You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/20690854

The Communicative Effectiveness Index: Development and psychometric


evaluation of a functional communication measure for adult aphasia

Article  in  The Journal of speech and hearing disorders · March 1989


Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

314 10,795

6 authors, including:

Laura Pickard
McMaster University
56 PUBLICATIONS   2,149 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Laura Pickard on 14 March 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Volume 54, 113-124, February 1989

THE COMMUNICATIVE EFFECTIVENESS INDEX:


DEVELOPMENT AND PSYCHOMETRIC EVALUATION OF A
FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION MEASURE FOR ADULT
APHASIA

JONATHAN L O M A S L A U R APICKARD
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada

STELLA BESTER HEATHER ELBARD


Chedoke-McMaster Hospitals,
Hamilton, Ontario, Canada

ALAN FINLAYSON
Chedoke-McMaster Hospitals and
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada

CAROLYN ZOGHAIB
Chedoke-McMaster Hospitals,
Hamilton, Ontario, Canada

Groups of aphasic patients and their spouses generated a series of communication situations that they felt were important in
their day-to-day life. Using criteria to ensure that the situations were generalizable across people, times, and places, we reduced
the number of situations to 36 and constructed an index that allowed the significant others of 11 recovering and 11 stable aphasic
individuals to rate their partners' performance in the situations on two occasions 6 weeks apart. These data were then used to
evaluate the psychometric properties of the Communicative Effectiveness Index (CETI) as a measure of change in functional
communication ability. Further application of a generalization criterion reduced the final index to 16 situations. Results- showed
the CETI to be internally consistent and to have acceptable test-retest and interrater reliability. It was valid as a measure of
functional communication according to the pattern of correlations found with other measures (Western Aphasia Battery, Speech
Questionnaire, and global ratings). Finally, it was responsive to functionally important performance change between testings.
Further research with the CETI and its usefulness for clinicians and researchers are discussed.

Historically, the focus of aphasia assessment has been Lincoln, 1982; Sarno, 1969) are all unsatisfactory for one
on language abilities with general communicative abili- or more of the following reasons: They are incomplete in
ties as only a secondary consideration. Furthermore, their documentation of psychometric properties (Skenes
assessment instruments have been validated with more & McCauley, 1985), correlate poorly with observation of
concern for their ability to discriminate aphasic from nonverbal communication (Behrmann & Penn, 1984), are
nonaphasic performance or one aphasia type from another not easy for the assessor to either administer or score
than for their ability to detect change in the severity of the (Houghton et al., 1982; Swisher, 1979), correlate so well
aphasia over time. The development of an instrument with existing language measures (Holland, 1980) that
with the intent to measure change does not guarantee that
they are probably not measuring any separate dimension
it will actually measure change (Kirshner & Guyatt, 1985;
of communication, and have not been shown to be sensi-
MacKenzie, Charlson, DiGioia, & Kelly, 1986).
tive to functionally important change over time. There-
Despite these historic trends there has been increased
fore, we developed a measure of functional communica-
interest recently in measuring pragmatic communication
tion for the adult with aphasia that could measure change
skills in aphasia and to do this in the context of within-
patient evaluation of change rather than between-patient in performance over time--the Communicative Effective-
discrimination (Behrmann & Penn, 1984; Beukelman, ness Index (CETI).
Yorkston, & Lossing, 1984; Holland, 1977; Houghton, Based on previous work in the area of disease-specific
Pettit, & Towey, 1982; Milton, Prutting, & Binder, 1983). quality of life measures (Guyatt, Bombardier, & Tugwell,
We were particularly interested in a measure of func- 1986; Kirshner & Guyatt, 1985), we established a set of
tional communication that could document the evolution properties that the instrument should possess. These
in aphasic individuals of both language and nonlanguage properties were relevant both to the development of
solutions to the communication problems encountered in items and the structure of the index, as well as to the
day-to-day living. The existing instruments developed to evaluation of the index, During development we asked
measure this area of aphasic performance (Holland, 1980; ourselves the following questions:

© 1989, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 113 0022-4677/89/5401-0113501.00/0

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


114 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 113-124 February 1989

Are both verbal and nonverbal communication as- the stroke. We selected the VAS (rather than a Likert-type
sessed? scale) because we believed it to be particularly sensitive
Are patients' values reflected in the instrument? to changes in performance over time (Bond & Lader,
Is performance in daily living assessed? 1974).
Is the instrument credible to users (face validity)? Because the focus of the CETI was on change in
Is the instrument simple and easy to administer? performance rather than absolute level of performance,
For the evaluation of the index we answered the follow- significant others rated the aphasic partner on the com-
ing questions: munication situations on a series of different occasions.
Is there a quantitative measure of change? The difference in ratings from one assessment to the next
Is the instrument reliable (i.e., is it reproducible across (the change score) was the score of interest. On the basis
time, people, and places)? of recent research showing that the size of the change
Is the instrument valid (i.e., does it really measure the score is unaffected but the variance around it is reduced
stated dimension of functional communication)? when raters are allowed to see their ratings from previous
Is the instrument responsive (i.e., does it measure occasions (Guyatt, Berman, Townsend, & Taylor, 1985),
functionally important change even if this change is we decided to adopt this strategy for the administration of
small)? the CETI. Raters therefore saw on the VAS where they
These properties or criteria are quite similar to those had rated the performance of the aphasic person on their
proposed by the American Psychological Association as previous assessments for each communication situation
standards for test development (APA, 1974) and are used (see Figure 1).
by others in their critical appraisals of language assess-
ment tools (McCauley & Swisher, 1984; Skenes & Mc-
Cauley, 1985). Index Content

To ensure that the communication situations that


METHOD would be rated were representative of patient values and
daily-living activities, we elicited situations from aphasic
individuals themselves. The procedures employed, and
lndex Structure the reliability or validity of the various judgments made
by experts during the process, have been described in
In deciding on the structure of the index we wished to more detail elsewhere (Lomas, Pickard, & Mohide, 1987).
ensure that performance (not potential) of communication What follows is a brief description and summary of the
in daily living was assessed. To do this one must directly results.
observe, or get reports of direct observation of, the apha- Under the auspices of the local Stroke Recovery Asso-
sic individual in actual communication situations. Alter- ciation, we assembled for a 4-hr meeting a group of 9
natively, one might role-play such situations with the
aphasic patient. Holland (1980), in her Communicative
Abilities in Daily Living, chose the role-playing option;
Sarno (1969), in her Functional Communication Profile, I THE COMMUNICATIVE EFFECTIVENESS INDEX I

assumed that the clinician-assessor would have some


direct observation experience of the aphasic patient's
performance. However, we wanted our instrument to be
simple and easy to administer and therefore rejected the
role-playing option. Furthermore, we were not confident
that clinician-assessors generally observe the aphasic
patient in daily-living situations; thus, they might confuse
potential with performance. Therefore, we rejected direct
observation by the clinician. We opted for reports from
direct observation made by the aphasic individual's sig-
nificant other (i.e., spouse, relative, neighbor, or friend)
who spends enough time with him or her to make accu-
rate judgments of communication performance.
Because the purpose of the CETI is to quantitatively
assess change in performance over time, the spouses' or
significant others' judgments were structured as ratings of
the aphasic person's performance in particular situations
on a 10-cm visual analogue scale (VAS). To emphasize
that we were interested in performance relative to pre-
morbid ability of the individual, not relative to some
absolute standard, the anchor on one end of the scale was Fiaum~ 1. Schematic of the use of the visual analogue scale for
not at all able and at the other end was as able as before rating at initial and repeat testing.

