You are on page 1of 23

14 CHAPTER 2

Articulatory Phonetics
2
SPEECH SOUND FORM

LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have finished this chapter, you should be able to:
• List the differences in production and function of vowels versus consonants.
• Identify the three descriptive parameters that are used for vowel articulations,
and classify the vowels of American English using those three parameters.
• Differentiate between monophthong and diphthong vowels.
• Define centering diphthongs.
• Differentiate between a phonemic and a nonphonemic diphthong.
• Identify the four parameters that are used to describe the articulation of consonants.
• Define the various manners of articulation.
• Classify the consonants of American English according to their organ, place, manner,
and voicing characteristics.
• Define coarticulation and assimilation, and describe the different types of
assimilatory processes.
• Understand the importance of syllable structure in the assessment process.

A rticulatory phonetics deals with the cat-


egorization and classification of the produc-
rary phonological theories have provided new
ways of viewing assessment and treatment
tion features of speech sounds. A thorough of these disorders, knowledge of the speech
knowledge of how vowels and consonants are sounds’ production features secures a firm ba-
generated remains essential for successful as- sis for utilizing such procedures. Without this
sessment and remediation of articulatory and knowledge, phonological process analysis, for
phonological disorders. Although contempo- example, is impossible.

14

ch02.indd 14 7/12/2007 8:04:38 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 15

This chapter discusses articulatory-


BOX 2.1 Selected Readings in Anatomy
phonetic aspects of the speech sounds of Gen-
and Physiology of the Speech and Hearing
eral American English. The specific goals are
Mechanisms

1. to provide a review of the production fea- Culbertson, W. R., Cotton, S. S., & Tanner, D. C.
(2006). Anatomy and physiology study guide
tures of vowels and consonants;
for speech and hearing. San Diego: Plural
2. to introduce the concepts of coarticulation Publishing.
and assimilation as a means of describing Kent, R. D. (1997). The speech sciences. San Diego:
how sounds change within a given articu- Singular Publishing.
latory context; and Perkins, W., & Kent, R. (1986). Functional anatomy
3. to examine the structure of syllables and of speech, language and hearing: A primer. Bos-
their clinical applicability in the assess- ton: Allyn & Bacon.
ment and treatment of impaired articula- Seikel, J. A., King, D. W., & Drumwright, D. G.
(2005). Anatomy and physiology for speech
tion and phonology.
and language (3rd ed.). Clifton Park, NY:
Delmar.
The production of vowels and con- Zemlin, W. R. (1997). Speech and hearing science:
sonants, and their subsequent language- Anatomy and physiology (4th ed.). Boston:
specific arrangements into syllables and Allyn & Bacon.
words, depends on articulatory motor pro-
cesses. If these processes are impaired, speech
sound production will be disordered. Articula-
tory motor processes depend in turn on many from those covering impaired articulation and
anatomical-physiological prerequisites, which phonology. This is because the clinical signifi-
include respiratory, phonatory, or resonatory cance of anatomical-physiological knowledge
processes. For example, the speech problems and its application to articulatory and phono-
of children with cerebral palsy often originate logical disorders is not always recognized. The
in abnormal respiratory, resonatory, and/ anatomical-physiological aspects of such dis-
or phonatory prerequisites for articulation. orders are not within the scope of this chap-
The proper function of such prerequisites, ter. Box 2.1 offers references as an incentive
therefore, must first be secured before any ar- for the reader to rediscover the wealth of in-
ticulatory improvement can be expected. Ar- formation essential to the clinical assessment
ticulatory motor ability is embedded in many and remediation of articulatory and phono-
different anatomical-physiological prerequi- logical impairments.
sites, which are of fundamental importance
to speech-language pathologists.
Basic knowledge in
these areas is typically For more information
VOWELS VERSUS CONSONANTS
gained from courses and about the respiratory, Speech sounds are commonly divided into
phonatory, resona-
textbooks covering anat- tory, and articulatory two groups: vowels and consonants. Vow-
omy and physiology of characteristics of els are produced with a relatively open vocal
the speech and hearing cerebral palsy, see tract; no significant constriction of the oral (and
mechanisms rather than Chapter 10. pharyngeal) cavities exists. The airstream from

ch02.indd 15 7/12/2007 8:04:40 AM


16 CHAPTER 2

the vocal folds to the lips is relatively unim- Table 2.1 IPA Symbols (Wise, 1958)
peded. Therefore, vowels are considered to be
open sounds. In contrast, consonants are pro- Consonants Vowels
duced with a significant constriction in the oral Commonly Commonly
and/or pharyngeal cavities during their pro- Symbol Realized In Symbol Realized In
duction. For consonants, the airstream from
the vocal folds to the lips and nostrils encoun- [p] pay [i] eat
[b] boy [] in
ters some type of articulatory obstacle along
[t] toy [e] ape
the way. Therefore, consonants are considered [d] doll [ε] egg
to be constricted sounds. [k] coat [] at
The sagittal midline of
For most consonants this the vocal tract refers [] goat [a] father*
constriction occurs along to the median plane [m] moon [u] moon
the sagittal midline of the that divides the vocal [n] not [υ] wood
vocal tract. This constric- tract into right and [ŋ] sing [oυ] boat
left halves. [θ] think [ɔ] father*
tion for consonants can
[ð] those [ɑ] hop
be exemplified by the first sound in top, [t], or
[f] far [a] tie
soap, [s]. For [t] the contact of the front of the
[v] vase [aυ] mouse
tongue with the alveolar ridge occurs along [s] sun [ɔ] boy
this midline while the characteristic s-quality [z] zoo [
] girl*
is made by air flowing along this median plane [ʃ] shop [] bird
as the tongue approximates the alveolar ridge. [ ] beige [] winner
By contrast, during all vowel productions the [tʃ ] chop [ ] cut
sagittal midline remains free. In addition, un- [d ] job [ə] above
der normal speech conditions, General Ameri- [j] yes
[w] win
can English vowels are always produced with
[] when*
vocal fold vibration; they are voiced speech [l] leap
sounds. Only during whispered speech are [r] red
vowels unvoiced. Consonants, on the other [h] hop
hand, may be generated with or without si-
multaneous vocal fold vibration; they can be *May be regional or individual pronunciations.
voiced or voiceless. Pairs of sounds such as [t]
and [d] exemplify this relevant feature. Pairs
of similar sounds, in this case differing only
in their voicing feature, are referred to as cog- tic energy they display. Vowels are highly res-
nates. Voicing features constitute the main onant, demonstrating at least two formant
linguistically relevant differences that sepa- areas. Thus, vowels are more intense than
rate the consonant cognates such as [s] from consonants; in other words, they are typi-
[z] or [f ] from [v]. The transcription of various cally louder than consonants. In this respect
vowels and consonants together with exam- we can say that vowels have greater sonority
ples of words in which these sounds can be than consonants. Sonority of a sound is its
heard are contained in Table 2.1. loudness relative to that of other sounds with
Vowels can also be distinguished from the same length, stress, and pitch (Ladefoged,
consonants according to the patterns of acous- 2006). Due to the greater sonority of vowels

ch02.indd 16 7/12/2007 8:04:40 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 17

