You are on page 1of 10

Wilbur, J., Marquez, D. X., Fogg, L., Wilson, R. S., Staffileno, B. A., Hoyem, R. L., Morris, M. C.

, Bustamante, E. E., Manning, A. F. (2012). The relationship between physical activity and cognition in
Staffileno,
older Latinos. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 10.1093/geronb/gbr137
67(5), 525–534, doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr137. Advance Access published on February 9, 2012

The Relationship Between Physical Activity and


Cognition in Older Latinos
JoEllen Wilbur,1 David X. Marquez,2 Louis Fogg,3 Robert S. Wilson,4,5 Beth A. Staffileno,6
Ruby L. Hoyem,1 Martha Clare Morris,7 Eduardo E. Bustamante,2 and Alexis F. Manning,1

1Women, Children, and Family Nursing, College of Nursing, Rush University, Chicago, Illinois.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


2Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition College of Applied Health Sciences, University of Illinois at Chicago.
3Community, Systems and Mental Health Nursing, College of Nursing,
4Department of Neurological Sciences, 5Department of Behavioral Sciences,
6Adult Health and Gerontological Nursing, College of Nursing and
7Section of Nutrition and Nutritional Epidemiology, Department of Internal Medicine,

Rush University, Chicago, Illinois.

Objectives. The purpose of this study was to explore the relationship between minutes spent participating in light and
moderate/vigorous-intensity physical activity (PA) and cognition in older Latinos, controlling for demographics, chronic
health problems, and acculturation.

Method. A cross-sectional study design was used. Participants were self-identified Latinos, without disability, who
had a score less than 14 on a 21-point Mini-Mental State Examination. Participants were recruited from predominantly
Latino communities in Chicago at health fairs, senior centers, and community centers. PA was measured with an acceler-
ometer, worn for 7 days. Episodic memory and executive function (inference control, inattention, and word fluency) were
measured with validated cognitive tests.

Results. Participants were 174 Latino men (n = 46) and women (n = 128) aged 50–84 years (M = 66 years). After
adjusting for control variables (demographics, chronic health problems) and other cognitive measures, regression analy-
ses revealed that minutes per day of light-intensity PA (r = −.51), moderate/vigorous PA (r = −.56), and counts per minute
(r=−.62) were negatively associated with lower word fluency.

Discussion. Findings suggest that the cognitive benefits of both light-intensity PA and moderate/vigorous PA may be
domain-specific.

Key Words: Cognition—Latino/Hispanic—PA.

Background Physical activity (PA) is thought to be one means of


The number of older adults in the United States is rapidly attenuating cognitive decline and improving cognitive ability
growing, and it is expected to grow fastest among Latinos. among older adults (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). Research
Between 2008 and 2030, the Latino population 65 years and providing support for the beneficial effects of PA on cogni-
older is expected to increase by 224% compared with just a tive function in older adults spans several decades but has
65% increase of the non-Latino White population 65 years recently been advanced by findings from observational
and older (Greenberg, 2009). Unfortunately, data from the prospective epidemiological studies (Hillman, Erickson, &
Health and Retirement Study, a large nationally representa- Kramer, 2008). Hamer and Chida (2009) identified 10 pro-
tive study of the 55 and older population, indicate that older spective cohort studies with healthy participants at baseline
Latinos are twice as likely as non-Latino Whites to exhibit that examined PA and cognitive decline. In nine of the 10
cognitive impairment with age (17.5% vs. 8.8%; Alzheimer’s studies, an inverse relationship was found between PA and
Association, 2010). Moreover, there is epidemiological cognitive decline. None of the studies, however, indicated
evidence linking age-associated cognitive impairment to that Latinos were included or mentioned translation of
diabetes (Kodl & Seaquist, 2008), for which Latinos in the instruments. The two studies that included race as a covari-
United States are more burdened compared with the domi- ate simply identified White versus non-White populations
nant culture (non-Latino White men 6.7% vs. Latino men (Albert et al., 1995; Sturman et al., 2005). Therefore, to
11.0%; non-Latino White women 5.6% vs. Latina women our knowledge, a majority of what is known about the PA–
10.9%; Roger et al., 2011). Given the costs associated with cognition relationship has been derived from the non-
care of the older people, it is imperative that we obtain a Latino dominant culture, and little is known about the possible
better understanding of modifiable health behaviors that can influence of acculturation on cognition in addition to the
prevent or stabilize cognitive impairment through the middle known influence of age and education (Plassman et al.,
and older years of life of our growing Latino population. 2007).
©
©The
All
TheAuthor
Author 2012.
All rights
2012. Published
rights reserved.
Published by
reserved. For
by Oxford
For permissions,
Oxford University
permissions, please
University Press
please e-mail:
Press on
on behalf
behalf of
ofThe
The Gerontological
Gerontological Society
e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
journals.permissions@oup.com.
Society of
ofAmerica.
America. 5251
Received
Received January
January 21,
21, 2011;
2011;Accepted
Accepted November
November 19,
19, 2011
2011
Decision
Decision Editor:
Editor: Robert
RobertWest,
West, PhD
PhD
2526 WILBUR
WILBUR

Closer scrutiny of these 10 studies revealed selected alternative to self-report (Hawkins et al., 2009). They are
measurement issues that may have particular bearing for small and unobtrusive and can be worn throughout the
Latinos. First, all relied on self-report measures that focused day without interfering with everyday movement. Another
heavily on leisure-time or structured PA obtained through unique feature is their ability to capture light-intensity PAs,
exercise and sport. According to 2009 National Health In- such as household cleaning, which is difficult to recall on
terview Survey, however, 44% of adult Latinos versus 28% self-report questionnaires. Although it is well-established
of non-Latino White adults reported no light, moderate, or that moderate-intensity PA reduces the risk of cardiovascu-
vigorous leisure PA (Roger et al., 2011). Furthermore, data lar disease and diabetes (U.S. Department of Health and
from Latinos indicate that the prevalence of leisure-time PA Human Services, 2008), recent work has shown that light-
is lowest among immigrants, and, although it increases over intensity PA is positively associated with physical health

