You are on page 1of 6

European Annals of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck diseases (2012) 129, 98—103

Available online at

www.sciencedirect.com

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Understanding the deafened brain: Implications for


cochlear implant rehabilitation
D.S. Lazard a,b,∗, A.-L. Giraud a, D. Gnansia c, B. Meyer b,d, O. Sterkers b,e

a
Inserm U960, école normale supérieure, 45, rue d’Ulm, 75005 Paris, France
b
Service d’ORL et de chirurgie cervico-faciale, hôpital Beaujon, AP—HP, 100, boulevard du Général-Leclerc,
92118 Clichy cedex, France
c
MXM-Neurelec, 2720, chemin Saint-Bernard, 06224 Vallauris, France
d
Université Paris-6 Pierre-et-Marie-Curie, 15, rue de l’École-de-Médecine, 75006 Paris, France
e
UMR-S 867, Inserm, université Paris-7 Denis Diderot, 75018 Paris, France

KEYWORDS Summary The cochlear implant (CI), by enabling oral communication in severely to profoundly
Cross-modality; deaf subjects, is one of the major medical advances over the last fifty years. Despite the globally
Functional MRI; very satisfactory results, individual outcomes vary considerably. The objective of this review
PET; is to describe the various factors influencing the results of CI rehabilitation with particular
Phonology; emphasis on the better understanding of neurocognitive mechanisms provided by functional
Plasticity; brain imaging.
Rehabilitation; The following aspects will be discussed:
Pre/post-lingual; 1. Peripheral predictors such as the degree of preservation of nerve structures and the
Maturation; positioning of the electrode array.
Predictor; 2. The duration of auditory deprivation whose influence on brain reorganization is now
Performance becoming more clearly understood.
3. The age of initiation of hearing rehabilitation in subjects with pre-lingual deafness influ-
encing the possibility of physiological maturation of nerve structures.
4. The concepts of sensitive period, decoupling and cross-modality.
5. In post-lingually deaf adults, brain plasticity can allow adaptation to the disability induced
by deafness, subsequently potentiating CI rehabilitation, particularly as a result of audiovisual
interactions.
6. Several studies provide concordant evidence that implanted patients present different
phonological analysis and primary linguistic capacities.
The results of CI rehabilitation are dependent on factors situated between the cochlea
and cortical associative areas. The importance of higher cognitive influences on the func-
tional results of cochlear implantation justify adaptation of coding strategies, as well as global
cognitive management of deaf patients by utilising brain plasticity capacities.
© 2011 Published by Elsevier Masson SAS.

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +33 1 40 87 55 71; fax: +33 1 46 25 23 62.


E-mail address: dianelazard@yahoo.fr (D.S. Lazard).

1879-7296/$ – see front matter © 2011 Published by Elsevier Masson SAS.


doi:10.1016/j.anorl.2011.06.001
Understanding the deafened brain: Implications for cochlear implant rehabilitation 99

