You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

ISSN: 1380-3395 (Print) 1744-411X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

Sensitivity of a Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive


Composite (PACC) to amyloid β load in preclinical
Alzheimer’s disease

Lisa Bransby, Yen Ying Lim, David Ames, Christopher Fowler, Joanne
Roberston, Karra Harrington, Peter J. Snyder, Victor L. Villemagne, Olivier
Salvado, Colin L. Masters & Paul Maruff for the AIBL Research Group

To cite this article: Lisa Bransby, Yen Ying Lim, David Ames, Christopher Fowler, Joanne
Roberston, Karra Harrington, Peter J. Snyder, Victor L. Villemagne, Olivier Salvado, Colin L.
Masters & Paul Maruff for the AIBL Research Group (2019): Sensitivity of a Preclinical Alzheimer’s
Cognitive Composite (PACC) to amyloid β load in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease, Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2019.1593949

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2019.1593949

Published online: 29 Mar 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ncen20
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2019.1593949

Sensitivity of a Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite (PACC) to amyloid β


load in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease
Lisa Bransbya, Yen Ying Lima, David Amesb,c, Christopher Fowlera, Joanne Roberstona, Karra Harringtona,d,
Peter J. Snydere, Victor L. Villemagnea,f,g, Olivier Salvadoh, Colin L. Mastersa and Paul Maruff for the AIBL
Research Groupa,i
a
The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia; bNational Ageing Research
Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia; cAcademic Unit for Psychiatry of Old Age, St. Vincent’s Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne,
VIC, Australia; dCooperative Research Centre for Mental Health, Parkville, Australia; eRyan Institute for Neuroscience, University of Rhode
Island, Kingston, RI, USA; fDepartment of Nuclear Medicine and Centre for PET, Austin Health, Melbourne, VIC, Australia; gDepartment of
Medicine, Austin Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia; hCommonwealth Scientific Industrial Research
Organization (CSIRO) Preventative Health National Research Flagship, Australian e-Health Research Centre-BiaMedIA, Brisbane, QLD,
Australia; iCogState Ltd., Melbourne, VIC, Australia

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Introduction: Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by amyloid-related cognitive Received 7 April 2018
decline. Reduction in this decline is used to determine the efficacy of drug therapies designed to Accepted 5 March 2019
forestall the disease in preclinical AD clinical trials, measured by a Preclinical Alzheimer’s KEYWORDS
Cognitive Composite (PACC). Most studies estimate rates of cognitive change by comparing Amyloid β; cognitive decline;
cognitively normal (CN) older adults with abnormally high beta-amyloid (Aβ+) to those with Mini-Mental State
low levels (Aβ–). However, participants of preclinical AD clinical trials must be Aβ+ for entry. Examination; Preclinical
Therefore, we estimated the effect of very high amyloid (Aβ++) and Aβ+ on cognitive change Alzheimer’s Cognitive
over three years measured by different versions of the PACC in individuals with preclinical AD. Composite; preclinical
Method: CN older adults underwent Aβ neuroimaging and neuropsychological assessments over Alzheimer’s disease
three years as part of the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study. Three
cognitive composite scores were computed: the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study
(ADCS)–PACC, the ADCS–PACC with no Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and the z-scores
of Attention, Verbal Fluency and Episodic Memory for Nondemented Older Adults (ZAVEN)
composite.
Results: Compared to the Aβ++ group, the Aβ+ group showed a slower rate of cognitive decline
with the largest magnitude of difference reflected by the ADCS–PACC (d = 0.85). The ADCS–PACC
excluding the MMSE and the ZAVEN also reflected a moderate to large magnitude of difference
between groups (d = 0.62, d = 0.72, respectively).
Conclusions: When all individuals have abnormal Aβ, the level of Aβ at baseline is associated
with the rate of subsequent decline. The ADCS–PACC was the most sensitive composite score in
showing that lower Aβ is associated with a slower rate of cognitive decline; however, there are
limitations to the use of the MMSE. These results provide a benchmark of comparison for
preclinical AD clinical trials aiming to slow cognitive deterioration.

There is now agreement that high levels of beta-amyloid executive function, when followed over a period of 12–-
(Aβ+), detected using cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) sampling 54 months (Baker et al., 2017). The consistency with which
or positron emission tomography (PET) neuroimaging, in these relationships have been observed across prospective
cognitively normal (CN) older adults indicate that the observational studies has provided the theoretical founda-
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) process has begun (Jack et al., tion for secondary prevention clinical trials of Aβ-reducing
2018; Sperling et al., 2011). A previous study has shown that drugs, where it has been proposed that preventing the
it can take an average of 12 years for an individual with low accumulation of Aβ in individuals without clinical symp-
levels of Aβ (Aβ–) to become Aβ+, and another 19.2 years toms of dementia will delay or even forestall disease pro-
to reach a diagnosis of AD (Villemagne et al., 2013). There gression in CN adults (Sperling, Jack, & Aisen, 2011;
is increasing evidence that, compared to Aβ– older adults, Sperling, Mormino, & Johnson, 2014; Sperling et al., 2014).
those who are Aβ+ have showed subtle but progressive Clinical trials of preclinical AD now use neuropsy-
cognitive decline, particularly in episodic memory and chological measures to detect underlying biological