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


LOMAS ET AL.: The C o m m u n i c a t i v e E f f e c t i v e n e s s I n d e x 115

stroke survivors who had been or still were aphasic; 7 of raters and possible categorizations precluded the use of
them were accompanied by their spouse or significant conventional measures of interrater reliability. However,
other. Using the nominal group process technique of we could use the percentage of item categorization deci-
Delbecq, Van de Ven, and Gustafson (1975), we asked for sions falling into each of the possible levels of agreement.
their responses to the question " I n which situations does In no case did fewer than four of the eight panelists agree
a stroke survivor have to be able to get his meaning across on a category assignment, and for the majority of the
and to understand what someone else means?" The assignments (70%-75%), six or more of the panelists were
nominal group process allows each participant 10 min of in agreement.
individual consideration of the question before responses The results of the comparability check are presented in
are elicited from members in turn--the aim is to provide Table 2. After exclusion of noncommunication situations,
each participant with an equal opportunity to respond. the number of situations generated by each group was
Responses are recorded in view of the entire group, and roughly comparable (33 and 38). The percentage in each
the process continues until no m e m b e r has any further of the communication categories was not significantly
responses to offer. As far as possible the exact wording of different between the two groups [×2(3) = 1.65, p = .65],
the respondents is recorded. Following this the group Finally, 52% of the communication situations from the
then judges redundancies between responses and re- second patient group were completely matched to situa-
words those that can be combined or that it feels need tions generated by the first patient group. We were
improvement, thus arriving at a final list. therefore satisfied that the patient groups were not gen-
The process was repeated with a second group of 5 erating unique and nongeneralizable communication sit-
aphasic stroke survivors and their spouses who were uations.
recruited from past patients of the speech pathology The final pool of situations for initial evaluation of the
service at Chedoke-McMaster Hospitals; this group pro- C E T I was arrived at in the following manner.
vided a comparability check on the representativeness of 1. An initial pool was constructed from all communi-
the situations generated by the first group. cation situations generated by the first patient group plus
Both groups had individuals with long-standing apha- all the unmatched situations from the second patient
sia and a variety of severities, ranging from mild residual group.
anomia to persisting global aphasia. In the case of the few 2. The expert panel met as a group and judged each
individuals in the latter category it was, obviously, the situation, retaining only those that they felt satisfied three
spouse or significant other who volunteered the wording criteria: not redundant with another situation on the list,
for responses. assessed performance rather than potential, and relevant
To perform the comparability check, an expert panel of to both institutional and community environments. The
eight (five speech-language pathologists and three neu- last of these criteria was important to ensure that the
ropsychologists) independently applied explicit rules de- C E T I would be applicable for aphasic persons living in
veloped by us (see Appendix A) to each of the group's both community and institutional settings.
lists after noncommunication situations had been re- There were 51 situations in the initial pool of finalized
moved. This was in order to (a) assign the communication items--those from the first patient group plus unmatched
situations to one of the four categories of basic need, life situations from the second patient group. After applica-
skill, social need, or health threat, thus comparing the tion of the three criteria by the expert panel, 36 situations
relative proportions in each category for each list; and (b) remained. These 36 situations, worded as closely as
use the first patient group's list as the "index set" and possible to the actual wording used by the item-genera-
match comparable situations from the second patient
group, thus dividing the latter into matched and un-
matched situations. TABLE 2. Comparability of the communication situations gener-
The levels of agreement on categorization among the ated by two patient groups.
eight raters are presented in Table 1. The large number of
Patient Patient
Group Group
TABLE 1. Levels of agreement between the eight raters on 1 2
categorization decisions.
Number of situations generated 40 44
Number of noncommunication
%of ~of situations generated 7 6
Patient Patient Number of communication situations
No. of raters assigning item Group 1 Group 2 generated 33 38
to category items items Percentage in each communication
category:
All 8 agreed on category 21.2 10.5 Basic Need 16% 20%
7 of 8 agreed on category 27.3 28.9 Health Threat 3% 9%
6 of 8 agreed on category 27.3 31.6 Life Skill 37% 36%
5 of 8 agreed on category 21.2 18.5 Social Need 44% 35%
4 of 8 agreed on category 3.0 10.5 Percentage of situations matched
Less than 4 agreed on category 0 0 to Patient Group 1 - 52%