Introduction Certain peripheral factors have also been proposed to


explain the variable performance of CI.
Severe to profound bilateral deafness, especially when it
occurs in subjects previously able to hear, can cause social
isolation and severe distress. The cochlear implant (CI), by Positioning of electrode array: angle, length, scalar
enabling good quality oral communication in quiet [1,2], placement
represents one of the major medical advances over the last
fifty years. CI can be proposed in patients with congenital The targets of electrical stimulation are the spiral ganglion
deafness (pre-lingual deafness), ideally before the age of 2 cells. There is a relationship between the distance to the
years, or in patients in whom severe hearing loss occurred spiral ganglion and the quantity of energy that needs to
after acquisition of language (post-lingual deafness). In be delivered. Electrode placement as close as possible to
1995, there were 12,000 implanted subjects throughout the these structures is recommended by inserting the electrode
world, with 120,000 in 2008, and 200,000 in 2010. Despite array in the scala tympani, while avoiding the scala vestibuli,
the globally very satisfactory results, individual outcomes with the shortest possible distance to the modiolus [5,6].
vary considerably. For example, the median recognition of Computed tomography methods have been developed to
disyllabic words is 70% 1 year after surgery (range: 100 to evaluate: the distance of the electrode array to the modi-
0%) [1]. This variability is observed in both post-lingually olus; the percentage placement inside the cochlear canal
deaf adults and congenitally deaf children [2]. The propor- [14]; or the length or angle of insertion1 [6,15]. Excessively
tion of patients with word recognition scores less than 10% is deep insertion of electrode arrays not specifically designed
difficult to assess, but appears to be about 10% [3]. However, for long insertion appears to be associated with an increased
patients with test scores less than 50% must be considered risk of cochlear damage (contortion, dislocation) and there-
to present oral communication difficulties. fore poorer performance [5,6].
This marked variability of outcomes can be partly In order to limit these peripheral factors of variability,
explained by peripheral predictors such as the degree of ‘‘soft surgery’’ introduction techniques [16] and the design
preservation of spiral ganglion cells [4], or the position of of electrode arrays have tried to decrease the distance
the electrode array [5,6]. The duration of auditory depriva- between the electrodes and spiral ganglion cells. A future
tion is another major predictive factor due to its impact on approach could consist of inserting the electrode array into
the peripheral [7] but especially the central nervous system. the modiolus [17] and/or promoting nerve growth by admin-
Functional brain imaging has improved our understanding of istration of neurotrophic factors. Finally, other types of
maturation of central auditory pathways in children, and the non-electrical (optic) stimulation are currently under inves-
cerebral changes induced by sensory deprivation in children tigation [18].
and in adults. The influence of cognitive factors appears to
be just as important if not more important than peripheral
structures in the performance of CI [8]. Duration of auditory deprivation
The objective of this review is to describe the var-
ious factors influencing the performance of CI hearing The aetiology most frequently proposed to explain the
rehabilitation, with particular emphasis on the better variable performance in congenitally deaf children and post-
understanding of neurocognitive mechanisms, provided by lingually deaf adults is the duration of auditory deprivation
functional brain imaging. [7,19,20]. However, many mechanisms are involved and
differ between pre- and post-lingually deaf populations.
Post mortem histological examination of the petrous tem-
Possible causes of variable performance poral bones of post-lingually deaf adults has concluded that
loss of inner hair cells induces retrograde neural degener-
ation [21]. The extent of this degeneration appears to be
Factors considered to have little or no influence correlated with the duration of deafness [4]. However, diver-
gent clinical and histological results are observed: patients
The older age of implantation of post-lingually deaf adults, with few residual spiral ganglion cells can obtain good
although resulting in lower performances, provides a high results [11,22] and a long duration of deafness may not
gain in life quality to this sub-population [9]. There is there- always result in excessive degeneration. Another structure
fore no upper age limit for cochlear implantation. The type influenced by auditory deprivation is the central nervous sys-
of implant or coding strategies at equivalent times has been tem: even when all of the peripheral prerequisites for good
excluded so far as factors of variable performance [10,11]. performances are present, functional cortical auditory net-
Published results on meningitis as a cause of deafness are works and synergistic audiovisual cooperation are essential
contradictory. Some authors consider this aetiology to be to derive meaningful information from the distorted sound
associated with poorer results due to more difficult and provided by the CI [8]. Central processes therefore play an
therefore incomplete insertion of electrodes [12] or due essential role in understanding of the CI message.
to central disorders (secondary hydrocephalus for example)
[9]. Other authors have reported similar performances to
those of patients implanted for congenital deafness, when
more progressive and higher stimulation thresholds (C and 1 The angle of insertion is more precise for evaluation of electrical
T levels) and higher stimulation rates are applied during tonotopy, ensuring a better correspondence with the Greenwood’s
fittings [13]. formula, relating cochlear place to characteristic frequency.
100 D.S. Lazard et al.