CONTACT Lisa Bransby lisa.bransby@florey.edu.au 30 Royal Parade, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia
© 2019 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 L. BRANSBY ET AL.

mechanisms as their primary endpoint (Sperling et al., greater sensitivity to Aβ+ related cognitive decline
2014). As such, the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative than composite scores that include the MMSE (Lim
Study–Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite et al., 2016).
(ADCS–PACC) has been developed to measure the The aim of this study was to understand whether
effects of the anti-Aβ medication solanezumab in the lower levels of Aβ burden are likely to translate to
Anti-Amyloid Treatment in Asymptomatic slower cognitive decline in Aβ+ CN older adults over
Alzheimer’s Disease (A4) trial (Donohue et al., 2014; 36 months, a similar time period to that of a clinical
Sperling et al., 2014). In fact, data on the extent to trial. This was achieved by estimating differences in the
which cognition declined in Aβ+ adults compared to rate of cognitive change over 36 months between cog-
Aβ– adults provided the basis for estimation of statis- nitively normal older adults with abnormally high
tical power in the A4 study where it was assumed that levels of Aβ (Aβ+) and those with very high levels of
the medication would lead to a 30% slowing of dete- abnormal Aβ (Aβ++). The first hypothesis was that
riorating performance on the ADCS–PACC (Sperling compared to Aβ++, Aβ+ would be associated with
et al., 2014). slower rates of decline in performance on composite
One potential limitation with anticipating the nature measures of episodic memory and executive function.
and magnitude of effects of anti-Aβ drugs from models The second aim of this study was to determine whether
comparing change in cognition between Aβ+ and Aβ– differences in Aβ+ related cognitive change between
groups is that, by definition, all individuals in trials of groups manifest differently using composite measures
preclinical AD must be Aβ+. A more accurate under- of episodic memory and executive function that did or
standing of the effects of anti-Aβ medication might be did not include the MMSE. The second hypothesis was
gained from modeling the effects of variation in Aβ that the exclusion of the MMSE would improve the
levels on cognitive decline in individuals whose Aβ sensitivity of cognitive composite scores to Aβ+ related
levels are abnormally high to begin with—that is, in cognitive decline. Finally, we also aimed to explore how
preclinical AD. Comparing cognitive change between Aβ level, when treated as a continuous variable, is
individuals with high (Aβ+) and very high (Aβ++) related to cognitive decline.
levels may therefore provide a more informative
model for estimating the potential effects of slowing
Aβ deposition with anti-Aβ medication. Method
The cognitive endpoint used in the A4 trial, the
Participants
ADCS–PACC, is a composite score of performance
on measures of episodic memory and complex atten- Participants were recruited from a group of cognitively
tion and scores from the Mini-Mental State normal (CN) older adults over the age of 60 years enrolled
Examination (MMSE; Donohue et al., 2014). in the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL)
However, there remains debate about the extent to study. The AIBL study is a prospective longitudinal study
which the inclusion of the MMSE in preclinical AD seeking to understand the natural history of AD. Details of
composite scores affects the sensitivity of such scores recruitment have been described previously (Ellis et al.,
to changes in Aβ levels. This is because in CN older 2009). Briefly, exclusion criteria included schizophrenia,
adults, the MMSE has shown poor test–retest relia- depression (15-item Geriatric Depression Score ≥6),
bility, ceiling effects, and low sensitivity to impair- Parkinson’s disease, symptomatic stroke, uncontrolled dia-
ment (Spencer et al., 2013). Thus, including the betes, sleep apnea, and alcohol consumption exceeding two
MMSE in a composite score might reduce its sensi- standard drinks per day for women or four per day for
tivity to changes in Aβ levels in CN adults (Lim men. Participants undergo medical, psychiatric, and neu-
et al., 2016; Spencer et al., 2013). Furthermore, as ropsychological assessments approximately every
studies have shown that verbal fluency measures, as 18 months. At each assessment, a clinical review panel
measures of executive function, are sensitive to Aβ- considers all available medical, psychiatric and neuropsy-
related change (Insel et al., 2015; Lim et al., 2014; chological information to classify clinical status (Ellis et al.,
Lim et al., 2016; Papp et al., 2016; Papp, Rentz, 2009). Clinical classification is blinded to neuroimaging
Orlovsky, Sperling, & Mormino, 2017), it was pro- results. Aβ positivity was a key selection criterion for the
posed that the MMSE measure be replaced by current study. This is in line with the aim of this study to
a measure of verbal fluency. This yielded the z-scores model rates of cognitive decline for different levels of
of Attention, Verbal Fluency and Episodic Memory abnormal Aβ in preclinical AD. As such, including Aβ–
for Nondemented Older Adults (ZAVEN) composite participants who are not considered to be in the preclinical
(Lim et al., 2016). This ZAVEN composite showed stage of AD in the analysis would detract from the aim of
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 3

this study. Additionally, as current preclinical AD trials Neuropsychological assessment


