You are on page 1of 11

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2002, 43, 227–237

Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers: The psychometric qualities


Blackwell Science Ltd

and dimensionality of an instrument to measure young drivers’


risk-taking attitudes
PÅL ULLEBERG and TORBJØRN RUNDMO
Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

Ulleberg, P. & Rundmo, T. (2002). Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers: The psychometric qualities and dimensionality of an
instrument to measure young drivers’ risk-taking attitudes. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 227–237.
Adolescents are proportionately more frequently involved in traffic accidents than are other age groups. A strategy for promoting road safety
is to change the attitudes likely to influence driving behavior. However, the lack of valid and reliable instruments to measure risk-taking attitudes
makes it difficult to evaluate the effects of measures aimed at changing attitudes among young drivers and their passengers. The present study
tested the psychometric qualities of a scale intended to measure adolescents’ risk-taking attitudes to driving. The results are based on a self-
completion questionnaire survey carried out among 3,942 adolescents and young adults, aged 16 –23 years, in Norway in 1998/1999. Using
both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, 11 dimensions of risk-taking attitudes were identified. Parametric as well as nonparametric
methods were applied to test the homogeneity of items within each attitude dimension. The reliability and validity of the dimensions were
satisfactory. The attitude dimensions were significantly correlated with self-reported driving behavior, as well as accident frequency. The applica-
tion of the new measurement instrument in studies aimed at evaluating safety campaigns is discussed.
Key words: Risk, attitudes, adolescents, traffic, dimensionality, questionnaire.
Pål Ulleberg, Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway . E-mail:
Pal.ulleberg@svt.ntnu.no

Adolescents are proportionately more frequently involved in strategy to increase road safety may be to change the atti-
traffic accidents than are other age groups (Bjørnskau, tude dimensions that influence adolescents’ driving behavior.
2000). Specifically, they are more involved in accidents such According to Festinger’s (1957) cognitive dissonance theory,
as driving off the road and head-on collisions with another changing the beliefs that underpin behavior can lead to
vehicle. These are accidents typically caused by speeding and behavioral change. This assumption has been incorporated
loss of control over the vehicle (Michels & Schneider, 1984; in social cognition models such as the theory of reasoned
Tränkle, Gelau & Metker, 1990). Insufficient skills and a action/planned behavior (Ajzen, 1988; Ajzen & Fishbein,
lack of experience have often been regarded as the main 1980) and health behavior models such as the health belief
causes of young drivers’ accidents. It is, however, acknow- model (Rosenstock, 1974). From these theories one can
ledged that several factors may influence their accident expect that a change in certain beliefs may reduce the prob-
involvement (see Gregersen & Bjurulf, 1996, for a review). ability of accidents.
A large number of studies have focused on perceived risk Several literature reviews have, however, concluded that
related to traffic hazards, as well as driving skills. Young the majority of traffic safety campaigns aimed at influencing
drivers, compared with other age groups, are more likely to attitudes have failed to document any effects on the number
underestimate the probability of the specific risks caused by of accidents (Elvik, Vaa & Østvik, 1989; OECD, 1994). A
traffic situations (Brown & Groeger, 1988; Deery, 1999). longitudinal study carried out among Norwegian drivers
They also tend to perceive the hazards in traffic less holist- (Assum, Midtland & Opdal, 1993) did not demonstrate any
ically (Deery, 1999; Milech, Glencross & Hartley, 1989), and predictive value of safety attitudes on the risk of accidents.
overestimate their own driving skills (Moe, 1986). On this basis it was concluded that attitude campaigns
It has also been hypothesized that young drivers are more should not be recommended as a measure to improve traffic
accident prone due to their risk-taking attitudes, meaning safety. On the other hand, a meta-analysis carried out
preferences towards risk-taking in traffic (Jessor, 1984). Such recently suggested that campaigns aimed at influencing atti-
attitudes have been found to correlate with aggressive driv- tudes may be the most efficient measure to improve safety
ing behavior (Parker, Lajunen & Strandling, 1998), fast driv- on the roads (Delhomme et al., 1999).
ing, and self-reported accident involvement (West & Hall, There may be several reasons for the attitude campaigns’
1997), and intention to commit driving violations (see apparent lack of success. According to the OECD (1994) and
Parker & Manstead, 1996, for a review). Hence, an effective Wilde (1993), one reason is the rather weak methodology

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations. Published by Blackwell Publishers, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and
350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
228 P. Ulleberg and T. Rundmo Scand J Psychol 43 (2002)