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


time since immigration, it remains lower than in nonimmi- and well-being (of which cognitive ability is one compo-
grants (Tremblay, Bryan, Peréz, Ardern, & Katzmarzyk, nent) in persons more than 65 years of age (Buman et al.,
2006). Conversely, the Latino population has been shown to 2010). To date, the only study we identified that used accel-
engage in a high level of nonleisure-time lifestyle PAs that erometers to relate PA with cognition found that total daily
are a part of everyday life (Berrigan, Troiano, McNeel, activity counts were related to higher cognitive levels
DiSogra, & Ballard-Barbash, 2006). Unfortunately, the few (Buchman, Wilson, & Bennett, 2008). Unfortunately, inten-
studies that included lifestyle PA were restricted to a single sity and duration of PA in minutes per day or week were not
item that queried participation in yard or garden work reported.
(Albert et al., 1995; Broe et al., 1998; Sturman et al., 2005; Finally, an additional issue was found with measures of
van Gelder et al., 2004). None of the studies addressed cognition, in that over half of the 10 studies relied on either
true lifestyle PA that incorporates all activity dimensions: a single global measure of cognition (Ho, Woo, Sham,
leisure-time, household, occupational, and transportation Chan, & Yu, 2003; Lytle, Vander Bilt, Pandav, Dodge, &
PA. To gain a better understanding of the cognition–PA Ganguli, 2004; Richards, Hardy, & Wadsworth, 2003;
relationship for Latinos, we need measures of PA that ade- Schuit, Feskens, Launer, & Kromhout, 2001; Yaffe et al.,
quately capture their lifestyle activity. 1999) or a composite measure (Albert et al., 1995; Sturman
Another limitation of this group of studies examining the et al., 2005; Weuve et al., 2004). There is literature that sug-
PA–cognition relationship has been the failure to adequately gests that some cognitive domains are affected by PA more
define intensity, frequency, and duration of PA. Therefore, it than others (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). Therefore, use of
is unknown if the 2008 Physical Activity Guidelines for global and composite measures may obscure relationships
Americans by the U.S. Department of Health and Human between PA and cognition that differ based upon the cogni-
Services (2008) calling for 150 min of moderate PA per tive domain.
week are protective for cognitive decline as well as for The purpose of this study was to explore the relationship
improving cardiovascular health. Only one study (Singh- between minutes spent participating in light- and moderate/
Manoux, Richards, & Marmot, 2003) used a self-report vigorous-intensity PA and different forms of cognition in
measure that assigned PAs a metabolic equivalence inten- older Latinos, controlling for sex and for factors shown to
sity code (metabolic equivalent of task [MET], with 1.0 being be associated with cognition. Our study extends previous
approximately equal to energy cost while laying quietly) literature because it examines an understudied, at-risk,
and then summed the amount of time spent in moderate/ rapidly growing population and uses accelerometers, an
vigorous-intensity PA to provide a calculation of the innovative, comprehensive means of measuring PA.
minutes/hours per week of moderate/vigorous-intensity PA.
They found that adults who spent the least amount of time
in moderate-intensity (<2 hr) or high-intensity activity (<1 Method
hr) over three 10-year points in time had greater cognitive
decline, thus suggesting a protective effect for cognition Design, Setting, and Sample
as well as cardiovascular disease. A major limitation of A descriptive, cross-sectional study design was used.
self-report measures of PA, however, is that they are largely Inclusion criteria were 50 years of age or older, Latino, no
inaccurate for assessing the quantity of light- and moderate- ambulation disability or assistive walking device, no evidence
intensity unstructured lifestyle activity frequently performed of suspected dementia, and a Chicago metropolitan address.
by older adults and immigrant populations (Brach, Kriska, To assess for suspected dementia, we used a modification of
Glynn, & Newman, 2008). The optimum intensity and the 30-point Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), a
duration of lifestyle PA needed to impact cognitive function global measure of cognition, for telephone administra-
requires further investigation and confirmation with objec- tion (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). The modified
tive measures. 21-point MMSE did not include items that could not be done
Accelerometers, PA monitors that can detect/record the over the telephone (9 points): orientation to room, naming a
actual magnitude of acceleration, offer a more robust objective pencil and watch, following a three-stage command, reading
RELATIONSHIP
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN
BETWEEN PA
PA AND
AND COGNITION
COGNITION 5273

and obeying a command, writing a sentence, and copying a was taken with the Omron HEM-907XL automated blood
design. To date 1,451 older people in the Rush Memory pressure machine (Omron Health Care Inc., Vernon Hills,
and Aging Project have completed the standard 30-point IL). It has reliability and reproducibility of mercury sphyg-
MMSE as part of their baseline evaluation (Bennett et al., momanometer measurements (El Assaad, Topouchian, Darne,
2005). The Pearson correlation between the standard version & Asmar, 2002). Blood pressure measurements followed
and the 21-point MMSE is 0.968 (p < .001). Previous studies the American Heart Association Recommendations for
on cognition and aging using the 30-point MMSE with par- human blood pressure determination (Pickering et al.,
ticipants of low-educational attainment have identified one- 2005), and the average of three measurements was recorded.
third incorrect as a cutpoint for impaired/poor cognition (Raji, Consistent with National Health and Nutrition Examination
Al Snih, Ostir, Markides, & Ottenbacher, 2010); therefore, Survey (NHANES), a participant was identified as having