require participants to be Aβ+ for enrolment, the inclusion The AIBL test battery comprises neuropsychological tests
of Aβ– participants in the analyses may skew estimations of that were selected to cover main cognitive domains affected
cognitive deterioration, thus making it less informative for by AD, and has been described previously (Ellis et al.,
power calculations. 2009). A selection of these have been used to compute the
composite scores. These include: the Mini-Mental State
Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Robins, & Helzer, 1983)
Materials and measures to measure global cognition; California Verbal Learning
Test–Second Edition (CVLT–II) delayed free recall (Delis,
Neuroimaging
Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 2000) to measure episodic mem-
Aβ imaging with PET was conducted using one of
ory; Logical Memory II (Wechsler Memory Scale, WMS;
three radioligands: 11C-Pittsburgh Compound B (PiB),
18 Story A only) delayed recall (Wechsler, 1945) to measure
F-Florbetapir (FBP) or 18F-Flutemetamol (FLUTE).
episodic memory; Delis–Kaplan Executive Function
A PET standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) was
System (D-KEFS) verbal (letter) fluency (Delis, Kaplan, &
calculated for each participant. A linear regression
Kramer, 2001) to measure executive function; and Digit
transformation was applied to the SUVR for each
Symbol–Coding subtests of the Wechsler Adult
F-18 Aβ tracer to transform it into a “PiB-like” SUVR
Intelligence Scale–Third Edition (WAIS–III; Wechsler,
unit.8. These were termed BeCKeT (Before the
1997) to measure complex attention (see Table 1).
Centiloid Kernel Transformation; Villemagne et al.,
2014). Participants were excluded if their SUVR/
BeCKeT score was below 1.4. Participants with Calculation of PACC scores
a SUVR between 1.4 and 1.9 (inclusive of 1.4) were Composite scores were calculated by averaging standar-
classified as high Aβ (Aβ+). Participants with a SUVR dized scores for each outcome measure from the neurop-
score of 1.9 or more were classified as very high Aβ sychological tests. The raw scores for the outcome
(Aβ++). The cutoff value of 1.4 was modified from 1.5, measures were standardized to z-scores using the baseline
which has been used previously and was obtained from mean and standard deviation derived from the current
a hierarchical cluster analysis (Lim et al., 2016; Rowe sample. The subtests that form the ADCS–PACC were
et al., 2013). A cutoff of 1.4 was used in the current selected based on a review of the literature that suggested
study to be more inclusive when analyzing Aβ+ parti- that the main cognitive domains to show deterioration in
cipants only. The cutoff value of 1.9 obtained from preclinical AD were episodic memory (both list learning
a ROC curve analysis was used as the optimal cut and paragraph recall), executive function, and orientation
point to discriminate age-matched CN older adults (Donohue et al., 2014). As such, a similar approach has
from AD patients (Lim et al., 2016; Rowe et al., 2013). been adopted to the construction of the current multido-
main cognitive composites, and the tests selected are reli-
able and well validated and are widely used across
Demographic and clinical characteristics
multiple AD cohorts (Lim et al., 2016).
Age and gender were self-reported. Premorbid IQ was
assessed using the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
(WTAR; Wechsler, 2001). The Hospital Anxiety and
Procedure
Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983) and
the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Yesavage et al., 1982) The current study was conducted based on data col-
were used to measure levels of depression and anxiety. The lected in the AIBL study. The process of consent,
Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale was also included to recruitment, and classification has been described in
contribute to the clinical information of the current sample. detail previously (Ellis et al., 2009). The AIBL study has

Table 1. Description of composite scores and neuropsychological tests included.


Composite score Abbreviation Neuropsychological tests included
Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study– ADCS–PACC MMSE, CVLT–II, Logical Memory II, and Digit
Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite score Symbol–Coding
Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study– ADCS–PACC no CVLT–II, Logical Memory II, and Digit Symbol–Coding
Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite score excluding the Mini- MMSE
Mental State Examination
Z-scores of Attention, Verbal Fluency and Episodic Memory for ZAVEN CVLT–II, Logical Memory II, Digit Symbol–Coding, and
Nondemented Older Adults D-KEFS verbal fluency
Note. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; CVLT–II = California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition; D-KEFS = Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System.
4 L. BRANSBY ET AL.