of some of the evaluation studies. Accident frequency is used who violates the traffic rules), concern for others, concern
as the criterion variable. This is, however, not always an for oneself, drinking and driving, and safety belts. Several
appropriate criterion for measuring effects, mainly because studies have shown that people’s attribution of accident
accidents are also influenced by other factors, such as causes as well as attitudes towards rule violations were
exposure (e.g. annual mileage), randomness, and weather significant predictors of risk-taking behavior (Parker &
conditions (see Friedstrøm, Ifver, Ingebrigtsen, Kulmala & Manstead, 1996; Rundmo, 1992, 1996). In the YDAS, there
Thomsen, 1995). Also, it is difficult to measure statistically was a lack of indicators aimed at measuring these aspects of
significant change in accident frequency since traffic acci- traffic safety attitudes. Thus, such additional attitude items
dents occur relatively seldom. should be included in order to cover relevant aspects of
Sutton (1998) has suggested nine methodological issues adolescents’ traffic safety attitudes.
in relation to why an underestimation of the correlation As suggested above, the use of a reliable and valid meas-
between attitudes and behavior may occur. Several of these urement of attitudes would be advantageous in evaluating
methodological issues are of particular relevance to attitudes the effects of traffic safety campaigns. The first step in the
and traffic accidents. One problem is the difference in the process of improving traffic safety by influencing attitudes
level of measurement. Attitudes are often measured at a should consequently be to validate a measure covering rele-
general level, that is through an aggregation of attitude vant aspects of adolescents’ traffic safety attitudes. Accord-
items, whereas an accident is a measure at a specific level. ingly, the overall aim of the present study was therefore to
In accordance with Ajzen and Fishbein’s (1980) principle of examine the reliability and validity of a measure of adoles-
compatibility, Sutton (1998; see also Aarø & Rise, 1996) cents’ risk-taking attitudes related to driving. This was done
suggests that a general attitude measure is a weak predictor in three specific steps.
of a specific behavior, such as an accident. A more appro-
priate criterion measure would be an aggregate of different
behaviors hypothesized to increase the risk of accidents, (1) Determining the multidimensionality of
namely a multiple-act criterion (Ajzen, 1988). This is also risk-taking attitudes
related to another issue pointed to by Sutton: random meas- The YDAS indicators were hypothesized to be multidimen-
urement error in behavior and /or attitudes causes an under- sional. Malfetti et al. (1989) found the internal consistency
estimation of the correlation between the measures. As of the attitude dimensions to be satisfactory. However, meas-
mentioned, specific measures, like accident measures, are ures of internal consistency are not aimed at determining the
influenced by numerous factors other than attitudes. Hence, multidimensionality of indicators (DeVellis, 1991). Therefore,
accidents can be seen as an unreliable criterion measure. a more thorough examination of the hypothesized multi-
Random measurement error in attitudes will also cause the dimensionality of risk-taking attitudes related to traffic still
same problem. Furthermore, many of the previous studies remains to be carried out. In addition, the structure of new
aimed at investigating the relationship between attitudes indicators intended to measure a broader aspect of adoles-
and traffic accidents have applied attitude measures with cents’ risk-taking attitudes has to be examined.
unknown psychometric properties (see OECD, 1994). Obvi-
ously, this imposes a limitation on the validity of the conclu-
sions drawn from these studies. Additionally, Sutton points (2) Comparing the suitability of parametric as well as
to a violation of the scale correspondence between the nonparametric methods for evaluating the homogeneity of
attitude and behavioral measures, as well as the unequal items within attitude dimensions
number of response categories for attitudes and behavior, to Parametric methods (e.g. Cronbach’s alpha) may in many
be causes for the underestimation of the attitudes–behavior cases be inappropriate for analyzing internal consistency,
correlation. Although these may be seen as important issues because some items may have a skewed distribution (Van
in some contexts, we will not focus on these in the present Schuur & Kiers, 1994). For instance, few respondents may
paper. This is mainly because these issues are most relevant “agree” or “strongly agree” on an item. The items may
to the relation between behavioral intention and behavior. therefore not be parallel, that is, not have identical true
A few studies have attempted to measure young drivers’ scores and variances. The parametric methods of evaluating
attitudes towards risk-taking in traffic. Malfetti, Rose, internal consistency have no safeguards against such items,
DeKorp and Basch (1989) conducted an extensive study on and the results obtained may be misleading (Carmines &
adolescents’ risk-taking attitudes to driving. Their Young Zeller, 1979). An alternative method is Mokken’s nonpara-
Driver Attitude Scale (YDAS) was based on a literature metric latent trait analysis for unidimensional scaling
review and interviews with groups of adolescents, concern- (Mokken, 1971). This is a nonparametric item response
ing teenagers’ risk-taking attitudes. Their work resulted in a model, which analyzes the probability of a positive or high
70-item attitude scale measuring seven attitude dimensions, value on one item compared with the values on other items.
which were attitudes towards speeding, safe driving, riding An advantage of applying this method is that the items do
with an unsafe driver (i.e. willingness to drive with a driver not have to fulfill the assumption of being parallel.

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


Scand J Psychol 43 (2002) Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers 229

(3) Analyzing the relationship between attitudes and Questionnaire


risk-taking behavior A total of 87 indicators measured traffic safety attitudes. Four
In accordance with Ajzen and Fishbein’s (1980) principle of attitude dimensions from the YDAS (Malfetti et al., 1989) were
compatibility and their multiple-act criterion, we would included: safe driving, speeding, riding with an unsafe driver, and
concern for others. Forty-one items made up these four dimensions.
expect attitudes to be most successful in predicting behavior
They were judged to be of particular relevance to the type of traffic
when both are at the same level of measurement. For this accidents in which adolescents are typically involved, that is,
research, this implies that general attitudes should be accidents caused by speeding and loss of control over the vehicle.
significant predictors of an aggregate of risk-taking beha- Also included were 46 other items based on studies carried out
viors. At the same time, we hypothesize general attitudes to by Rundmo (1992, 1996, 1998). These indicators were intended to
measure attitudes towards violating rules of traffic in general, accid-
be weak predictors of actual traffic accidents. This is
ent causation, and risk of traffic accidents. Ratings on all items were
because the specific behavioral criteria of traffic accidents made on a five-point scale in Likert format, ranging from “strongly
are thought to be influenced by numerous factors in addi- agree” (scored 1) to “strongly disagree” (scored 5).
tion to the attitude assessed. In addition, a scale was included consisting of 15 items measuring
self-reported acts of risk-taking in traffic, such as speeding, tailgat-
ing, not stopping when the traffic light turned red, driving too close
METHOD to the car in front, and so on (Rundmo, 1996; Rundmo & Ulleberg,
2000). Only the respondents who possessed a driving licence were
asked to fill out this part of the questionnaire. The respondents were
Sample asked to indicate how often they committed the different acts of
A questionnaire survey was carried out among 4,500 adolescents risk-taking, ranging from “never” (scored 1) to “very often” (scored
and young adults in Norway in 1998/1999. The survey was con- 5). A complete list of the behavioral items with their mean score and
ducted at the start of a traffic safety campaign, scheduled to last standard deviation is presented in Table 1. The respondents with a
until 2003 (see Rundmo & Ulleberg, 2000). The study was initiated driving licence were also asked to report how many times they, as a
by the Norwegian Authorities of Public Roads, in cooperation with driver, had been involved in a traffic accident.
the police, the Norwegian Society of Road Safety, and the Traffic
Safety Committees of two Norwegian counties. The respondents
were from randomly selected high school classes, and the question-
Statistical analysis
naires were completed individually while the students were at Confirmatory as well as exploratory factor analyses were carried
school. A total of 3,942 respondents returned the questionnaire, a out to examine the structure of the attitude items. First, confirmat-
response rate of 88%. Of the respondents, 56% were female and 44% ory factor analysis (maximum-likelihood method) was performed to
were male. Their mean age was 18.5 years (modal average = 18 years) assess how well the original four-factor structure of the YDAS
and the age ranged from 16 to 23 years. Of the 2,032 respondents fit the data. The covariance matrix of the YDAS was analyzed by
who had a driving licence, the majority (84%) had possessed it for means of the LISREL 8 program (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993).
more than three months. Thereafter, a principal components analysis (PCA), with varimax

Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of the behavioural items (respondents with driver licence, n = 1,963)

How often do you . . . Mean SD

Exceed the speed limit in built-up areas (by more than 10 km/h) 2.79 1.14
Exceed the speed limit on country roads (by more than 10 km/h) 3.24 1.11
Drive through an amber light when it is about to turn red 2.49 1.12
Disregard a red light on an empty road 1.45 0.88
Overtake the car in front when it is driving at the speed limit 2.37 1.10
Drive too close to the car in front 2.56 0.93
Drive the wrong way down a one-way street 1.52 0.85
Drive fast because the opposite sex enjoys it 1.61 0.89
Ignore traffic rules to in order to get ahead in traffic 2.24 0.96
Bend the traffic rules in order to get ahead in traffic 2.65 0.96
Break traffic rules because they are too complicated to follow 1.78 0.92
Break traffic rules due to peer pressure 1.67 0.88
Drive recklessly because others expect me to do it 1.76 0.95
Drive fast to show others that I am tough enough 1.68 0.93
Drive fast to show others I can handle the car 1.84 0.97
Total mean score 2.11 0.66

α = 0.92, H = 0.48.