over consonants, vowels are also referred to as Table 2.2 Features Differentiating Vowels
sonorants. and Consonants
Due to the production features of a special
Vowels Consonants
group of consonants and their resulting so-
nority, certain consonants are also labeled so- No significant Significant constriction
norants. Sonorant consonants are produced constriction of the of the vocal tract
with a relatively open expiratory passageway. vocal tract
When contrasted to other consonants, so- Open sounds Constricted sounds
norant consonants demonstrate less obstruc-
tion of the airstream during their production. Sagittal midline Constriction occurs
The sonorant consonants include the nasals, of vocal tract along sagittal midline
remains open of the vocal tract
the liquids, and the glides. The sonorants are
distinguished from the obstruents, which are Voiced Voiced or unvoiced
characterized by a complete or narrow constric-
tion between the articulators hindering the ex- Acoustically more Acoustically less intense
intense
piratory airstream. The obstruents include the
stop-plosives, the fricatives, and the affricates. Demonstrate more Demonstrate less
There are also functional differences be- sonority sonority
tween vowels and consonants. In other words,
Function as syllable Only specific consonants
vowels and consonants play different linguis- nuclei can function as syllable
tic roles. This has often been referred to as the nuclei
“phonological difference” between vowels
and consonants (Crystal, 1987; Hyman, 1975).
The term consonant actually indicates this: con
American English Vowels
meaning “together with” and -sonant reflect-
ing the tonal qualities that characterize vowels. Vowels are commonly described according to
Thus, consonants are those speech sounds that certain parameters (Abercrombie, 1967; Crys-
function linguistically together with vowels. As tal, 1987; Heffner, 1975; Kantner and West,
such, vowels serve as the center of syllables, as 1960; Kent, 1998; Shriberg and Kent, 2003):
syllable nuclei. Vowels can constitute syllables
1. The portion of the tongue that is involved
all by themselves, for example, in the first syl-
in the articulation. Example: front versus
lable of a-go or e-lope. Vowels can also appear
back vowels.
together with one or more consonants, ex-
2. The tongue’s position relative to the pal-
emplified by blue, bloom, or blooms. Although
ate. Example: high versus low vowels.
there are many types of syllables, the vowel is
3. The degree of lip rounding or unrounding.
always the center of the syllable, its nucleus. A
small group of consonants When transcribing, The four-sided form called a vowel quadrilat-
can serve as the nuclei of syllabic consonants eral is often used to demonstrate schemati-
syllables. A consonant that need a special nota- cally the front–back and high–low positions.
functions as a syllable nu- tion. This is discussed The form roughly represents the tongue posi-
in Chapter 3.
cleus is referred to as a syl- tion in the oral cavity (see Figure 2.1).
labic. These form and functional differences The terms tense/lax and open/close are also
are summarized in Table 2.2. used to describe vowels. Tense and lax refer to

ch02.indd 17 7/12/2007 8:04:41 AM


18 CHAPTER 2

Front Central Back 2006). The initial segment, the beginning por-
tion of such a diphthong, is phonetically re-
High High ferred to as the onglide, its end portion as the
offglide. Using this notation system, the fol-
lowing descriptions for the most common vow-
els of General American English are offered.
Mid Mid
Front Vowels
[i] a high-front vowel, unrounded, close
Low Low and tense.
[] a high-front vowel, unrounded, open
and lax.
Figure 2.1 Vowel Quadrilateral of General [e] a mid-front vowel, unrounded, close
American English Vowels
and tense. In General American English,
this vowel is typically produced as a
the degree of muscular activity involved in the diphthong, especially in stressed sylla-
articulation and to the length of the vowels bles or when articulated slowly.
in question (Shriberg and Kent, 2003). There-
[ε] a mid-front vowel, unrounded, open
fore, tense vowels are considered to have rela-
and lax.
tively more muscle activity and are longer in
duration than lax vowels. The vowel [i] is con- [] a low-front vowel, unrounded, open
sidered to be a tense vowel, whereas [] is lax. and lax.
When contrasting tense versus lax, one has to [a] a low-front vowel, unrounded, close
keep in mind that these oppositions refer to and tense. In General American En-
pairs of vowels that are productionally similar, glish, the use of this vowel depends on
to vowel cognates. For example, [i] and [] are the particular regional dialect of the
considered to be “ee” type vowels, and [u] and speaker. In the New England dialect of
[υ] are “oo” type vowels. the Northeast, one might often hear it.
The terms close and open refer to the rela-
All front vowels show various degrees of
tive closeness of the tongue to the roof of the
unrounding (lip spreading), with the high-
mouth (Abercrombie, 1967). Again, only vowel
front vowels showing the most. The lip
cognates are usually characterized with these
spreading becomes less as one moves from the
terms. Using the previous examples, [i] is more
high-front vowels to the mid-front vowels, fi-
close and [] more open, [u] close and [υ] open.
nally becoming practically nonexistent in the
There are two types of vowels: monoph-
low-front vowels.
thongs and diphthongs. Monophthongs re-
main qualitatively the same throughout their
Back Vowels
entire production. They are “It should be noted
pure vowels (Abercrombie, that although [u] a high-back vowel, rounded, close and
1967; Shriberg and Kent, monophthongs are tense.
2003). Diphthongs are often referred to as [υ] a high-back vowel, rounded, open and
‘pure’ vowels, no
vowels in which there is a special virtue attaches lax.
change in quality during to them” (Abercrom- [o] a mid-back vowel, rounded, close and
their duration (Ladefoged, bie, 1967, p. 60). tense. This vowel is typically produced

ch02.indd 18 7/12/2007 8:04:42 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 19

cally more intense, has a higher fun-


There are differences of opinion as to whether
certain vowels (specifically [ɔ] and [ɑ]) are tense damental frequency, and has a longer
or lax. This is based partially on definitional dif- duration when it is compared to a simi-
ferences. Heffner (1975) and Kantner and West lar unstressed vowel such as [].
(1960) define tense and lax according to the [] a central vowel, rounded, lax with
degree of muscular activity. Shriberg and Kent
r-coloring. Again, lip rounding may
(2003) point out that this has not been verified by
experimental studies (e.g., Neary, 1978; Raphael
vary from speaker to speaker. This lax
and Bell-Berti, 1975), and they add the dimension vowel is an unstressed vowel.
of length: Tense vowels are longer in duration [] a central vowel, rounded, tense. [] is
than lax ones. Ladefoged (2006) defines tense very similar in pronunciation to [],
and lax according to the type of syllable in which but it lacks any r-coloring. This vowel
the vowel can occur. Only tense vowels can occur
is heard in certain dialects. [] might be
in open syllables, that is, in those without a con-
sonant following the vowel (as in the words bee found in a Southern dialect pronuncia-
and do); all other vowels must be considered lax. tion of bird or worth, for example. Also,
it could be heard in the speech of chil-
dren having difficulties producing the
as a diphthong, especially in stressed “r” sound.
syllables or when articulated slowly.
[] a lax, unrounded central vowel. It is a
[ɔ] a low mid-back vowel, rounded, open stressed vowel.
and lax (Heffner, 1975). The use of
[ə] a lax, unrounded central vowel. It is an
this vowel depends on regional
unstressed vowel.
pronunciation.
[ɑ] a low-back vowel, unrounded, open and
lax (Kantner and West, 1960). There
seems to be some confusion in tran- CLINICAL APPLICATION
scribing [ɔ] and [ɑ], although acoustic Do Children Have Difficulties
differences certainly exist. One distin- Producing Vowels?
guishing feature: the [ɔ] shows some Vowel errors in children developing phonological skills
degree of lip rounding, whereas [ɑ] in a normal manner are relatively uncommon. However,
does not. children with phonological disorders may show deviant
vowel patterns. Several studies (e.g., Gibbon, Shockey,
Back vowels display different degrees of and Reid, 1992; Penney, Fee, and Dowdle, 1994; Pol-
lock, 2002; Pollock and Keiser, 1990; Reynolds, 1990;
lip rounding in General American English.
Robb, Bleile, and Yee, 1999; Stoel-Gammon and Her-
The high-back vowels [u] and [υ] often show a rington, 1990) have documented the presence of
fairly high degree of lip rounding, whereas the specific vowel problems in phonologically disordered
low-back vowel [ɑ] is commonly articulated as children. Although certain vowel substitutions seem
an unrounded vowel. to be articulatory simplifications that could also oc-
cur in normal development, other errors appear to be
Central Vowels idiosyncratic. Assessment of vowel qualities should be
a portion of every diagnostic protocol. This can easily
[] a central vowel, rounded, tense with be achieved with any formal articulation test by tran-
r-coloring. Rounding may vary, how- scribing the entire word rather than just the sound
ever, from speaker to speaker. [] is a being tested.
stressed vowel. It is typically acousti-