The auditory brain is called cross-modality. Non-implanted profoundly deaf


subjects therefore develop visual cross-modality of the
Functional brain imaging (functional MRI (fMRI) or positron auditory cortex.
emission tomography (PET)), evoked potentials, electroen-
cephalography or magnetoencephalography, have eluci- These modifications are now known to be influenced by
dated the way in which brain structure is modified by the the duration of auditory deprivation. The study of cortical
presence or, on the contrary, the absence of auditory stimuli. auditory evoked potentials (latency and morphology of the
P12 wave) in children implanted at various ages has demon-
Maturation of multi-sensory and associative cortical strated a sensitive period during which the auditory system is
particularly plastic and able to develop almost normal cen-
auditory areas in pre-lingually deaf children
tral maturation. This maturation occurs as a result of the
auditory stimulation delivered by the CI [29]. The sensitive
Cortical responses generated by non-auditory stimuli (visual
period is situated before the age of 3.5 years. After the age
[23] or somatosensory [24]) were studied by fMRI in pro-
of 7 years, speech integration and comprehension become
foundly pre-lingually deaf adults using sign language. These
much more difficult and the results of CI are much more
non-auditory stimuli, not related to sign language, gen-
variable. Between these two extremes, there is an individual
erated activation in areas normally devoted to hearing
variability of plastic capacities and therefore performances.
(temporal cortex) (Fig. 1). This mainly visual colonization
Part of this variability is independent of the duration of
of the auditory cortex is secondary to the absence of audi-
auditory deprivation and may be explained by cross-modal
tory stimulation and not to the practice of sign language
reorganization of the auditory cortex. This reorganization
[25]. Normally functioning modalities deploy supplementary
seems to start during fetal life and to be irreversible despite
resources and do not suffer from the sensory restriction.
early cochlear implantation [30,31]. Some areas of the tem-
Non-implanted profoundly deaf subjects therefore develop
poral cortex may already be reused by modalities other than
better spatial attentional capacities [26].
hearing and may prevent physiological auditory maturation.
These higher capacities compared to normal-hearing sub-
Inversely, other areas may remain devoid of any cognitive
jects develop as a result of:
use, allowing the possibility of good performance of CI if
maturation of these areas is triggered by the auditory inputs
• potentiation of cortical areas that are already multimodal provided by the CI, even at a relatively advanced age of
[25,27]. Multimodal areas are defined as regions of the implantation [32]. The factors influencing these maturation
brain able to process and integrate physiologically inputs mappings have not been identified.
of various nature. An example of a multimodal area is the At the end of the sensitive period, in the absence of
associative auditory cortex, which integrates visual and auditory stimulation, primary and secondary auditory areas
auditory inputs [28]; become decoupled: these areas are no longer able to
• modification of the intrinsic properties of the primary develop interconnections, even when cochlear implantation
auditory cortex. Colonization by other sensory modalities is performed [33]. Feedback loops and high-level cognitive
of cortical areas not stimulated by their usual modality modulations controlling auditory processing fail to develop.
Auditory areas are then available for reorganizations involv-
ing aberrant connections and neo-couplings. Children who
are implanted at an older age may therefore present audi-
tory activation in visual and parieto-temporal areas with no
benefit in terms of speech comprehension [29].
Basic research has formally demonstrated the need for
early cochlear implantation in children. Although the sen-
sitive period for satisfactory afferentation and maturation
of the auditory cortex occurs very early in childhood, a cer-
tain amount of plasticity nevertheless remains possible at
all ages [34—36]. The brain of post-lingually deaf adults can
also undergo changes induced by auditory deprivation and
then by CI rehabilitation.

Plasticity and cross-modality in implanted


Figure 1 Schematic surface representation of left auditory post-lingually deaf subjects
and visual cortices in a setting of physiological sensory matura-
tion. Despite progress in coding strategies, the auditory infor-
White: zones of activation responding to auditory input in the mation delivered by the CI remains coarse. In implanted
primary and secondary auditory cortices and Wernicke’s area
(temporal lobe); black: zones of activation responding to visual
inputs in the primary and secondary visual cortices (occipi- 2 The P1 wave represents the sum of the activities of synapses
tal lobe). In the case of non-implanted profound congenital situated on auditory pathways from the thalamus to high-level cog-
deafness, visual or somatosensory inputs activate the primary nitive areas. It reflects associative auditory processing using neural
auditory cortex. loops between primary and secondary auditory areas.
Understanding the deafened brain: Implications for cochlear implant rehabilitation 101

that compete with auditory recognition tasks. In contrast,


normal-hearing control subjects and cochlear-implanted
subjects with good performances are not bothered by visual
distractors [40]. Excessive visual reorganization of audi-
tory areas can therefore induce integration conflicts in the
case of incongruent audiovisual stimuli, even when they are
devoid of any linguistic content [40].