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


116 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 113-124 February 1989

tion groups, were structured as our initial CETI. It was TABLE 3. Description of patient samples.
then administered to the significant others of two groups
of aphasic persons to evaluate its reliability, validity, and Characteristic Recovering Stable
sensitivity to change.
Number 11 11
Age M 65.4 57.0
The Patient Sample SD 8.3 14.2
Sex M 8 10
F 3 1
The C E T I was administered on two occasions, 6 weeks Etiology CVAa 11 8
apart, to 11 patients recovering from recent onset of Aneurysm 0 2
Unknown 0 1
aphasia and 11 stable aphasic patients. Inclusion criteria Time of initial testing
for both groups were: spoken and written knowledge of (weeks postonset) M 7.2 163
English, a spouse or significant other was available who SD 2.4 61
had contact with the aphasic individual at least three Time between initial and
repeat testing (weeks) M 7.1 8.2
times a week, a score of less than 93.8 on the Western SD 1.3 1.1
Aphasia Battery [this score is the cutoff value for aphasic Range 5.7-9.7 6.0-9.5
vs. nonaphasic individuals (Kertesz, 1979)]. In addition, Severity (WAB Aphasia
for the recovering group, initial test administrations had Quotient) b
to be possible within 6--10 weeks postonset. For the Initial test M 28.1 60.0
SD 21.3 26.4
stable group, the initial administrations had to be 15 Repeat test M 33.7 59.0
months or more postonset, the attending clinician had to SD 33.7 29.8
judge the patient as stable, and patients could not be Description of significant
receiving formal language treatment. The attending cli- others Spouse 8 11
Child 2 0
nicians used informal assessments to judge whether the Sibling 1 0
patients were stable. The aphasic patients involved in the
initial generation of communication situations were ex- aCVA = cerebral vascular accident, bWAB = Western Aphasia
cluded from this portion of the study. Battery (Kertesz, 1982).
The patients were recruited from two regional referral
centers from admission log books (recovering group) or recovering group and greater than 65 weeks for the stable
from consecutive speech pathology department files go- group. Second assessments were completed at slightly
ing back from 15 months prior to the study start date longer than our planned 6-week interval but were not
(stable group). The recovering group were all receiving significantly different between the recovering (7.1 weeks)
language treatment from the regional referral centers. and stable (8.2 weeks) groups, t(20) = 1.1, n.s.
Table 3 describes the two patient groups. The slightly The relative severity of the aphasia in the two groups,
greater age of the recovering sample (65.4 years vs. 57 as measured by the Aphasia Quotient (AQ) of the Western
years) represents a recent shift in the demographics of Aphasia Battery, was as expected; at initial assessment
stroke patients referred to the centers. the value of the recovering group's AQ was about half that
The majority of patients in both patient groups had of the stable group. Furthermore, the AQ did show
suffered a cerebral vascular accident (CVA). In the recov- significant improvement in the recovering group between
ering group, 10 patients were suffering from their first first (M = 28.1) and second (M = 33.7) testing [t(10) =
unilateral infarct and all but one of these were in the left 3.16, p = .01], supporting the fact that they were recov-
hemisphere; the other patient had a recent left hemi- ering; the stable group's AQ showed little change (Ms =
sphere infarct after a previous right hemisphere lesion. At 60, 59).
initial testing the types of aphasia, according to t h e Finally, the existence of a period of spontaneous recov-
classification system of Kertesz (Kertesz & Poole, 1974), ery in aphasic patients (Culton, 1969; Lomas & Kertesz,
were: 4 global, 5 Broca, 1 Wernicke, and 1 transcortical 1978) enabled us to use the recent onset recovering group
sensory. In the stable group, 9 patients had unilateral left to assess the C E T I ' s ability to detect functionally impor-
hemisphere lesions with no previous infarcts, and 2 tant change. I f the index was responsive to change, then
patients had previous infarcts prior to the latest left there would be a significant improvement in scores from
hemisphere lesion--one in the left and one in the right initial to second testing in recovering patients. The stable
hemisphere. The types of aphasia were: 7 Broca, 1 con- group enabled us to assess the test-retest reliability of the
duction, and 3 anomic. CETI. I f scores on the index were reproducible over
Only one potential subject for the study was excluded time, there would be a strong relationship between the
because of the unavailability of a significant other meet- scores at initial and second testing in stable patients. Data
ing the criterion of at least three contacts per week. Eight on both groups combined contributed to assessments of
of the significant others in the recovering group were the internal reliability and the validity of the index.
spouses, 2 were daughters, and 1 was a sister. All 11 were
spouses for the stable group. Test Administration
The number of weeks postonset for initial assessment
fell within our desired ranges of 6--10 weeks for the Tests were administered by a trained research assistant

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


LOMAS ET AL.: The Communicative Effectiveness Index 117

either at the centers or the aphasic patients' homes. In generalizability of each item was revealed by the number
addition to the CETI, a number of other assessments of significant others who actually chose to rate their
were conducted. The Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, partner on it (i.e., the number who had actually observed
1982) was administered to obtain a score on a traditional performance of the behavior described in an item). To
"language" assessment tool. The Speech Questionnaire guarantee that the final items were as generalizable as
(Lincoln, 1982) was used as an alternate measure of possible, we adopted the stringent criterion that we
functional communication that, although poorly docu- would include only those situations for which 21 of the
mented as to its validity and significantly focused on possible 22 significant others gave a rating at both initial
functional language, was easy and simple to administer. and repeat testings.
This assessment was completed by the significant other Application of the above criterion excluded 20 items to
in the presence of the research assistant. The significant leave a final set of 16 communication situations on the
others were also asked to complete a single global rating C E T I (see Appendix B). Eight of the 16 items were rated
scale that asked them to assess the partner's "overall by all 22 significant others at both initial and repeat
language and communication skill during the previous testing, and 8 items by 21 of the 22 at both initial and
week" (possible score of 1--extremely poor to 7--excel- repeat testing. All remaining reliability and validity eval-
lent). Finally, 11 aphasic patients had available at least uations were carried out on these 16 items. For the
two eligible significant others. We had them both inde- analyses that follow, the 8 missing values were imputed
pendently complete the C E T I to provide some informa- using a program that took the patient's scores on all the
tion on the interrater reliability of the index. other items and weighted it by all other patients' scores
The C E T I requires a brief training period with the on that particular item.
significant other to familiarize him/her with the use of the
VAS. In addition, raters were explicitly instructed to
think not only of verbal communication but also of all DISTRIBUTION OF SCORES
other forms of communication and understanding when
rating the performance of the aphasic individual in the
Figure 2 displays the distribution of total scores on the
specific communication situations. It was stressed that
C E T I for both groups at the initial assessment. Table 4
the assessment was of the individual's overall ability to
provides the actual scores of the individuals in both
get his/her meaning across or understand someone else's
groups for the initial and repeat assessments.
meaning in daily-living situations, using any communi-
The mean C E T I scores for the group reflected the
cation means at their disposal.
greater severity of the recovering group (44.8) compared
The raters were also told to feel free not to rate a
to the stable group (68.0). This shows that, although the
particular communication situation if they felt that it was
C E T I was not developed for use on the absolute scores
not relevant or did not apply to their partner's situation.
(the focus is on the change in score over time), the
This instruction was included to facilitate the final item
absolute scores may have some validity as indicators of
reduction by revealing which communication situations
the severity of an aphasic individual's problems. Of
were considered generally applicable across the variety
course, on an individual patient basis, some of the recov-
of settings and people to whom the C E T I would eventu-
ally be administered. Perhaps most important, it also
ensured that raters provided assessments only on items RECOVERING SAMPLE (N=11)
describing situations in which they had actually observed
their partner's performance; raters were explicitly in-
structed to rate actual performance and not to infer # OF

I/ I,
PAT I ENTS
potential performance for unobserved situations.
The rating for each situation was converted into a score
by laying a template marked with l-ram divisions over the
10-cm VAS and reading off a value between i and 100. The
MEAN : 44.8 S,D, = 12,7
total C E T I score was converted to a 100-point maximum by
dividing the sum of the individual situation ratings by the
STABLE SAMPLE (N=11)
total number of situations. A high score indicated good
performance and a low score, poor performance.