ch02.indd 19 7/12/2007 8:04:42 AM


20 CHAPTER 2

Diphthongs. As previously defined, a diph- and [ɑ], which can be heard in dear [d],
thong is a vowel sound that demonstrates ar- bear [bε], or farm [fɑm], are far more prev-
ticulatory movement during its production. Its alent than [i] or [u]. Lowe (1994) refers to
initial portion, the onglide, is acoustically more the diphthongs which are paired with [] as
prominent and usually longer than the off- rhotic diphthongs. Centering diphthongs are
glide. Common diphthongs in General Ameri- also seen transcribed with [r]. Thus, dear is tran-
can English are rising diphthongs. This means scribed as [dr], bear as [bεr], or farm as [farm].
that in producing these diphthongs, essential There are several different ways to char-
portions of the tongue move from a lower acterize diphthongs as single phonemic units
onglide to a higher offglide position; thus, rel- in contrast to two separate vowels. Some tran-
ative to the palate, the tongue moves in a ris- scribers use a bar or bow either above or be-
ing motion. This can be demonstrated on the low the two vowel symbols—[e], [e
], or [e],
vowel quadrilateral as well (see Figure 2.2). for example. The author has chosen to use the
Certain diphthongs are referred to as cen- transcription that elevates the offglide portion
tering diphthongs. In this case, the offglide, of the diphthong to indicate its typically lesser
or less prominent element of the diphthong, is intensity and length.
a central vowel. In British English, and in some Discrepancies may be noted between the
dialects of General American English, this may transcriptions of diphthongs offered in this
be a schwa vowel [ə]. Thus, fear may be pro- text and the ones in other books. Because pho-
nounced as [fə] or far as [fɑə]. More common netic transcription is purely descriptive, never
in General American English is the use of the prescriptive, any transcription will, of course,
central vowel with r-coloring [] as the off- vary according to the actual pronunciation.
glide. Thus, fear is often pronounced as [f], See Shriberg and Kent (2003) for a thorough
far as [fɑ], and bear as [bε] (Ball and Rahilly, discussion of the various ways diphthongs
1999; Heffner, 1975). Theoretically, any vowel have been transcribed.
may be combined with [ə] or [] to form a cen-
tering diphthong; however, in General Ameri- [e] a nonphonemic diphthong
can English certain centering diphthongs are It is nonphonemic in the sense that the
more common than others. Thus, [], [ε], meaning would not change in a particu-
lar word if the vowel were to be pro-
Front Central Back nounced as a monophthong [e] versus
a diphthong [e]. Therefore, the mean-
High High ing would not change if just the onglide
was realized. Words pronounced [bek]
or [bek], for example, would be recog-
Mid Mid
nized as the same word.
[oυ] a nonphonemic diphthong
[a] a phonemic diphthong
Low Low It is phonemic in the sense that the
meaning would change in a particular
word if only the vowel onglide was pro-
Figure 2.2 Vowel Quadrilateral with Rising duced. Therefore, the vowel was real-
Diphthongs ized as a monophthong. A realization of

ch02.indd 20 7/12/2007 8:04:43 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 21

[a] instead of [a] will change the mean- meaning-differentiating sound feature
ing in General American English as the of English.
words sod [sad] versus sighed [sad] [aυ] a phonemic diphthong
demonstrate.
Oppositions such as [mas], moss, versus
[ɔ] a phonemic diphthong [maυs], mouse, exemplify its phonemic
The opposition [ɔ], jaw, versus [ɔ], value.
joy, exemplifies its phonemic value as a

CLINICAL APPLICATION
Analyzing the Vowel System of a Child
Occasionally, the vowel system of a client may be re- George was being seen in the clinic for his phono-
stricted or show deviant patterns. In this case, a more logical disorder. He was a gregarious child who loved
in-depth analysis of the vowels produced may be to talk and would try to engage anyone in conver-
necessary. Vowel systems can be analyzed using the sation who would listen. The only problem was that
vowel quadrilateral and knowledge of the diphthongs George was almost unintelligible. This made dialogue
as guiding principles. Front, back, and central vowels difficult, possibly more so for those who would pa-
as well as diphthongs can be checked in relationship tiently and diligently try to understand his continuing
to their accuracy and their occurrence in the appropri- attempts to interact.
ate contexts. George, age 5;3, is an example of a child In addition to his many consonant problems, the
with a deviant vowel system. following vowel deviations were noted:

VOWEL ERRORS

Norm Actual Word



Production Production Examples Transcriptions

[e] → [ε] grapes [reps] → [dε]


table [tebl ] → [tεboυ]
[i] → [] feet [fit] → [f]
teeth [tiθ] → [t]
three [θri] → [d]

[ε] → [] bed [bεd] → [bt]


feather [fεð] → [fvə]
[u] correct [u] shoe [ ʃu] → [tu]
spoon [spun] → [mun]
[υ] correct [υ] book [bυk] → [bυ]
[oυ] correct [oυ] stove [stoυv] → [doυ]
nose [noυz] → [noυ]
[ɑ] correct [ɑ] mop [mɑp] → [mɑ]
blocks [blɑks] → [bɑ]

ch02.indd 21 7/12/2007 8:04:43 AM


22 CHAPTER 2

George’s productions of the back vowels [u], [υ], latory position of the vowel substitution for [e] is real-
[oυ], and [ɑ] are on target. The front vowels do show ized lower as [ε]. This tendency to lower vowels is also
a deviant pattern, however. Not only is the diphthong noted in the other productions with front vowels, in
[e] produced as a monophthong, but also the articu- which [i] becomes [] and [ε] becomes [].