Reorganization of phonological processing


circuits: a possible key to the performance of
the cochlear implantees with post-lingual
deafness
Due to degradation of the auditory information delivered
by the CI, reconstitution of phonemic cues in cochlear-
implanted post-lingually deaf subjects, regardless of their
Figure 2 Word recognition learning curve during the first year performance, is a cognitively more difficult task than
after cochlear implantation. Median results of a population semantic processing [41]. Implanted subjects are also not
of 55 post-lingually deaf adults expressed as the percentage equivalent in terms of primary analysis of auditory infor-
recognition of dissyllabic words derived from Fournier lists and mation. For example, some subjects with poor performance
presented at 65 dB in quiet. perceive the auditory input, but do not activate the area
specific for recognition of the human voice, unlike normal-
post-lingually deaf subjects, progressive improvement of hearing control subjects and implanted subjects with good
performance during the first year demonstrates the capacity performance. Poor performance could be explained by a
of the brain to adapt to these deformed auditory inputs that defect of extraction of the human voice from the audi-
differ from those to which it is accustomed [8] (Fig. 2). tory message [42]. Absence of recruitment has also been
During the period of deafness, subjects maintain oral demonstrated in the left inferior prefrontal cortex [43], a
comprehension by developing lip reading, the efficacy of multimodal region participating in phonological and seman-
which varies from one subject to another. They use visual tic processing of language.
clues to complete the limited auditory information avail- The absence of recruitment of these regions could corre-
able. It could be logical to assume that the use of lip spond to:
reading would decrease with restoration of hearing. How-
ever, in practice, the information transmitted by the CI is • an inappropriate routing of auditory information due
too rudimentary and approximate (particularly in noise) to to cross-modal reorganization of these regions. Audi-
be sufficient and requires the persistent use of the visual tory information can no longer be analysed because the
clues provided by lip reading [37]. dedicated neurons now process other sensory modalities
Over the months or even years following cochlear and/or;
implantation, activation of the associative auditory cortex • poor correspondence between the perceived sounds and
gradually increases for stimuli containing speech informa- their internal phonemic representations.
tion, but remains stable for noise. A central learning process
therefore occurs in implanted patients enabling them to dis- Phonological decomposition is an initial auditory step
tinguish information with a linguistic content from other allowing the subsequent semantic analysis. Subjects with
information [35]. This constitutes the intra-modal plastic- poor performance would therefore lack linguistic analy-
ity of the auditory cortex. In parallel, the primary visual sis and a valid correspondence between perceived and
cortex develops activation in response to auditory stimuli. memorized phonology, limiting extraction of speech com-
As in the auditory cortex, the rate of activation depends prehension. These difficulties of matching the sounds
on the speech information contained [35]. This modifica- provided by the CI and internal phonological representations
tion of the properties of the visual cortex corresponds to tend to worsen with the duration of auditory deprivation
cross-modal plasticity. A basic inter-modal audiovisual coop- [44]. Progressive deterioration of phonological memory and
eration is present in all individuals, but due to the ambiguity its dorsal route (parieto-frontal) has been demonstrated
of the auditory signal of the CI, patients make greater use of in post-lingually deaf subjects (Fig. 3). Subjects with the
the visual component compared to normal-hearing subjects longest duration of deafness use the ventral semantic route
and develop synergistic interactions [34,36,38]. In the case (occipito-temporal) to palliate the insufficient elementary
of low performance with the CI (< 50% of word recognition), phonological decomposition. This leads to difficulties in pro-
due to insufficient auditory cues, a supplementary compen- cessing the information delivered by the CI due to global
sation is created by the development of visual responses auditory analysis, resulting in erroneous segmentation of
within the auditory cortex [39]. This cross-modal reorgani- the message. In contrast, subjects with maintained phono-
zation of the auditory cortex can even become deleterious to logical skills sustained by audiovisual interactions, present
speech comprehension when it is over-developed, as it has good performances with their CI and generally present the
been shown that implanted subjects with poor performance shortest durations of auditory deprivation [44].
present deterioration of their word recognition scores in the The study of non-implanted pre-lingually deaf subjects
presence of visual stimuli (colour screens or moving points) who have developed good reading skills shows that they
102 D.S. Lazard et al.

