You are on page 1of 8

Update: Applications of

Research in Music Education


Volume 27 Number 1
November 2008 41-48

Creativity Research in Music Education © 2008 MENC: The National


Association for Music Education
10.1177/8755123308322280
http://update.sagepub.com
hosted at
A Review (1980–2005) http://online.sagepub.com

Donald J. Running
Bridgewater State College

This article lays a foundational groundwork of what is currently known regarding creativity and music education to
encourage future research. It explores principal research avenues within various scholarly journals related to creativity and
music education, including definitions of creativity, empirical measures of creativity, and effects of music instruction on
general creativity scores. Definitions (and, consequently, assessments) of creativity fall into three general categories:
product based, process based, and performance based. These definitions have generated a number of new theories and
tests designed to assess the creativity of products and individuals.

Keywords: music; education; creativity; measures; measurement; testing

C reativity is a much discussed and debated topic in


music education today. Researchers have explored
what it is, if it can be assessed, if it can affect (or is
There has been much research into creativity in the
25 years since Richardson’s review; it is time to reex-
amine how far research in this area has progressed.
attached to) other areas of academic study, and if it can As an extension to Richardson’s examination
be taught. Creativity has been explored quantitatively, (1983), this review is limited to articles written for
qualitatively, historically, and philosophically. Much of scholarly journals published between 1980 and 2005.
this research into creativity was brought about by the The studies under consideration are categorized by
decision to include improvisation and composition in research topic. The first section discusses various defi-
the National Standards for Arts Education (Consortium nitions of creativity. The second examines various
of National Arts Education Associations, 1994). empirical measures of musical creativity, and the third
Empirical assessment based on No Child Left Behind explores the effects of music instruction on general cre-
makes exploration of the content and context of cre- ativity scores. Although every attempt was made to be
ativity as important as ever (Moran, 2005). This article comprehensive, this review may not be all-inclusive.
represents an exploration into the research based on
definitions and measurements of creativity in music
education from 1980 until 2005. Definitions of Creativity
Richardson’s previous review of literature (1983)
relating to creativity and music education explored A formal agreement regarding the definition of cre-
research from 1922 until 1979, with the bulk of the ativity has eluded scientists, artists, philosophers, and
presented material between 1962 and 1979. Since researchers alike—from Socrates to Csikszentmihalyi.
1980, creativity has remained a topic that has gener- Much of the more recent research has focused on
ated much interest, research, and a little controversy. deciding (or defining) exactly what creativity is and
Authors such as Henry (1996) and Rohwer (1997) have who has it. This exploration had led researchers such
completed more recent reviews in creative composition as Balkin (1990) to claim that creativity is “overused,
and assessment of creativity. Yet a comprehensive misused, confused, abused, and generally misunder-
exploration is overdue that targets contemporary studies stood” (p. 29). Is creativity a gift, or can it be devel-
regarding how creativity and music education relate. oped? Can it be learned? Can it be taught? The

Donald J. Running is a assistant professor of bands at Bridgewater State College. Address correspondence to Donald J. Running, Maxwell
Library, Room 312G, 10 Shaw Road, Bridgewater, MA 02325; e-mail: DRunning@bridgew.edu.