American English Consonants structures actively involved in the articulation


of the consonants of General American En-
Four phonetic categories are used to transcribe
glish and the resulting phonetic descriptors
consonants: (1) organ of articulation, (2) place
are contained in Table 2.3. Figure 2.3 displays
of articulation, (3) manner of articulation, and
the divisions of the tongue.
(4) voicing features. Most textbooks state that
only place, manner, and voicing are used to
characterize individual consonants (Edwards, Place of Articulation. The place of articula-
2003; Lowe, 1994; Shriberg and Kent, 2003). tion denotes the area within the vocal tract
However, they nevertheless often include the that remains motionless during consonant ar-
organ of articulation. For example, the term ticulation, that is, the passive articulator; it is
lingual as in lingua-dental or lingua-palatal, the part that the organ of articulation as ac-
designates the active organ of articulation. tive articulator approaches or contacts directly
However, when contrasting the lingua-dental (Crystal, 1987). The upper lip and teeth, the
sounds [θ] and [ð] to the lingua-palatal sounds palate, and the velum are the main places of ar-
[ ʃ ] and [ ], it becomes clear that different por- ticulation when describing the consonants of
tions of the tongue are actively involved in General American English. The passive struc-
the articulation. The term lingual alone does tures of articulation and their resulting pho-
not specify these differences. This text empha- netic descriptors are contained in Table 2.4.
sizes the detailed knowledge of production Figure 2.4 displays the structures of the oral
features for specific therapy goals. By adding cavity as organs and places of articulation.
a category specifically designating the active
articulator, the organ of articulation, valu- Manner of Articulation. The manner of ar-
able clarification of consonant articulation is ticulation refers to the type of constriction
achieved. the organ and place of articulation produce
for the realization of a particular consonant.
Organ of Articulation. Consonants are sounds There are various manners of articulation,
characterized by the articulators creating a ranging from complete closure for the produc-
partial or total obstruction of the expiratory tion of stop-plosives to a very limited constric-
airstream. There are active and passive articu- tion of the vocal tract for the production of
lators. Active articulators, the so-called organs glides. The following manners of articulation
of articulation, are the parts within the vocal are used to account phonetically for the con-
tract that actually move to achieve the articu- sonants of General American English.
latory result (Crystal, 1987). In describing the
consonants of General American English, we Stop-Plosives. During the production of stop-
are referring specifically to the movements of plosives, complete occlusion is secured at spe-
the lower lip and portions of the tongue. The cific points in the vocal tract. Simultaneously,

ch02.indd 22 7/12/2007 8:04:43 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 23

Table 2.3 Phonetic Description: Organ of Articulation

Organ of Articulation Phonetic Descriptor Examples


Lower lip Labial [p], [b], [m], [f ], [v], [w], []

Tip of tongue Apical [s], [z], [θ], [ð], [r],1 [l]

Lateral rims of tongue2 Coronal [t], [d], [n], [ ʃ ], [ ]

Surface of tongue Dorsum


anterior portion predorsal [s], [z]
central portion mediodorsal [ j ], [r]
posterior portion postdorsal [k], [g], [ŋ]

1. The transcription used officially by the International Phonetic Association for the American English
“r” is [ɹ]. See explanation under rhotics.
2. The term coronal designates the apex and the lateral rims of the tongue. While the term blade of
the tongue also includes its apex, it characterizes an extension into predorsal areas as well. In order to
delineate the action of the organ of articulation as closely as possible, the terms coronal and predorsal
will be used instead of blade.

the velum is raised so that no air can escape Fricatives. Fricatives result when organ and
through the nose. The expiratory air pressure place of articulation approximate each other
builds up naturally behind this closure (stop); so closely that the escaping expiratory air-
compression results, which is then suddenly stream causes an audible friction. As with
released (plosive). Examples of stop-plosives the stops, the velum is raised for all fricative
are [p] and [b]. sounds. Two examples of fricatives are [f ] and
[v]. Some fricatives, referred to as sibilants,
have a sharper sound than others due to the
presence of high-frequency components. In
General American English [s], [z], [ ʃ ], and [ ]
medio- post-
corona dorsum dorsum belong to the sibilants.

Nasals. These consonants are produced with


apex epiglottis
the velum lowered so that the air can pass
predorsum dorsum
freely through the nasal cavity. However, there
is complete occlusion within the oral cav-
ity between organ and place of articulation.
medio- post-
dorsum dorsum These sounds have been called nasal stops due
to the closure in the oral cavity and the ensu-
ing free air passage through the nasal cavity
(Ball and Rahilly, 1999). [m], [n], and [ŋ] are
the nasal speech sounds of General American
Figure 2.3 Divisions of the Tongue English.

ch02.indd 23 7/12/2007 8:04:43 AM


24 CHAPTER 2

Table 2.4 Phonetic Description: Place of Articulation

Place of Articulation Phonetic Descriptor Examples


Upper lip Labial [p], [b], [m], [w], []

Upper teeth Dental [f], [v], [θ], [ð]

Alveolar ridge Alveolar [t], [d], [n], [s], [z], [l]

Surface of hard palate Palatal


anterior portion prepalatal [ ʃ ], [ ],1 [r]
central portion mediopalatal [ j ], [r]
posterior portion postpalatal (does not normally occur in General American English)

Soft palate Velar [k], [g], [ŋ]

1. [ ʃ ] and [ ] are also referred to as postalveolar sounds, indicating a place of articulation just posterior to the highest point
of the alveolar ridge. This text will include both of these places of articulation to describe [ ʃ ] and [ ].

Affricates. For affricate sounds, two phases and place of articulation. As a consequence
can be noted. First, the velum is raised as a of these articulatory conditions, expiratory
complete closure is formed between organ air pressure builds up behind the blockage
formed by the organ and place of articula-
tion, the stop phase. Second the stop is then
slowly (in comparison to the plosives) released
orally, resulting in the friction portion of the
speech sound. Affricates should not be viewed
as a stop plus fricative combination similar to
consonant blends or clusters, such as [ks], in
which the stop portion is formed by a different
organ and at a different place of articulation
than the fricative portion. Rather, affricates
are single uniform speech sounds character-
ized by a slow release of a stopping phase into
a homorganic (hom = same) friction element.
The two most prominent affricates of General
American English are [ ] and [].

Glides. For the realization of glides, the con-


striction between organ and place of articu-
lation is not as narrow as for fricatives. In
addition to this relatively wide articulatory
Figure 2.4 Structures of the Oral Cavity as posture, glides are also characterized by a glid-
Organs and Places of Articulation ing movement of the articulators from a rela-

ch02.indd 24 7/12/2007 8:04:44 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 25

Laterals. These sounds are established by a


Stop-plosives are sometimes referred to as stops
and sometimes as plosives, depending on the midline closure but lateral openings within
phase of production one wants to draw attention the oral cavity. Consequently, the expira-
to. Such a division appears at first glance rather tory airstream can pass only around one or
academic. There are situations, however, when both sides of the tongue. [ l ] is the only lat-
this distinction becomes important. For exam- eral consonant of General American English.
ple, a client has difficulties realizing a complete The laterals together with the rhotics are col-
occlusion of the lips. This can occur in cases of
lectively referred to as liquids. According to
paralysis of the facial nerve, such as in myasthe-
nia gravis (Thiele, 1980). Such a client has trouble
the classification system of the International
with the stop portion of the production. Other Phonetic Alphabet [l] is considered a lateral
clients—for example, children with developmen- approximant.
tal verbal dyspraxia—have difficulties with rapid
movement patterns of speech. These children can
realize the static articulatory postures of the oc- Rhotics. The phonetic characteristics of the
clusion, but they cannot necessarily release it sud- rhotics are especially difficult to describe.
denly enough (Velleman and Strand, 1994). They, First, there are at least two types of rhotic pro-
therefore, have problems with the plosive phase ductions: retroflexed and bunched (Shriberg
of the realization and need to be treated quite and Kent, 2003). Second, the actual forming
differently.
of rhotics is highly context dependent. Thus,
the production easily changes depending on
the features of the surrounding sounds (Kant-
tively constricted into a more open position. ner and West, 1960). In addition, the posi-
The sounds [w] and [j] are considered glides. tioning of the tongue for individual speakers
According to the classification of the Interna- is highly variable (Shriberg and Kent, 2003).
tional Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), [w] and [j] are Generally, the retroflexed rhotics are produced
considered approximants. Approximants are with the tongue tip in a retroflexed position
consonants in which there is a much wider (retro = back, flex = turn). The bunched rhot-
passage of air resulting in a smooth (as op- ics, on the other hand, show an elevation of
posed to turbulent) airflow for these voiced the whole corpus of the tongue toward the
sounds (Ball and Rahilly, 1999). palate. Perhaps a better classification for [r]
is the term approximant, which is used within
the International Phonetic Alphabet. In this
According to the symbols used by the Interna- case, [r] is a central approximant. According
tional Phonetic Association (IPA), the American to the International Phonetic Alphabet, there
English rhotics are officially transcribed as [ɹ], an are two symbols used for the central rhotic ap-
upside down r, while the retroflexed is character-
proximants. The [ɹ] is a postalveolar approx-
ized by [ ], an upside-down r with a retroflexed
diacritic. According to the IPA, the [r] symbol is imant in which the tongue tip is raised and
officially reserved for the alveolar trilled “r” sound, points directly upward toward the rear of the
which can be heard in Spanish, for example. Be- alveolar ridge. The [ ] is a retroflex produc-
cause trilled “r” sounds do not exist in General tion characterized by the tongue tip elevated
American English, and in order not to complicate and bent backward in a more retroflexed
matters unnecessarily, it is customary to use the position. Officially, there is no IPA symbol for
[r] symbol for both the bunched and the retro-
the bunched r-production (Ball and Rahilly,
flexed “r” sounds.
1999). Table 2.5 contains the various manners