# OF
RESULTS PATIENTS

FINAL ITEM REDUCTION 0-10 '11-20'21-30 "31-40 '81-9 J 91-1


MEAN = 68.0 S.D, = 16,8

The 36 communication situations were initially se- CETI TOTAL SCORE

lected to be generalizable across people and settings on FIGURE 2. Distribution of total scores on the Communicative
the basis of the expert panel's judgments. The true Effectiveness Index at initial testing.

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


118 Journal o f Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 113-124 F e b r u a r y 1989

TABLE 4. Scores on the Communicative Effectiveness Index e x c e p t i o n of 3 patients (7, 8, a n d 11), the C E T I shows
(CETI) (max. = 100) and change scores on the Western Aphasia s m a l l e r changes in p e r f o r m a n c e than the WAB for this
Battery (WAB; Kertesz, 1982) (max. = 10O) and the Speech
Questionnaire (SQ; Lincoln, 1982) (max. = 18). clinically d e t e r m i n e d stable p o p u l a t i o n who are not re-
c e i v i n g s p e e c h therapy. I n d e e d , 7 of the 11 patients have
change scores of zero or one on the C E T I , w h e r e a s none
Initial Repeat
test score test score Change score have such small c h a n g e scores on t h e WAB. T h e greater
Patient CETI CETI CETI WAB SQ t e n d e n c y for the WAB to show changes in performance,
in this stable p o p u l a t i o n , m a y reflect its r e l i a n c e on
Recovering group a s s e s s m e n t of p e r f o r m a n c e on a specific occasion (when
1 57 65 8 8 4
2 47 65 18 13 -2 patients m a y or m a y not b e "'at t h e i r best"); as o p p o s e d to
3 31 49 18 -3 0 the C E T I ' s r e q u e s t for ratings of p e r f o r m a n c e at the
4 46 66 20 21 2 general t i m e i m m e d i a t e l y p r e c e d i n g the a s s e s s m e n t ses-
5 64 72 8 4 0 sion (thus a v e r a g i n g the " g o o d " and " b a d " days).
6 48 54 6 17 2
7 24 28 4 0 4
8 53 59 6 9 1
9 47 62 15 0 5 RELIABILITY
10 25 44 19 8 3
11 48 51 3 2 5
Internal Reliability
M 44.8 55.9 11.4 7.2 2.2
SD 12.7 12.6 6.6 7.6 2.3
I f the items w i t h i n the a s s e s s m e n t tool are all measur-
Stable group ing the same d i m e n s i o n (in this case, c o m m u n i c a t i o n
1 57 64 7 10 1 effectiveness), t h e r e s h o u l d b e a strong r e l a t i o n s h i p both
2 62 63 1 4 2
3 75 75 0 -14 0 a m o n g t h e m and b e t w e e n each one a n d the total score
4 51 51 0 -2 -4 (i.e., the tool s h o u l d b e i n t e r n a l l y consistent). O f course,
5 79 80 1 2 -1 this r e l a t i o n s h i p s h o u l d not b e perfect; o t h e r w i s e t h e r e
6 73 74 1 7 0 w o u l d b e no justification for h a v i n g m o r e than one item in
7 77 88 11 5 0
8 98 89 -9 5 0 the index.
9 84 85 1 -9 0 Two m e a s u r e s of internal c o n s i s t e n c y w e r e u n d e r t a k e n
10 41 40 -1 -5 -1 on the scores of b o t h groups c o m b i n e d (N = 22). T h e s e
11 52 40 -12 -4 1 results, and the results of the o t h e r r e l i a b i l i t y assess-
ments, are p r e s e n t e d in T a b l e 5. First, w e c a l c u l a t e d the
M 68.0 68.1 0 -0.1 -0.2
SD 16.8 18.0 6.3 7.4 1.5 correlations b e t w e e n the score o b t a i n e d on each i n d i v i d -
ual item a n d the total score (item-total correlations).
Second, w e c a l c u l a t e d the C r o n b a c h a l p h a statistic
(Cronbach, 1951), w h i c h c o n c e p t u a l l y c o m p u t e s the com-
e r i n g group still had absolute scores h i g h e r than the
posite correlation from all the correlations of split-half
stable group.
comparisons of all the item scores.
T h e scores approximate a normal distribution with evi-
T h e overall t r e n d in t h e item-total correlations is
d e n c e that significant others were using all or most of the
toward high values (11 of the 16 items c o r r e l a t e d at .50 or
VAS w h e n doing their ratings with enough room left at the
greater with the total), i n d i c a t i n g that scores on the
top and bottom of the scale for both deterioration and
i m p r o v e m e n t in performance to b e d e t e c t e d (MacKenzie &
Charlson, 1986). TABLE 5. Reliability assessments of the Communicative Effec-
Because the i n t e n t of the C E T I is not to d i s c r i m i n a t e tiveness Index.
b e t w e e n a p h a s i a types, no a t t e m p t was m a d e to relate
these total scores to the nature of the aphasic patient's 1. Internal reliability (calculated on initial scores of both
deficits. patient samples, N = 22)
T a b l e 4 also p r o v i d e s the change scores from the AQ of a) Item-total correlation #items
the W e s t e r n A p h a s i a Battery (WAB) and from the S p e e c h >.75 4
.5-.75 7
Questionnaire. 1 It is i n t e r e s t i n g to note that in the recov- .25-.50 3
e r i n g group, with the exception o f 2 patients (6 a n d 8), the <.2,5 2
C E T I reveals t h e same or larger i m p r o v e m e n t s in perfor- b) Cronbach's alpha: .90 Total 16
m a n c e than the WAB. Also, in the stable group, with the 2. Test-retest reliability (calculated on stable group only,
(n = i1)
Intraclass correlation = .94
95% confidence limits = .87-.99
1Although Lincoln (1982) advises against pooling the scores on 3. Interrater reliability (calculated on change scores of eligible
the Speaking and Understanding scales of the Speech Question- from both patient samples, n = 1I)
naire, we have done so in this report of change scores for Intraclass correlation = .73
simplicity of presentation. When used in the validity analysis 95% confidence limits = .62-.81
later in the paper, the scores on the two scales are kept separate.

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


LOMAS ET AL.: The Communicative Effectiveness Index 119

individual items are strongly related to, but not totally correlation is acceptable, espeeially given the fairly sub-
coincident with, the total score. The Cronbach alpha of jective nature of the rating judgments that were required
.90 is high and supports the good internal reliability of the of the significant others, and suggests that the change
index. scores obtained from one rater are generalizable to others.