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


230 P. Ulleberg and T. Rundmo Scand J Psychol 43 (2002)

rotation, was used to determine the underlying dimensionality of scale, H between 0.40 and 0.50 is regarded as a moderately strong
the YDAS items. In order to exclude unreliable items from the scale, and H greater than 0.50 is understood to be a strong scale.
YDAS, items with a factor loading below 0.50 were excluded. The The computer program MSP (Molenaar et al., 1994) was applied to
final clusterings of items obtained in the PCA were interpreted to analyze the data with the Mokken scale model.
indicate different dimensions of the risk-taking attitudes of the Item analysis was also carried out by analyzing the corrected
YDAS. Each factor’s theoretical substance was evaluated on the item–scale correlation of the items within each subscale. The MSP
basis of the content of the items clustering on the factor. program also makes it possible to analyze the contribution of indi-
With the purpose of comparing the fit of the factor structure vidual items to the scale by computing a scalability coefficient for
suggested by Malfetti et al. (1989) with the factor structure sug- each item in addition to one for the total scale. It is therefore pos-
gested by the exploratory PCA, various fit indexes were used: the sible to identify items with an unsatisfactory fit to the rest of the
goodness-of-fit index (GFI), the adjusted goodness-of-fit index scale. After evaluating item scalability, the subscales were con-
(AGFI), the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error structed by adding the items belonging to each scale without differ-
of approximation (RMSEA), and the expected cross-validation ential weighting.
index (ECVI). Traditionally, a GFI, an AGFI, and a CFI above 0.90 The last phase of the analysis was to establish the validity of the
have been agreed-upon cut-off criteria, indicating a close fit between proposed scales. This was done by an assessment of discriminant
the model and the data (Hoyle & Panter, 1995; Hu & Bentler, 1995; validity. The item discriminant validity was examined by correlating
Loehlin, 1998). However, Hu and Bentler (1999) later concluded each item with the total scale score. Next, the intercorrelations
that the CFI should be close to 0.95 in order to claim a good fit between the scales’ total scores were studied. The scales were
between the hypothesized model and the observed data. An expected to be correlated, but not too strongly if they were to reflect
RMSEA of 0.05 or less is also thought to indicate a very good conceptually different dimensions of risk-taking attitudes.
model fit, and the lower the ECVI value the better the fit (Browne Finally, criterion validity was examined by studying external cor-
& Cudeck, 1993; Loehlin, 1998). relates of the scales. Based on previous research (Evans, 1991), we
We also wanted to examine whether the 46 items selected from expected women to show less preferences for risk-taking attitudes
the safety attitude questionnaires (Rundmo, 1992, 1996, 1998) than men. We also hypothesized the scales to be related to risk-
represented attitude dimensions separate from the dimensions taking behavior and accident involvement among the respondents
identified in the YDAS. In order to do this, items from both the with a driving licence. The more “ideal” attitudes the respondents
YDAS and the safety attitude questionnaires were included in the reported, the less risk-taking behavior and accident involvement we
same exploratory PCA analysis. The criterion for factor extraction expected them to report.
and item selection was the same as for the exploratory analysis of
the YDAS.
In order to test the robustness of the factor model identified in the
exploratory analysis, the sample was randomly split in half. Explor- RESULTS
atory factor analysis was carried out on the first half and confirmatory
factor analysis was performed on the second half. We also wanted Dimensionality of risk-taking attitudes
to compare the factor structure of the attitude items between ado-
lescents who had a driving licence and those who did not. Hence, To test the structure of the original four-factor model of the
separate exploratory factor analyses, as well as confirmatory factor YDAS, a confirmatory factor analysis was carried out. The
analyses, were performed for the two groups of respondents. items were allowed to load on one factor only (i.e. the factor
Cronbach’s (1951) alpha coefficient was applied to evaluate the
homogeneity of the item within the attitude dimensions. Nunnaly
they were hypothesized to reflect). As indicated in Table 2,
(1978) recommends that the alpha coefficient should be equal or the fit of the four-factor model was poor.
higher than 0.70 if a set of items are to make up a scale. However, We then used a PCA with varimax rotation on an explor-
the alpha coefficient tends to increase as a function of the number atory basis in order to detect latent sources of variation and
of items. Therefore, it is easier to obtain a satisfactory alpha with covariation in the YDAS items. Of the original 41 items
many than with few items, given the same average inter-item corre-
lation. Consequently, one should keep the number of items in mind
from the YDAS, 19 were excluded because they did not
when homogeneity is evaluated using this method. fulfill the criterion for item selection (as described in the
The nonparametric alternative, a Mokken scale analysis for Method section). The remaining 22 items resulted in five
polytomus items, was also applied to evaluate item homogeneity. factors of risk-taking attitudes. In order to test the robust-
The Mokken model was first developed for dichotomous items, but ness of the factor structure, a separate PCA was performed
has been generalized to Likert-type items (Molenaar, Debets, Sijtsma
& Hemker, 1994). The Mokken model uses Loevinger’s weighted H
for respondents with a driver licence and for those with-
coefficient as a measure of item homogeneity. The H coefficient out a driver licence. The PCA resulted in a nearly identical
varies from 0 to 1, where a set of items is said to constitute a scale five-factor structure for both drivers and non-drivers. There
if H is greater than or equal to 0.30. H = 0.30 is regarded as a weak was only one item in the drivers group, pertaining to the

Table 2. Goodness-of-fit indices for the original four-factor model and the five-factor model of the YDAS (n = 3,942)

χ2 d.f. p GFI AGFI CFI ECVI RMSEA

Original four-factor model of the YDAS (41 items) 11068.57 773 0.0001 0.83 0.810 0.74 4.23 0.072
Revised five-factor model of the YDAS (21 items) 1639.06 179 0.0001 0.95 0.94 0.95 0.53 0.050

Note: GFI = goodness-of-fit index, AGFI = adjusted goodness-of-fit index, CFI = comparative fit index,
ECVI = expected cross-validation Index, RMSEA = root mean of square of approximation.