been approved by the regulations of three institutional fluency as individual measures entered as the depen-
research and ethics committees, with all participants dent variable. This was done to elicit a comparison
providing written informed consent prior to participa- between these two measures, the inclusion or exclu-
tion. Participants attended the research facility for clin- sion of which is what differentiates each composite
ical and neuropsychological assessments every score.
18 months. Neuropsychological assessments were con-
ducted in 90-min sessions by trained research assistants Relationship between Aβ level as a continuous
according to standardized protocols. For participants variable and cognitive decline
included in the current study, neuroimaging aligned Three planned comparisons were conducted using
with one of these time points. repeated measures LMMs for each cognitive composite
score to determine the relationship between Aβ level as
a continuous variable and cognitive change. In the
Data analysis
analyses, SUVR, time (baseline, 18 months, 36 months)
Data selection and SUVR × Time interaction were entered as fixed
Data from participants who had not undergone neu- factors, participant as a random factor, and cognitive
roimaging or did not have two subsequent cognitive composite score as the dependent variable. Statistical
assessments following neuroimaging were excluded significance was set at p < .05.
from analysis. For the data included, the time point at
which neuroimaging had taken place was now classi-
Results
fied as baseline, and the two subsequent time points
were now classified as 18- and 36-month visits. All data Figure 1 summarizes the data selection process for the
from other time points were excluded. current study. Participants who had not undergone
neuroimaging (n = 343), who did not have two subse-
Demographic and clinical characteristics quent neuropsychological visits following neuroima-
Analysis began by investigating differences in demo- ging (n = 328), and who were classified as being Aβ–
graphic and clinical characteristics between Aβ+ and (n = 215) were excluded from the current study. This
Aβ++ groups to determine whether they needed to be resulted in a total of 66 CN participants.
added as covariates in subsequent analyses. An inde-
pendent samples t test was used to analyze age. A chi-
Demographic and clinical characteristics
square test of independence was used to analyze sex.
Due to distributional characteristics a Mann–Whitney At baseline, there were no statistically significant dif-
test was used to analyze premorbid IQ, depression, and ferences between the Aβ+ and Aβ++ groups on any
anxiety. For all analyses, Aβ status was entered as demographic or clinical characteristics (Table 2).
a dichotomous and categorical independent variable. However, age was added as a covariate to adjust for
any covariance between age and cognitive decline.
Difference in rates of cognitive change between Aβ+
and Aβ++ groups
Effect of Aβ group on cognitive change using
Three planned comparisons were conducted using
cognitive composite scores
repeated measures linear mixed-effects models
(LMMs) with maximum likelihood estimation and The LMM analyses indicated statistically significant
an unstructured covariance matrix, for each cogni- group by time interactions for each composite score.
tive composite score. Linear mixed modeling was Table 3 summarizes these interactions and the group
used because it is able to model both fixed and mean slopes for both groups for each cognitive com-
random effects, accounting for variability typical of posite score. The group mean slopes for both groups
longitudinal studies (Lim et al., 2016). In the ana- are displayed in Figure 2 for each composite score.
lyses, group (Aβ+, Aβ++), time (baseline, 18 months, Relative to the Aβ++ group, the Aβ+ showed a slower
36 months) and Group × Time interaction were rate of decline on each of the composite scores.
entered as fixed factors, participant as a random
factor, and cognitive composite score as the depen-
The magnitude of difference between Aβ groups
dent variable. Cohen’s d was calculated to determine
measured by the cognitive composite scores
the magnitude of difference between rates of cogni-
tive change between groups over 36 months. These The magnitude of the difference in rate of change
analyses were repeated with the MMSE and letter between groups for each composite was moderate to
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 5

group, the magnitude of this difference was slightly less


than that observed for the ADCS–PACC (d = 0.85, p
< .01) and the ZAVEN (d = 0.72, p < .01). Reanalyses
with age as a covariate did not alter the nature or
343 participants who had not
undergone neuroimaging were magnitude of group differences on the composite
excluded scores.

Comparison of the effect of Aβ on MMSE and


verbal fluency individual scores over 36 months
328 participants who did not have The LMM analyses did not indicate statistically signifi-
two subsequent
neuropsychological visits cant group by time interactions on the MMSE or verbal
following neuroimaging were fluency scores. Similarly, both the MMSE (d = 0.15, p
excluded
= .54) and verbal fluency (d = 0.24, p = .34) reflected
small and statistically insignificant magnitudes of dif-
ference between Aβ groups.

(N=281)
Relationship between Aβ level at baseline and
cognitive performance
215 Aβ- participants were
excluded The LMMs indicated statistically significant relation-
ships between SUVR at baseline and each composite
score over 36 months. Similar to the previous analyses,
the ADCS–PACC reflected a z-score change of −0.42
(0.78) per 18 months associated with each unit increase
in SUVR, which was larger than the other composite
scores. However, the rate of decline on each composite
score was not statistically significant from each other
Figure 1. Sample selection process. CN = cognitively normal; (95% confidence interval, CI, overlap). Table 3 and
Aβ+ = beta-amyloid positive; Aβ– = beta-amyloid negative; Figure 3 summarize these results.
AIBL = Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle study.

Discussion
large. While cognitive decline measured using the The first hypothesis that, compared to CN older adults
ADCS–PACC with no MMSE (d = 0.62, p = .01) was with very high levels of abnormal Aβ (Aβ++), CN older
significantly less in the Aβ+ group than in the Aβ++ adults with high levels (Aβ+) would show a slower rate

Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample by Aβ status.


CN Aβ+ (n = 34) CN Aβ++ (n = 32)
M (SD) n (%) M (SD) n (%) p
Female sex 18 (52.9) 13 (40.6) .316
Age (years) 72.47 (6.57) 75.19 (6.89) .106
Premorbid IQ 109.73 (5.96) 110.47 (6.47) .345
GDS 0.61 (0.97) 0.75 (0.88) .322
HADS Depression 2.41 (2.02) 2.97 (2.49) .372
HADS Anxiety 3.62 (2.10) 3.81 (3.14) .887
CDR 0.03 (0.12) 0.03 (0.12) .951
CDR sum of boxes 0.03 (0.12) 0.03 (0.12) .951
MMSE 28.74 (1.02) 28.91 (1.17) .530
Digit Symbol 57.71 (13.76) 57.06 (13.69) .964
Logical Memory II 11.97 (5.03) 10.48 (3.93) .272
CVLT–II 11.88 (3.46) 11.56 (3.06) .442
Letter Fluency (FAS) 42.82 (12.69) 45.22 (9.08) .019
Note. Aβ = beta-amyloid; CN = cognitively normal; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale;
CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; CVLT–II = California Verbal Learning Test delayed recall.
6 L. BRANSBY ET AL.