Test-Retest Reliability VALIDITY

Just as it is important to demonstrate that an instrument


can detect change when real change in performance has Face Validity
occurred (see Validity section), it is equally important to
be able to show that it does not detect any change over
time when performance has stabilized (i.e., test-retest Face validity pertains to the degree to which an instru-
reliability). ment appears to be measuring what it was designed for.
We calculated test-retest reliability using the stable As such it relates significantly to our criterion of "credible
group of patients, which, given our inclusion criteria for to users." The extensive involvement of the aphasic
the group, should not show any change in mean perfor- patients themselves in generating the items used on the
mance on the C E T I between the two testings (see Table C E T I should ensure that the index has high face validity.
5). We did indeed find this to be the case with a mean Furthermore, the item reduction process involved clini-
score at first testing of 68.0 (SD = 16.8) and at second cians, the aphasic individuals, and their significant others
testing of 68.1 (SD = 18.1). The standard error of this in culling from an initial pool of 51 items the 16 that were
mean difference of 0.1 between first and second testing most representative of communicative effectiveness in
was estimated to be 5.87.~ The intraclass correlation (rho) the aphasic patient. Thus, clinicians familiar with the
was .94, with a confidence interval of .87 to .99. deficits encountered in aphasia should find the items
The individual patient scores shown in Table 4 indi- listed in Appendix B credible as measures of functional
cate that this test-retest reliability was a group effect, with communication.
the scores of some aphasic patients in the stable group
changing by up to 12 points. It is not clear whether the
changes in these patients' scores reflect measurement Construct Validity
error or the variability in clinicians' judgments about
what constituted a "stable patient." Clarification of the The construct validity of an instrument is a measure of
meaning of these individual patient score changes will the extent to which it actually measures what it was
have to await further study of the properties of the CETI. designed for. This is assessed by comparing the scores on
Either way, we were most interested in the group effect, the instrument to scores obtained from other measures of
which clearly demonstrated test-retest reliability. the same individual's performance. We had three concur-
rent measures available for comparison with the CETI.
First, we used the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB), a
Interrater Reliability traditional measure of language ability that has b e e n
validated as being able to reliably discriminate among
In addition to being reproducible across time, the aphasic types and between individuals who do or do not
ratings made of one individual's performance should also have aphasia (Shewan & Kertesz, 1980). However, al-
be reproducible across different raters at the same point though the WAB has been used, it has not been evaluated
in time (interrater reliability). With two significant others as a measure of change in aphasic performance over time.
available for 11 of our 22 patients (6 recovering and 5 We used the Aphasia Quotient (AQ) as the WAB score of
stable), we were able to calculate the interrater reliability interest (maximum score = 100). The second measure
of the CETI. Because the focus of the instrument is on the was the Speech Questionnaire (SQ), a brief self-adminis-
change in score from one assessment to the next, we tered instrument designed as a measure of functional
calculated the intraclass correlation coefficient (rho) on language ability in aphasia, which has b e e n shown to be
the change score produced by the 11 pairs of raters (see internally reliable using Guttman scaling and to have
Table 5). For these 11 patients the mean change score of good test-retest reliability (Lincoln, 1982) but has not
the first raters was 6.4 (SD = 6.5) and of the second raters been assessed for its validity against other measures or for
8.3 (SD = 9.4); the intraclass correlation, adjusted to its ability to measure change over time. We used the
represent the population of potential raters (Berk, 1979), scores from the Speaking (maximum = 13) and Under-
was .73 with 95% confidence limits of .62 to .81. This standing (maximum = 5) subscales separately because
Lincoln reports that they cannot be combined. Finally,
we had the single global ratings done by each of the
2This estimate of the standard error is based on the estimated significant others to evaluate the overall ability of their
variance of the difference score, derived from the pooled vari- partner in both language and communication (maximum
ance of the scores on first and second testing, corrected for test score = 7). Performance was rated as excellent, good,
reliability using the formula SE = SDV~T~-~ fairly good, fair, not too good, poor, or extremely poor.

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


120 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 113-124 February 1989

Each of these measures was obtained at both the initial cautiously insofar as the other instruments combine mea-
testing and the repeat testing. surement of speaking and understanding, whereas the SQ
Commensurate with our a priori hypothesis that func- measures them separately.
tional communication is a separate but overlapping di- The pattern of correlations found between the C E T I
mension to language, we made a series of predictions and these other measures suggests that it possibly mea-
about the relationship between the scores on the C E T I sures a separate but overlapping dimension to the tradi-
and scores on these other measures. We predicted that tional language assessment instrument.
the strongest associations would be seen between the
C E T I and the global ratings by the significant others;
although because these global ratings included more
Sensitivity to Change in Performance
assessment of language ability than the C E T I (communi-
cation and language were assessed by the global ratings), Because the principal objective of the C E T I is to assess
we did not expect the correlations to be extremely high. changes in performance over time (i.e., evaluate prog-
Shewan and Kertesz (1980) claim that the WAB has some ress), it was particularly important to demonstrate that it
components that measure functional communication, but was sensitive to and able to measure functionally impor-
the focus of the WAB on language assessment led us to tant change. It was for this reason that we selected
expect only a moderate but nevertheless significant cor- patients for a "recovering group"; spontaneous recovery
relation with the CETI. Finally, the correlation of both assured us of a patient sample in which functionally
the Speaking and Understanding subseales of the SQ important change would likely occur. I f the C E T I failed
with the C E T I was not expected to be very different from to show significantly improved scores for this group
that between the C E T I and the WAB, despite the sup- between the initial and repeat testing, then it would
posed focus of the SQ on functional language ability. This clearly not be sensitive to or able to measure this func-
a priori hypothesis was based on scrutiny of the items in tionally important change.
the SQ; they exclusively concerned language issues (e.g., Therefore, using only the 11 aphasic patients from the
"Does he/she say phrases spontaneously?") rather than recovering group, we did a repeated measures analysis of
communication performance in different daily-living sit- variance on their initial and repeat test scores on the
uations. CETI. The mean total score at initial testing was 44.8 (SD
The Spearman rank correlation coefficients between = 12.7) and at repeat testing was 55.9 (SD = 12.6). The
the C E T I and the three measures for initial and repeat difference between these mean scores was significant,
testings were calculated on all 22 of the aphasic individ- F(1, 10) = 32.4, p < .002. No such significant difference
uals from both groups (except for some of the global was found for the stable patients, where the mean total
ratings where ratings for fewer than 22 patients were score at initial testing (68.0) was not significantly different
available) (Table 6). We did indeed find that the highest from that at repeat testing (68.1).
correlations were with the global ratings by significant The mean change score for the recovering group was
others at initial and repeat testing. The WAB had the next 11.4, with a standard deviation of 6.6 and a range of 3-19.
highest correlations and the SQ similar but slightly lower This change, however, was not significantly correlated
correlations. All the correlations between the C E T I and with the group's change in AQ scores on the Western
the other measurements at initial and repeat testing were Aphasia Battery; the Spearman rank correlation coeffi-
significant at at least the .05 level. cient was only .36 (df = 11, p > .D.
Our belief that the SQ, despite its stated objective, was
measuring language rather than communication found
DISCUSSION
some support from the fact that it correlated highly with
the WAB: .89 at initial testing and .87 at repeat testing.
However, correlations involving the SQ should be treated This study used aphasic patients to generate items for a
functional communication measure and patients' signifi-
cant others to rate performance on these items. On the
TABLE 6. Correlations between the Communicative Effective- basis of psychometric evaluation of 11 recovering and 11
ness Index and three other measures of aphasic performance. stable aphasic patients, the resulting 16-item Communi-
cative Effectiveness Index appears to be reliable and
Initial Repeat valid.
N test test Both the inclusion/exclusion criteria and the scores on
traditional language measures indicate that one group
Global ratings by significant others a .79** .62** was indeed recovering function, whereas the other was
Western Aphasia Battery b 22 .61"* .52**
Speech stable. Although some individuals in each group may not
Questionnaire ° have shown recovering or stable scores, the two groups
Speaking 22 .46* .43* were selected to have the potential to recover or to have
Understanding 22 .47* .56* a high probability of being stable. Obviously, this did not
~The number of raters at initial testing was 19 and at repeat guarantee that all group members would recover or be
testing was 21. bKertesz, 1982. °Lincoln, 1982. stable, but it did result in group means that clearly
*p < .05. **p < .01. represent recovering and stable samples. The stable pa-