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


below 14 was our cutpoint for suspected dementia. hypertension if he or she was taking medication for hyper-
Recruitment sites (5 senior centers, 2 community health tension or if the systolic blood pressure was ≥140 mmHg or
care centers, 15 health fairs, 1 senior housing center) were diastolic blood pressure was ≥90 mmHg (Roger et al.,
located in 11 Chicago community areas that have a moderate- 2011). Chronic health problems were 0 “does not have
to-large Latino population (10%–89%) and in 1 suburban hypertension or diabetes,” 1 “has either hypertension or
community that is 8% Latino but adjacent to a community diabetes,” or 2 “has both hypertension and diabetes.”
that is 18% Latino (City of Chicago, n.d.). Recruitment
materials, available in Spanish and English, were reviewed Acculturation. The Acculturation Rating Scale for
by four bilingual adults who met the study eligibility crite- Mexican Americans—II measures orientation toward the
ria but did not participate in the study. They reviewed both Latino and the Anglo culture independently using two
to determine comprehension and consistency between the subscales: a Latino orientation subscale (17 items, origi-
versions. Sign-up sheets were available at the study sites. nal α = .88) and an Anglo orientation subscale (13 items,
Overall, 342 persons signed up on a recruitment sheet at original α = .83; Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995).
all data collection sites; 282 were reached, and, of these, We revised this measure to include all Latino subgroups
224 agreed to be screened. Of those screened, 25 did not (e.g., Puerto Ricans, Dominicans). Acculturation was
meet the inclusion criteria (11 a score of <14 on the short- determined by subtracting the mean score for the Latino
ened MMSE, 6 too young, 6 used a walking assistive subscale from the mean score for the Anglo subscale.
device, 1 not Latino, 1 lived outside area); of the 199 eligi- Based on the scores, Cuellar and colleagues (1995) iden-
ble volunteers, 18 decided not to participate and 7 could not tified five acculturative categories: very Latino oriented
schedule a time for an assessment, leaving 174 who partic- (less than −1.33), Latino oriented to approximately bal-
ipated. There were no significant differences between anced bicultural (greater than or equal to −1.33 and less
eligible volunteers who did and did not participate on than or equal to −.07), slightly Anglo oriented (greater
either mean age (both 66 years) or MMSE scores (18.73 than −.07 and <1.19), strongly Anglo oriented (≥1.19 and
vs. 19.31). <2.45), and very assimilated (>2.45). Good reliability
was demonstrated in our study for the Spanish (α = .81)
and the English (α = .73) versions.
Measures
Measures of background characteristics and cognitive Cognitive function.—Cognitive function was assessed
function had been previously translated and were available with a battery of seven performance tests: two of episodic
in Spanish or English. memory and five of executive function processes. Executive
function tests were selected to represent domains of execu-
Background characteristics.—Background character- tive function including interference control, inattention,
istics included demographics, chronic health problems, and and verbal fluency (Murphy, Barkley, & Bush, 2001). All
acculturation. tests were chosen because they have been shown to test
functions that decline in old age (Wilson, Beckett et al., 2002),
Demographics. Demographic variables were age, sex (1 = shown to test functions that are likely to be related to PA
men, 2 = women), and educational level (1 = no high school (Kramer, Erickson, & Colcombe, 2006), and translated
degree, 2 = high school degree or higher). Descriptive into Spanish and shown to be valid across ethnic and
demographic characteristics included marital status, employ- socioeconomic backgrounds (Krueger, Wilson, Bennett, &
ment status, and country of origin. Aggarwal, 2009). The battery of tests met the practical
demands of the study (i.e., brief, portable, acceptable to
Chronic health problems. Both diabetes and stroke place older people).
persons at risk for cognitive decline (Haan et al., 2003). Par- The bilingual data collectors participated in 1 day of
ticipants were asked to respond yes or no if they were on training with follow-up observation. They were certified on
medication for diabetes and/or hypertension. Blood pressure each test using performance-based criteria before they
4528 WILBUR
WILBUR

began data collection. The cognitive function tests are The ActiGraph monitor provides a valid assessment of PA
administered in the following fixed order: East Boston in adult men and women during treadmill walking/running
Memory Test (immediate recall), Stroop Neuropsychological and daily activity (Hendelman, Miller, Baggett, Debold, &
Screening Test, East Boston Memory Test (delayed recall), Freedson, 2000). It records vertical accelerations as “counts.”
numbers comparison, and category fluency. Participants were instructed to wear the monitor on their
hips for seven consecutive days. Participants were given an
East Boston Memory Test. Episodic memory was mea- accelerometer log on which they recorded the hours they
sured with the East Boston Memory Test, which involves wore it. The monitor was initialized and set to collect count
immediate and delayed recall of a brief story (Albert et al., values at 1-min sampling intervals (epochs). To be included
1991). The story, which has 12 key elements, was read in the analyses, ≥10 hr of data or wear time had to be available

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


slowly to the participant. The participant was asked to in a 24-hr period on at least 3 days (Matthews, Ainsworth,
immediately recall the story. After an approximate 3-min Thompson, & Bassett, 2002). As in Troiano and colleagues
delay (during which time the Stroop was administered), the (2008), nonwear time was defined as at least 60 consecutive
participant was asked to recall the story. Scores ranged from minutes of 0 activity counts.
0 to 12 story ideas recalled on immediate and delayed PA intensity was categorized by cutpoints of Miller,
recall. The episodic summary score was the mean of the Strath, Swartz, and Cashin, (2010) used with persons 60–69
immediate and delayed recall. years of age. The mean number of minutes of PA was
defined as: sedentary <100 counts/min, light 100–1,565
Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Interference counts/min (<3 METs), moderate 1,566–6,139 counts/min
control was measured with the modified color–word task (3.0–6.0 METs), and vigorous ≥6,140 counts/min (≥6.1
(Wilson et al., 2005) of the Stroop Neuropsychological METs). We report the number of minutes of light-intensity
Screening Test (Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe, & Leber, 1989). PA per day and combine the number of minutes of moderate
The participant named the color of ink in which a list of and vigorous PA per day due to low participation in vigor-
color names were provided. In some cases, the color of ink ous activity. In addition, we provide the number of activity
in which the word was printed was the same as the word and counts per minute, which is an estimate that corrects for the
for others the color differed from the word. For example, amount of wear time.
the word red was in blue ink. The scores were the number of
colors correctly identified in 30 s, the number of incorrect
responses in 30 s, and the number of colors answered cor- Procedures
rectly minus the number of incorrect responses (Wilson Interested volunteers were screened in person or over the
et al., 2005). Prior to administration of the color–word task, telephone for age, ethnicity, and disability and for cognition
the participant read a list of color names as quickly as pos- with the MMSE. Eligible persons were scheduled for an ap-
sible for 30 s. According to Trenerry and colleagues (1989), pointment at a location of their choosing, including their
the color task may have a priming effect on the interference homes (n = 65, 37%), one of the community recruitment
shown in the color–word task. Three participants did not sites (n = 99, 57%), or our program offices (n = 10, 6%). To
complete the measure due to literacy problems. closely simulate the privacy of the community sites and pro-
gram office setting, participants who chose their home were
Numbers Comparison Test. The Numbers Comparison asked to identify a room and time when there would be no
Test (Ekstron, French, Harmen, & Kermen, 1976), a measure outside interruptions. At the appointment, a bilingual staff
of inattention, requires participants to classify pairs of 3- to member further explained the study and read the consent
10-digit sequences as the same or different. The attention form aloud in the participant’s preferred language. After
score was the number of pairs classified correctly in 90 s consent was given, the health and demographics question-
minus the number classified incorrectly. naire was administered followed by assessment of blood
pressure, resting heart rate, height, weight, and waist circum-
Category Fluency Test. Word fluency was assessed by ference. Then the cognitive function measures were adminis-
asking participants to generate as many examples as possible tered. Next, additional questionnaires related to determinants
from two semantic categories (animals, fruits, vegetables) of PA and health (e.g., barriers to PA, social support, and
in separate 60-s trials (Welsh et al., 1994). The total number nutritional history), to be reported in a later manuscript
of unique examples was the performance measure. A word related to these issues, were administered. Once the ques-
fluency score was arrived at by summing the number of tionnaires were completed, participants watched a demonstra-
animals generated to the number of fruits and vegetables tion of the accelerometer and were then asked to demonstrate
generated. their competence. Participants wore their accelerometer for
the next 7 days. A second appointment was made to download
Physical Activity.—PA was monitored with the portable the accelerometer data, go over the results, and complete
GT1M ActiGraph accelerometer (Tryon & Williams, 1996). any questionnaires administered after the cognitive function
RELATIONSHIP
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN
BETWEEN PA
PA AND
AND COGNITION
COGNITION 5295