Table 3. Effect of abnormal Aβ level, as a group and continuous variable, on cognitive decline over 36 months in CN older adults.
CN Aβ+ CN Aβ++
Group Time Group × Time (n = 34) n = 32) SUVR
Slope Slope
F (df) p F (df) p F (df) p M (SD) M (SD) slope M (SD) p
ADCS–PACC 1.19 (1, 139.25) .277 20.44 (1, 129.24) <.001 11.92 (1, 129.24) <.001 −0.04 (0.29) −0.29 (0.29) −0.42 (0.78) <.001
ADCS–PACC (with no 0.22 (1, 120.22) .638 25.10 (1, 128.17) <.001 6.29 (1, 128.17) .013 −0.08 (0.25) −0.23 (0.25) −0.35 (0.67) <.001
MMSE)
ZAVEN 0.80 (1, 113.14) .372 19.91 (1, 129.16) <.001 8.49 (1, 129.16) .004 −0.04 (0.22) −0.20 (0.22) −0.32 (0.58) <.001
Note. Aβ = beta-amyloid; CN = cognitively normal; SUVR = standardized uptake value ratio; ADCS–PACC = Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Preclinical
Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite score; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; ZAVEN = Z-scores of Attention, Verbal Fluency and Episodic Memory for
Nondemented Older Adults. Mean slope is calculated as z-score decline per year.

a. of cognitive decline was supported. Decline over three


years in performance on each of the composite scores
studied was less in the Aβ+ group than in the Aβ++
groups. The effect size observed for differences in this
rate of cognitive decline varied between 0.62 and 0.85
(Table 3, Figure 2). The error associated with the
estimate of difference in rates of cognitive decline for
each composite overlapped, indicating that these did
not differ from one another. These data show that
within preclinical AD, lower Aβ levels at baseline are
b. associated with slower rates of cognitive decline.
The second hypothesis that the exclusion of the
MMSE from composite measures of cognitive func-
tion would improve their sensitivity to Aβ+ related
cognitive decline was not supported. The removal of
the MMSE from the ADCS–PACC reduced the mag-
nitude of difference in rate of cognitive change
detected between groups, with Cohen’s d decreasing
from 0.85 to 0.62. The finding that the MMSE did
contribute to the sensitivity of cognitive composite
c. scores to Aβ-related cognitive decline in
preclinical AD is consistent with the data showing
the validity of the ADCS–PACC initially (Donohue
et al., 2014). However, in these analyses, claims about
sensitivity of the ADCS–PACC were based on com-
parisons of performance between Aβ– and Aβ+ indi-
viduals. Given that Aβ+ is a necessary criterion for
preclinical AD, this study was comparing cognitive
decline between individuals for whom AD had already
begun and individuals without AD. The current data
extend this by showing that when all individuals are
Figure 2. Differences in rates of cognitive decline measured by (a) Aβ+ (i.e., meet criteria for preclinical AD), the MMSE
the ADCS–PACC, (b) the ADCS–PACC excluding the MMSE, and (c) does play a role in the sensitivity of the ADCS–PACC
the ZAVEN between Aβ groups over 36 months. Slope is calculated to Aβ.
as z-score decline per year. Shaded regions represent 95% CIs.
ADCS–PACC = Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Preclinical
Our exploration of Aβ level at baseline, when treated
Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite score; MMSE = Mini-Mental State as a continuous measure, showed that a higher SUVR
Examination; ZAVEN = Z-scores of Attention, Verbal Fluency and was associated moderately with worsening cognitive
Episodic Memory for Nondemented Older Adults; Aβ = beta- performance over time on each of the cognitive com-
amyloid; CI = confidence interval; BL = baseline. posite scores. These results provide further evidence
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 7