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


41
42 Update

following section explores these questions and appears in the research of Oehrle (1986) and Hickey
describes what is thought about creativity and music and Webster (2001).
education. Researchers such as Perkins (1981), The importance of product is included in the
Weisberg (1986), Balkin (1990), and Clark (1986) research of Amabile (1983), Symes (1983), Tang and
have put forth a variety of explanations regarding Leonard (1985), and Clark (1986). Amabile suggested
who is creative and what creativity is. that “a product or response is creative to the extent that
The theories of creativity put forth by Perkins appropriate observers independently agree it is cre-
(1981) and Weisberg (1986) stated that creativity is a ative. Appropriate observers are those familiar with the
process of application of knowledge, logical reason- domain in which the product was created or the
ing, memory recovery, and visualization. Both response articulated” (p. 359). This consensual assess-
authors related artistic creativity to puzzle solving ment has become the cornerstone of much research
(i.e., Person A asks Person B a question and confirms into the evaluation of creative work. Clark (1986)
or denies the answer until Person B answers cor- added that for the product to be considered creative, it
rectly). In his 1990 retort, Leddy (1990) played the must possess descriptive and evaluative functions that
role of devil’s advocate by offering several contradic- are novel and deemed desirable by society. Because
tions and alternatives to the authors’ view of creativ- society is ultimately responsible for determining
ity. Leddy believed that true creativity requires more whether an individual is creative (through the product
than the sum of a person’s prior knowledge and a that one delivers), creativity is (in part) taken out of the
novel means of applying it. hands of the creator. Tang and Leonard (1985) added
Contra Perkins (1981) and Weisberg (1986), Balkin that the truly creative product must be the “unique
(1990) defined creativity through a process of con- solution to a problem” (p. 7). In musical terms, this
trasting it with other words and concepts commonly problem is the need for a product that will be consid-
confused with creativity. For example, Balkin com- ered original and desirable.
pared creativity with talent, by explaining that talent is Elliott (1989, 1995) described creativity as a
an innate and unlearnable gift whereas creativity is an family of concepts that are confused with originality.
acquired and developable behavior. Talented people Elliott stated that one engages in a creative process
are often creative, and creative people can be talented, through actions such as composing and improvising,
but there is no causal correlation that exists between which result in a final product. This product will not
these two concepts. Balkin also contrasted creativity be considered creative unless there is a substantial
with originality (i.e., flamboyant or bizarre behavior), level of quality (determined in a sociocultural envi-
IQ, and cleverness. Again, creative people often pos- ronment). This theory led to Elliott’s head-and-shoul-
sess these characteristics but not necessarily so. In fact, ders model. Elliott believed that creative products are
although theorists differ on their views of what consti- a combination of the familiar and the unfamiliar
tutes a creative person, a general agreement exists—as based on a network of the creator’s prior musical
summarized by Balkin—that creative people tend to experience. Consider, for example (as Elliot does),
demonstrate certain characteristics (e.g., confidence, Beethoven’s Eroica Symphony. Beethoven did not
intellectually playful, persistent). invent symphonic music or create a bizarre work of
If creativity is not directly connected to talent, IQ, art. Beethoven created a timeless work of quality art
originality, or cleverness, then what is it? Balkin that combines the ordinary and the extraordinary on
(1990) claimed that what separates creativity from the “shoulders” of past creations.
simple spontaneity is a final result that represents an Johnson-Laird (1987) hypothesized that creativity
important contribution to society—a product. This can be expressed as an algorithm (a limited set of
product is the result of a four-stage process (first pos- instructions). To demonstrate this point, Johnson-
tulated in 1926 by Wallas). The first step involves Laird designed a computer program that would test
preparation wherein one gathers the necessary infor- three creative algorithms by “improvising” jazz bass
mation and skills needed to complete the task. This is lines, melodies, and chord sequences. These pro-
followed by an incubation period in which one allows grams were instructed to use the grammar (existing
the unconscious to develop ideas and concepts. motifs) of jazz to create improvisations. These com-
Incubation is followed by an illumination or eureka puter-generated improvisations varied widely in qual-
moment where the great idea is formed. The last ity and led to Johnson-Laird’s describing the creative
stage involves one’s verification of this great idea process of a successful improvisation as having two
through time and testing. This four-stage process also stages: First, combine and modify components within