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


Scand J Psychol 43 (2002) Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers 231

acceptability of riding with an unsafe driver, which failed naires, in addition to the 19 items mentioned above. This
to fulfill the criteria for item selection. The item showed a was due to lower factor loadings, as well as substantial load-
lower than acceptable factor loading, as well as substantial ings on other factors. On this basis, we decided to exclude
cross-loadings to other factors. For this reason, we decided these items from further analysis.
to exclude this item from further analysis. Finally, a confirmatory factor analysis of the suggested
Thus, the results of the exploratory factor analyses sug- structure of 11 factors was carried out on the total sample.
gested a five-factor structure consisting of 21 items. Three of The items were allowed to load on their respective factor
the original subscales from the YDAS, “speeding”, “riding only. The results indicated a satisfactory fit of the data:
with an unsafe driver”, and “concern for others”, appeared χ2 (889, n = 3,942) = 6266.93 ( p > 0.0001), GFI = 0.93,
as separate factors. Since several of the original items within AGFI = 0.92, CFI = 0.93, ECVI = 1.76, RMSEA = 0.041.
these subscales did not fulfill the criteria for item selection, Still, the CFI was somewhat lower than the suggested cut-off
the three factors identified in the exploratory analysis can be value of 0.95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999), indicating that the model
regarded as shortened versions of three of the original could benefit from refinement. However, we decided not to
YDAS factors. In addition, two new factors were identified. modify the model further in order to keep it as parsimoni-
Three of the items, which originally belonged to the factor ous as possible. The 11-factor structure and standardized
“riding with an unsafe driver”, appeared as a separate factor factor loadings are shown in Table 3.
in the analysis. These were all items that questioned the Carrying out a confirmatory factor analysis on the same
acceptability of riding with a driver who had been drinking sample as for the exploratory analysis increases the prob-
alcohol, and the fourth factor was therefore named “drinking ability of obtaining high fit indexes. Thus, this may lead to
and driving”. Likewise, three of the items originally within a self-fulfilling confirmation of the 11-factor structure. In
the “speeding” factor in the YDAS made up a separate order to test the robustness of the 11-factor model, the sam-
factor. All three items were statements pertaining to the ple was randomly split in two. Exploratory factor analysis
demonstration of one’s own driving skills to others, and the was carried out on one half and confirmatory factor analysis
fifth factor was thus named “showing off skills to others”. was performed on the other. The results showed no note-
The five-factor model of the YDAS was also subjected to worthy difference in either factor structure or fit indexes
a confirmatory factor analysis in order to compare the fit of compared with the total sample.
this model with the original four-factor structure. The items
were allowed to load on one factor only. As can be seen
from Table 2, the five-factor model fit the data far better Evaluation of scalability
than the original four-factor model: the GFI and AGFI We also examined the homogeneity of the items within each
were all above the accepted level of 0.90, and the CFI was factor (hereafter called a subscale). This was done by both
0.95. Moreover, the ECVI and RMSEA were both within an computing Cronbach’s alpha and applying a Mokken scale
acceptable level for the five-factor model. analysis. At the same time, we performed an item analysis of
In order to investigate whether the 46 items selected from each item within the subscales. This was done by computing
the safety attitude questionnaires (Rundmo, 1992, 1996, the corrected item–scale correlation for each item, and the
1998) represented separate attitude dimensions, items from H coefficient of each item. The item–scale analysis and the
both the YDAS and the safety attitude questionnaires were Mokken scale analysis gave identical results; all items
included in the same exploratory PCA. The criterion for showed a satisfactory item–scale correlation (r > 0.30), as
item selection was identical to the exploratory analysis of well as an acceptable H coefficient (H > 0.30). The final
the YDAS. The analysis resulted in 11 factors (or subscales) subscale scores were constructed by adding the items within
of risk-taking attitudes. It is interesting to note that the each subscale without differential weighting. Before the
items from our previous questionnaires did not load on the items were added, they were recoded. A high score indicated
same factors as the YDAS items: six additional factors con- an “ideal” attitude, that is, less preference for risk-taking.
sisting of items from these questionnaires were identified in This means that a high score on an attitude items such as
the analysis. The item clustering on these factors was as “If you possess good driving skills, speeding is OK” indic-
expected, and the factors can thus be seen as identical to ated that the respondent disagreed or strongly disagreed
those obtained in the previous studies (Rundmo, 1992, 1996, with the item. Thus, a high score on a subscale implies low
1998). Of the 46 items from the safety attitude question- preferences for risk-taking. Table 4 presents descriptive stat-
naires, 19 were excluded on basis of this analysis. istics, Cronbach’s alpha, average inter-item correlation and
We also performed a PCA separately for respondents with Loevinger’s H coefficient of the final subscales.
a driving licence and for those without. The PCA resulted Table 4 demonstrates similar results from both methods of
in a nearly identical 11-factor structure for both drivers estimating scalability. Subscales 1, 2, 4 and 6–9 have both
and non-drivers. However, the PCA carried out among the alpha coefficients above 0.70 and H coefficients above 0.40.
respondents with driving licence suggested the exclusion of This indicates that the items within these scales have satis-
three items originating from the safety attitude question- factory homogeneity according to both methods. The alpha

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


232 P. Ulleberg and T. Rundmo Scand J Psychol 43 (2002)

Table 3. Standardized factor loadings for the attitude items

Factor 1. Riding with an unsafe driver (items from the YDAS)


I would get into my friend’s car even though she/he is known to be an unsafe driver 0.79
I would probably ride with a friend who drives unsafly if I trusted him or her 0.76
I might get into the car with friends who I know are unsafe drivers 0.72
I would get into the car with a reckless driver if I had no other way to get home 0.72
I might get in the car with an unsafe driver if my friends did 0.65
I would rather walk a hundred miles than get into a car with an unsafe driver 0.64
I would ask my friend to let me out of the car immediately if she/he drove recklessly 0.59
Factor 2. Speeding (items from the YDAS)
It is acceptable to drive in 100 km / h on a straight road if there are no other vehicles within a mile distance 0.76
If you are a safe driver, it is acceptable to exceed the speed limit by 10 km/h in areas where it is permitted 0.75
to drive at 80–90 km/h
I think it’s OK to speed if the traffic conditions allow you to do so 0.77
Driving 5 or 10 miles above the speed limit is OK because everyone does it 0.72
If you have good skills, speeding is OK 0.70
Factor 3. Concern about hurting others (items from the YDAS)
Hurting someone else with my car would scar me for life 0.85
I couldn’t live with myself if I hurt another human being in traffic 0.74
If I should cause an accident, I hope to be the one who’s hurt 0.49
Factor 4. Drinking and driving (items from the YDAS)
I might get in the car with a driver who has been drinking 0.77
I would not even consider riding with a drunk person 0.70
I would get in the car with a driver who has been drinking if I knew and trusted him or her 0.70
Factor 5. Showing off driving skills to others (items from the YDAS)
Most people like to show off their skills by driving fast 0.69
When people drive they like to be different – not to be ordinary, cautious drivers 0.61
People usually (or will usually) drive faster when their friends are in the car 0.57
Factor 6. Traffic flow vs. rule obedience
Sometimes it is necessary to bend the rules to keep traffic going 0.79
It is better to drive smoothly than always to follow the traffic rules 0.72
Sometimes it is necessary to break the traffic rules in order to get ahead 0.70
Sometimes it is necessary to take chances in traffic 0.69
Sometimes it is necessary to bend the traffic rules to arrive on time 0.68
There are many traffic rules which cannot be obeyed in order to keep up the traffic flow 0.68
It is more important to keep up the traffic flow rather than always follow the traffic rules 0.65
Sometimes it is necessary to ignore violations of traffic rules 0.62
A person who take chances and violates some traffic rules is not necessarily a less safe driver 0.55
Factor 7. Joyriding
Speeding and excitement belong together when you are driving 0.90
Driving is more than transportation, it is also speeding and fun 0.82
Adolescents have a need for fun and excitement in traffic 0.76
Factor 8. Dare to speak up to an unsafe driver
A driver who is speeding is a more attractive person than a driver who always follow the rules 0.82
I would be very unpopular if I should ask the person driving to drive more carefully 0.63
Boys prefer girls who dare to get into a car when you are speeding 0.61
If I should ask my friends to drive more carefully, it would be perceived as hassle 0.47
Factor 9. Risk of accidents
Drunk driving is not as risky as people think it is 0.78
The risk of dying young in an traffic accident is so low that you can ignore it 0.75
Driving-off-the-road accidents are so rare that there is no need to worry 0.62
Factor 10. Fatalism
Most accidents could be prevented if the authorities had put more effort into prevention measures 0.71
Traffic accidents are due to poor road standards 0.59
The number of old cars in Norway make accidents unavoidable 0.55
Factor 11. Violation of traffic rules
You should always follow the traffic rules, regardless of the driving conditions 0.73
You should always obey laws while driving 0.71