a. composite scores was lower than that observed in indi-


viduals with a SUVR of 1.9 or higher at baseline.
However, there was substantial variability surrounding
the differences in the rate of decline between groups
(Figure 2), which was likely due to the small sample
size of the current study and reinforces the need for the
results to be replicated in a larger group.
In the context of AD, Aβ levels are not associated
with clinical characteristics and, in particular, with
level of cognitive impairment. This conclusion is
b. based mainly on postmortem studies that have
reported the strength of associations between Aβ pla-
que counts and clinical disease severity prior to death,
as well as on data from studies of living patients on
associations between SUVR and performance on neu-
ropsychological tests (Aizenstein et al., 2008; Bennett
et al., 2006). Importantly, many clinical trials targeting
Aβ in participants with mild to moderate AD have
failed to show any improvement on cognition (Egan
et al., 2018). Such data have been used to argue that
clinical trials of Aβ lowering medications are mis-
guided (Herrup, 2015). However, the results from the
c. present study indicate that in individuals with
preclinical AD, higher Aβ levels are associated with
more rapid cognitive decline. Thus, keeping Aβ levels
low at this preclinical stage is likely to be associated
with slower disease progression (Sperling et al., 2011;
Sperling et al., 2014).
The current results also show that the magnitude of
association between Aβ level and subsequent cognitive
decline is related to the combination of tests used to
measure cognition. Both the ADCS–PACC (d = 0.85)
and ZAVEN (d = 0.72) composite scores showed
a large effect of Aβ load in the preclinical AD sample.
Figure 3. The relationship between Aβ level at baseline, as
a continuous measure, and cognitive performance on (a) the However, previous comparisons have indicated that the
ADCS–PACC, (b) the ADCS–PACC excluding the MMSE, and (c) inclusion of the MMSE in a composite score will
the ZAVEN over 36 months. Slope is calculated as z-score reduce its sensitivity to cognitive change (Lim et al.,
decline per year. Shaded regions represent 95% CIs. ADCS– 2016). In CN older adults, the MMSE is known to have
PACC = Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Preclinical poor test–retest reliability, ceiling effects, and restricted
Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite score; MMSE = Mini-Mental
State Examination; ZAVEN = Z-scores of Attention, Verbal
sensitivity to subtle cognitive impairment (Spencer
Fluency and Episodic Memory for Nondemented Older Adults; et al., 2013). However, the ADCS–PACC that includes
Aβ = beta-amyloid; CI = confidence interval; SUVR = standar- the MMSE (d = 0.85) reflected the largest effect size out
dized uptake value ratio. of all composite scores in the current study, and
removing the MMSE from the ADCS–PACC reduced
its sensitivity to differences in rates of cognitive decline
that when all individuals have abnormal Aβ, the level associated with Aβ level (d = 0.62). This controversial
of Aβ is associated with cognitive decline. result needs to be considered taking into account the
The current study is one of the first to show that integrity of the MMSE. Previous analysis has demon-
variation in the extent of Aβ positivity, even when all strated that the MMSE is not a robust or reliable
individuals are Aβ+, is associated with the nature and assessment tool, as patients with moderate to severe
rate of subsequent cognitive decline (Lim et al., 2016). memory impairment were still able to achieve near to
Specifically, for individuals whose SUVR was above 1.4 perfect scores (Lacy, Kaemmerer, & Czipri, 2014).
but below 1.9, the rate of decline detected by all Therefore, the MMSE might accurately screen the
8 L. BRANSBY ET AL.

mental state of the subject at the time, but it might fail assessments on Aβ– adults will clarify whether number
to reliably detect any underlying pathology. of years of Aβ+ provides predictive information over
The MMSE was excluded from the ZAVEN on the and above that obtained from continuous Aβ levels, as
basis that no studies have shown performance on this has been presented in this study. A final limitation of
test to be abnormal, or to decline over time in the current study is that Aβ is included as the sole
preclinical AD (Lim et al., 2016). Further, a previous biomarker, and was only measured at a single time
study showed that adding a measure of letter fluency to point. Future studies should explore the role of bio-
a cognitive composite score increased its sensitivity to markers and their relationship with cognition, and the
Aβ-related cognitive decline, and this was replicated in relationship between change in biomarker levels and
the current results showing an increase in Cohen’s subsequent change in cognitive function.
d from 0.62 to 0.72 (Lim et al., 2016). In the current The current results demonstrate that there is impor-
study there was a statistically significant difference on tant information that needs to be understood beyond
letter fluency scores between Aβ groups at baseline, the traditional dichotomy of Aβ levels as positive or
with the Aβ++ group performing on average three negative (Lim et al., 2016). Our results show that even
words better than the Aβ+ group (Table 2). After within the Aβ+ group, there remains much variance in
36 months the Aβ++ group performed slightly worse the rate of cognitive decline. Other studies have also
on the measure than the Aβ+ group. However, this shown that the rate of Aβ accumulation is related to
difference was not statistically significant over time (d the rate of cognitive decline (Villemagne et al., 2013).
= 0.24, p = .34). There was also no significant differ- As these factors have implications for clinical trial out-
ence between Aβ groups on the MMSE (d = 0.15, p comes, future studies should investigate the role of
= .54) as an individual measure. These results were both Aβ levels at baseline and rate of Aβ accumulation
expected given that the ADCS–PACC and ZAVEN in predicting cognitive decline in CN older adults.
did not significantly differ from one another. These limitations notwithstanding, the current study
However, the ZAVEN should be considered as demonstrated that when CN individuals have abnor-
a better composite score as it still demonstrated sensi- mal Aβ, the level of Aβ at baseline is associated with
tivity to the difference between Aβ groups (d = 0.72, p the rate of subsequent cognitive decline. Our results
< .01), is composed of measures that are not prone to suggest that in Aβ+ CN older adults, lower levels of Aβ
ceiling effects (unlike the MMSE), and is more reliable, burden are likely to translate to slower cognitive
demonstrated by tighter standard deviations than the decline over a time period similar to that of a clinical
ADCS–PACC, suggesting that the inclusion of the trial. Differences in the rate of cognitive change over
MMSE in the ADCS–PACC is associated with more 36 months between CN older adults with abnormally
variance. high levels of Aβ (Aβ+) and those with very high levels
There are limitations in the current study that war- of abnormal Aβ (Aβ++) were estimated for three cog-
rant consideration. First, the sample size of the current nitive composite scores. It was also demonstrated that
study was relatively small and is likely to have played Aβ level, as a continuous variable, is related to cogni-
a role in the large variability around the results. tive decline over 36 months. Although the ADCS–
Therefore, it is important for results to be replicated PACC reflected the largest difference between Aβ
in a larger sample of CN older adults with abnormal groups, previous research suggests that the MMSE is
Aβ. Additionally, the AIBL study is not an epidemio- not a reliable measure of cognitive decline in CN older
logical sample, but rather a convenience sample adults. Thus, the ZAVEN might provide better utility
(Brodaty et al., 2014). As a result, cognitively normal for characterizing Aβ-associated rates of cognitive
participants recruited were well educated and had few change in preclinical AD and will likely be sensitive
untreated medical or psychiatric illnesses. On average, to detecting the benefits in cognition associated with
premorbid IQ was relatively high. Therefore, it would lowering abnormal levels of Aβ in a clinical trial. Taken
also be important for these findings to be replicated in together, these results provide a benchmark of compar-
population-based samples. Another limitation of cur- ison for preclinical AD clinical trials that aim to slow
rent older cohort studies of preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive deterioration.
disease is that a substantial portion (30%) already
meet criteria for Aβ positivity. As it is not possible to
determine how long these individuals have been Aβ+ Acknowledgments
for, we have used baseline Aβ levels as an estimation Alzheimer’s Australia (Victoria and Western Australia)
(i.e., someone with higher Aβ levels may have been Aβ assisted with promotion of the study and the screening of
+ for longer). Future studies that conduct serial Aβ telephone calls from volunteers. The AIBL team wishes to
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 9