Table 1. Background Characteristics correlation analyses to determine how the dependent vari-
n % ables were related to the predictors.
Age
50–59 54 31.0
60–69 52 29.9
Results
70–79 51 29.3
≥80 17 9.8 Background Characteristics
Education The mean age of the participants was 66 years (SD =
Did not complete high school 99 56.9 9.12; range 50–84 years; Table 1). The majority of the
Completed high school or GED 29 16.7
Some college or vocational school 26 14.9 participants were women (74%), unmarried (59%), and not

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


Completed college or higher 20 11.5 working (76%). Over half (56%) of the participants had not
Sex graduated from high school. The main country of origin was
Men 46 26.4
Mexico (55%). A total of 144 (83%) participants chose to
Women 128 73.6
Marital respond in Spanish. The mean acculturation score was
Never married 13 7.5 −1.47 (SD = 1.16, range −3.23 to 1.98), which is in the
Married 72 41.4 “very Latino-oriented” range. Nearly 70% of the partici-
Widowed 46 26.4
Separated 13 7.5
pants had a self-reported chronic health problem. Eight par-
Divorced 26 14.9 ticipants had diabetes only, 78 had hypertension only, and
Member of an unmarried couple 4 2.3 35 had both hypertension and diabetes.
Employment
Working 42 24.1
Retired 89 51.2 PA and Cognitive Function
Other 43 24.7
Country of origin Table 2 shows the mean score and standard deviation for
USA 29 16.7 minutes of accelerometer-assessed PA and cognitive function
Mexico 95 54.6 domains. Of the 174 participants, 151 had valid accelerom-
Puerto Rico 24 13.8
Central American 2 1.1
eter data, 4 lost their accelerometers, 6 had no valid data due
South American 11 6.3 to accelerometer malfunctions, and 13 had only 1 or 2 days
Other/missing 13 7.5 of valid data. More minutes were spent in light-intensity
Acculturation levels than in moderate/vigorous-intensity PA. There were no sig-
Very Latino oriented 107 62.6
Latino to balanced bicultural 39 22.8
nificant differences on background characteristics or cogni-
Slightly Anglo bicultural 21 12.3 tive function tests between the participants who had valid
Strongly Anglo 4 2.3 accelerometer data and those who did not. Test scores from
Chronic health problems
one of the cognitive function domains (episodic memory)
0 52 30.1
1–2 121 69.9 had a negatively skewed distribution, whereas the compo-
Note. N = 174.
nents of executive function (interference control, inattention,
word fluency) were relatively symmetric.

tests that remained. At the end of the second visit, partici- Correlations Among Background Characteristics, PA, and
pants were given the oral and written results of their blood Cognitive Function
pressure, weight, and height information and PA informa- Age was significantly negatively correlated with light-
tion from the American Heart Association in Spanish or intensity PA, minutes of moderate/vigorous-intensity PA,
English. Participants were compensated $10 at the end of and activity counts (Table 2). Correlations of small magni-
the first appointment and received another $10 at the end of tude were found between chronic health problems and
the second appointment. minutes of light-intensity PA a day (r = −.221). There was a
correlation between sex and moderate/vigorous-intensity
Analyses PA, with men having more minutes in moderate/vigorous-
All analyses were conducted using SPSS V16.0. The intensity PA than women.
bivariate correlations between background/control measures Correlations were assessed between the PA measures and
and PA with the cognitive measures were conducted to cognitive measures. In addition, we looked at the composite
determine if further multiple regression analysis was justified. scores for episodic memory (Boston immediate and Boston
Next, a multivariate multiple regression was done with delay) and PA measures. The pattern of correlations for
episodic memory, interference control, inattention, and the the component and summary/composite scores for episodic
two-word fluency measures (animals and fruits and vegeta- memory was nearly identical across all three PA measures.
bles) as the dependent variables and the background and PA Consequently, the subsequent analyses examined only the
variables as predictors. This was followed by a canonical episodic composite.
Table 2. Correlations Among Background Characteristics, Physical Activity (PA), and Cognition
M (SD) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15.
Background characteristics
1. Age
2. Sexa −.012
3. Educationb −.268*** .000
4. Chronic health problems .296*** −.203* −.117
5. Acculturation −.020 −.010 .277*** .176*
PA
6. Lightc 259.4 (73.3) −.438*** .096 .139 −.221** .087
7. Moderate/vigorousc 31.2 (30.5) −.318*** −.255** −.036 −.093 .036 .321***
8. Counts per minute 267.0 (181.9) −.280*** −.112 −.022 −.151 .041 .368*** .865***
Cognition
9. Boston immediate 8.8 (2.0) −.000 .165* .054 −.121 .078 .085 .013 .014
10. Bostondelay 8.6 (2.0) −.023 .216** .076 −.117 .112 .091 .056 .067 .771***
6530 WILBUR
WILBUR