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


Scand J Psychol 43 (2002) Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers 233

Table 4. Descriptive statistics, Cronbach’s alpha and Loevinger’s weighted H coefficient of the final subscales (n = 3,942)

Number
Subscale of items Mean score SD Min. score Max. score Skewness Kurtosis α H

1. Riding with an unsafe driver 7 19.4 4.71 7 35 0.06 0.24 0.84 0.48
2. Speeding 5 14.6 4.09 5 25 −0.11 −0.37 0.84 0.56
3. Concern about hurting others 3 12.4 2.13 3 15 −0.90 0.88 0.62 0.40
4. Drinking and driving 3 12.0 2.72 3 15 −0.70 −0.32 0.76 0.58
5. Showing off skills to others 3 8.2 2.06 3 15 0.02 −0.07 0.63 0.41
6. Traffic flow vs. rule obedience 9 26.5 5.88 9 45 −0.06 0.20 0.86 0.45
7. Joyriding 3 10.2 2.70 3 15 −0.30 −0.13 0.83 0.66
8. Dare to speak up to an unsafe driver 4 18.8 3.43 5 25 −0.46 0.37 0.74 0.41
9. Risk of accidents 3 12.8 2.06 3 15 −1.30 2.11 0.77 0.59
10. Fatalism 3 9.9 2.11 3 15 −0.34 0.38 0.63 0.36
11. Violation of traffic rules 2 6.8 1.76 2 10 −0.18 −0.31 0.64 0.51

Table 5. Intercorrelations between the subscales (n = 3,942)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. Riding with an unsafe driver –


2. Speeding 0.56 –
3. Concern about hurting others 0.12 0.19 –
4. Drinking and driving 0.39 0.33 0.21 –
5. Showing off skills to others 0.17 0.13 −0.06 0.06 –
6. Traffic flow vs. rule obedience 0.50 0.68 0.20 0.32 0.15 –
7. Joyriding 0.45 0.52 0.19 0.28 0.26 0.54 –
8. Dare to speak up to an unsafe driver 0.26 0.26 0.18 0.19 0.29 0.31 0.43 –
9. Risk of accidents 0.25 0.32 0.31 0.36 0.06 0.32 0.35 0.36 –
10. Fatalism 0.22 0.33 0.18 0.19 0.14 0.42 0.33 0.28 0.30 –
11. Violation of traffic rules 0.31 0.40 0.18 0.21 −0.05 0.43 0.25 0.10 0.17 0.14

Note: All correlations are significant at the 0.01 level.

is, however, low on the subscales 3, 5, and 11, ranging from relative to other constructs. In order to obtain discriminant
0.62 to 0.64, whereas the H coefficient indicated satisfactory validity, an attitude item should show the strongest correla-
homogeneity of these items. The difference between the tion with the scale it is hypothesized to represent, compared
alpha and H coefficient may be due to the fact that these with other scales. Satisfactory discriminant validity was
subscales consisted of only two or three items each, which obtained for all 45 items. Another way of establishing item
increases the difficulty of obtaining a satisfactory alpha. discriminant validity is to examine substantial factor load-
Due to the low number of items in subscales 3, 5, and 11, ings of the items on the other subscales, that is a “cross-
we judged the items to have satisfactory homogeneity. On loadings” above 0.50 to other factors identified in the
the other hand, subscale 10, named “Fatalism”, demonstrated exploratory factor analysis. No cross-loadings of this magni-
both a low alpha and a low H coefficient, indicating weak tude were found.
scalability. This suggests that further work should be done A third way of establishing discriminant validity is to
in order to improve the reliability of the “Fatalism” subscale. examine the intercorrelations between the subscales (Table
Table 4 also shows the symmetry and shape, in terms of 5). We expected the subscales to be positively correlated,
skewness and kurtosis, of the subscales’ distributions. Most although not too highly, if they really are measuring dif-
of the subscales demonstrate a symmetrical distribution. ferent latent variables. In general, the discriminant validity
However, subscales 3, 4, and 9 were relatively negatively was satisfactory for the subscales. However, the highest
skewed, indicating that the respondents in general had high correlation was found between subscale 2, “Speeding”, and
scores on these scales, meaning they reported low prefer- subscale 6, “Traffic flow vs. rule obedience” (r = 0.68),
ences for risk-taking. indicating that these dimensions are quite similar. The high
correlation between these two subscales may imply that the
two scales measure the same concept, and not separate con-
Discriminant validity cepts. In order to test this hypothesis, a confirmatory factor
In an item discriminant analysis, items are evaluated with analysis with 10 factors was carried out. The fit indexes indic-
respect to how well they represent a particular construct ated a clearly poorer model fit than the 11-factor structure.