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


Running / Creativity Research in Music Education 43

the limit of the constraints; second, make an impul- understanding of what creativity is and how it can be
sive decision from the available options. developed. The latter half of the 20th century has
Not only is there controversy over what creativity been an exciting time for the development of empiri-
is, but numerous music educators have inquired cal measures of creativity in music. The Torrance
whether creativity can be taught. Clark (1986) Tests of Creative Thinking (Torrance, 1966) and the
explains that the product of creativity is not a separate structure of intellect model (Guilford & Hoepfner,
action from the creative processes any more than 1971) did much to standardize the evaluation of cre-
winning a race is not a separate act from running the ativity. Much of the research from 1980 to the present
race. Educators may teach all the tools necessary for has been based on the foundational work of these pio-
a student to be creative, but this does not directly neers. Thus, this section examines the work of other
yield a creative product. However, Clark also believes researchers who have developed means to calculate
that “much that students learn from their teachers, and quantifiably assess creativity.
and especially in the area of values and dispositions, Webster’s investigations (1990) into measuring
is learned by example” and that “this is especially creativity have been highly influential on other
likely to be true in teaching creativity” (p. 31). This researchers’ attempts to explore and quantify creativ-
belief in the importance of a teacher’s openness to ity. Originally published in 1980, Webster’s Measure
personal creativity is shared by others, including of Creative Thinking in Music (MCTM) is directly
DeTurk (1989) and Harris and Hawksley (1989). related to the prior research of Guilford and Hoepfner
Gordon (1993) stated that the degree to which a (1971), Vaughan (1973), Gordon (1993). The MCTM
child is creative is directly dependent on the child’s is designed to evaluate the musical creativity and
tonal and rhythmic vocabulary. Morin (2002) agreed expressivity of children (ages 6–10) by engaging
and explored teaching composition through the them in a 10-task guided-improvisatory session last-
expansion of the student’s base of knowledge. Morin ing 20–25 min. Their work is then scored by one or
suggested that for students to display creativity, they more judges. Based on roughly 300 students, the
must have a fundamental knowledge of melody, har- interscorer reliability has been found to average .70.
mony, and rhythm. Morin offered examples of her Hickey and Webster (1999) reviewed and
method in action. improved Webster’s previous work with the MCTM
Numerous educators have forwarded theories of by incorporating a MIDI-based instrument into the
how to incorporate creativity into the classroom. The test (i.e., musical instrument digital interface), princi-
majority of these activities are in the areas of impro- pally to make judging results both easier and more
visation (Addison, 1988; Fratia, 2002; Hickey, consistent. Participants for their research consisted of
1997a; Nolan, 1995; Rooke, 1990) and composition third-grade students (N = 28), and like the original
(Collins, 2005; Dunbar-Hall, 1999; Reynolds, 2002; MCTM, this instrument was designed to test for cre-
Stauffer, 2001, 2002; Wiggins, 1999; Wilson, 2001). ative elements that might not appear on other stan-
Other researchers have explored ways in which the dardized tests or teacher ratings. Hickey and Webster
curriculum may be structured to promote creativity not only improved reliability for the MTCM but also
(Byrne, 2002; Davidson, 1990; Kratus, 1990; Moore, simplified its administration.
1990; Sullivan, 2002). Finally, a great deal has been Schmidt and Sinor (1986) employed Webster’s
written about the use of technology to aid in the MCTM along with Gordon’s Primary Measures of
teaching of creativity (Demonline, 1999; Howell & Musical Audiation (1979) and Kagan’s Matching
Murphy, 1993; Pike, 2000; Reese, 2001). Familiar Figures (Kagan, Roseman, Day, Albert, &
Clearly, a great deal of thought, time, and research Philips, 1964) to investigate whether creative
has been undertaken in the name of creativity. As achievement in convergent and divergent musical
such, the next section examines various methods in assignments is connected with the cognitive dimen-
which musical creativity has been analyzed and sions of reflection/impulsivity. The researchers
quantified since 1980. employed second-grade students (N = 34) as their
participants, and although they discovered that 15%
of the variance of the Primary Measures of Musical
Empirical Measures of Musical Creativity Audiation can be attributed to reflection/impulsivity,
they did not find any significant relationships
Research into a means of quantifying the creative between reflection/impulsivity and the various
value of an individual or a work is essential to the dimensions of musical creativity.