ch02.indd 25 7/12/2007 8:04:45 AM


26 CHAPTER 2

Table 2.5 Phonetic Description: Manner of Articulation

Manner of Articulation Phonetic Descriptor Examples

Complete blockage Stop-plosive [p], [b], [t], [d], [k], [g]

Partial blockage Fricative [f ], [v], [s], [z], [ ʃ ], [ ], [θ], [ð]

Nasal emission Nasal [m], [n], [ŋ]

Release of stop portion to a Affricate [tʃ ], [d ]


homorganic fricative portion

Gliding motion from a more closed Glide [w], [], [j]


to a more open position

Lateral airflow Lateral [l]

Retroflex blade or bunched dorsum Rhotic [r]

of articulation with examples of the conso-


nants of General American English. CLINICAL APPLICATION
When Organ, Place, Manner,
Voicing. Voicing is the term used to denote and Voicing Are Not Enough
the presence or absence of simultaneous vi-
bration of the vocal cords resulting in voiced In analyzing the articulatory requisites for the realiza-
or voiceless consonants. The voiced and voice- tion of [ ʃ ], we find that it can be described—according
less consonants of General American English to voicing, articulatory organ, place, and manner—as
are summarized in Table 2.6. a voiceless coronal-prepalatal fricative. Although that
is a generally satisfactory phonetic description, an-
Far more precision may often be neces-
other production characteristic is lip rounding. De-
sary to describe how specific consonants are
scribing such an additional feature becomes necessary
produced. However, this framework of organ because some children with “sh” problems do not re-
of articulation, place of articulation, manner alize the rounding. In fact, the resulting aberrant pro-
of articulation, and voicing provides a fairly duction may be due entirely to the absence of this
accurate description of General American En- lip-rounding feature.
glish consonants.

Table 2.6 Phonetic Description: Voicing

Voicing Phonetic Descriptor Examples

With vocal fold vibration Voiced [b], [d], [g], [m], [n], [ŋ], [v], [z], [ ], [ð], [w], [ j ], [l], [r]

Without vocal fold vibration Voiceless [p], [t], [k], [f], [s], [ ʃ ], [θ], [], [h]

ch02.indd 26 7/12/2007 8:04:45 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 27

The following phonetic descriptions clas- The [θ] and [ð] are typically produced
sify the consonants of General American En- with either the tongue tip resting be-
glish according to the parameters of voicing, hind the upper incisors (i.e., apico-
organ, place, and manner.1 dental) or with the tongue tip between
the upper and lower incisors (i.e.,
[p] voiceless bilabial stop-plosive interdental).
(Because both organ and place of articu- [ð] voiced apico-dental or interdental
lation are the lower and upper lips, fricative
respectively, one should actually say [m] voiced bilabial nasal
labio-labial. However, the term bilabial
is usually preferred.) [n] voiced coronal-alveolar nasal

[b] voiced bilabial stop-plosive [ŋ] voiced postdorsal-velar nasal


[t] voiceless coronal-alveolar stop-plosive [w] voiced labial-velar glide or approximant
[d] voiced coronal-alveolar stop-plosive [] voiceless labial-velar fricative (IPA,
1996)
[k] voiceless postdorsal-velar stop-plosive
[j] voiced mediodorsal-mediopalatal glide
[] voiced postdorsal-velar stop-plosive
or approximant
[f ] voiceless labio-dental fricative
[l] voiced apico-alveolar lateral or lateral
[v] voiced labio-dental fricative approximant
[s] voiceless apico-alveolar or predorsal- [r] voiced mediodorsal-mediopalatal rhotic
alveolar fricative approximant (bunched) or voiced
[s] (and [z]) can be produced in one of apico-prepalatal rhotic approximant
two ways: with the tongue tip up (i.e., (retroflexed), officially [ɹ]
as apico-alveolar fricative [sibilant]) or Here, the term apico refers to the under-
with the tongue tip resting behind the side of the apex of the tongue.
lower incisors (i.e., predorsal-alveolar
fricative [sibilant]). [h] voiceless unlocalized open consonant
that is, an aspirate
[z] voiced apico-alveolar or predorsal-
alveolar fricative Although this sound is sometimes clas-
sified as a laryngeal or glottal fricative,
[ʃ] voiceless coronal-prepalatal or coronal- in General American English, there is
postalveolar fricative with lip rounding normally no constriction at the laryn-
[ ] voiced coronal-prepalatal or coronal- geal, pharyngeal, or oral levels. See
postalveolar fricative with lip rounding Heffner (1975) for a discussion of the
[θ] voiceless apico-dental or interdental [h] production in General American
fricative English.
[ ] voiceless coronal-alveolar stop portion
followed by a voiceless coronal-prepala-
1. The organ, place, manner, and voicing features are tal fricative portion
based on the phonetic descriptions provided by Bron-
stein (1960) and Kantner and West (1960). These features
[] voiced coronal-alveolar stop portion
are seen as descriptive and may, therefore, vary somewhat followed by a voiced coronal-prepalatal
from speaker to speaker. fricative portion

ch02.indd 27 7/12/2007 8:04:46 AM


28 CHAPTER 2

CLINICAL APPLICATION
Rhotic Errors versus Central Vowels with R-Coloring
Children with “r” problems, thus, rhotic consonant in Latoria’s speech from the Word Articulation Subtest
difficulties, often produce the central vowels with of the Test of Language Development, Primary, Sec-
r- coloring ([] and []) in error as well. However, that ond edition (Newcomer and Hammill, 1988).
is not always the case. Note the following patterns seen

Norm Actual Word



Production Production Example Transcriptions

Rhotics
[tr] → [tw] tree [tri] → [twi]
[br] → [bw] bridge [br] → [bw ]
[r] → [w] ring [rŋ] → [wŋ]
[br] → [bw] zebra [zibrə] → [zibwə]
[r] → [w] garage [ərɑ ] → [əwɑ]
[θr] → [θw] thread [θrεd] → [θwεd]
[tr] → [tw] treasure [trε ] → [twε ]
Central Vowels with R-Coloring
[] correct [] feather [fεð] → [fεd]
[] correct [] soldier [soυld ] → [soυ ]
[z] correct [z] scissors [szz] → [szz]
[] correct [] birthday [bθde] → [bde]

On the one hand, Latoria has a [w] for [r] sub- the other, she can produce the central vowels with
stitution ([r] → [w]) for the rhotic consonant [r]. On r-coloring accurately.