thank the clinicians who referred patients with AD to the Delis, D., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. (2001). Delis-Kaplan
study: Brian Chambers, Edmond Chiu, Roger Clarnette, executive function system (D-KEFS). Canadian Journal
David Darby, Mary Davison, John Drago, Peter Drysdale, of School Psychology, 20(1–2), 117–128.
Jacqueline Gilbert, Kwang Lim, Nicola Lautenschlager, Dina Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (2000).
LoGiudice, Peter McCardle, Steve McFarlane, Alastair California verbal learning test – Second edition. Adult
Mander, John Merory, Daniel O’Connor, Ron Scholes, version. manual. Test.
Mathew Samuel, Darshan Trivedi, and Michael Woodward. Donohue, M. C., Sperling, R. A., Salmon, D. P., Rentz, D.
We thank all those who participated in the study for their M., Raman, R., Thomas, R. G., … Aisen, P. S. (2014).
commitment and dedication to helping advance research into The preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite. Journal
the early detection and causation of AD. of the American Medical Association Neurology, 71(8),
961.
Egan, M. F., Kost, J., Tariot, P. N., Aisen, P. S., Cummings, J. L.,
Vellas, B., … Michelson, D. (2018). Randomized trial of ver-
Disclosure statement ubecestat for mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease. The New
England Journal of Medicine, 378(18), 1691–1703.
C.L.M. is an advisor to Prana Biotechnology Ltd and Ellis, K. A., Bush, A. I., Darby, D., De Fazio, D., Foster, J.,
a consultant to Eli Lilly. P.J.S. is a scientific consultant to Hudson, P., … Ames, D. (2009). The Australian imaging,
Cogstate Ltd. P.M. is a full-time employee of Cogstate Ltd. D. biomarkers and lifestyle (AIBL) study of aging:
A. has served on scientific advisory boards for Novartis, Eli Methodology and baseline characteristics of 1112 indivi-
Lilly, Janssen, and Pfizer Inc. V.L.V. served as a consultant duals recruited for a longitudinal study of Alzheimer’s
for Bayer Pharma. disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 21(4), 672.
Folstein, M. F., Robins, L. N., & Helzer, J. E. (1983). The
mini-mental state examination. Archives of General
Funding Psychiatry, 40(7), 812.
Funding for the study was provided in part by the study Herrup, K. (2015). The case for rejecting the amyloid cascade
partners: Commonwealth Scientific Industrial and research hypothesis. Nature Neuroscience, 18(6), 794–799.
Organization (CSIRO), Edith Cowan University (ECU), Insel, P. S., Mattsson, N., Mackin, R. S., Kornak, J., Nosheny,
Mental Health Research institute (MHRI), National Ageing R., Tosun-Turgut, D., … Weiner, M. W. (2015).
Research Institute (NARI), Austin Health, CogState Ltd. The Biomarkers and cognitive endpoints to optimize trials in
study also received support from the National Health and Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Clinical and Translational
Medical Research Council (NHMRC); the Dementia Neurology, 2(5), 534–547.
Collaborative Research Centres program (DCRC2); the Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C.,
Science and Industry Endowment Fund (SIEF); and the Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., & Silverberg, N. (2018). NIA-
Cooperative Research Centre for Mental Health (CRCMH); AA research framework: Toward a biological definition of
and the New Energy and Industrial Technology Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 14, 535–
Development Organization (NEDO) Japan [grant to V.L.V.]. 562.
Lacy, M., Kaemmerer, T., & Czipri, S. (2014). Standardized
mini-mental state examination scores and verbal memory
performance at a memory center: Implications for cogni-
References tive screening. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease
and Other Dementias, 30(2), 145–152.
Aizenstein, H. J., Nebes, R. D., Saxton, J. A., Price, J. C., Lim, Y. Y., Maruff, P., Pietrzak, R. H., Ames, D., Ellis, K. A.,
Mathis, C. A., Tsopelas, N. D., … Klunk, W. E. (2008). Harrington, K., … Rowe, C. C. (2014). Effect of amyloid
Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive on memory and non-memory decline from preclinical to
impairment among the elderly. Archives of Neurology, 65 clinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 137(1), 221–231.
(11), 1509–1517. Lim, Y. Y., Snyder, P. J., Pietrzak, R. H., Ukiqi, A., Villemagne, V.
Baker, J. E., Lim, Y. Y., Pietrzak, R. H., Hassenstab, J., L., Ames, D., … Maruff, P. (2016). Sensitivity of composite
Snyder, P. J., Masters, C. L., & Maruff, P. (2017). scores to amyloid burden in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease:
Cognitive impairment and decline in cognitively normal Introducing the Z-scores of attention, verbal fluency, and
older adults with high amyloid-β: A meta-analysis. episodic memory for nondemented older adults composite
Alzheimer’s & Dementia (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 6, score. Alzheimer’s and Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and
108–121. Disease Monitoring, 2, 19–26.
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Arvanitakis, Z., Kelly, J. F., Papp, K. V., Mormino, E. C., Amariglio, R. E., Munro, C., Dagley,
Aggarwal, N. T., Shah, R. C., & Wilson, R. S. (2006). A., Schultz, A. P., … Rentz, D. M. (2016). Biomarker validation
Neuropathology of older persons without cognitive of a decline in semantic processing in preclinical alzheimer’s
impairment from two community-based studies. disease. Neuropsychology, 30(5), 624–630.
Neurology, 66(12), 1837–1844. Papp, K. V., Rentz, D. M., Orlovsky, I., Sperling, R. A., &
Brodaty, H., Mothakunnel, A., de Vel-Palumbo, M., Ames, Mormino, E. C. (2017). Optimizing the preclinical
D., Ellis, K. A., Reppermund, S., … Sachdev, P. S. (2014). Alzheimer’s cognitive composite with semantic proces-
Influence of population versus convenience sampling on sing: The PACC5. Alzheimer’s and Dementia:
sample characteristics in studies of cognitive aging. Annals Translational Research and Clinical Interventions, 3(4),
of Epidemiology, 24(1), 63–71. 668–677.
10 L. BRANSBY ET AL.