Figure 3 Functional MRI exploration phonological memory of post-lingually deaf adults, candidates for cochlear implantation (CI).
Surface representation of the left hemisphere.
The task consisted of phonological segmentation exercises by performing rhyme tests. The figure illustrates the correlation between
the neural networks used during the task and word recognition performances obtained prospectively after 6 months of implantation.
The physiological phonological network uses a dorsal network (superior) including fronto-parietal areas, while the ventral network
(inferior) extracts semantic information and uses inferior temporo-occipital, anterior temporal and inferior frontal areas. Deaf
subjects who use the dorsal phonological network will obtain good results with the CI. In contrast, subjects who use the ventral
semantic network (global analysis) to perform the rhyme task will obtain poorer performance with their CI. The use of the dorsal
network significantly decreases with increasing duration of auditory deprivation.

are able to successfully complete fine phonological anal- The results of CI rehabilitation are dependent on multiple
ysis tasks (rhymes) despite the fact that they have never factors situated between the cochlea peripherally and high-
received any auditory information [45,46]. Functional brain level central associative areas. The importance of higher
imaging has demonstrated the existence of a dorsal net- cognitive influences on the functional results of CI would
work, less well developed and less lateralised to the left justify adaptation of transmission of the peripheral message
than in normal-hearing subjects, but nevertheless present. but also global cognitive management of deaf patients in
It is based on more marked articulatory cues and internal order to take maximum advantage of their brain plasticity
repetition [45,46]. Post-lingually deaf implanted subjects capacities.
with good performance may therefore also use articulatory Funding: this article was made possible by the financial
correspondences that have been maintained active by audio- support of Neurelec (MXM, Vallauris, France) and the Asso-
visual cooperation [44]. This audiovisual cooperation would ciation des anciens internes des hôpitaux de Paris, with no
be less functional in subjects with poor performance, and conflict of interest.
may even become deleterious when visual colonization of
auditory areas becomes predominant [40,47].
More extensive investigation of the auditory memory of Disclosure of interest
post-lingually deaf subjects has shown modifications of net-
works evoking environmental and non-speech sounds. These The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest
reorganizations depend on the duration of deafness and are concerning this article.
designed to palliate the phonological deficit. In some cases,
they can be deleterious and associated with a poor prognosis
of cochlear implantation [44,48]. References
It would therefore be tempting to propose a cognitive
rehabilitation designed to promote internal phonological [1] Lazard DS, Bordure P, Lina-Granade G, et al. Speech percep-
segmentation and correspondences, and to preserve global tion performance for 100 post-lingually deaf adults fitted with
auditory memory. Studies are currently underway in this Neurelec cochlear implants: comparison between Digisonic(R)
field. Convex and Digisonic(R) SP devices after a 1-year follow-up.
Acta Otolaryngol 2010;130:1267—73.
[2] Lee HJ, Kang E, Oh SH, et al. Preoperative differences of cere-
Conclusion bral metabolism relate to the outcome of cochlear implants in
congenitally deaf children. Hear Res 2005;203:2—9.
Auditory deprivation is clearly responsible for socioeco- [3] Bodmer D, Shipp DB, Ostroff JM, et al. A comparison of post-
nomic disability, but also induces modifications of brain cochlear implantation speech scores in an adult population.
organization in both pre-lingual and post-lingual deafness. Laryngoscope 2007;117:1408—11.
Understanding the deafened brain: Implications for cochlear implant rehabilitation 103