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


234 P. Ulleberg and T. Rundmo Scand J Psychol 43 (2002)

Table 6. Results of t-tests for gender differences in mean score on the 11 subscales

Women Men
(n = 2,207) (n = 1,735)

Mean SD Mean SD t

1. Riding with an unsafe driver 20.7 4.41 17.9 4.64 18.98*


2. Speeding 15.8 3.70 13.1 4.04 22.10*
3. Concern about hurting others 13.0 1.83 11.7 2.25 19.77*
4. Drinking and driving 12.5 2.54 11.5 2.85 11.26*
5. Showing off skills to others 8.3 2.00 7.9 2.11 6.24*
6. Traffic flow vs. rule obedience 28.1 5.40 24.6 5.94 18.61*
7. Joyriding 11.0 2.46 9.4 2.72 19.55*
8. Dare to speak up to an unsafe driver 19.5 3.08 17.8 3.59 16.21*
9. Risk of accidents 13.2 1.78 12.3 2.24 14.79*
10. Fatalism 10.3 1.87 9.3 2.22 15.10*
11. Violation of traffic rules 7.0 1.68 6.4 1.81 10.52*

Note: *p < 0.001.

We also judged the item content to be somewhat different on Table 7. Partial correlations between the attitude subscales, risk-
the two subscales, and thus decided to keep the 11-factor taking behavior and accident involvement among respondents with a
driving licence (n = 1,963), controlling for gender differences
structure of the attitude measure.
With the exception of the correlation between subscale 2 Risk-taking Accident
and 6, the remaining subscales were moderately to weakly Attitude subscale behavior involvement
correlated. This strengthens the subscales’ discriminant
validity, indicating that risk-taking attitudes in relation to 1. Riding with an unsafe driver −0.35** −0.02
2. Speeding −0.45*** −0.05**
driving are multidimensional. However, subscale 5, “Show-
3. Concern about hurting others −0.24*** −0.10***
ing off skills to others”, was characterized by weak and 4. Drinking and driving −0.29*** −0.06***
sometimes negative correlations with the other subscales. 5. Showing off skills to others −0.20*** −0.03*
This may indicate that this subscale measures something 6. Traffic flow vs. rule obedience −0.45*** −0.05*
different than the other attitude scales, for instance self- 7. Joyriding −0.45*** −0.09***
8. Dare to speak up to an unsafe −0.35*** −0.05**
assertiveness. Nevertheless, the positive correlations with
driver
the other subscales suggest that some aspects of risk-taking 9. Risk for accidents −0.36*** −0.10***
attitudes are present in the scale. 10. Fatalism −0.29*** −0.11***
11. Violation of traffic rules −0.26*** −0.07***

External correlates of the subscales Note: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

If the subscales really are indicators of risk-taking attitudes,


women would be expected to score higher on them than (Ajzen, 1988), the relationship between attitudes (measured
men, that is, to express less preference for risk-taking. Thus, at a general level) and traffic accident frequency (a criterion
women were hypothesized to have more “ideal” attitudes at a specific level) was weak. However, all correlations were
than men by showing a higher mean score on the subscales. negative and almost all were significant. Thus, the respond-
Table 6 shows significant gender differences on all the sub- ents with “ideal” attitudes towards risk-taking reported less
scales, with women having a higher mean score, indicating accident involvement than those with less “ideal” attitudes.
that they do indeed show more “ideal” risk-taking attitudes Finally, a multiple regression analysis was performed
than men. in order determine the total influence of the scores on the
Next, the relationship between attitudes, self-reported risk attitude dimensions for self-reported risk-taking behavior
behavior, and accident frequency among the respondents (Table 8). Together, the attitude dimensions explained 50%
with a driving licence was examined. In order to control for of the total variance in self-reported risk-taking behavior.
gender differences, partial correlation coefficients were used.
As hypothesized, the attitude subscales were negatively cor-
related with self-reported risk-taking behavior (Table 7). DISCUSSION
Hence, the higher score on the subscales, the less risk-taking The major aim of the present study was to develop a reliable
behavior the respondents reported. Table 7 also shows the and valid measurement instrument of adolescents’ risk-taking
relationship between self-reported accident frequency and attitudes to driving. The structure of the instrument was
attitudes. In accordance with the principle of compatibility thought to be multidimensional, indicating that the different

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


Scand J Psychol 43 (2002) Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers 235

Table 8. Prediction of risk-taking behavior from attitudes: multiple criteria for item selection, whereas the present study selected
regression analysis based on respondents with a driving licence the items on basis of a factor analysis. The latter method is
(n = 1,963)
traditionally recommended in order to examine multidimen-
Standardized sionality among items (Kline, 1995). Still, three of the four
regression dimensions we included from the YDAS could be recovered
Attitude sub-scale coefficient in the factor analysis. This suggests that a similar structure
of risk-taking attitudes exists among Norwegian and US
1. Riding with an unsafe driver −0.10***
adolescents.
2. Speeding −0.18***
3. Concern about hurting others −0.02 In order to obtain univariate scales of the latent attitude
4. Drinking and driving −0.07*** dimensions, the items that were selected on the basis of the
5. Showing off skills to others −0.08*** exploratory analysis had to fulfill the criteria of a factor
6. Traffic flow vs. rule obedience −0.12*** loading above 0.50. This causes a selection of items within
7. Joyriding −0.15***
clusters that discriminate most with other item clusters. In
8. Dare to speak up to an unsafe driver −0.09***
9. Risk of accidents −0.17*** other words, the criteria may have maximized the differences
10. Fatalism −0.07*** between the final subscales of risk-taking attitudes. The
11. Violation of traffic rules −0.05** satisfactory fit shown by the indexes of the confirmatory
factor analysis was interpreted as evidence for the multidimen-
Notes: R2 = 0.50. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
sionality of risk-taking attitudes. However, the evidence of
multidimensionality may be the result of a self-fulfilling
attitude subscales reflect different dimensions of risk-taking prophecy. Although the statistical criteria applied in the
attitudes. The results support this assumption: 11 factors present paper cannot guarantee the validity of multidimen-
were identified on the basis of an exploratory factor analysis. sionality in risk-taking attitudes, there is still reason to
In addition, a confirmatory factor analysis demonstrated a believe that multidimensionality in risk-taking attitudes is
satisfactory fit to the data of the suggested 11-factor struc- conceptually meaningful. In line with critics of exploratory
ture. Further evidence for multidimensionality was found factor analysis (see Bynner, 1988), we believe that content
in discriminate analysis of the scales. As expected, the validity criteria of factors are more important than empir-
subscales turned out to be intercorrelated, but not strongly ical criteria based on factor analytic considerations. The
so. They can thus be considered to represent different content of the items clustering on each factor was evaluated
constructs. Moreover, the content of the items clustering on as logically and conceptually associated with the factor they
each factor was evaluated as logically and conceptually asso- were thought to represent. Furthermore, the sample was ran-
ciated with the factor they were thought to represent. The domly split in two parts to be compared, with an exploratory
different attitude dimensions (factors) were also similar to factor analysis on one half and confirmatory analyses of the
those previously found in the studies by Rundmo (1992, 11-factor model on the other half. The results yielded no
1996, 1998), as well as the dimensions included from the difference in either the factor structure or model fit com-
YDAS (Malfetti et al., 1989). The study supported the pared with the same analyses carried on the total sample. This
assumption of multidimensionality on an empirical as well further strengthens the assumption of multidimensionality.
as a theoretical basis. The majority of the different attitude subscales demon-
A 45-item scale with 11 subscales was constructed from strated sufficient reliability and item homogeneity. However,
the original pool of 87 items. A total of 42 items did not four of the subscales had alpha coefficients below 0.70, and
fulfill the criteria for item selection. There may be several one of these had an H coefficient below 0.40. This suggests
reasons for this. One explanation may be that the items ori- that the attitude measure should be viewed as a good pre-
ginating from the YDAS were translated from English into liminary measure that could benefit from refinement. The
Norwegian. The translation may have changed both word- reliability analysis also gave similar results for both the par-
ing and meaning, causing the items to measure something ametric and the nonparametric method for evaluating item
other than what was originally intended. This may have scalability. This indicates that the estimation of internal con-
caused the items to fail to show the expected relation to sistency by means of Cronbach’s alpha was quite robust
other attitude items. A second explanation for why items against the skewness in item distributions. Some of the items
failed to load may be cultural differences. The YDAS was that were selected in the final scales indeed showed a skewed
developed from a sample of adolescents in the USA. Thus, distribution.
the excluded items may be of particular relevance for the As hypothesized, the correlation between attitudes and
North American culture, and not equally appropriate for the aggregated measure of self-reported behavior was con-
Norwegian adolescents. A third explanation may be the siderably stronger than the attitude–accident correlation. This
use of different statistical methods by Malfetti et al. (1989) is not an unexpected result, considering that it is unlikely
compared with the present study. Malfetti et al. (1989) that accident involvement will capture all aspects of risky
used internal consistency and item–scale correlation as the driving behavior, since only a few of the risky actions taken

