You are on page 1of 6

680263

research-article2017
CDPXXX10.1177/0963721416680263Costa et al.On Language Processing Shaping Decision Making

Current Directions in Psychological

On Language Processing Shaping Science


2017, Vol. 26(2) 146­–151
© The Author(s) 2017
Decision Making Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0963721416680263
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416680263
www.psychologicalscience.org/CDPS

Albert Costa1,2, Marc–Lluís Vives1, and Joanna D. Corey1


1
Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, and 2Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis
Avançats (ICREA), Barcelona, Spain

Abstract
Recent research has revealed that people’s preferences, choices, and judgments are affected by whether information
is presented in a foreign or a native language. Here, we review this evidence, focusing on various decision-making
domains and advancing a variety of potential explanations for this foreign-language effect on decision making. We
interpret the findings in the context of dual-system theories of decision making, entertaining the possibility that
foreign-language processing reduces the impact of intuition and/or increases the impact of deliberation on people’s
choices. In closing, we suggest future research directions for progressing our understanding of how language and
decision-making processes interact when guiding people’s decisions.

Keywords
foreign-language processing, decision making, judgment, bilingualism

Decision-Making Machines With evaluate the risks and benefits, also considering your
Talking Heads preferences. In this context, your decision should be
independent of whether you are interacting with a doctor
We are constantly making decisions, from mundane in your native or foreign language. However, this is not
ones—such as what to eat or wear—to more significant the case.
ones—such as whether to undergo a surgical procedure
or enroll in a retirement plan. In effect, we are decision-
making machines. But humans are also talking heads, Using a Foreign Language Modifies
since we love to tell others about our feelings and ideas. Decision-Making Tendencies
Thus, we are decision-making machines with talking Our decisions are driven by different forces. One is auto-
heads. Consequently, we make decisions based on infor- matic, intuitive, and cost-free, and the other is more con-
mation that is largely communicated linguistically. How trolled and deliberative (see Evans, 2008, for a review).
do language and decision-making processes interact These processes are constantly at play when we make
when guiding our decisions? We assess this issue by decisions and do not always favor the same responses or
exploring how using a foreign versus a native language have the same weight on our choices (Kahneman, 2003).
affects our revealed preferences, choices, and judgments. Put simply, on some occasions we go with our gut feeling
Understanding how our talking heads affect our deci- and on others we slow down, thinking twice about the
sions is fundamental to promoting better and wiser situation and the options at stake. Our reliance on each
decisions. of these processes depends on several factors, such as
In principle, provided that information is understood, our mood at a given moment or how demanding or
the language in which it is presented should not be a
major factor affecting our decisions. Consider, for exam-
Corresponding Author:
ple, that you have to decide between having surgery and Albert Costa, Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu
undergoing a more conservative treatment. You are pre- Fabra, Carrer de Tànger, 122, 08018 Barcelona, Spain
sented with the available information and carefully E-mail: costalbert@gmail.com
On Language Processing Shaping Decision Making 147

