical care. have therefore labeled validity criteria tive) treatment effects than do random¬
This second article begins the discus¬ as "primary"—those few that can quickly
ized trials.'01" The beauty of randomiza¬
sion of how to use a report dealing with be applied by readers with limited time— tion is that it assures, if sample size is
therapy or prevention. In this article, and "secondary"—those that, though still sufficiently large, that both known and
we will use the term "therapy" in a broad unknown determinants of outcome are
sense. As we've described elsewhere,5
important, can be reserved for articles
that pass the initial guides and for read¬ evenly distributed between treatment and
the same guides can be applied to evalu¬ ers who have both the need and the time control groups.
ation of therapeutic interventions (di¬ for a deeper review. What can the clinician do if no one has
rected at reducing symptoms and cur¬ done a randomized trial of the thera¬
ing disease) and preventive interven¬ ARE THE RESULTS OF THIS peutic question she faces? She still has
tions (directed at reducing the risk of ARTICLE VALID? to make a treatment decision, and so
disease or disease complications). must rely on weaker studies. In a later
Primary Guides article in this series devoted to deciding
THE FRAMEWORK Was the Assignment of Patients to whether a therapy or an exposure causes
As with articles on other clinical ques¬ Treatment Randomized?—During the harm (a situation when randomization is
tions, one can usefully pose three ques¬ 1970s and early 1980s surgeons increas¬ usually not possible), we deal with how
tions about an article on therapy. ingly undertook extracranial-intracra- to assess weaker study designs. For now,
nial bypass (that is, anastomosis of a you should bear in mind that nonran¬
Are the Results of the Study Valid? branch of the external carotid artery, domized studies provide much weaker
This question has to do with the va¬ the superficial temporal, to a branch of evidence than do randomized trials.
lidity or accuracy of the results and con¬ the internal carotid artery, the middle Were All Patients Who Entered
siders whether the treatment effect re¬ cerebral). They believed it prevented the Trial Properly Accounted for and
ported in the article represents the true strokes in patients whose symptomatic Attributed at Its Conclusion?—This
direction and magnitude of the treat¬ cerebrovascular disease was otherwise guide has two components: was follow-
ment effect. Another way to state this surgically inaccessible. This conviction up complete and were patients analyzed
question is this: Do these results rep¬ was based on the comparison of clinical in the groups to which they were ran¬
resent an unbiased estimate of the treat¬ outcomes among nonrandomized cohorts domized?
ment effect, or have they been influ¬ of patients who, for whatever reason, Was Follow-up Complete?—Every
enced in some systematic fashion to lead had and had not undergone this opera¬ patient who entered the trial should be
to a false conclusion? tion, for the former appeared to fare accounted for at its conclusion. If this is
much better than the latter. To the sur¬ not done, or if substantial numbers of
What Were the Results?
prise of many and the indignation of a patients are reported as "lost to follow-
If the results are valid and the study few, a large multicenter randomized trial up," the validity of the study is open to
likely yields an unbiased assessment of in which patients were allocated to re¬ question. The greater the number of sub¬
treatment effect, then the results are ceive or forego this operation using a jects who are lost, the more the trial
worth examining further. This second process analogous to flipping a coin, dem¬ may be subject to bias because patients
question considers the size and preci¬ onstrated that the only effect of surgery who are lost often have different prog¬
sion of the treatment's effect. The best was to make patients worse off in the noses from those who are retained, and
estimate of that effect will be the study immediate postsurgical period; long- may disappear because they suffer ad¬
findings themselves; the precision of the term outcome was unaffected.6 verse outcomes (even death) or because
estimate will be superior in larger Other surprises generated by random¬ they are doing well (and so did not re¬
studies. ized trials that contradicted the results of turn to the clinic to be assessed).

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a World Health Organization User on 08/01/2013


Readers can decide for themselves Secondary Guides chance), but rather the magnitude of
when the loss to follow-up is excessive these differences. If they are large, the
by assuming, in positive trials, that all Were Patients, Their Clinicians, and validity of the study may be compro¬
patients lost from the treatment group Study Personnel "Blind" to Treat¬ mised. The stronger the relationship be¬
did badly, and all lost from the control ment?—Patients who know that they tween the prognostic factors and out¬
group did well, and then recalculating are on a new, experimental treatment come, and the smaller the trial, the more
the outcomes under these assumptions. are likely to have an opinion about its the differences between groups will
If the conclusions of the trial do not efficacy, as are their clinicians or the weaken the strength of any inference
change, then the loss to follow-up was other study personnel who are measur¬ about efficacy.