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


LOMAS ET AL.: The Communicative Effectiveness Index 121

tients were used to assess test-retest reliability, and the Assessment of construct validity presents a particular
recovering patients contributed to the assessment of the problem for the development of instruments in new areas
CETI's sensitivity to change in performance. The remain- because of the absence of an alternative measure of the
ing aspects of instrument evaluation combined both dimension under consideration with which to compare
groups of patients for analysis. the new measure. For aphasia assessment tools that focus
The evaluations of reliability produced clearly accept- on discriminating among types of aphasia and relating
able values for use of the CETI in group studies and this to localization of the damaged areas of the brain (e.g.,
adequate values for use with individual patients. How- the Western Aphasia Battery or the Boston Diagnostic
ever, further studies would be useful to clarify the CETI's Aphasia Examination) such a "gold standard" for compar-
ison (and, therefore, validation) is available from CT
reliability for use with the latter. The most contentious
scans or other "hard" evidence of the actual location of
question is whether the results support the validity of the
damage. However, what can be considered the "gold
CETI as a measure of functional communication. There
standard" of functional communication against which the
are two aspects to this question. First, there is the ques- CETI can be validated? One of the reasons to develop
tion of whether significant others are unbiased reporters such a measure is the belief that existing aphasia assess-
of aphasic patient performance. Second, there is the issue ment instruments do not, or do not fully, assess functional
of whether the relationship of scores on the CETI and communication and would not, therefore, correlate par-
scores on the other measures supports the hypothesis of a ticularly highly with the CETI.
dimension of functional communication that is separate We adopted the approach of specifying, a priori, the
from but overlapping with the language dimension. relationship we would expect to see between the CETI
Spouses (or significant others) have been shown to have and three other measures (WAB, Speech Questionnaire,
a good level of agreement with aphasic patients on the and global ratings) if the CETI was measuring a separate
presence or absence of particular communication prob- dimension of functional communication (Cronbach &
lems, as well as on the severity of the problem (Shewan & Meehl, 1955). The actual correlations we obtained were
Cameron, 1984). Of particular interest for the CETI is that consistent with these specifications. Further evidence for
when there is disagreement between the spouse and the the recovery of functional communication separately from
aphasic patient on the precise level of severity, spouses the recovery of language ability came from the observa-
are equally likely to underestimate (31% of problems) as tion that the recovering group's change scores on the
to overestimate (27%) severity as judged by the patients CETI did not correlate significantly with their change
themselves (Shewan & Cameron, 1984). In contrast, some scores on the WAB. This result is not inconsistent with
studies have shown that, relative to clinicians (not rela- our hypothesis of a separate dimension of functional
tive to the aphasic patient), spouses overestimate perfor- communication, with a separate recovery course from
mance (e.g, Helmiek, Watamori, & Palmer, 1976). How- language, and that this dimension is being measured by
ever, because we were looking for validation of spouse the CETI but not by traditional instruments. A similar
assessments as proxies for the aphasic patients, we did argument has been made by Sarno in discussing the
not consider as relevant those studies that contrasted relationship between the Functional Communication
spouse and clinician assessments. Profile and the Neurosensory Centre Comprehensive
Furthermore, the validity of clinicians' assessments of Examination for Aphasia (Sarno, Sarno, & Levita, 1971).
as subjective an area as functional communication in the It might be argued that validation should involve cor-
aphasic patient is potentially problematic because of their relations of the CETI scores with direct observations of
limited opportunity to view the patient when he or she is actual communication behavior. This approach was used
actively engaged in functional "real-life" behaviors. In- by Holland in validation of the Communicative Abilities
deed, we have shown elsewhere (Lomas et al., 1987) that in Daily Living (Holland, 1980). However, such a valida-
the types of communication situations that are considered tion strategy for the CETI would have been redundant
important for functional behavior in aphasia are signifi- and, if the observations were rated or scored by clini-
cantly different for clinicians and aphasic patients. This cians, potentially misleading. It would have been redun-
finding also reinforced our decision to go directly to the dant because the structure of the index was such that
patients and their partners for item generation for the spouses were already rating the functional communica-
CETI. It also calls into question the widespread use of tion ability of their aphasic partners based on observa-
clinicians' views as the basis for constructing items for tions of their actual communication behavior. Our accept-
any "quality of life" assessment instrument. able levels of interrater reliability attest to the stability of
Thus, for the purpose of rating performance, we believe this rating.
that spouses are reasonable proxy respondents for the If the ratings of actual communication behavior were
aphasic patient, with no apparent bias toward overesti- performed by clinicians, then they would be, to some
mating ability. Furthermore, for the CETI it would not extent, subjective, and we have no reason to believe that
even matter if spouses did overestimate ability because of they would be any more valid as a measure of a particular
the focus of the index on the change score. As long as patient's functional communication ability than ratings
ability was overestimated an equivalent amount at each done by the spouse. In fact, we have reason to believe
testing, then the validity of the change score would that they would be less valid compared to a spouse--
remain intact. there is only limited exposure of the clinician to the