[4] Nadol Jr JB, Eddington DK. Histopathology of the inner [27] Bavelier D, Dye MW, Hauser PC. Do deaf individuals see better?
ear relevant to cochlear implantation. Adv Otorhinolaryngol Trends Cogn Sci 2006;10:512—8.
2006;64:31—49. [28] Arnal LH, Morillon B, Kell CA, et al. Dual neural rout-
[5] Finley CC, Holden TA, Holden LK, et al. Role of electrode ing of visual facilitation in speech processing. J Neurosci
placement as a contributor to variability in cochlear implant 2009;29:13445—53.
outcomes. Otol Neurotol 2008;29:920—8. [29] Sharma A, Gilley PM, Dorman MF, et al. Deprivation-induced
[6] Escude B, James C, Deguine O, et al. The size of the cochlea cortical reorganization in children with cochlear implants. Int
and predictions of insertion depth angles for cochlear implant J Audiol 2007;46:494—9.
electrodes. Audiol Neurootol 2006;11:27—33. [30] Giraud AL, Lee HJ. Predicting cochlear implant outcome
[7] O’Donoghue GM, Nikolopoulos TP, Archbold SM. Determinants from brain organisation in the deaf. Restor Neurol Neurosci
of speech perception in children after cochlear implantation. 2007;25:381—90.
Lancet 2000;356:466—8. [31] Lee HJ, Giraud AL, Kang E, et al. Cortical activity at
[8] Moore DR, Shannon RV. Beyond cochlear implants: awakening rest predicts cochlear implantation outcome. Cereb Cortex
the deafened brain. Nat Neurosci 2009;12:686—91. 2007;17:909—17.
[9] Skinner MW, Ketten DR, Holden LK, et al. CT-derived estima- [32] Lee DS, Lee JS, Oh SH, et al. Cross-modal plasticity and
tion of cochlear morphology and electrode array position in cochlear implants. Nature 2001;409:149—50.
relation to word recognition in Nucleus-22 recipients. J Assoc [33] Sharma A, Nash AA, Dorman M. Cortical development, plastic-
Res Otolaryngol 2002;3:332—50. ity and re-organization in children with cochlear implants. J
[10] Spahr AJ, Dorman MF, Loiselle LH. Performance of patients Commun Disord 2009;42:272—9.
using different cochlear implant systems: effects of input [34] Giraud AL, Price CJ, Graham JM, et al. Functional plasticity
dynamic range. Ear Hear 2007;28:260—75. of language-related brain areas after cochlear implantation.
[11] Khan AM, Handzel O, Burgess BJ, et al. Is word recognition Brain 2001;124:1307—16.
correlated with the number of surviving spiral ganglion cells [35] Giraud AL, Price CJ, Graham JM, et al. Cross-modal plastic-
and electrode insertion depth in human subjects with cochlear ity underpins language recovery after cochlear implantation.
implants? Laryngoscope 2005;115:672—7. Neuron 2001;30:657—63.
[12] Rotteveel LJ, Snik AF, Vermeulen AM, et al. Three-year [36] Giraud AL, Truy E. The contribution of visual areas to speech
follow-up of children with postmeningitic deafness and partial comprehension: a PET study in cochlear implants patients
cochlear implant insertion. Clin Otolaryngol 2005;30:242—8. and normal-hearing subjects. Neuropsychologia 2002;40:
[13] Eshraghi AA, Telischi FF, Hodges AV, et al. Changes in pro- 1562—9.
gramming over time in postmeningitis cochlear implant users. [37] Rouger J, Lagleyre S, Fraysse B, et al. Evidence that cochlear-
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;131:885—9. implanted deaf patients are better multisensory integrators.
[14] Ketten DR, Skinner MW, Wang G, et al. In vivo measures Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104:7295—300.
of cochlear length and insertion depth of nucleus cochlear [38] Rouger J, Fraysse B, Deguine O, et al. McGurk effects
implant electrode arrays. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl in cochlear-implanted deaf subjects. Brain Res 2008;1188:
1998;175:1—16. 87—99.
[15] Xu J, Xu SA, Cohen LT, et al. Cochlear view: postop- [39] Doucet ME, Bergeron F, Lassonde M, et al. Cross-modal reor-
erative radiography for cochlear implantation. Am J Otol ganization and speech perception in cochlear implant users.
2000;21:49—56. Brain 2006;129:3376—83.
[16] Fraysse B, Macias AR, Sterkers O, et al. Residual hear- [40] Champoux F, Lepore F, Gagne JP, et al. Visual stimuli can impair
ing conservation and electroacoustic stimulation with the auditory processing in cochlear implant users. Neuropsycholo-
nucleus 24 contour advance cochlear implant. Otol Neurotol gia 2009;47:17—22.
2006;27:624—33. [41] Giraud AL, Truy E, Frackowiak RS, et al. Differential
[17] Volckaerts B, Corless AR, Mercanzini A, et al. Technology devel- recruitment of the speech processing system in healthy
opments to initiate a next generation of cochlear implants. subjects and rehabilitated cochlear implant patients. Brain
Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2007;2007:515—8. 2000;123:1391—402.
[18] Wenzel GI, Balster S, Zhang K, et al. Green laser light activates [42] Coez A, Zilbovicius M, Ferrary E, et al. Cochlear implant ben-
the inner ear. J Biomed Opt 2009;14:044007. efits in deafness rehabilitation: PET study of temporal voice
[19] Blamey P, Arndt P, Bergeron F, et al. Factors affecting auditory activations. J Nucl Med 2008;49:60—7.
performance of postlinguistically deaf adults using cochlear [43] Mortensen MV, Mirz F, Gjedde A. Restored speech compre-
implants. Audiol Neurootol 1996;1:293—306. hension linked to activity in left inferior prefrontal and
[20] Green KM, Bhatt YM, Mawman DJ, et al. Predictors of audi- right temporal cortices in postlingual deafness. Neuroimage
ological outcome following cochlear implantation in adults. 2006;31:842—52.
Cochlear Implants Int 2007;8:1—11. [44] Lazard DS, Lee HJ, Gaebler M, et al. Phonological processing
[21] Hinojosa R, Marion M. Histopathology of profound sensorineural in post-lingual deafness and cochlear implant outcome. Neu-
deafness. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1983;405:459—84. roimage 2010;49:3443—51.
[22] Blamey P. Are spiral ganglion cell numbers important for speech [45] MacSweeney M, Brammer MJ, Waters D, et al. Enhanced activa-
perception with a cochlear implant? Am J Otol 1997;18:S11—2. tion of the left inferior frontal gyrus in deaf and dyslexic adults
[23] Finney EM, Fine I, Dobkins KR. Visual stimuli activate auditory during rhyming. Brain 2009;132:1928—40.
cortex in the deaf. Nat Neurosci 2001;4:1171—3. [46] Aparicio M, Gounot D, Demont E, et al. Phonological processing
[24] Auer Jr ET, Bernstein LE, Sungkarat W, et al. Vibrotactile acti- in relation to reading: an fMRI study in deaf readers. Neuroim-
vation of the auditory cortices in deaf versus hearing adults. age 2007;35:1303—16.
Neuroreport 2007;18:645—8. [47] Tremblay C, Champoux F, Lepore F, et al. Audiovisual fusion
[25] Fine I, Finney EM, Boynton GM, et al. Comparing the effects of and cochlear implant proficiency. Restor Neurol Neurosci
auditory deprivation and sign language within the auditory and 2010;28:283—91.
visual cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 2005;17:1621—37. [48] Lazard DS, Giraud AL, Truy E, et al. Evolution of non-
[26] Dye MW, Bavelier D. Attentional enhancements and deficits in speech sound memory in postlingual deafness: implica-
deaf populations: an integrative review. Restor Neurol Neurosci tions for cochlear implant rehabilitation. Neuropsychologia
2010;28:181—92. 2011;49:2475—82.

You might also like