11. Episodic memory 8.7 (2.0) −.012 .203* .069 −.126 .101 .093 .037 .043 .939*** .943***
12. Interference control 14.8 (9.5) −.486*** .026 .258** −.140 .192* .234** .150 .137 .295*** .319*** .327***
13. Inattention 16.7 (7.3) −.311*** .002 .494*** −.086 .244** .119 .062 .058 .197* .139 .178* .495***
14. Number of animals 17.2 (4.5) −.247** .009 .244** −.040 .028 .253** .231** .240** .276** .277** .294*** .389*** .401***
15. Number of fruits and 17.1 (5.2) −.211** .306*** .204* −.164* .029 .246** .079 .114 .204* .263** .249** .334*** .341*** .574***
vegetables
16. Word fluency 34.3 (8.7) −.260** .165* .254** −.109 .032 .281*** .181* .205* .273*** .305*** .308*** .410*** .421*** .868*** .905***
Notes. n = 151.
a 1. = men; 2. = women.
b 1. = no high school degree; 2. = high school degree or higher.
c Minutes per day at named intensity.

*p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01; ***p ≤ .001.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


RELATIONSHIP
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN
BETWEEN PA
PA AND
AND COGNITION
COGNITION 5317

Table 3. The Correlations Between Background and Physical Table 3 also displays the extent to which the four func-
Activity (PA) Predictor Variables and Canonical Functions tions are correlated with cognitive functioning. Functions
Canonical functions 1 and 3 both predict interference control, and inattention
Predictor variables 1 2 3 4 Function 2 is most correlated with episodic memory and
Background word fluency (fruits and vegetables recalled). Function 4 is
Age .81 −.04 .54 .05 most highly correlated with the two measures of word flu-
Sex .05 −.92 −.12 .04
Education −.72 −.13 .63 −.10
ency, the number of fruit and vegetables recalled (r = −.44)
Chronic health problems .19 −.41 .18 .15 and the number of animals recalled (r =−.83). Function 4 is
Acculturation −.38 −.07 .21 .40 composed of the three measures of PA and one measure of
PA acculturation. From these results, it is clear that there is a

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


Light −.34 −.24 −.35 −.51
Moderate/vigorous −.23 −.13 −.23 −.56
relationship between PA and word fluency.
Counts per minute −.21 .05 −.23 −.62
Dependent variables
Episodic memory −.09 −.56 .04 .01 Discussion
Interference control −.83 −.08 −.45 .11 The relationship between PA and cognitive function was
Inattention −.84 −.12 .51 .03
examined in a sample of older Latinos who had low levels
Word fluency animals −.48 −.10 .00 −.83
Word fluency fruits and vegetables −.38 −.75 −.12 −.44 of education, were highly Latino acculturated, and most of
Note. Loadings above .40 are marked in bold.
whom were born in Mexico. These individuals were recruited
from predominantly Latino Chicago communities and rep-
resent a population for which little is known about their
Light-intensity PA had significant positive correlations cognition and its relationship to PA. Another distinctive fea-
with interference control. Minutes of light-intensity PA per ture of this study is that PA was assessed with accelerometers.
day, minutes of moderate/vigorous PA per day, and PA We know of only one other study to date that has used accel-
counts/minute all had significant positive correlations with erometers to measure PA and examined its relationship with
word fluency. PA was not significantly correlated with epi- cognition, and that study was restricted to a primarily
sodic memory or inattention. non-Latino White (92%) sample (Buchman et al., 2008).
Age had significant negative correlations, and education had Problems with misrepresentation of PA in Latinos using
significant positive correlations with interference control, inat- self-report PA measures are well-documented (Ham &
tention, and word fluency (animals, fruits, and vegetables), but Ainsworth, 2010). Although Latinos are shown to have
not with episodic memory. Women had higher scores on tests lower levels of leisure-time PA than non-Latino White par-
of episodic memory and word fluency. Chronic health prob- ticipants, recent evidence suggests that moderate/vigorous
lems were significantly negatively correlated with only number PA measured with an accelerometer is actually higher for
of fruits and vegetables. Acculturation had significant positive Latinos than for non-Latino Whites (Troiano et al., 2008).
correlations with both interference control and inattention but This is thought to be due to the inclusion of occupational
not with episodic memory or word fluency. and transportation PA captured with accelerometers. There-
fore, accelerometers may better capture the influence of
Regression of Cognitive Function on Background lifestyle PA on cognition in this population.
Characteristics and PA Overall, scores on the cognitive tests from the current
The overall multivariate regression analysis showed a sample are comparable to previous work of Krueger and
significant relationship among the background measures, colleagues (2009) with older, nondemented community-
PA measures, and dependent cognitive measures. For the based Latinos. With the exception of interference control,
multivariate regression, the approximate F-ratio, based on all cognitive function tests were slightly higher in our study
Pillai’s trace was 3.38 (degrees of freedom = 40, 710; p < (episodic memory: immediate recall 8.8 vs. 8.5, delayed
.001; Bock, 1975). From this analysis, four canonical func- recall 8.6 vs. 7.4; interference control: 14.8 vs. 15.0; inat-
tions were extracted, and they combined to account for tention: number comparison 16.7 vs. 13.3; word fluency:
82.7% of the variance associated with the five dependent category fluency 34.3 vs. 30.5). This slight discrepancy may
variables. The percent of variance associated with these be due to the lower age limit in our study compared with that
four functions was 36.4%, 18.5%, 9.8%, and 18.0%, respec- of Krueger and colleagues (2009; 50 vs. 55 years of age).
tively. Table 3 displays the correlations between the eight Mean PA counts of Latinos in our study (263) were compa-
predictor variables and the four functions. Age and education rable to NHANES findings for Mexican Americans 60 years
load on the first and third functions. Sex and chronic health of age and older (283.5 men; 214.6 women) but higher than
problems load on the second function. PA (minutes of light for non-Latino Whites (221.6 men; 208.7 women; Troiano
activity r = −.51, minutes of moderate PA r = −.56, counts et al., 2008). Participants in our study spent a lot of time per-
per minute r = −62) and acculturation (r = .40) load on the forming light-intensity activities, similar to older adult partici-
fourth function. pants in the senior quality-of-life study (Buman et al., 2010).
8532 WILBUR
WILBUR