not excessive. If the conclusions would ing responses to therapy. These opin¬ All is not lost if the treatment groups
change, the strength of inference is ions, whether optimistic or pessimistic, are not similar at baseline. Statistical
weakened (that is, less confidence can can systematically distort both the other techniques permit adjustment of the
be placed in the study results). The ex¬ aspects of treatment and the reporting study result for baseline differences. Ac¬
tent to which the inference is weakened of treatment outcomes, thereby reduc¬ cordingly, readers should look for docu¬
will depend on how likely it is that treat¬ ing our confidence in the study's results. mentation of similarity for relevant base¬
ment patients lost to follow-up all did In addition, unblinded study personnel line characteristics and, if substantial
badly, while control patients lost to fol¬ who are measuring outcomes may pro¬ differences exist, should note whether
low-up all did well. vide different interpretations of mar¬ the investigators conducted an analysis
Were Patients Analyzed in the Groups ginal findings or differential encourage¬ that adjusted for those differences. WTien
to Which They Were Randomized?—As ment during performance tests, either both unadjusted and adjusted analyses
in routine practice, patients in random¬ one of which can distort their results.20 reach the same conclusion, readers jus¬
ized trials sometimes forget to take their The best way of avoiding all this bias tifiably gain confidence in the validity of
medicine or even refuse their treatment is double-blinding (sometimes referred the study result.
altogether. Readers might, on first blush, to as double-masking), which is achieved Aside From the Experimental Inter¬
agree that such patients who never ac¬ in drug trials by administering a pla¬ vention, Were the Groups Treated
tually received their assigned treatment cebo, indistinguishable from active treat¬ Equally?—Care for experimental and
should be excluded from analyses for ment in appearance, taste, and texture, control groups can differ in a number of
efficacy. Not so. but lacking the putative active ingredi¬ ways besides the test therapy, and dif¬
The reasons people don't take their ent, to the control group. When you read ferences in care other than that under
medication are often related to progno¬ reports on treatments (such as trials of study can weaken or distort the results.
sis. In a number of randomized trials, surgical therapies) in which patients and If one group received closer follow-up,
noncompliant patients have fared worse treating clinicians cannot be kept blind, events might be more likely to be re¬
than those who took their medication as you should note whether investigators ported, and patients may be treated
instructed, even after taking into ac¬ have minimized bias by blinding those more intensively with nonstudy thera¬
count all known prognostic factors, and who assess clinical outcomes. pies. For example, in trials of new forms
even when their medications were pla¬ Were the Groups Similar at the Start of therapy for resistant rheumatoid ar¬
cebos!1419 Excluding noncompliant pa¬ of the Trial?—For reassurance about a thritis, ancillary treatment with systemic
tients from the analysis leaves behind study's validity, readers would like to steroids (extremely effective for reliev¬
those who may be destined to have a be informed that the treatment and con¬ ing symptoms), if administered more
better outcome and destroys the unbi¬ trol groups were similar for all the fac¬ frequently to the control group than
ased comparison provided by random¬ tors that determine the clinical outcomes to the treatment group, could obscure
ization. of interest save one: whether they re¬ an experimental drug's true treatment
The situation is similar with surgical ceived the experimental therapy. Inves¬ effect (unless exacerbation requiring
therapies. Some patients randomized to tigators provide this reassurance when steroids were itself counted as an out¬
surgery never have the operation because they display the entry or baseline prog¬ come).
they are too sick or suffer the outcome of nostic features of the treatment and con¬ Interventions other than the treat¬
interest (such as stroke or myocardial in¬ trol patients. Although we never will ment under study, when differentially
farction) before they get to the operating know whether similarity exists for the applied to the treatment and control
room. If investigators include such pa¬ unknown prognostic factors, we are re¬ groups, often are called "cointerven-
tients, who are destined to do badly, in assured when the known prognostic fac¬ tions." Cointervention is a more serious
the control arm but not in the surgical tors are nicely balanced. problem when double-blinding is absent,
arm of a trial, even a useless surgical Randomization doesn't always pro¬ or when the use of very effective non-
therapy will appear to be effective. How¬ duce groups balanced for known prog¬ study treatments is permitted at the
ever, the apparent effectiveness of sur¬ nostic factors. When the groups are physicians' discretion. Clinicians gain
gery will come not from a benefit to those small, chance may place those with ap¬ greatest confidence in the results when
who have surgery, but the systematic ex¬ parently better prognoses in one group. permissible cointerventions are de¬
clusion of those with the poorest progno¬ As sample size increases, this is less and scribed in the "Methods" section and
sis from the surgical group. less likely (this is analogous to multiple documented to be infrequent occur¬
This principle of attributing all pa¬ coin flips: one wouldn't be too surprised rences in the results.
tients to the group to which they were to see seven heads out of 10 coin flips, The foregoingfive guides (two pri¬
randomized results in an intention-to- but one would be very surprised to see mary and three secondary), applied in
treat analysis. This strategy preserves 70 heads out of 100 coin flips). sequence, will help the reader deter¬
the value of randomization: prognostic The issue here is not whether there mine whether the results of an article on
factors that we know about, and those are statistically significant differences therapy are likely to be valid. If the
we don't know about, will be, on aver¬ in known prognostic factors between results are valid, then the reader can
age, equally distributed in the two treatment groups (in a randomized trial proceed to consider the magnitude of
groups, and the effect we see will be just one knows in advance that any differ¬ the effect and the applicability to her
that due to the treatment assigned. ences that did occur happened by patients.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a World Health Organization User on 08/01/2013


ARE THE RESULTS all the other patients were analyzed in We judge that the methods in this trial
OF THE STUDY VALID? the group to which they had been as¬ were, overall, strong and provide a valid
THE PLASMAPHERESIS TRIAL signed. With respect to secondary cri¬ start for deciding whether or not to ad¬
teria, the study was not blinded, the two minister plasmapheresis to our patient
Readers may be interested in how groups were comparable at the start of with severe lupus nephritis.