SOUNDS IN CONTEXT: lips, whereas there is lip rounding in Sue. This


difference is due to the influence of the follow-
COARTICULATION AND ASSIMILATION
ing vowel articulations: [i], a vowel with lip
Until now, this textbook has discussed artic- spreading, facilitates this feature in the [s] pro-
ulatory characteristics of General American duction in see, whereas the lip rounding of [u]
English speech sounds as discrete units. How- influences the production of [s] in Sue. These
ever, the articulators do not move from sound types of modifications are grouped together
to sound in a series of separate steps. Speech under the term coarticulation. Coarticulation
consists of highly variable and overlapping describes the concept that the articulators are
motor movements. Sounds within a given continually moving into position for other
phonetic context influence one another. For segments over a stretch of speech (Fletcher,
example, if the [s] production in see is con- 1992). The result of coarticulation is referred
trasted to the one in Sue, it can be seen that [s] to as assimilation. The term assimilation re-
in see is produced with some spreading of the fers to adaptive articulatory changes by which

ch02.indd 28 7/12/2007 8:04:46 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 29

one speech sound becomes similar, sometimes “telephone” [tεləfoυn] → [tεdəfoυn]


identical, to a neighboring sound segment. Manner of articulation is impacted when the
Such a change may affect one, several, or all [l] is changed from a lateral to a stop-plosive,
of a sound’s phonetic constituents; that is, a similar to the [t] at the beginning of the word.
sound may change its organ, place, manner,
2. Assimilations can be either progressive or
and/or voicing properties under the articula-
regressive. In progressive assimilation, a sound
tory influence of another sound. Assimilation
segment influences a following sound. This
processes are perfectly natural consequences
is also referred to as perseverative assimilation
of normal speech production and are by no
(Crystal, 1987; Ladefoged, 2006). The previ-
means restricted to developing speech in
ously noted contact assimilations for jump-
young children. Because the two segments be-
ing and skunk and the remote assimilation for
come more alike, assimilatory processes are
telephone are examples of progressive assimi-
also referred to as harmony processes.
lation. A previously articulated sound influ-
There are different types and degrees of as-
enced a following sound.
similatory processes. In regard to the different
In regressive assimilation, a sound seg-
types of assimilatory processes, the following
ment influences a preceding sound. If “is she”
should be noted:
[z ʃi] is pronounced [ ʃi], changing [s] into
1. Assimilatory processes modifying directly [ ], regressive assimilation is noted. Regressive
adjacent sounds are called contact (or contigu- assimilations are also known as anticipatory as-
ous) assimilations. If at least one other segment similations (Crystal, 1987; Ladefoged, 2006).
separates the sounds in question, especially The following are examples of progressive
when the two sounds are in two different syl- and regressive assimilation processes:
lables, one speaks of remote (or noncontiguous)
assimilation (Heffner, 1975). Progressive
The following assimilation processes were “ice cream” [askrim] → [astrim]
noted in the results of children’s articulation
Organ and place of articulation for [s] influ-
tests:
ence the following stop-plosive, changing it
from a postdorsal-velar to a coronal-alveolar
Contact
stop-plosive production: This is progressive
“jumping” [mpn] → [mbn] contact assimilation.
The voiced [m] impacts the normally voice- “television” [tεləv ən] → [tεdəv ən]
less [p].
Manner of articulation is impacted when
“skunk” [skŋk] → [stŋk] the stop-plosive [t] impacts the following [l],
The organ and place of articulation for [s] in- changing it from a lateral to a stop-plosive:
fluence the stop-plosive, changing it from a This is progressive remote assimilation.
postdorsal-velar to a coronal-alveolar.
Regressive
Remote “pumpkin” [pmkn] → [pŋkn]
“yellow” [jεloυ] → [lεloυ] Organ and place of articulation of [k] influ-
Organ, place, and manner of articulation are ence [m], which is changed from the bilabial
impacted when the [j] at the beginning of the to the postdorsal-velar nasal [ŋ]: This is regres-
word becomes identical to the following [l]. sive contact assimilation.

ch02.indd 29 7/12/2007 8:04:46 AM


30 CHAPTER 2

“bathtub” [bθtb] → [θθtb] The following are examples of total and


Organ, place, and manner of articulation are partial assimilation processes:
impacted as [θ] influences the previous segment
[b]: This is regressive remote assimilation. Total “window” [wndoυ] → [wnoυ]
“Pontiac” [pɑntik] → [pɑnik]
In regard to the different degrees of assim- Partial “handkerchief” [hnkf ] → [hŋkf ]
ilatory influence, one distinguishes between
phonemic assimilation and phonetic simili- The term coalescence is used when two
tude (Ball and Rahilly, 1999). If an altered seg- neighboring segments are merged into a new
ment is perceived to be a different phoneme and different segment. An example of coales-
altogether, this is termed phonemic assimilation. cence would be the realization of sandwich
Phonetic similitude occurs when the change in [snw ] as [sm ]. The bilabial features for
the segment is such that it is still perceived by the articulation of [w] have impacted the orig-
speakers of a language as nothing more than a inal coronal-alveolar nasal (regressive assim-
variation or allophone of the original segment. ilation), which now is changed to a bilabial
A phonemic assimilation could be exemplified nasal [m].
by the change in ten girls [tεn lz] to [tεŋ lz], Children at different stages of their speech-
the [n] changing to [ŋ] due to the influence of language development tend to utilize assimi-
the following postdorsal-velar stop-plosive []. lation processes in systematic ways. This is
An example of a phonetic similitude would be of obvious interest to cli-
the lip rounding of [s] in soup [swup] as the [s] nicians whose task is to Typical assimilation
is influenced by the lip rounding of the follow- separate normal from im- processes and the
ages at which these
ing [u]. paired phonological de- processes occur in
Assimilation processes can also be total velopment. In normally children are discussed
or partial. Total assimilation occurs when the developing children and in Chapter 5.
changed segment and the source of the influ- those with disordered
ence become identical. Partial assimilation ex- phonology, syllable structure can impact their
ists when the changed segment is close to, but production possibilities. This will be discussed
not identical with, the source segment. in the next section.