We examined correlations between all study variables to provide cognitive benefits for word fluency as does moder-
identify congruence with prior studies on age, education, ate/vigorous PA. This could be particularly important for
sex, PA, and cognition. As expected, age was negatively older adults who, like participants in our study, tend to
correlated and education positively correlated with the spend more time participating in light-intensity than more
majority of the dimensions of cognitive function (Plassman vigorous activities. We must keep in mind that our estimates
et al., 2007). The lack of a correlation between age and epi- between moderate PA and cognition may be somewhat mis-
sodic memory was surprising. There is evidence suggesting leading because of the floor effects. It may be that increases
that frequent participation in cognitively stimulating activ- in moderate PA will lead to substantial increases in cogni-
ities is associated with reduced risk of dementia (Wilson tive functioning, but we were not able to find this relation-
et al., 2005). Therefore, we speculate that the rich tradition ship due to the limited range of moderate/vigorous PA in

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


of oral history that is characteristic of the Latino community this study. However, this needs further study using prospec-
may provide some protective effects on episodic memory tive research designs before substantive recommendations
(Caspe, 2009). can be made.
As demonstrated in prior studies, there was a negative Despite the unique findings, there are several limitations
correlation between chronic health problems and all cogni- to this study. A major limitation is that it is cross-sectional;
tive measures, but only one component of word fluency was thus, we cannot identify the true direction of associations
significant (Kodl & Seaquist, 2008; Plassman et al., 2007). between PA and cognitive function. Another limitation is
Findings related to sex have been unequivocal, with several that the study is composed of volunteers, so the findings have
earlier studies finding women to be at greater risk for to be cautiously generalized beyond the study population.
dementia than men (Bachman et al., 1992; Canadian Study The participants, however, were recruited from multiple
of Health and Aging Working Group, 1994), but recent U.S. communities in the Chicago area, thus reflecting those of
prevalence data found no sex differences in cognitive func- Mexican, Puerto Rican, and other Latino backgrounds.
tion (Plassman et al., 2007). We found women perform better The strengths of this study include the inclusion of highly
on episodic memory and word fluency. Consistent with Latino–acculturated participants who had the opportunity to
prior studies on PA and aging in Latinos, we found a signif- respond in their preferred language. Also, using accelerom-
icant negative correlation between age and PA, and men eters allowed us to assess lifestyle activity (i.e., activity dur-
tended to be more active than women (Troiano et al., 2008). ing leisure, work, homemaking, walking for transportation).
We selected tests that assess executive function because Unfortunately, the accelerometer does not allow us to
prior studies show it has the largest fitness-induced cogni- identify the specific types of activity in which participants
tive benefits (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). Accordingly, we engaged.
found PA (minutes of both light and moderate/vigorous PA) In summary, we found an association between higher levels
correlated significantly with two of the three domains of of PA and higher levels of cognitive function for word
executive function (word fluency and interference control). fluency in older Latinos similar to other cross-sectional and
Not surprisingly, there was no relationship with episodic epidemiological studies. Most importantly, we found that
memory, which does not tap into executive function. cognitive benefits may be gained from both light-intensity
Subsequently, in the regression analyses, only word fluency and moderate/vigorous activity. Longitudinal work needs to
retained a relationship with PA, and the effect was larger for be done to identify the effects of PA on the various domains
those with a more Latino orientation. It is uncertain why PA of executive function.
was more strongly related to rule-based retrieval of acquired
knowledge (identification of animals, fruits, vegetables) Funding
compared with other aspects of executive function such as This work was supported by the Rush University Medical Center Pilot
Funding.
interference control and numbers comparison. Comparing
our findings with studies of cohorts with healthy people at Acknowledgments
baseline was a challenge due to considerable variation in We acknowledge Janet Flores, research assistant, who assisted in
cognitive tests across studies and the use of single global recruitment and data collection. We thank Kevin Grandfield for editorial
and composite scores. There is a call for researchers to identify assistance.
a small group of standard measurement instruments to allow
Correspondence
for better comparison across studies (Snowden et al., 2011).
Correspondence should be addressed to JoEllen Wilbur, PhD, College
Our findings seem to suggest that some domains of executive of Nursing, Rush University Medical Center, 600 S Paulina, Suite 1062A,
function may be more susceptible to PA and that, not all, may Chicago, IL 60612. E-mail: joellen_wilbur@rush.edu.
be equally affected. Further research is needed in identifying
References
the effects of PA on specific domains of executive function.
Albert, M. S., Jones, K., Savage, C. R., Berkman, L., Seeman, T.,
We believe that this is one of the first studies to objec- Blazer, D., . . . Rowe, J. W. (1995). Predictors of cognitive change
tively measure light-intensity PA in relation to cognitive in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychol-
function. Our findings suggest that light-intensity PA may ogy and Aging, 10, 578–589. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.578
RELATIONSHIP
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN
BETWEEN PA
PA AND
AND COGNITION
COGNITION 5339