familiar the situation is (Shiv & Fedorikhin, 1999; see judge the wrongness of moral transgressions—actions
Kahneman, 2011, for a review). Using a foreign language that break moral or social norms but have no tangible
affects the contributions of intuition and deliberation to consequences—less harshly when the transgressions are
our decisions—a phenomenon known as the foreign- presented in a foreign language (Geipel, Hadjichristidis,
language effect. This effect has been explored in three & Surian, 2015b).
domains. To the extent that our choices in these contexts are the
First, the language in which information is presented product of the interaction between intuitive and delibera-
affects people’s treatment of losses, gains, and risk. For- tive processes, language status seems to alter this rela-
eign-language processing leads to a reduction of loss and tionship. This raises the question of why and how this
risk aversion. For example, in a foreign-language context, effect arises.
people are less sensitive to whether options are pre-
sented in terms of gains or losses—they are less affected On the Origin of the Foreign-Language
by the way choices are framed, showing more consistent
preferences. Also, people are more willing to take risks in
Effect
foreign-language contexts, at least for positive expected The foreign-language effect has been interpreted in the
outcomes. For example, most people prefer accepting a context of dual-process theories of decision making. But
sure outcome of €1 to gambling with an even chance of how are the contributions of intuitive and deliberative
winning €2.50 or nothing. This goes against normative processes affected by language status? Does foreign-
behavior, given that taking the gamble has a higher language use reduce the impact of intuition and/or
expected value (€1.25) than the sure outcome (€1). Using increase the impact of deliberation? There are reasons to
a foreign language reduces this tendency (see Costa, believe that both might be true.
Foucart, Arnon, Aparici, & Apesteguia, 2014; Keysar, Reading, writing, listening, and speaking are more
Hayakawa, & An, 2012). Consistent with this, foreign- costly and largely less fluent in a foreign language than
language use also affects people’s judgments of risks and in a native language (see Costa & Sebastián-Gallés, 2014,
benefits—reducing the perception of risk and increasing for a review). Although there is still debate about the
the perception of benefit (Hadjichristidis, Geipel, & effects of processing difficulty on decision making, some
Savadori, 2015). authors have suggested that processing difficulty is used
Second, foreign-language contexts affect the way we as a signal to engage in more thoughtful and deliberative
make sense of the events we encounter in the world. In processing, which reduces the effects of intuition (Alter,
particular, foreign-language processing reduces illusory Oppenheimer, Epley, & Eyre, 2007; Gervais & Norenzayan,
correlations. Consider, for example, the hot-hand fallacy: 2012; Oppenheimer, 2008; but see Meyer et al., 2015, for
the tendency to expect a positive outcome after a series contrasting evidence). Put simply, situations that involve
of prior positive outcomes, even when the events are processing difficulties are less subject to the impact of
independent. Using a foreign language reduces this fal- intuition and prompt more deliberation.
lacy effect (Gao, Zika, Rogers, & Thierry, 2015). Similarly, In fact, independent research has revealed that for-
foreign-language use can reduce illusions of causality, eign-language processing recruits brain areas related to
whereby people perceive a causal link between two suc- control processes to a greater extent than native-language
cessive independent events (Díaz-Lago & Matute, 2014). processing—the inferior prefrontal and dorso-anterior
The third, and perhaps most striking, domain in which cingulate cortices (Branzi, Della Rosa, Canini, Costa, &
language affects people’s choices is morality. In particu- Abutalebi, 2016; see Abutalebi & Green, 2016, for a recent
lar, foreign-language processing prompts more utilitarian review on the relationship between language control and
choices than native-language processing. For example, executive control), areas that may be involved in the
when presented with the well-known footbridge decision-making contexts described above (Greene,
dilemma, in which saving the life of five people involves Nystrom,  Engell, Darley, & Cohen, 2004; Tom, Fox,
actively sacrificing the life of another person (Foot, 1978; Trepel, & Poldrack, 2007). Hence, according to this view,
Thomson, 1986), people are more willing to make this the foreign-language effect could be the result of a
sacrifice if the dilemma is presented in a foreign lan- decrease in processing fluency that prompts people to
guage (Costa, Foucart, Hayakawa, Aparici, & Apesteguia, slow down and think more carefully about the decision-
2014). This phenomenon has been replicated with sev- making situation.
eral native and foreign languages (Cipolletti, McFarlane, Emotion is another factor associated with foreign-
& Weissglass, 2016; Geipel, Hadjichristidis, & Surian, language processing that can alter the interaction between
2015a), showing that it is not restricted to specific lan- intuition and deliberation. Messages processed in a for-
guages or cultures (see also Geipel, Hadjichristidis, & eign language usually elicit a milder emotional response
Surian, 2016). Consistent with this effect, people tend to compared to those processed in a native language (see
148 Costa et al.