CLINICAL APPLICATION
Assimilation Processes and Articulation Testing
Assimilatory or harmony processes often occur during tively impacted. The following assimilation processes
an articulation test. It is important to recognize these have been frequently observed by the author:
processes so that the test scoring will not be nega-

Word Expected Response Child’s Response Impact on Scoring

Santa [sntə] [snə] total assimilation Could be scored as an omission of [t]


sandwich [snw ] [sm ] total assimilation Could be scored as an omission of [w]
(coalescence) and an [m]/[n] substitution
presents [prεzənts] [prεzəns] total assimilation Could be scored as an omission of [t]

ch02.indd 30 7/12/2007 8:04:47 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 31

Word Expected Response Child’s Response Impact on Scoring

A less common example was observed for Danny, age 4;3:


bath [bθ] [θθ] [θ]/[b] substitution
bathtub [bθt b] [θθt b] [θ]/[b] substitution

However, Danny could produce [b] correctly in all the end of bathtub. This was an example of a regres-
other contexts. Note the correct production of [b] at sive remote assimilation.

ken and written syllable boundaries is impor-


SYLLABLE STRUCTURE tant for speech-language specialists.
If we are asked to break words down into com- This is especially critical because a dic-
ponent parts, syllables seem to be more nat- tionary of rules for the boundaries of spoken
ural than sounds. For example, speakers of syllables does not exist. Thus, two competent
unwritten languages will characteristically use speakers of a given language may syllabify the
syllable, not sound, divisions. They may even same word in different ways. Words such as
resist the notion that any further breakdown hammer and window would probably not cause
is possible (Ladefoged, 2006). Also, preschool problems. However, how should one syllabify
children use syllabification if they try to ana- telephone, as [tε lə foυn] or as [tεl ə foυn]? That
lyze a word. It is only after children are ex- is, does [l] belong to the second or to the first
posed to letters and writing that they begin to syllable? Variations in the syllabification of
understand the possibility of dividing words spoken words do indeed exist between speak-
into sounds. Thus, syllables appear to be easily ers. To understand this, a look at the syllable
recognizable units. structure might be a good way to begin.
Counting the number of syllables in a Structurally, the syllable can be divided
word is a relatively simple task. Probably all into three parts: peak, onset, and coda (Sloat,
will agree on the number of syllables in the Taylor, and Hoard, 1978). The peak is the most
word away or articulation, for example. What prominent, acoustically most intense part of
we might disagree on are the beginning and the syllable. Although vowels are clearly more
end points of the syllables in question. To ar- prevalent as syllable peaks, consonants are not
rive at a consensus, it is first necessary to differ- strictly excluded. Consonants that serve as the
entiate between written and spoken syllables. syllable peak are referred to as syllabics. A peak
If one consults a dictionary, written syl- may stand alone, as in the first syllable of the
labification rules are found. We learn that word a-way, or it can be surrounded by other
the word cutting is to be divided cut-ting. sounds, as in tan or bring.
However, differences may, and often do, ex- The onset of a syllable consists of all the
ist between written and spoken syllables. The segments prior to the peak, whereas the coda
written syllabification rules for cutting do not is made up of all the sound segments of a sylla-
reflect the way we would syllabify the word ble following its peak. The segments that com-
when speaking. The divisions [k tŋ] would pose the onset are also termed syllable releasing
be more probable during normal speech. An sounds, and those of the coda are termed syl-
awareness of existing differences between spo- lable arresting sounds. Thus, the onset of meet

ch02.indd 31 7/12/2007 8:04:47 AM


32 CHAPTER 2

[mit] is [m]; that is, [m] is the syllable releasing picture of the child’s articulatory capabilities
sound. The coda, or syllable arresting sound, of than can individual sound productions. The
meet is [t]. This applies also to consonant blends ease of syllable production can be affected by
within one syllable. The onset of scratched is at least three circumstances: (1) the number of
[skr], its peak is [], and the coda [tʃt]. Not all syllables an utterance contains, (2) the type of
syllables have onsets or codas. Both syllables syllable (open versus closed), and (3) the degree
of today [tu de] lack a coda, whereas off [ɑf] of syllable stress (stressed or unstressed) (Flem-
does not have an onset. The number of seg- ing, 1971; Kent, 1982). Generally, fewer syl-
ments that an onset or a coda may contain is lables, open syllables, and stressed syllables
regulated by rules of the language in question. usually facilitate accurate productions of spe-
General American English syllables can have cific target sounds.
one to three segments in an onset (ray, stay, The designs of most articulation tests doc-
stray) and one to four segments in a coda (sit, ument a striking lack of attention to these vari-
sits, sixth [sksθ], sixths [sksθs]) ables. Most assessment instruments focus on
The peak and coda together are referred the beginning-initial, the middle-medial, and
to as the rhyme (Carr, 1999). Therefore, in the end-final sound positions within words.
the word sun, the onset is “s” and the rhyme At first glance, it may seem as if initial could
is “un.” Syllables that do not contain codas be related to the syllable onset, medial to syl-
are called open or unchecked syllables. Ex- lable peak, and final to syllable coda. However,
amples of open, unchecked syllables are do this is not the case. For example, the word
[du], glee [li], or the first syllable of rebound [ri window may be used in an articulation test to
baυnd]. Syllables that do have codas are called assess the production of the word-medial [d]
closed or checked syllables, such as in stop sound, while the word bathtub is used to test
[stɑp] or the first syllable in window [wn]. the word-medial [θ] and [t] sounds. The elici-
The use of specific syllable structures is of- tation of the word pajamas tests [d ] medially.
ten neglected when analyzing the speech char- From these examples, it appears that medial
acteristics of children. However, they do seem indicates anything between the beginning
to play an important developmental role. A and the end of an utterance.
child’s first words consist typically of open Is there any comparability between these
or unchecked syllables, such as [bɑ] for ball “medial” positions? Let’s examine the syllable
or [m] for milk. If children start to produce structures of these three words:
closed syllables, they usually contain only
“window” [wn-doυ] target [d]
single-segment codas. Similarly, two-syllable
1st syllable stressed onset-peak-coda
words at this stage of development consist
2nd syllable unstressed onset-peak
usually of open syllables (e.g., Ingram, 1976;
Menn, 1971; Velten, 1943; Vihman, Ferguson, “bathtub” [bθ-tb] target [θ] and [t]
and Elbert, 1986). Productions such as [be bi] 1st syllable stressed onset-peak-coda
for baby or [ti pɑ] for teapot are examples. 2nd syllable unstressed onset-peak-coda

“pajamas” [pəd məz] target [d ]


Syllable Structure: Clinical Implications 1st syllable unstressed onset-peak
The syllable is also an important unit when 2nd syllable stressed onset-peak
3rd syllable unstressed onset-peak-coda
assessing and treating children with articula-
tory or phonological disorders. Sometimes, As one can see, the medial sound [d] in win-
the syllable unit can give us a more accurate dow is actually the onset of an unstressed, open

ch02.indd 32 7/12/2007 8:04:47 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 33

syllable. The preceding syllable ends with a An analysis of an articulation test accord-
coda, thus, two consonants (i.e., [n] + [d]) must ing to the syllable structure rather than the
be produced in immediate succession. The me- word unit would eliminate these problems. If
dial [θ] tested in bathtub poses a different prob- onset, peak, and coda for each syllable are ex-
lem. It represents the coda of a stressed syllable. amined, the results also become more accurate
Again, there is the complication of two conso- and, therefore, clinically more valid. Accuracy
nants in sequence, [θ] and [t]. The word-medial of any assessment process is the key to suc-
[t] now appears as a syllable onset in a closed cessful treatment. The information attained
syllable. The third example of medial [d ] in from examining sound articulation with the
pajamas exemplifies a quite different articula- syllable as a basic structural unit comple-
tory situation again. Here, a three-syllable word ments the word-based results and gives addi-
is elicited in which the medial [d ] is actually tional insight into the child’s true articulatory
an onset of a stressed open syllable. abilities.