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


44 Update

Like Webster (1990), Gorder (1980) used the three overarching categories: product-centered
research of Guilford and Hoepfner (1971) and measurements, process-centered measurements, and
Torrance (1966) to design a new empirical method of performance-centered measurements (a combination
evaluating creative work. Gorder’s Measures of of product and process).
Musical Divergent Production evaluates, among
instrumental music students, the divergent abilities of Product-centered methods of measuring creativity
music fluency (producing musical ideas from sup- in music. Amabile (1983) put forward perhaps the
plied music information), flexibility (producing most influential independent empirical measure of
musical ideas that emphasize shifts in musical char- creativity. Amabile believed that appropriate
acter, such as from staccato to legato), originality observers (i.e., experts within the field) are able to
(producing musical ideas that emphasize musical independently judge the creative merit of a product
concepts rarely found in the overall population), elab- and that by examining the analysis of multiple
oration (producing musical ideas emphasizing detail experts, one can have a reasonably reliable measure
or complexity), and quality (producing musical ideas of the creative value of a product. Amabile called this
that are musically desirable). The Measures of process of evaluation consensual assessment,
Musical Divergent Production was administered to founded on the concept that the most valid means of
80 randomly selected junior high school and high judging creativity is through the subjective assess-
school students, and Gorder discovered that a ments of experts within the domain.
student’s ability to improvise and one’s musical cre- Hickey (1997b) employed this consensual assess-
ativity rating served as significant predictors for flex- ment model when she explored a possible relation-
ibility (R2 = .301) and elaboration (R2 = .433). ship between a child’s musical explorations and the
Although these two variables, when combined with quality of his or her compositions. Participants for
age, were substantial predictors of quality (R2 = this study were fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade
.573), the musical creativity rating provided only a students (N = 21) working on MIDI keyboards con-
weak predictor of originality (R2 = .168). nected to computers. These students were efficiently
The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking divided into groups based on their ability level (low,
(Torrance, 1966), along with the Vaughan Test of middle, and high) as determined by a panel of experts
Musical Creativity (Vaughan, 1973), appeared in the evaluating the students’ creative work. An analysis of
research of Kiehn (2003), who studied students (N = the various products demonstrated that what sepa-
89) in Grades 2, 4, and 6 on two measures of creativ- rated students in the high-ability level from the mid-
ity. The results of the Vaughan test were scored by dle- and low-ability levels was an advanced ability to
expert judges to determine the creative quality of the manipulate and experiment with musical motifs, as
students’ improvisations. Keihn discovered that boys well as invent new musical ideas quickly.
scored significantly higher than girls (p < .05); they Amabile’s work on creativity (1983) resurfaced in
also found significant differences between grades another study by Hickey (2001), who tested Amabile’s
(p < .01). A Tukey test revealed a significant differ- consensual assessment technique on the musical com-
ence between Grade 2 and Grades 4 and 6 but no sig- positions of fourth- and fifth-grade students. Five inde-
nificant difference between Grades 4 and 6. Kiehn pendent groups of judges examined the students’
stated that there is a leveling of creativity that occurs compositions and discovered the consensual assess-
between Grades 4 and 6 and that boys scored signifi- ment to be a reasonably reliable means of judging
cantly higher on the tests for fluency, originality, and student creativity, particularly when the most knowl-
composite scores. edgeable judges are employed. Of the five groups of
judges, general music/choral teachers displayed the
highest interactive agreement for creative quality (.81),
Other Methods for Evaluation of Creativity
whereas second-grade students and mixed-experienced
Although certain researchers have defined creativity teachers (i.e., teachers with experience in instrumental
as being fundamentally product centered (Amabile, and vocal/general music) displayed the least agreement
1983; Balkin, 1990; Elliott, 1995), others believe that (.50 and .53, respectively). Interestingly, composers
creativity is process centered (see Johnson-Laird, had such little agreement (.04) that their input was
1987). These differing concepts of creativity have lead removed from the study.
to multiple means of measurement and quantification. Priest (2001) designed an experiment in which
This review categorizes various methodologies into undergraduate students (N = 54) who were enrolled