Pavlenko, 2012, and Caldwell-Harris, 2015, for reviews).1 that intuitive and deliberative processes have in the
Interestingly, people often use their emotional reaction to course of decision making.
a given problem to guide their decision instead of engag-
ing in more deliberative reasoning (Loewenstein, Weber,
Future Directions
Hsee, & Welch, 2001; Slovic, Finucane, Peters, &
MacGregor, 2007). In other words, we go with our feelings The foreign-language effect on decision making is a very
regarding what is good or bad rather than considering the recent discovery, with most studies on the topic pub-
options in a thoughtful manner. But foreign-language use lished in the last 3 years. Consequently, we are still far
lowers emotional reactivity, as, for example, shown by from having a good description of the phenomenon. We
reduced electrodermal responses to swear words and need to advance our knowledge of its generalizability
childhood reprimands (Harris, Aycicegi, & Gleason, 2003; and boundaries. Which decision-making contexts are
see Dewaele, 2004, for similar behavioral evidence). More- sensitive to the language of presentation, and which ones
over, reading emotionally charged passages in a foreign are not? For example, up to now, the foreign-language
language elicits less activation in brain areas related to effect has mostly been tested in contexts involving a cer-
emotional processing in the amygdala (Hsu, Jacobs, & tain emotional connotation. Hence, we do not yet know
Conrad, 2015). Interestingly, this brain structure has been whether foreign-language use affects people’s response
shown to be relevant in decision-making contexts involv- tendencies in domains that do not carry emotionally
ing gains and losses (De Martino, Kumaran, Seymour, & arousing information. This can be assessed by looking,
Dolan, 2006). Hence, according to this view, the foreign- for example, at the effect of language on certain emotion-
language effect could result from a reduction in the emo- ally neutral biases such as anchoring, hindsight bias, and
tional reactivity elicited by the decision-making scenario. the conjunction fallacy. In relation to the boundaries of
This, in turn, could reduce the impact of affective states on the phenomenon, it is also critical to assess how foreign-
people’s decisions, therefore allowing for more delibera- language proficiency modulates the presence and magni-
tive processing. tude of the phenomenon. It is reasonable to predict that
Processing difficulty and emotionality may have col- when language proficiency approaches native-like levels,
lateral effects on the way people relate to and construe a language status should have a minimal effect—although
given situation (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2008; Van Boven, this may depend on the context of learning and use. On
Kane, McGraw, & Dale, 2010). In particular, foreign- the other hand, much lower levels of language profi-
language use may lead to an increase in psychological ciency may pose too heavy a burden on processing,
distance—it may aid in taking a more objective perspec- reducing the availability of the resources needed to
tive of the situation and construing the options in a more engage in deliberation. Hence, the current observations,
global/abstract way (Trope & Liberman, 2010). Interest- arguably from studies involving participants with
ingly, psychological distance and foreign-language use medium-high levels of foreign-language proficiency,
can affect people’s decisions in a similar manner (Aguilar, should be complemented with studies exploring the role
Brussino, & Fernández-Dols, 2013; Lermer, Streicher, of language proficiency in the interaction between intu-
Sachs, Raue, & Frey, 2015; Raue, Streicher, Lermer, & ition and deliberation.
Frey, 2015). Indeed, foreign-language use has been This information would allow us to design more con-
shown to lead to a detachment effect, such that people trolled experiments assessing the origin of the phenom-
are able to discuss certain topics that they would enon and to determine more precisely the contributions
otherwise find too aversive (e.g., Bond & Lai, 1986; of intuition and deliberative processing. Does using a
Halberstadt, 1994; Marcos, 1976; see also Ivaz, Costa, & foreign language both reduce intuitive processing and
Duñabeitia, 2015). Thus, according to this view, the for- promote deliberative processing? If so, in which way are
eign-language effect could result from an increase in psy- these processes affected? For example, foreign-language
chological distance, which would affect the contributions use may not prompt more deliberation per se; rather, it
of intuition and deliberation to people’s choices. Note, may shape the cues used by intuitive processes. Indeed,
however, that the way in which psychological distance advancing in these explanations would require exploring
affects people’s judgments is still debated, especially in the roles of cognitive fluency, cognitive load, emotional
the context of moral choice (Eyal, Liberman, & Trope, reactivity, and their interactions with foreign-language
2008; but see also Gong & Medin, 2012; Zezelj & Jokic, processing. This enterprise would benefit from a multi-
2014). disciplinary approach, in which psychophysical (e.g.,
These three explanations are not mutually exclusive facial electromyographic responses, electrodermal activ-
and are probably complementary and related to each ity, pupil dilation) and brain-imaging techniques would
other to some extent. However, all of them stress that the provide fundamental information. Furthermore, this
foreign-language effect stems from changes in the weights research could also constrain current models of decision
On Language Processing Shaping Decision Making 149