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


236 P. Ulleberg and T. Rundmo Scand J Psychol 43 (2002)

in traffic situations can be expected to lead to an accident. taking behavior represent sensitive information about the
Moreover, the incompatibility in the level of measurement of individual, and the respondents may choose not to report
attitudes and accident is likely to cause an underestimation such behaviors in order to present themselves in a socially
of the correlation. This may suggest that accident frequency desirable way. However, West, French, Kemp and Elander
is an inappropriate criterion for the study of the attitude– (1993) have concluded that self-reports of driver behavior,
behavior relationship. including deviant driving behavior, can be used as a surrog-
However, one should draw this conclusion with caution. ate for actual driving behavior. Their conclusion was based
First, the measure of accident involvement did not specify on the correlation coefficients found between observers’
whether the driver caused the accident or simply was ratings of a driver’s behavior and the driver’s self-reported
involved through no fault of his or her own. Ideally, a meas- behavior. A similar conclusion was reached in a longitudinal
ure of the former kind should be included in order to elim- study of young drivers in Finland (Hattaka, Keskinen,
inate this kind of “measurement error”. Second, the attitude Katila & Laapotti, 1997). They found a significant relation-
measure developed in this study is previously untested, and ship between self-reported driving habits and future accident
further studies should be done in order to test both the fac- involvement. A relationship between self-reported driving
tor structure and the validity of the instrument. habits and police-registered traffic violations was also found
Nevertheless, the attitude dimensions were quite success- in their study.
ful in predicting self-reported risk behavior; in fact, the When it comes to the practical implementation of the
dimensions accounted for a total of 50% of the variance in measurement instrument, we see potential in the area of
behavior. However, the correlation, as well as the standard- road safety programs. One possibility would be to adminis-
ized regression coefficient between the various attitude ter the scales longitudinally, to obtain change scores over
dimensions and behavior, differed in magnitude. This sug- time. For instance, an assessment of adolescents joining road
gests that some dimensions were more important predictors safety programs could be undertaken at an initial point in
of behavior than others. Thus, the attitude dimensions with the program and then again later, in order to see if attitudes
the highest correspondence with self-reported behavior could had changed. Assessments can lend insight into whether
be given special attention in safety programs. This may also interventions such as road safety programs can change atti-
be an additional explanation of why attitude campaigns seem tudes, and, if so, which attitudes are most likely to change,
to be unsuccessful, as mentioned in the Introduction. Sev- and how the changes are related to changes in behaviors in
eral of the campaigns were aimed at influencing safety atti- traffic. Another possible application of the instrument
tudes in general, and did not, to a proper extent, focus on would be to administer it to the target group before a safety
the specific attitudes likely to influence risk-taking behavior. program was conducted, in order to identify which areas
However, the term “predictor” should be used with cau- had the most potential for improvements. Such attitudes
tion. Risk-taking behavior was measured by self-report could then be given particular attention when the program
measures at the same time as the attitudes were measured. was carried out.
Thus, the use of self-reports also makes it difficult to claim
that the attitude scales predict behavior, because reports of This study was supported by the Norwegian Authorities of Public
Roads (Statens Vegvesen). Especially, the authors would like to
the latter are provided after the behavior has occurred. In
thank the steering committee of the campaign, and the following
other words, attitudes may correspond to behavior in order members of the project group: Per Gjerde, Erik Hartmann, Rolf
to justify previous actions, not vice versa (Bem, 1967; Robertsen and Bård Morten Johansen, as well as all the respond-
Festinger, 1957). Furthermore, a relationship with actual ents who answered our questionnaire. We also would like to thank
driving behavior instead of self-reports would also give more the two anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments and
suggestions for revisions.
powerful evidence of the influence of attitudes on behavior.
On the other hand, self-reports have several advantages over
some of the alternatives of studying driving behavior, REFERENCES
namely direct observations or simulation of the driving task. Aarø, L. E. & Rise, J. (1996). Den menneskelige faktor [The human
First, both the alternative methods have the disadvantage factor] (SF-report nr. 5–96). Oslo: Skadeforebyggende Forum (in
that they place the individual under observation, which Norwegian).
may cause the driver to be more disciplined than normal. Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality and behaviour. Buckingham:
Open Press University.
Second, these methods are expensive and time consuming. Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and pre-
Third, an advantage of self-report is that it represents a sum- dicting social behaviour. New York: Prentice-Hall.
mary judgment of information in a variety of situations, and Assum, T., Midtland, K. & Opdal, L. (1993). Bilføreres holdninger
may therefore be the most suited measure according to the og risiko for ulykker [Drivers’ attitudes and risk for accidents]
principle of compatibility (Ajzen, 1988). (report no. 223). Oslo: Institute of Transport Economics (in
Norwegian).
Another measurement problem is the validity of self- Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception theory: An alternative interpreta-
report measures in relation to actual behavior in traffic. tion of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review,
Behaviors like violating traffic rules and engaging in risk- 74, 183–200.