well the trial of plasmapheresis in pa¬ the trial, and the authors provide little So, in part A of this two-part essay,
tients with lupus nephritis met the tests information about comparability of other we have described how to answer the
of validity. With respect to primary cri¬ treatments. question: Are the results of the study
teria, randomization was rigorously con¬ In the introductory article in this se¬ valid? Part will describe how to an¬
ducted, as treatment was assigned ries, we described the concept of strength swer the second and third questions:
through a phone call to the study's Meth¬ of inference. The final assessment of va¬ What are the results of the trial? and
ods Center. One patient assigned to stan¬ lidity is never a "yes" or "no" decision Will the results help me in caring for my
dard therapy was lost to follow-up, and and must, to some extent, be subjective. patient?
References
1. Levey AS, Lan SP, Corwin HL, et al. Progres- Study. JAMA. 1987;257:2043-2046. 11:339-352.
sion and remission of renal disease in the Lupus 7. Bone RC, Fisher CJ, Clemmer TP, et al. Con- 14. Coronary Drug Project Research Group. In-
Nephritis Collaborative Study: results of treatment trolled trial of high-dose methylprednisolone in the fluence of adherence treatment and response of
with prednisone and short-term oral cyclophospha- treatment of severe sepsis and septic shock. N Engl cholesterol on mortality in the Coronary Drug
mide. Ann Intern Med. 1992;116:114-123. J Med. 1987;317:653-658. Project. N Engl J Med. 1980;303:1038-1041.
2. Pohl MA, Lan SP, Berl T. Plasmapheresis does 8. Carette S, Marcoux S, Truchon R, et al. A con- 15. Asher WL, Harper HW. Effect of human
not increase the risk for infection in immunosup- trolled trial of corticosteroid injections into facet chorionic gonadotropin on weight loss, hunger,
pressed patients with severe lupus nephritis: the joints for chronic low back pain. N Engl J Med. and feeling of well-being. Am J Clin Nutr. 1973;
Lupus Nephritis Collaborative Study Group. Ann 1991;325:1002-1007. 26:211-218.
Intern Med. 1991;114:924-929. 9. Miller FW, Leitman SF, Cronin ME, et al. Con- 16. Hogarty GE, Goldberg SC. Drug and sociotherapy
3. Lewis EJ, Hunsicker LG, Lan SP, Rohde RD, trolled trial of plasma exchange and leukapheresis in the aftercare of schizophrenic patients: one-year
Lachin JM. A controlled trial of plasmapheresis in polymyositis and dermatomyositis. N Engl J relapse rates. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1973;28:54-64.
therapy in severe lupus nephritis. N Engl J Med. Med. 1992;326:1380-1384. 17. Fuller R, Roth H, Long S. Compliance with
1992;326;1373-1379. 10. Sacks HS, Chalmers TC, Smith H Jr. Random- disulfiram treatment of alcoholism. J Chronic Dis.
4. Oxman AD, Sackett DL, Guyatt GH, for the ized versus historical assignment in controlled clini- 1983;36:161-170.
Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group. Users' cal trials. Am J Med. 1983;309:1353-1361. 18. Pizzo PA, Robichaud KJ, Edwards BK, Schu-
guides to the medical literature, I: how to get started. 11. Chalmers TC, Celano P, Sacks HS, Smith H Jr. maker C, Kramer BS, Johnson A. Oral antibiotic
JAMA. 1993;270:2093-2095. Bias in treatment assignment in controlled clinical prophylaxis in patients with cancer: a double-blind
5. Sackett DL, Haynes RB, Guyatt GH, Tugwell P. trials. N Engl J Med. 1983;309:1358-1361. randomized placebo-controlled trial. J Pediatr. 1983;
Clinical Epidemiology: A Basic Science for Clini- 12. Colditz GA, Miller JN, Mosteller F. How study 102:125-133.
cal Medicine. 2nd ed. Boston, Mass: Little Brown design affects outcomes in comparisons of therapy, 19. Horwitz RI, Viscoli CM, Berkman L, et al. Treat-
& Co; 1991. I: medical. Stat Med. 1989;8:441-454. ment adherence and risk of death after myocardial
6. Haynes RB, Mukherjee J, Sackett DL, Taylor 13. Emerson JD, Burdick E, Hoaglin DC, et al. An infarction. Lancet. 1990;336:542-545.
DW, Barnett HJM, Peerless SJ. Functional status empirical study of the possible relation of treat- 20. Guyatt GH, Pugsley SO, Sullivan MJ, et al.
changes following medical or surgical treatment for ment differences to quality scores in controlled ran- Effect of encouragement on walking test perfor-
cerebral ischemia: results in the EC/IC Bypass domized clinical trials. Controlled Clin Trials. 1990; mance. Thorax. 1984;39:818-822.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a World Health Organization User on 08/01/2013

You might also like