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


Running / Creativity Research in Music Education 45

in a foundational music course were assigned three Performance-centered methods of measuring cre-
composition projects. The final project was judged by ativity in music. Madura (1996) undertook an investi-
an independent panel of eight judges and was rated gation into how creativity can be a predictor of a
for perceived creativity. The projects were then student’s achievement level in vocal jazz improvisa-
divided into three groups of 18 based on the per- tion. Madura measured 18 elements in vocal impro-
ceived quality of the works (i.e., low, middle, and visations in college students (N = 101) who were
high). The participants then completed Creativity and studying vocal jazz. The results of this study suggest
Craftsmanship Assessments to describe five composi- that the most significant predictors of a student’s abil-
tions from a previous class. Priest discovered that the ities to improvise in a vocal jazz style were as
students in the high-creativity group tended to describe follows: jazz experience, knowledge of jazz theory,
music in temporal terms (statements relating to musical and imitative ability. Madura suggested that in teach-
transformations over time) whereas those in the middle ing vocal jazz improvisation, emphasis should be
and low groups tended to use metaphors and similes placed on jazz theory, imitative exercises, and meth-
(describing the music using nonmusical descriptors). ods to increase a student’s jazz experiences (such as
critical listening). Madura also emphasized the
Process-centered methods of measuring creativity importance of attending and giving live jazz perfor-
in music. Another school of research in creativity and mances. General creativity, as measured by the
music education focuses on the cognitive and experi- Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, was not found to
mental processes that lead to creativity. Wang’s be a significant predictor of achievement in vocal
Measures of Creativity in Sound and Music (MCSM; jazz. Laczó (1981) discovered similar results and
1985) was designed to measure “the fluency and claimed that the quality of a student’s ability to
imagination factors of divergent thinking skills of improvise is primarily determined by musical experi-
lower-elementary grade children” (para. 1) and was ences and education. Age was found to be signifi-
based on the model of the Torrance Tests of Creative cantly less important than other factors when
Thinking. This four-activity test is designed for examining creative improvisation.
students between the ages of 4 and 8 and has proven It is important to note that the majority of mea-
to be a consistent measure of a student’s ability to sures described above were designed with elementary
demonstrate fluency and imagination. Currently, the school children as participants. As such, Lapp and
MCSM remains unpublished. Lungren (2000) reminded educators that teaching
The reliability of Wang’s MCSM (1985) was the creativity should not be limited to elementary
focus of a study by Baltzer (1988), who randomly students. They cited a high school’s song-writing
selected male and female participants (N = 32) from course for increases in individual and group partici-
eight second-grade classrooms to take the MCSM. pation. Class participation also greatly expanded the
The scores of these students were tabulated by two students’ knowledge of music.
judges and correlated with Likert-type questions
administered to the students’ classroom and music
instructors to estimate the students’ musical and gen- Effects of Music Instruction on
eral creativity. The results of the MCSM were also General Creativity Scores
correlated with the students’ Stanford Achievement
Test scores, age (in months), and sex. Baltzer indi- Another area for investigation in creativity and
cated that there were no significant differences within music education involves the effect that formal music
the MCSM scores for the variables of age or sex and training has on general creativity. Hamman,
that ratings given by teachers for individual creativity Bourassa, and Aderman (1990) investigated whether
displayed a higher correlation with Stanford undergraduate music majors are significantly more
Achievement Test scores than with MCSM scores. creative than nonmusic majors, as measured by the
Interjudge reliability coefficient scores for imagina- Consequences Form A-1 (designed to test one’s abil-
tion and fluency were .90 and .99, respectively, and ity to find original ideas out of new and unusual cir-
inter-item coefficients fluxuated between .83 and .92. cumstances) of Guilford and Guilford’s test for
Baltzer concluded that the MCSM is a valid measure individual creativity (1980). The researchers discov-
of musical creativity in elementary schools and can ered that the scores of the music majors were signifi-
be a valuable tool in future research into musical cantly higher (p < .02) than the scores of the nonmusic
creativity. majors. Significantly higher scores (p < .02) were also