Albert, M. S., Smith, L. A., Scherr, P. A., Taylor, J. O., Evans, D. A., & Greenberg, S. (2009). A profile of older Americans: 2009. Retrieved from
Funkenstein, H. H. (1991). Use of brief cognitive tests to identify http://www.aoa.gov/AoAroot/Aging_Statistics/Profile/2009/docs/
individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s 2009profile_508.pdf
disease. International Journal of Neuroscience, 57, 167–178. Haan, M. N., Mungas, D. M., Gonzalez, H. M., Ortiz, T. A., Acharya, A., &
doi:10.3109/00207459109150691 Jagust, W. J. (2003). Prevalence of dementia in older Latinos: The
Alzheimer’s Association. (2010). 2010 Alzheimer’s disease facts and influence of type 2 diabetes mellitus, stroke and genetic factors. Journal
figures. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 6, 158–194. doi:10.1016/ of American Geriatrics Society, 51, 169–177. doi:10.1046/j.1532-
j.jalz.2010.01.009 5415.2003.51054.x
Bachman, D. L., Wolf, P. A., Linn, R., Knoefel, J. E., Cobb, J., Belanger, A., Ham, S. A., & Ainsworth, B. E. (2010). Disparities in data on Healthy
. . . White, L. R. (1992). Prevalence of dementia and probable People 2010 physical activity objectives collected by accelerometry
senile dementia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham Study. and self-report. American Journal of Public Health, 100, S263–S268.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


Neurology, 42, 115–119. Retrieved from http://www.neurology.org/ doi:10.2105/AJPH.2009.180075
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Buchman, A. S., Mendes de Leon, C., Hamer, M., & Chida, Y. (2009). Physical activity and risk of neurodegen-
Bienias, J. L., & Wilson, R. S. (2005). The rush memory and aging erative disease: A systematic review of prospective evidence. Psy-
project: Study design and baseline characteristics of the study cohort. chological Medicine, 39, 3–11. doi:10.1017/S0033291708003681
Neuroepidemiology, 25, 163–175. doi:10.1159/000087446 Hawkins, M. S., Storti, K. L., Richardson, C. R., King, W. C., Strath, S. J.,
Berrigan, D., Troiano, R. P., McNeel, T., DiSogra, C., & Ballard-Barbash, R. Holleman, R. G., & Kriska, A. M. (2009). Objectively measured
(2006). Active transportation increases adherence to activity recom- physical activity of USA adults by sex, age, and racial/ethnic groups:
mendations. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 31, 210–216. A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition
doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2006.04.007 and Physical Activity, 6, 31. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-6-31
Bock, R. D. (1975). Multivariate statistical methods in behavioral research. Hendelman, D., Miller, K., Baggett, C., Debold, E., & Freedson, P. (2000).
New York: McGraw-Hill. Validity of accelerometry for the assessment of moderate intensity
Brach, J. S., Kriska, A. M., Glynn, N. W., & Newman, A. B. (2008). Physical physical activity in the field. Medicine and Science in Sports and
activity and the older adult: Measurement, benefits, and risks. Current Exercise, 32, S442–S449. doi:10.1097/00005768-200009001-00002
Cardiovascular Reports, 2, 305–310. doi:10.1007/s12170-008-0056-6 Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise
Broe, G. A., Creasey, H., Jorm, A. F., Bennett, H. P., Casey, B., Waite, L. M., . . . your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews
Cullen, J. (1998). Health habits and risk of cognitive impairment and Neuroscience, 9, 58–65. doi:10.1038/nrn2298
dementia in old age: A prospective study on the effects of exercise, Ho, S. C., Woo, J., Sham, A., Chan, S. G., & Yu, A. L. (2003). A 3-year
smoking and alcohol consumption. Australian and New Zealand Journal follow-up study of social, lifestyle and health predictors of cognition
of Public Health, 22, 621–623. doi:10.1111/j.1467-842X.1998.tb01449.x impairment in a Chinese older cohort. International Journal of
Buchman, A. S., Wilson, R. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2008). Total daily activity is Epidemiology, 30, 1389–1396. doi:10.1093/ije/30.6.1389
associated with cognition in older persons. American Journal of Geri- Kodl, C. T., & Seaquist, E. R. (2008). Cognitive dysfunction and diabetes
atric Psychiatry, 16, 697–701. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e31817945f6 mellitus. Endocrine Reviews, 29, 494–511. doi:10.1210/er.2007-0034
Buman, M. P., Hekler, E. B., Haskell, W. L., Pruitt, L., Conway, T. L., Cain, Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., & Colcombe, S. J. (2006). Exercise, cognition,
K. L., . . . King, A. C. (2010). Objective light-intensity physical activity and the aging brain. Journal of Applied Physiology, 101, 1237–1242.
associations with rated health in older adults. American Journal of doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00500.2006
Epidemiology, 172, 1155–1165. doi:10.1093/aje/kwq249 Krueger, K. R., Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., & Aggarwal, N. T. (2009).
Canadian Study of Health and Aging Working Group. (1994). Canadian A battery of tests for assessing cognitive function in older Latino
study of health and aging: Study methods and prevalence of dementia. persons. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 23, 384–388.
Canadian Medical Association Journal, 150, 899–913. Retrieved doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e31819e0bfc
from http://www.cmaj.ca/ Lytle, M. E., Vander Bilt, J., Pandav, R. S., Dodge, H. H., & Ganguli, M.
Caspe, M. (2009). Low-income Latino mother’s booksharing styles and (2004). Exercise level and cognitive decline: The MoVIES project.
children’s emergent literacy development. Early Childhood Re- Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 18, 57–64. doi:00002093-
search Quarterly, 24, 306–324. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.03.006 200404000-00003
City of Chicago. (n.d.). Zoning and land use planning: Community Area Matthews, C. E., Ainsworth, B. E., Thompson, R. W., & Bassett, D. R., Jr.
2000 Census Profiles. Retrieved from http://www.cityofchicago.org/ (2002). Sources of variance in daily physical activity levels as
content/city/en/depts/dcd/supp_info/community_area_2000census measured by an accelerometer. Medicine and Science in Sports
profiles.html and Exercise, 34, 1376–1381. doi:10.1097/00005768-200208000-
Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive 00021
function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, Miller, N. E., Strath, S. J., Swartz, A. M., & Cashin, S. E. (2010). Estimating
14, 125–130. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430 absolute and relative physical activity intensity across age via accel-
Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating erometry in adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 18, 158–170.
Scale of Mexican Americans II—A revision of the original ARSMA Retrieved from http://journals.humankinetics.com/japa
Scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275–304. Murphy, K. R., Barkley, R. A., & Bush, T. (2001). Executive functioning
doi:10.1177/07399863950173001 and olfactory identification in young adults with attention deficit-
Ekstron, R. B., French, J. W., Harmen, H. H., & Kermen, D. (1976). Manual hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15, 211–220. doi:10.1037//
for kit of factor-referenced cognitive test. Princeton, NJ: Educational 0894-4105.15.2.211
Testing Service. Pickering, T. G., Hall, J. E., Appel, L. J., Falkner, B. E., Graves, J., Hill,
El Assaad, M. A., Topouchian, J. A., Darne, B. M., & Asmar, R. G. (2002). M. N., . . . Rocella, E. J. (2005). Recommendations for blood pressure
Validation of the Omron HEM-907 device for blood pressure mea- measurement in humans and experimental animals: Part 1: Blood pres-
surement. Blood Pressure Monitoring, 7, 237–241. doi:10.1097/ sure measurement in humans: A statement for professionals from the
00126097-200208000-00006 Subcommittee of Professional and Public Education of the American
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. Heart Association Council on High Blood Pressure Research. Hyper-
A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the tension, 45, 142–161. doi:10.1161/01.CIR.0000154900.76284.F6
clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. doi:10.1016/ Plassman, B. L., Langa, K. M., Fisher, G. G., Heeringa, S. G., Weir, D. R.,
0022-3956(75)90026-6 Ofstedal, M. B., . . . Wallace, R. B. (2007). Prevalence of dementia in
534 WILBUR
10 WILBUR