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


122 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 113--124 February 1989

particular communication foibles of a particular patient. patient's progress at functionally communicating in ev-
The existing evidence comparing clinician and spouse eryday life situations.
judgments demonstrated that they do not coincide (Hel- The critical value for the C E T I ' s change scores, above
mick et al., 1976; Lomas et al., 1987). Therefore, in the which individual aphasic patients can be d e e m e d to have
area of functional communication measurement, it would made a clinically important improvement, can be esti-
not appear to be a reasonable premise that ability can be mated by two methods. If one assumes that the combined
more objectively defined by expert clinicians than by treatment effect and spontaneous recovery of the recov-
individuals such as spouses who have significantly ering group is equivalent to clinically important improve-
greater exposure to the aphasic patients' behavior. ment, then the difference between the mean initial and
Obviously, the small number of patients involved in repeat scores of this group (11.4) represents the critical
the evaluation of the C E T I means that further validation value--scores at or above this level signify improvement.
Alternatively, one may consider that the critical value is
and reliability assessments should be undertaken. Future
the change score that would exclude all patients in the
work can profitably explore the relationship between
stable group from having exhibited clinically important
changes on the C E T I and other language and communi-
improvement [i.e., the largest change score in the stable
cation measures; this will assist in specifying more group (12.0)]. Both methods yield a change score of
clearly the degree to which functional communication is approximately 12 over a 6--8 week period as the initial
separate from other aspects of aphasic patients' diffi- estimate of the critical value. Of course, if some of the
culties. The higher scores of the stable group compared to recovering patients were not in fact recovering, or if some
the recovering group suggest that, although not designed of the stable patients were not in fact stable, this will be
for this purpose, the C E T I could be useful in measuring an overestimate of the critical value. Corroboration or
absolute severity of, and not just change over time in, revision of this initial estimate will have to await further
functional communication deficits. Studies in other pa- development work with the CETI.
tient groups would clarify this potential use of the index. One final characteristic of the C E T I worth mentioning
Finally, studies on larger numbers may clarify the reli- is its reliance on assessments made by significant others.
ability of the C E T I for use with individual patients. This not only gives, in a structured fashion, first-hand
Future work with the C E T I would also help to answer evidence on communication performance to the clini-
some important questions that were not addressed by the cian, but it also involves an often motivated and con-
current evaluation. For instance, the high proportion of cerned spouse in assessment of progress and orients him
men in both our patient groups (18 out of 22) raises the or her to the full repertoire of potential communication
question of whether the index is truly generalizable behaviors available to his or her partner. Given that the
across the sexes. With more women would different clinician can only be in contact with the aphasic individ-
results have been obtained or would different communi- ual for a few hours a week, increased involvement of
cation situations have been included as items? However, those who live with or spend large amounts of time with
it should be r e m e m b e r e d that more males than females him/her may lead to improved results in therapy out-
generally become aphasic (McGlone, 1980). Neverthe- comes. The Communicative Effectiveness Index is an
less, the items on the C E T I appear, and were constructed attempt to make available an instrument that will be able
to be, generalizable across the sexes (as well as across to measure some of these more functional changes that,
places and time). It is interesting to note that of the 16 though occurring during the aphasic patient's recovery
final items on the index, 8 were from the category of process, have so far eluded measurement.
Social Need, 3 from Life Skill, 4 from Basic Need, and
only 1 from the Health Threat category (see Appendix A).
The major value of this instrument is as a measure of ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
change in communicative effectiveness, thus enabling
clinicians to evaluate individual patients' progress in We wish to thank the following for their assistance during the
therapy and researchers to evaluate recovery of commu- study: the Hamilton-Wentworth Stroke Recovery Association;
nication ability in group studies of the effectiveness of Allan Kroll and the staffof Niagara Region Rehabilitation Center;
and Drs. Geoffrey Norman, Gordon Guyatt, Cindy Shewan, and
therapy. The C E T I does not purport to be a comprehen- Judith Trotter.
sive aphasia assessment instrument; it does not substitute This study was supported by grants from the Ontario Ministry
for the traditional language assessments. It has not been of Health (#01265) and Bauer Funds from the Health Sciences
designed (nor has it been validated) as a tool to discrim- Gerontological Research Group, McMaster University. The first
inate among aphasic types or between those with aphasia author is supported as a career scientist by the Ontario Ministry
of Health (#0135).
and those without aphasia, although it does appear to
potentially reflect levels of severity in an aphasic popu-
lation. It should not be used to predict responsiveness to
REFERENCES
therapy unless future work is able to validate it for such a
use. In its present form and at the present stage of
AMEmCAN PSYCHOLOGICALASSOCIATION.(1974). Standards for
evaluation, clinicians and researchers should feel confi- educational and psychological tests. Washington, DC: Author.
dent using the change scores obtained from one testing to BEHRMANN, M., ~ PENN, C. (1984). Non-verbal communication
the next as valid and reliable measures of an aphasic of aphasic patients. British Journal of Disorders of Communi-

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


LOMAS ET AL.: The Communicative Effectiveness Index 123

cation, 19, 155-168. of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49, 34-42.