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


46 Update

discovered among those of students who had more that student composers who were most actively
than 10 years of arts experiences. In 1991, Hamann engaged in the task produced significantly (p < .01)
and Aderman replicated their study using high school more creative work.
students (N = 144) to determine the extent to which
their creativity scores were correlated with gender,
grade point average, and participation in the arts Conclusions and Suggestions
(jazz, music, art, theater, and combined experiences). for Future Research
The researchers found no significant differences
between creativity scores by gender, visual arts, jazz, There is a large body of research regarding cre-
or combined arts. In fact, grade point average repre- ativity and music education. The research described
sented nearly all the variation between these scores. here represents selected studies conducted subse-
Those who rated themselves as low and moderate in quent to 1980. As mentioned, research before that
musical experiences had significantly lower scores year has been reviewed by Richardson (1983). Much
than those who rated themselves as high. research remains to be done on measuring musical
Luftig (2000) designed an experiment to deter- creativity. The majority of the tests cited in this
mine whether second-, fourth-, and fifth-grade review that relate directly to the assessment and mea-
students would be positively affected—in terms of surement of creativity were designed between the
creativity, scholastics, self-esteem, and locus of con- years 1980 and 1986. Reexamining previous methods
trol—by being enrolled in a schoolwide SPECTRA+ for measuring creativity is important for increased
program, an interdisciplinary arts education program application and reliability, and technological
emphasizing improved performance in all facets of a advancements in computer hardware and software
student’s education through arts integration. offer potential for new methods for evaluating cre-
Participants (N = 615) were separated into two con- ativity. New and established methods of evaluating
trol groups and one experimental group and given a creativity should be employed to explore the appro-
pretest before 2 days of instruction within the SPEC- priateness of using product-centered methods of mea-
TRA+ environment. Following the instruction period, suring creativity in music versus process-centered or
a posttest was given to all participants. Luftig con- performance-centered methods. Also, it is important
cluded that the SPECTRA+ program had a signifi- to determine whether there are relationships between
cant impact (p < .019) on the students’ creativity students and their scores on product-centered mea-
scores (as measured using the Torrance Tests of sures of creativity and their scores on process-
Creative Thinking), particularly in the areas of elabo- centered and performance-centered methods.
ration (p < .01) and originality (p < .01). Luftig An additional avenue should be an attempt to
displayed mixed results for scholastic ability, self- determine whether creativity is something that can be
esteem, and locus of control. taught, and if so, the best methods for teaching it
Byrne, MacDonald, and Carlton (2003) used should be established. Madura (1996) found creativ-
Csikszentmihalyi’s concept of flow (a sense of opti- ity in vocal jazz improvisation to be related to knowl-
mal and effortless involvement in an engaging activ- edge of theory and jazz experience—both teachable
ity) in combination with student compositions to see concepts. Similarly, Luftig (2000) linked creativity to
whether there were any correlations between creativ- classroom participation in a schoolwide arts program.
ity and engagement in the process. University Conversely, Hamann and Aderman (1991) found cre-
students (N = 45) were separated into small groups ativity in high school students to be most correlated
and asked to select one of three musical stimuli to with general grade point averages.
build their compositions around. Following group Creativity is a vital factor within the context of a
compositional sessions, the students filled out complete education in music. The research investi-
Experience Sampling Forms to help them reflect on gated here covers an extensive assortment of feasible
the process of writing music—specifically, how they topics and has practical applications to current musi-
felt while accomplishing the compositional activity. cal instruction. Amabile’s technique of consensual
A panel of experts evaluated the compositions for cre- assessment (1983) provides an excellent means of
ativity and found significant agreement (r = .76, p < judging the relative creativity of musical products (see
.01) for compositional creativity. When these creativ- also, Hickey, 1997b, 2001). In the process of teaching and
ity scores were compared to the scores of students’ evaluating creativity, classroom teachers could readily
Experience Sampling Forms, the researchers found employ consensual assessment. Another practical