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


Scand J Psychol 43 (2002) Risk-taking attitudes among young drivers 237

Bjørnskau, T. (2000). Road traffic risk in Norway 1997/98 (report attitudes. New York: New York Teachers College, Columbia
no. 483). Oslo: Institute of Transport Economics. University.
Brown, I. D. & Groeger, J. A. (1988). Risk perception and decision Michels, W. & Schneider, P. (1984). Traffic offences: Another
taking during the transition between novice and experienced approach to description and prediction. Accident Analysis and
driver status. Ergonomics, 31, 585–597. Prevention, 16, 223–238.
Browne, M. W. & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing Milech, D., Glencross, D. & Hartley, L. (1989). Skill acquisition by
model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural young drivers: Perceiving, interpreting and responding to the driv-
equation models ( pp. 136 –162). Newbury Park: Sage. ing environment (report no. MR4). Canberra: Federal Office of
Bynner, J. (1988). Factor analysis and the construct indicator rela- Road Safety.
tionship. Human Relations, 41, 389– 405. Moe, D. (1986). Young drivers. Relation between perceived and real
Carmines, E. G. & Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and validity ability. Behavioural studies (report STF63 A92002). Trondheim:
assessment. Beverly Hills: Sage. SINTEF Samferdselsteknikk (in Norwegian).
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure Mokken, R. J. (1971). A theory and procedure of scale analysis. The
of tests. Psychometrica, 16, 297–334. Hague: Mouton.
Deery, H. A. (1999). Hazard and risk perception among young Molenaar, I. W., Debets, P., Sijtsma, K. & Hemker, B. T. (1994).
novice drivers. Journal of Safety Research, 30, 225–236. MSP. A program for Mokken scale analysis for polytomus items,
Delhomme, P., Vaa, T., Meyer, T., Gordon, H., Goldenbeld, C., version 3.0. Groeningen: Iec ProGamma.
Järmark, S., Christie, N. & Rehnova, V. (1999). Evaluated road Nunnaly, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-
safety campaigns: An overview of 265 evaluated campaigns and Hill.
some meta-analysis on accidents. Gadget project, work package OECD (1994). Improving road safety by attitude modifications
4, contract no. RO-97-SC.2235. (report prepared by an OECD scientific expert group). Paris:
DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development: Theory and applications. OECD.
London: Sage. Parker, D., Lajunen, T. & Strandling, S. (1998). Attitudinal predic-
Elvik, R., Vaa, T. & Østvik, E. (1989). Trafikksikkerhetshåndboka tors of interpersonally aggressive violations on the road. Trans-
[Handbook of traffic safety]. Oslo: Institute of Transport Eco- portation Research Part F, 1, 11–24.
nomics (in Norwegian). Parker, D. & Manstead, A. S. R. (1996). The social psychology of
Evans, L. (1991). Traffic safety and the driver. New York: Van driver behaviour. In G. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.), Applied social
Nostrand Reinhold. psychology (pp. 198–224). London: Sage.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief
Row Peterson. model. Health Education Monographs, 2, 1–8.
Friedstrøm, L., Ifver, J., Ingebrigtsen, S., Kulmala, R. & Thomsen, Rundmo, T. (1992). Økt trafikksikkerhet ved motivasjon og beløn-
L. K. (1995). Measuring the contribution of randomness, expos- ning. Resultater fra en spørreskjema-undersøkelse [Increased
ure, weather, and daylight to the variation in road accident traffic safety through motivation and rewards. Results from a
counts. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 27, 1–20. questionnaire survey] (report no. 0328). Oslo: Institute of Trans-
Gregersen, N. P. & Bjurulf, P. (1996). Young novice drivers: port Economics (in Norwegian).
Towards a model of their accident involvement. Accident Analysis Rundmo, T. (1996). Associations between risk perception and
and Prevention, 28, 229–241. safety. Safety Science, 24, 197–209.
Hattaka, M., Keskinen, E., Katila, A. & Laapotti, S. (1997). Self- Rundmo, T. (1998). Organizational factors, safety attitudes and risk
reported driving habits are valid predictors of violations and behaviour, main report, prepared for Norsk Hydro (report no
accidents. In I. T. Rothengatter & E. Carbonell Vaya (Eds.), 24). Trondheim: Rotunde.
Traffic and transport psychology: Theory and application Rundmo, T. & Ulleberg, P. (2000). Var det aerdt det? Evaluering av
(pp. 295–303). Oxford: Pergamon. 18– 40 Aksjonen. Resultatrapport [Evaluation of the 18– 40 cam-
Hoyle, R. H. & Panter, A. T. (1995). Writing about structural equa- paign. Main report] (report no. 43). Trondheim: Rotunde (in
tion models. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Norwegian).
Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 158–175). London: Sage. Sutton, S. (1998). Predicting and explaining intentions and beha-
Hu, L. & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. viour: How well are we doing? Journal of Applied Social Psycho-
Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and logy, 28, 1317–1338.
applications (pp. 76–99). London: Sage. Tränkle, U., Gelau, C. & Metker, T. (1990). Risk perception and
Hu, L. & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in age-specific accidents of young drivers. Accident Analysis and
covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new Prevention, 22, 119–125.
alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. Van Schuur, W. H. & Kiers, H. A. L. (1994). Why factor analysis
Jessor, R. (1984). Adolescent development and behavioural health. often is incorrect model for analysing bipolar concepts, and
In J. Matarazzo, A. J. Herd, N. M. Miller & S. M. Weiss (Eds.), what model to use instead. Applied Psychological Measurement,
Behavioural health: A handbook of health enhancement and dis- 18, 97–110.
ease prevention (pp. 69–90). New York: Wiley. West, R., French, D., Kemp, R. & Elander, J. (1993). Direct observa-
Jöreskog, K. A. & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equa- tions of driving, self reports of driver behaviour and accident
tion modelling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: involvement. Ergonomics, 36, 557–567.
Scientific Software. West, R. & Hall, J. (1997). The role of personality and attitudes
Kline, P. (1995). The handbook of psychological testing. London: in traffic accident risk. Applied Psychology: An International
Routledge. Review, 46, 253 –264.
Loehlin, J. C. (1998): Latent variable models. An introduction to factor, Wilde, G. J. S. (1993). Effects of mass media communication on
path and structural analysis. 3rd edn. London: Lawrence Erlbaum. health and safety habits: An overview of issues and evidence.
Malfetti, J. L., Rose, P. R., DeKorp, N. A. & Basch, C. E. (1989) Addiction, 88, 983 –996.
The Young Driver Attitude Scale. The development and field
testing of an instrument to measure young drivers’ risk-taking Received 2 November 2000, accepted 14 February 2001

© 2002 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

You might also like