Rowe, C. C., Bourgeat, P., Ellis, K. A., Brown, B., Lim, Y. Y., study: Stopping AD before symptoms begin? Science
Mulligan, R., … Villemagne, V. L. (2013). Predicting Translational Medicine, 6(228), 228fs13–228fs13.
Alzheimer disease with beta-amyloid imaging: Results Villemagne, V. L., Burnham, S., Bourgeat, P., Brown, B., Ellis,
from the Australian imaging, biomarkers, and lifestyle K. A., Salvado, O., … Masters, C. L. (2013). Amyloid β
study of ageing. Annals of Neurology, 74(6), 905–913. deposition, neurodegeneration, and cognitive decline in
Spencer, R. J., Wendell, C. R., Giggey, P. P., Katzel, L. I., sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: A prospective cohort study.
Lefkowitz, D. M., Siegel, E. L., & Waldstein, S. R. (2013). The Lancet Neurology, 12(4), 357–367.
Psychometric limitations of the mini-mental state exam- Villemagne, V. L., Dore, V., Yates, P., Brown, B., Mulligan,
ination among nondemented older adults: An evaluation R., Bourgeat, P., et al. (2014). En attendant centiloid.
of neurocognitive and magnetic resonance imaging corre- Journal of Advanced Research, 2, 723–729.
lates. Experimental Aging Research, 39(4), 382–397. Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for clin-
Sperling, R., Mormino, E., & Johnson, K. (2014). The evolu- ical use. The Journal of Psychology, 19(1), 87–95.
tion of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Implications for Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring
prevention trials. Neuron, 84, 608–622. manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Corporation. doi:10.1177/1073191102009001003
Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., … Phelps, C. H. (2011). Toward Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler test of adult reading:
defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging- Corporation.
Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guide- Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V.,
lines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s and Dementia. Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O. (1982). Development and vali-
doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003 dation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preli-
Sperling, R. A., Jack, C. R., & Aisen, P. S. (2011). Testing the minary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37–
right target and right drug at the right stage. Science 49.
Translational Medicine, 3(111), 111cm33–111cm33. Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety
Sperling, R. A., Rentz, D. M., Johnson, K. A., Karlawish, J., and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67
Donohue, M., Salmon, D. P., & Aisen, P. (2014). The A4 (6), 361–370.

You might also like