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


Running / Creativity Research in Music Education 47

measure of creativity, Hickey and Webster’s updated Gordon, E. E. (1979). Primary measures of music audiation.
version of the MCTM (1999), was redesigned to be Chicago: GIA.
Gordon, E. E. (1993). Learning sequences in music: Skill, con-
more accurate and easier to administer. This new ver-
tent, and patterns. Chicago: GIA.
sion of the MCTM could assist with the longitudinal Guilford, J. P., & Guilford, J. S. (1980). Consequences: Manual
exploration of how a student’s creativity changes of instructions and interpretations. Orange, CA: Sheridan
from a young age through high school or college. Psychological Services.
Although there is still much to be learned about Guilford, J. P., & Hoepfner, R. (1971). The analysis of intelli-
developing the creative abilities of student musicians, gence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hamann, D., & Aderman, M. (1991). Arts experiences and cre-
it is my hope that this examination of the literature ativity scores of high school students. Contributions to Music
will lead to superior teaching methods and to the Education, 18, 36–47.
establishment of an environment that develops Hamann, D. L., Bourassa, R., & Aderman, M. (1990). Creativity
students’ capabilities as independent music learners. and the arts. Dialogue in Instrumental Music Education,
14(2), 59–68.
Harris, R., & Hawksley, E. (1989). Composing in the classroom.
References Australian Journal of Music Education, 1, 82–83.
Henry, W. (1996). Creative processes in children’s musical com-
Addison, R. (1988). A new look at musical improvisation in edu- positions: A review of the literature. Update: Applications of
cation. British Journal of Music Education, 5(3), 255–267. Research in Music Education, 15(1), 10–15.
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New Hickey, M. (1997a). Teaching ensembles to compose and impro-
York: Springer-Verlag. vise. Music Educators Journal, 83, 17–21.
Balkin, A. (1990). What is creativity? What is it not? Music Hickey, M. (1997b). Understanding children’s musical creative
Educator’s Journal, 76, 29–32. thinking process through qualitative analysis of their MIDI
Baltzer, S. (1988). A validation study of a measure of musical data. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education,
creativity. Journal of Research in Music Education, 36(4), 131, 29–30.
232–249. Hickey, M. (2001). An application of Amabile’s consensual
Byrne, C. (2002). A spider’s web of intrigue. In L. R. Bartel assessment technique for rating the creativity of children’s
(Ed.), Creativity and music education (pp. 195–205). Toronto, musical compositions, Journal of Research in Music
Ontario, Canada: Britannia. Education, 49(3), 234–244.
Byrne, C., MacDonald, R., & Carlton, L. (2003). Assessing cre- Hickey, M., & Webster, P. (1999). MIDI-based adaptation and
ativity in musicalcompositions: Flow as an assessment tool. continued validation of the Measures of Creative Thinking in
British Journal of Music Education, 20(3), 277–290. Music (MCTM). Bulletin of the Council for Research in
Clark, W. H., Jr. (1986). Some thoughts on creativity. Journal of Music Education, 142, 93–94.
Aesthetic Education, 20(4), 27–31. Hickey, M., & Webster, P. (2001). Creative thinking in music:
Collins, D. (2005). A synthesis process model of creative thinking Rather than focusing on training children to be creative, it
in music composition. Psychology in Music, 33(2), 193–216. might be better for music teachers to nurture children’s inher-
Consortium of National Arts Education Associations. (1994). The ent ability to think creatively in music. Music Educators
National Standards for Arts Education. Reston, VA: MENC. Journal, 88, 19–23.
Davidson, L. (1990). Tools and environments for musical cre- Howell, S., & Murphy, K. (1993). Proceedings, the 68th annual
ativity. Music Educators Journal, 76, 47–51. meeting: Meeting of Region Three—Creative and critical
Demonline, K. (1999). Educational applications of Band-in-a- thinking: Can music technology assist both? National
Box: As accompaniment, as creative tool, and as aid for teach- Association of Schools of Music, 81, 103–112.
ing styles of music. Canadian Music Educator, 41, 29–30. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Reasoning, imagining and creating.
DeTurk, M. (1989). Critical and creative musical thinking. In Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 95, 71–87.
E. Boardman (Ed.), Dimensions of musical thinking (pp. Kagan, J., Roseman, B. L., Day, D., Albert, J., & Philips, W.
21–32). Reston, VA: MENC. (1964). Information processing in the child: Significance of
Dunbar-Hall, P. (1999). Composition as the site of music teach- analytic and reflective attitudes. Psychological Monographs,
ing: Pre-service students’ attitudes to teaching through cre- 78(1), 1-37.
ative activities. Australian Journal of Music Education, 1, Kiehn, M. T. (2003). Development of music creativity among ele-
44–62. mentary school students. Journal of Research in Music
Elliott, D. J. (1989). The concept of creativity. In J. W. Richmond Education, 51(4), 278–288.
(Ed.), The proceedings of the suncoast music education forum Kratus, J. (1990). Structuring the music curriculum for creative
on creativity (pp. 14–39). Tampa: University of South Florida. learning. Music Educators Journal, 76, 33–37.
Elliott, D. (1995). Music matters: A new philosophy of music edu- Laczó, Z. (1981). A psychological investigation of improvisation
cation. New York: Oxford University Press. abilities in the lower and higher classes of the elementary
Fratia, M. A. (2002). The creative link: An introduction to jazz school. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music
improvisation. Canadian Music Educator, 43, 16–17. Education, 66–67, 39–45.
Gorder, W. D. (1980). Divergent production abilities as con- Lapp, D., & Lungren, L. (2000). Musical creativity: Exclusively
structs of musical creativity. Journal of Research in Music an elementary school concept? American Music Teacher, 50,
Education, 28, 34–42. 35–36.