making in general, since it is not obvious how these Declaration of Conflicting Interests
models can capture the complexities of the phenome- The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
non. Indeed, it is possible that the various models make respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
different predictions about how language status affects
our choices in different contexts. Funding
Perhaps the greatest challenge would be the explora- This study was partially funded by grants from the Spanish
tion of the foreign-language effect in the context of real- Ministry of Economy and Finance (PSI2013-42091-P), the Gov-
life decisions. Can we use language status as a lever to ernment of Catalonia (2014 SGR 1210), and the European
promote better decisions, at least in certain decision- Research Council under the European Community’s Seventh
making contexts? If foreign-language use can indeed Framework (FP7/2007 2013 Cooperation Grant Agreement
help people engage in a colder, dispassionate, delibera- 613465-AThEME). Marc–Lluís Vives was supported by a grant
tive thinking style, then this might help promote self- from the Government of Spain (BES-2015-071581). Joanna D.
control and avoidance of temptation. For example, it is Corey was supported by a grant from the Government of Cata-
possible that language could be used as a nudge to pro- lonia (FI-DGR).
mote better choices, such as what to eat or how to evalu-
ate the options for a clinical treatment (for preliminary Note
evidence, see Klesse, Levav, & Goukens, 2015). This 1. Some evidence suggests that variables may affect this reduc-
would be important for many choices that involve long- tion in emotionality associated with a foreign-language use,
term beneficial outcomes but require current sacrifices. such as language proficiency, age of acquisition, and language
Finally, it is crucial to study the role of language in inter- distance (i.e., the degree of difference between a native and
active contexts, such as interpersonal conflict and nego- foreign language). Hence, this phenomenon is not always pres-
tiation. At present, all the available evidence has come ent (e.g., Eilola & Havelka, 2011; Ferré, García, Fraga, Sánchez-
from studies in which people were presented with indi- Casas, & Molero, 2010).
vidual decision-making contexts, and we do not know
whether the phenomenon will extend to interactive sce- References
narios. And, indeed, the answer to this question is not Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of lan-
obvious: Although foreign-language use can prompt a guage control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve.
deliberative thinking mode, the cognitive load and anxi- Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 689–698.
ety often elicited when interacting in a foreign language Aguilar, P., Brussino, S., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2013).
may end up leading to more automatic and less thought- Psychological distance increases uncompromising conse-
ful decisions. quentialism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49,
We have reviewed how foreign-language use affects 449–452.
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Effects of fluency on
people’s choices. In several contexts, foreign- and native-
psychological distance and mental construal (or why New
language processing lead to differential recruitment of
York is a large city, but New York is a civilized jungle).
intuition and deliberation, with foreign-language pro- Psychological Science, 19, 161–167.
cessing prompting a more deliberative approach. But do Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N.
not be mistaken—this evidence does not imply that peo- (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty acti-
ple’s choices would always be better in foreign-language vates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
contexts. Indeed, sometimes it is good to go with our gut, ogy: General, 136, 569–576.
and other times it is better to think twice and consider Bond, M. H., & Lai, T. M. (1986). Embarrassment and code-
the options in a more thoughtful manner. Hence, finding switching into a second language. Journal of Social Psy-
out how our talking heads affect the likelihood of using chology, 126, 179–186.
one approach or the other is fundamental to understand- Branzi, F. M., Della Rosa, P. A., Canini, M., Costa, A., & Abutalebi,
J. (2016). Language control in bilinguals: Monitoring and
ing the decision-making machines that we are.
response selection. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 2367–2380.
Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2015). Emotionality differences between
Recommended Reading a native and foreign language implications for every-
Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2015). (See References). An accessible day life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24,
review about the reduced emotionality in a foreign lan- 214–219.
guage in comparison to a native one and its possibilities Cipolletti, H., McFarlane, S., & Weissglass, C. (2016). The moral
and consequences for society. foreign-language effect. Philosophical Psychology, 29, 23–40.
Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S. L., & An, S. G. (2012). (See References). Costa, A., Foucart, A., Arnon, I., Aparici, M., & Apesteguia, J.
The first exploration of how using a foreign language (2014). “Piensa” twice: On the foreign language effect in
changes our decisions when dealing with risk. decision making. Cognition, 130, 236–254.
150 Costa et al.

Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Apesteguia, Hadjichristidis, C., Geipel, J., & Savadori, L. (2015). The effect
J., Heafner, J., & Keysar, B. (2014). Your morals depend of foreign language in judgments of risk and benefit: The
on language. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e94842. doi:10.1371/journal role of affect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied,
.pone.0094842 21, 117–129.
Costa, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2014). How does the bilingual Halberstadt, A. (1994). On the use of language in psychother-
experience sculpt the brain? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, apy with émigrés from the former Soviet Union. Journal of
15, 336–345. Jewish Communal Service, 71, 93–99.
De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Harris, C. L., Aycicegi, A., & Gleason, J. B. (2003). Taboo
Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactiv-
brain. Science, 313, 684–687. ity in a first language than in a second language. Applied
Dewaele, J. M. (2004). The emotional force of swearwords Psycholinguistics, 24, 561–579.
and taboo words in the speech of multilinguals. Journal Hsu, C. T., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2015). Can Harry
of Multilingual & Multicultural Development, 25, 204–222. Potter still put a spell on us in a second language? An fMRI
Díaz-Lago, M., & Matute, H. (2014, October). Foreign lan- study on reading emotion-laden literature in late bilinguals.
guage reduces the illusion of causality. Paper presented Cortex, 63, 282–295.
at the Meeting of the Sociedad Española de Psicología Ivaz, L., Costa, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2015). The emotional
Experimental (SEPEX), Murcia, Spain. impact of being myself: Emotions and foreign-language
Eilola, T. M., & Havelka, J. (2011). Behavioural and physiologi- processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
cal responses to the emotional and taboo stroop tasks in Memory, and Cognition, 42, 489–496.
native and non-native speakers of English. International Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice:
Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 353–369. Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58,
Evans, J., St. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reason- 697–720.
ing, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY:
Psychology, 59, 255–278. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Eyal, T., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). Judging near and Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S. L., & An, S. G. (2012). The foreign-
distant virtue and vice. Journal of Experimental Social language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces deci-
Psychology, 44, 1204–1209. sion biases. Psychological Science, 23, 661–668.
Ferré, P., García, T., Fraga, I., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Molero, Klesse, A. K., Levav, J., & Goukens, C. (2015). The effect of
M. (2010). Memory for emotional words in bilinguals: Do preference expression modality on self-control. Journal of
words have the same emotional intensity in the first and in Consumer Research, 42, 535–550.
the second language? Cognition and Emotion, 24, 760–785. Lermer, E., Streicher, B., Sachs, R., Raue, M., & Frey, D. (2015).
Foot, P. (1978). The problem of abortion and the doctrine of The effect of construal level on risk-taking. European
the double effect. In P. Foot, Virtues and vices: And other Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 99–109.
essays in moral philosophy (pp. 19–32). Oxford, England: Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001).
Blackwell. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286.
Gao, S., Zika, O., Rogers, R., & Thierry, G. (2015). Second Marcos, L. R. (1976). Bilinguals in psychotherapy: Language as
language feedback abolishes the “hot hand” effect during an emotional barrier. American Journal of Psychotherapy,
even-probability gambling. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 30, 552–561.
5983–5989. Meyer, A., Frederick, S., Burnham, T., Guevara Pinto, J. D.,
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2015a). The foreign Boyer, T. W., Ball, L. J., . . . Schuldt, J. P. (2015). Disfluent
language effect on moral judgment: The role of emotions fonts don’t help people solve math problems. Journal of
and norms. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0131529. doi:10.1371/journal Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 16–30.
.pone.0131529 Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2015b). How foreign in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 237–241.
language shapes moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Pavlenko, A. (2012). Affective processing in bilingual speak-
Social Psychology, 59, 8–17. ers: Disembodied cognition? International Journal of
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2016). Foreign lan- Psychology, 47, 405–428.
guage affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes Raue, M., Streicher, B., Lermer, E., & Frey, D. (2015). How
to moral judgment. Cognition, 154, 34–39. far does it feel? Construal level and decisions under risk.
Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking pro- Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4,
motes religious disbelief. Science, 336, 493–496. 256–264.
Gong, H., & Medin, D. L. (2012). Construal levels and moral Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict:
judgment: Some complications. Judgment and Decision The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision
Making, 7, 628–638. making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278–292.
Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G.
Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Opera-
and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44, 389–400. tional Research, 177, 1333–1352.
On Language Processing Shaping Decision Making 151

Thomson, J. J. (1986). Rights, restitution, and risk: Essays in Van Boven, L., Kane, J., McGraw, A. P., & Dale, J. (2010). Feeling
moral theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. close: Emotional intensity reduces perceived psychological
Tom, S. M., Fox, C. R., Trepel, C., & Poldrack, R. A. (2007). The distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,
neural basis of loss aversion in decision-making under risk. 872–885.
Science, 315, 515–518. Zezelj, I. L., & Jokic, B. R. (2014). Replication of experiments
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psy- evaluating impact of psychological distance on moral judg-
chological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440–463. ment. Social Psychology, 45, 223–231.

You might also like