BERK, R. A. (1979). Generalizability of behavioral observations: MCGLONE, J. (1980). Sex differences in human brain asymmetry:
A clarification of interobserver agreement and interobserver A critical survey. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 3, 215-226.
reliability. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 83, 460- MILTON, S. B., PRUTTING, C. A., & BINDER, G. M. (1983). Ap-
472. praisal of communicative competence in head injured adults.
BEUKELMAN, D. R., YORKSTON, K. M., & LOSSlNG, C. A. (1984). In R. H. Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology: Conference
Functional communication assessment of adults with neuro- proceedings (pp. 114-123). Minneapolis: BRK Publishers.
genie disorders. In A.S. Halperin & M.J. Fuhrer (Eds.), SAmqO, M. T. (1969). Functional communication profile. New
Functional assessment in rehabilitation (pp. 101-115). Balti- York: New York University Medical Centre.
more: Paul H. Brookes. SARNO, M.T., SARNO, E., & LEVITA, E, (1971). Evaluating
BOND, A., & LADER, M. (1974). The use of analogue scales in language improvement after completed stroke. Archives of
rating subjective feelings. British Journal of Medical Psychol- Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 52, 73-78.
ogy, 47, 211-218. SHEWAN, C. M., & CAMERON, H. (1984). Communication and
CRONBACH, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha in the internal struc- related problems as perceived by aphasic individuals and their
ture of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297-334. spouses. Journal of Communication Disorders, 17, 175-187.
CRONBACH,L. J., & MEEHL, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in SHEWAN, C. M., & KERTESZ, A. (1980). Reliability and validity
psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281-300. characteristics of the Western Aphasia Battery (WAR). Journal
CULTON,G. L. (1969). Spontaneous recovery from aphasia.Jour- of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45, 308-324.
nal of Speech and Hearing Research, 12, 825-832. SKENES, L. L., & McCAULEY, R. J. (1985). Psychometric review
DELBECQ, A.L., VAN DE VEN, A. H., & GUSTAFSON, D . H . of nine aphasia tests. Journal of Communication Disorders, 18,
(1975). Group techniques for program planning--A guide to 461-474.
nominal group and delphi processes. Boston: Glenview, Scott, SWISHER, L. (1979). Functional Communication Profile (FCP)
Foresman, and Co. (Review). In F. L. Darley (Ed.), Evaluation of appraisal tech-
GUYATT, G. H., BERMAN, L. B., TOWNSEND, M., & TAYLOR, niques in speech and language pathology (pp. 17-20). Red-
D.W. (1985). Should study subjects see their previous re- ding, MA: Addison Wesley.
sponses? Journal of Chronic Diseases, 38, 1003-1007.
GUYATT, G. H., BOMBARDIER, C., & TUGWELL, P. (1986). Mea- Received September 30, 1986
suring disease-specific quality of life in clinical trials. Cana- Accepted January 14, 1988
dian Medical Association Journal, 134, 889-895.
HELMICK, J.W., WATAMORI, T. S., & PALMER, J.M. (1976). Requests for reprints should be sent to Jonathan Lomas,
Spouses' understanding of the communication disabilities of Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, McMaster University,
aphasic patients.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 41, 1200 Main Street West, Hamilton, Ontario L8N 3Z5 Canada.
238-243.
HOLLAND, A. L. (1977). Some practical considerations in aphasia
rehabilitation. In M. Sullivan & M. Kommers (Eds.), Rationale APPENDIX A
for adult aphasia therapy (pp. 345-349). Lincoln: University
of Nebraska Medical Centre.
HOLLAND, A. L. (1980). Communicative abilities in daily living: The following definitions were provided to the expert panel:
A test of functional communication for aphasic adults. Balti-
more: University Park Press. Communication Categories
HOUGHTON, P. M., PETTIT, J. M., & TOWEY, M. P. (1982). Mea-
suring communication competence in global aphasia. In Pro- 1. Basic Need--Communication is required to meet basic
ceedings of the Clinical Aphasiology Conference (pp. 28-37). needs (e.g., toileting, eating, grooming, positioning).
Minneapolis: BRK Publishers. 2. Health Threat--Physical well-being or health is dependent
KERTESZ, A. (1979). Aphasia and associated disorders: Taxon- on effective communication (e.g., calling for help after falling,
omy, localization and recovery. New York: Grune and Strat- giving or receiving information about one's medical condition).
ton. 3. Life Skill--Giving or receiving information that is neces-
KERTESZ,A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune sary to accomplish everyday living (e.g., shopping, home main-
and Stratton. tenance, use of telephone, understanding traffic symbols).
KERTESZ, A., & POOLE, E. (1974). The Aphasia Quotient: The 4. Social Need--Communication that is primarily social in
taxonomic measurement of aphasic disability. Canadian Jour- nature (i.e., communication with others as an end in and of itself
nal of Neurological Sciences, 1, 1-17. such as dinner table conversation, playing cards, writing a letter
KIRSHNER, B., & GUYATT, G. (1985). A methodological frame- to a friend).
work for assessing health indices.Journal of Chronic Diseases,
8, 27-36. Matching
LINCOLN, N. B. (1982). The Speech Questionnaire: An assess-
ment of functional language ability. International Rehabilita- Situations should be considered matched if the inclusion of
tion Medicine, 4, 114-117. both of the situations in a communication assessment scale
LOMAS,J., ~ KERTESZ,A. (1978). Patterns of spontaneous recov- would provide no extra information beyond the inclusion of only
ery in aphasic groups: A study of adult stroke patients. Brain one of the situations.
and Language, 5, 388-401.
LOMAS, J., PIGKARD, L., & MOHIDE, A. (1987). Patient versus
clinician item generation for quality of life measures: The case APPENDIX B
of language disabled adults. Medical Care, 25, 764-769.
MACKENZIE, R. C., & CHARLSON, M. E. (1986). Standards for the
use of ordinal scales in clinical trials. British Medical Journal, F i n a l 16 Items of the C o m m u n i c a t i v e Effectiveness
292, 40-43. Index (CETI)
MACKENZIE,R. C., CHARLSON,M. E., DIGIOlA, D., & KELLY,K.
(1986). Can the Sickness Impact Profile measure change? An Please R a t e 's ability a t . . .
example of the scale assessment. Journal of Chronic Diseases,
39, 429-438. 1, Getting somebody's attention.
McCAULEY, R., & SWISHER, L. (1984). Psychometric review of 2, Getting involved in group conversations that are about him/
language and articulation tests for preschool children. Journal her.

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


124 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 113-124 F e b r u a r y 1989

3. Giving yes and no answers appropriately. 10. Having a spontaneous conversation (i.e., starting the conver-
4. Communicating his/her emotions. sation and/or changing the subject).
5. Indicating that he/she understands what is being said to him/ i i . Responding to or communicating anything (including yes
her. or no) without words.
6. Having coffee-time visits and conversations with friends and 12. Starting a conversation with people who are not close family.
neighbors (around the bedside or at home). 13. Understanding writing.
7. Having a one-to-one conversation with you. 14. Being part of a conversation when it is fast and there are a
8. Saying the name of someone whose face is in front of him/ number of people involved.
her. 15. Participating in a conversation with strangers,
9. Communicating physical problems such as aches and pains. 16. Describing or discussing something in depth.

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


The Communicative Effectiveness Index: Development and Psychometric Evaluation
of a Functional Communication Measure for Adult Aphasia

Jonathan Lomas, Laura Pickard, Stella Bester, Heather Elbard, Alan Finlayson, and
Carolyn Zoghaib
J Speech Hear Disord 1989;54;113-124

This article has been cited by 13 article(s) which you can access for free at:
http://jshd.asha.org/cgi/content/abstract/54/1/113#otherarticles

This information is current as of April 26, 2010

This article, along with updated information and services, is


located on the World Wide Web at:
http://jshd.asha.org/cgi/content/abstract/54/1/113

Downloaded from jshd.asha.org on April 26, 2010


View publication stats

You might also like