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016


48 Update

Leddy, T. (1990). Is the creative process in art a form of puzzle Rohwer, D. A. (1997). The challenges of teaching and assessing
solving? Journal of Aesthetic Education, 24(3), 83–97. creative activities. Update: Applications of Research in Music
Luftig, R. L. (2000). An investigation of an arts infusion program Education, 15(2), 8–12.
on creative thinking, academic achievement, affective func- Rooke, M. (1990). Technique and creativity, par 4: To coin a
tioning and arts appreciation on children at three grade levels. phrase. Music Teacher, 69, 14–15.
Studies in Art Education, 41(3), 208–227. Schmidt, C. P., & Sinor, J. (1986). An investigation of the relation-
Madura, P. D. (1996). Relationships among vocal jazz improvi- ships among music audiation, musical creativity, and cognitive
sation achievement, jazz theory, knowledge, imitative ability, style. Journal of Research in Music Education, 34(3), 160–172.
musical experience, creativity and gender. Journal of Stauffer, S. L. (2001). Composing with computers: Meg makes
Research in Music Education, 44(3), 252–267. music. Bulletin for the Council for Research in Music
Moore, J. L. (1990). Strategies for fostering creative thinking. Education, 150, 1–20.
Music Educators Journal, 76, 38–42. Stauffer, S. L. (2002). Connections between the musical and life
Moran, J. (2005). Creativity, critical skills, and context. Teaching experiences of young composers and their compositions.
Music, 12, 8. Journal of Research in Music Education, 50, 301–322.
Morin, F. (2002). Finding the music “within”: An instructional Sullivan, T. (2002). Creativity in action. In L. R. Bartel (Ed.),
model for composing with children. In L. R. Bartel (Ed.), Creativity and music education (pp. 179–194). Toronto,
Creativity and music education (pp. 152–178). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Britannia.
Ontario, Canada: Britannia. Symes, C. (1983). Creativity: A divergent point of view. Journal
Nolan, E. (1995). Focus on improvisation: Music Content of Aesthetic Education, 19(2), 83–96.
Standard 3: Orchestra—Drawing creativity out of your Tang, P. C. L., & Leonard, A. R. (1985). Creativity in art and
students. Teaching Music, 2(5), 28–29. science. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 19(3), 5–19.
Oehrle, E. (1986). A method of evaluating the extent to which Torrance, P. E. (1966). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking.
music education texts support creativity—An important Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Services.
aspect of contemporary music education. International Vaughan, M. (1973). Music as model and metaphor in the culti-
Society for Music Education, 13, 169–178. vation and measurement of creative behavior in children
Perkins, D. N. (1981). The mind’s best work. Cambridge, MA: (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1971).
Harvard University Press. Dissertation Abstracts International, 32(10), 5833A.
Pike, J. (2000). Sampling music technology: Creativity and elec- Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt,
tro-acoustic composition. Music Teacher, 79, 35. Brace.
Priest, T. (2001). Using creativity assessment experience to nur- Wang, C. (1985). Measures of Creativity in Sounds and Music
ture and predict compositional creativity. Journal of Research (MCSM). Retrieved February 16, 2005, from http://www.uky
in Music Education, 49(3), 245–257. .edu/~cecilia/MCSM
Reese, S. (2001). Tools for thinking in sound: Technology tools Webster, P. (1990). Measures of Creative Thinking–II (MCTM-II):
for composing and improvising are revolutionizing teaching Administrative guidelines. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University.
methods that encourage creativity in sound among students of Weisberg, R. W. (1986). Creativity: Genius and other myths. New
all ages. Music Educators Journal, 88, 42–26. York: Freeman.
Reynolds, N. (2002). Computers, creativity and composition in Wiggins, J. (1999). Teacher control and creativity: Carefully
the primary school: An analysis of two compositions. designed compositional experiences can foster students’ cre-
Australian Journal of Music Education, 1, 16–26. ative processes and augment teachers’ assessment efforts.
Richardson, C. P. (1983). Creativity research in music education: Music Educators Journal, 85, 30–35.
A review. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Wilson, D. (2001). Guidelines for coaching student composers.
Education, 74, 1–21. Music Educators Journal, 88, 28–33.

Downloaded from upd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 16, 2016

You might also like