SUMMARY

This chapter presented an overview of the tory consequences that regularly occur in the
form and function of vowels and consonants speech of individuals. Assimilatory processes
of General American English. Both vowels and were defined according to the type and degree
consonants were classified according to their of sound modification. Examples were given
articulatory production features and their lin- of assimilatory processes in children as well
guistic functions. Phonetic descriptors were as of the possible impact these processes could
given to provide the clinician with a detailed have on articulation test results. The last sec-
account of articulatory action during norm tion, on syllable structure, defined the parts
production of vowels and consonants. These of the syllable. Variations in syllable structure
features can later be contrasted to those noted do not seem to be accounted for when testing
in the impaired sound realizations of children individual sounds within most articulation
and adults with articulatory-phonological tests. However, this may be a factor that could
impairments. affect the articulatory proficiency of children
In the second portion of this chapter, co- and adults with impaired speech. An analysis
articulation, assimilation processes, and syl- of syllable structures would provide the clini-
lable structure were defined and examined. cian with additional knowledge when evaluat-
Coarticulation and resulting assimilatory ing individuals with articulatory-phonological
processes were described as normal articula- disorders.

CASE STUDY

The following sample is from Tina, age 3;8. fan [vεn] ring [wŋ]
yes [wεt] thumb [dm]
dig [dε] cat [tt] boat [bot] that [zt]
house [haυθ] bath [bt] cup [tυp] zip [wp]
knife [naf ] red [led] lamp [wmp] key [di]
duck [dυt] ship [sp] goat [dot] win [jn]

ch02.indd 33 7/12/2007 8:04:47 AM


34 CHAPTER 2

Compare the typical vowel productions to Compare the typical consonant productions
those noted in the sample according to (1) the to those noted in the sample according to
portion of the tongue that is involved in the voicing, organ, place, and manner character-
articulation (front, central, back) and (2) the istics. For example:
tongue’s position relative to the palate (high,
house [haυθ] a voiceless apico-alveolar
mid, low). For example:
(predorsal-alveolar)
dig [dε] a high-front vowel fricative is changed to
changed to a mid-front a voiceless interdental
vowel (apico-alveolar) fricative

THINK CRITICALLY

1. Some younger children have trouble produc- 4. Identify the following syllable structures ac-
ing [s] and [z]; they substitute [θ] and [ð] for cording to (a) onset, peak, and coda and
these sounds. Thus, the word Sue would be (b) closed or open syllables. For example:
pronounced [θu] and zoo as [ðu]. Both of the
win.dow → [wn.doυ]
target sounds and the substitutions are frica-
tives. Compare the two articulations and see if 1st syllable: onset-peak-coda, closed syllable
you might be able to describe to a child what 2nd syllable: onset-peak, open syllable
he or she would have to do to change the ar-
telephone
ticulation from [θ] and [ð] to [s] to [z].
wagon
2. Children often have trouble with the lip
shovel
rounding associated with the sh-sounds
banana
([ ʃ ] and [ ]). Which type of vowel contexts
pajamas
would promote lip rounding? Can you find
five words that you could use to assist the lip 5. You are testing [k] sounds in the initial, me-
rounding of [ ʃ ] or [ ]? dial, and final positions with a child who is
3. Identify the following assimilation processes 4 years old with a [t] for [k] substitution. You
according to the following parameters: con- would like to keep the syllable structure and
tact versus remote, progressive versus re- the stress consistent for all the words used.
gressive, phonemic assimilation, phonetic Therefore, all words should be two syllables in
similitude, or coalescence. length, stress should be on the same syllable,
and syllable structures should be comparable.
news [nuz] however newspaper [nuspeIp]
Find six words that could be used for a 4-
panty [pnti] → [pni]
year-old child that would test [k] under these
did you [dd ju] → [dd u]
conditions.
incubate [nkjubeIt] → [ŋkjubeIt]
misuse [msjuz] → [mʃuz]

TEST YOURSELF

1. Vowels are defined as c. having a relatively unimpeded airstream


a. under normal circumstances having no from the vocal folds to the lips
simultaneous vocal fold vibration d. having relatively less acoustic intensity
b. having articulatory constriction along the 2. Which consonants are considered to be
sagittal midline of the vocal tract sonorant consonants?

ch02.indd 34 7/12/2007 8:04:48 AM


ARTICULATORY PHONETICS 35

a. fricatives and affricates b. progressive phonemic assimilation


b. stop-plosives c. regressive phonetic similitude
c. all voiced consonants d. coalescence
d. nasals, liquids, and glides 7. A young child says [nɔni] for noisy. This is
3. The vowel [i] is described phonetically as a which type of assimilation process?
a. high-front vowel that is unrounded and a. progressive contact phonemic assimilation
lax b. regressive contact phonemic assimilation
b. mid-front vowel that is unrounded and c. progressive remote phonemic assimilation
tense d. progressive remote phonetic similitude
c. high-front vowel that is unrounded and 8. Which one of the following words has an
tense unchecked syllable structure?
d. high-back vowel that is unrounded and a. cupcake
tense b. tomato
4. The consonant [l] is described phonetically as c. jumping
a. voiced apico-alveolar lateral approximant d. bathtub
b. voiced coronal-alveolar glide 9. What is the rhyme of “reached”?
c. voiced predorsal-alveolar lateral a. [i]
approximant b. [itʃt]
d. none of the above c. [itʃ]
5. Sibilants are characterized by the presence of d. none of the above
high-frequency components. Which one of 10. If you were testing [s] in the medial position,
the following is not a sibilant? which one of the following words would
a. [θ] have the same syllable and stress structure as
b. [s] “cassette”?
c. [z] a. message
d. [ ʃ ] b. receipt
6. A very young child says [ɑ] for dog. This is c. basic
which type of assimilation process? d. Lassie
a. regressive phonemic assimilation

WEBSITES

www.uiowa.edu/~acadtech/phonetics/about.html though the tongue as organ of articulation is not


mentioned. It does give some basic definitions and
This website provides an animated articulatory dia-
examples of manners of articulation and defines
gram of each consonant and vowel as well as a de-
vowels according to tongue height, front–back di-
scription of how the sound is produced. It seems
mensions, and lip rounding. Nasal vowels and the
to be very user-friendly. Some of the terminology
concept of tense versus lax are also a portion of this
is a bit different from that used in this text. For ex-
webpage. Several links are provided, for example,
ample, the term lingua-, as organ of articulation,
to the International Phonetic Alphabet. Other links
is used for all tongue placements and the terms
are humorous and the webpage is worded in a light
tongue blade and tongue back are descriptors for
style.
what has been referenced here as pre-, medio-, and
postdorsal. en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Vowel and
en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Consonant
www.everything2.com/index.pl?node_id=441666
These two websites give basic definitions of the
This website gives some basic definitions of the vowel and consonant concepts as well as many
various articulators for consonant production, al- links to other webpages that are both informative

ch02.indd 35 7/12/2007 8:04:49 AM


36 CHAPTER 2

and detailed. These are good reference sources for University), deals with syllable structure. Basic def-
information. initions are given and several examples are pro-
vided. Although the website gives information
cla.calpoly.edu/~jrubba/phon/syllables.html
beyond what this chapter covers, the examples on
This website, developed by Dr. Johanna Rubba syllable structure will be helpful.
(English Department, Linguistics, Cal Poly State

FURTHER READINGS

Ashby, P. (2005). Speech sounds. London: Rout- Ladefoged, P. (2005). Vowels and consonants (2nd
ledge. ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Davenport, M., & Hannahs, J. (2006). Introducing Yavaş, M. (2005). Applied English phonology. Malden,
phonetics and phonology. London: Arnold. MA: Blackwell.
Garn-Nunn, P., & Lynn, J. (2004). Calvert’s descrip-
tive phonetics (3rd ed.). New York: Thieme.

ch02.indd 36 7/12/2007 8:04:49 AM

You might also like