the United States: The aging, demographics, and memory study. Trenerry, M., Crosson, B., DeBoe, J., & Leber, W. (1989). Stroop Neuro-
Neuroepidemiology, 29, 125–132. doi:10.1159/000109998 psychological Screening Test manual. Adessa, FL: Psychological
Raji, M. A., Al Snih, S., Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., & Ottenbacher, K. J. Assessment Resources (PAR).
(2010). Cognitive status and future risk of frailty in older Mexican Troiano, R. P., Berrigan, D., Dodd, K. W., Masse, L. C., Tilert, T., &
Americans. The Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological McDowell, M. (2008). Physical activity in the United States measured
Sciences and Medical Sciences, 65, 1228–1234. doi:10.1093/gerona/ by accelerometer. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 40,
glq121 181–188. doi:10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a51b3
Richards, M., Hardy, R., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2003). Does active leisure Tryon, W. W., & Williams, R. S. (1996). Fully proportional actigraphy:
protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Social A new instrument. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and
Science and Medicine, 56, 785–792. doi:S0277953602000758 Computers, 28, 392–403. doi:10.3758/BF03200519
Roger, V. L., Go, A. S., Lloyd-Jones, D. M., Adams, R. J., Berry, J. D., U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2008). 2008 Physical

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/67/5/525/657933 by guest on 05 May 2019


Brown, T. M., . . . Wylie-Rosett, J. (2011). Heart disease and stroke activity guidelines for Americans. Washington DC: Author.
statistics—2011 update: A report from the American Heart Asso- van Gelder, B. M., Tijhuis, M. A., Kalmijn, S., Giampaoli, S., Nissinen, A., &
ciation. Circulation, 123, e18–e209. doi:10.1161/CIR.0b013e3 Kromhout, D. (2004). Physical activity in relation to cognitive
182009701 decline in elderly men: The FINE Study. Neurology, 63, 2316–2321.
Schuit, A. J., Feskens, E. J., Launer, L. J., & Kromhout, D. (2001). Physi- doi:10.1016/S0197-4580(04)81579-3
cal activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein Welsh, K. A., Butters, N., Mohs, R. C., Beekly, D., Edland, S., Fillenbaum,
e4 allele. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 33, 772–777. G., & Heyman, A. (1994). The consortium to establish a registry for
Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/acsm-msse/pages/default. Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the
aspx neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 44, 609–614.
Singh-Manoux, A., Richards, M., & Marmot, M. (2003). Leisure activities Weuve, J., Kang, J. H., Manson, J. E., Breteler, M. M., Ware, J. H., &
and cognitive function in middle age: Evidence from the Whitehall II Grodstein, F. (2004). Physical activity, including walking, and cogni-
study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57, 907–913. tive function in older women. The Journal of the American Medical
doi:10.1136/jech.57.11.907 Association, 292, 1454–1461. doi:10.1001/jama.292.12.1454
Snowden, M., Steinman, L., Mochan, K., Grodstein, F., Prohaska, T. R., Wilson, R. S., Barnes, L. L., Krueger, K. R., Hoganson, G., Bienias, J. L., &
Thurman, D. J., . . . Anderson, L. A. (2011). Effect of exercise Bennett, D. A. (2005). Early and late life cognitive activity and cog-
on cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults: nitive systems in old age. Journal of the International Neuropsycho-
Review of intervention trials and recommendations for public health logical Society, 11, 400–407. doi:10.1017/S1355617705050459
practice and research. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 59, Wilson, R. S., Beckett, L. A., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A., Bach, J.,
704–716. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03323.x Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2002). Individual differences in rates
Sturman, M. T., Morris, M. C., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Bienias, J. L., of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychology and
Wilson, R. S., & Evans, D. A. (2005). Physical activity, cognitive Aging, 17, 179–193. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.17.2.179
activity, and cognitive decline in a biracial community population. Wilson, R. S., Mendes De Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A.,
Archives of Neurology, 62, 1750–1754. doi:10.1001/archneur.62. Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2002). Participation in
11.1750 cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer dis-
Tremblay, M. S., Bryan, S. N., Peréz, C. E., Ardern, C. I., & Katzmarzyk, ease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 742–748.
P. T. (2006). Physical activity and immigrant status: Evidence from doi:10.1001/jama.287.6.742
the Canadian Community Health Survey. Canadian Journal of Public Yaffe, K., Blackwell, T., Gore, R., Sands, L., Reus, V., & Browner, W. S.
Health, 97, 277–282. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ (1999). Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in nondemented
entrez / query. fcgi ? cmd = Retrieve & db = PubMed & dopt = Citation & elderly women: A prospective study. Archives of General Psychiatry,
list_uids=16967745 56, 425–430. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.5.425

You might also like