You are on page 1of 9

31. Tentori F, Hunt WC, Rohrscheib M et al. Which targets in clinical practice 40.

40. Palmer SC, Hayen A, Macaskill P et al. Serum levels of phosphorus, para-
guidelines are associated with improved survival in a large dialysis organ- thyroid hormone, and calcium and risks of death and cardiovascular dis-
ization? J Am Soc Nephrol 2007; 18: 2377–2384 ease in individuals with chronic kidney disease: a systematic review and
32. Voormolen N, Noordzij M, Grootendorst DC et al. High plasma phosphate meta-analysis. JAMA 2011; 305: 1119–1127
as a risk factor for decline in renal function and mortality in pre-dialysis 41. Binder H, Sauerbrei W, Royston P. Comparison between splines and frac-
patients. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2007; 22: 2909–2916 tional polynomials for multivariable model building with continuous cov-
33. Covic A, Kothawala P, Bernal M et al. Systematic review of the evidence ariates: a simulation study with continuous response. Stat Med 2013; 32:
underlying the association between mineral metabolism disturbances and 2262–2277
risk of all-cause mortality, cardiovascular mortality and cardiovascular events 42. Malloy EJ, Spiegelman D, Eisen EA. Comparing measures of model selec-
in chronic kidney disease. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2009; 24: 1506–1523 tion for penalized splines in Cox models. Comput Stat Data Anal 2009; 53:
34. Bradbury BD, Fissell RB, Albert JM et al. Predictors of early mortality 2605–2616
among incident US hemodialysis patients in the Dialysis Outcomes and 43. Govindarajulu US, Malloy EJ, Ganguli B et al. The comparison of alterna-
Practice Patterns Study (DOPPS). Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 2007; 2: 89–99 tive smoothing methods for fitting non-linear exposure-response rela-
35. Noordzij M, Korevaar JC, Dekker FW et al. Mineral metabolism and mor- tionships with Cox models in a simulation study. Int J Biostat 2009; 5:
tality in dialysis patients: a reassessment of the K/DOQI guideline. Blood Article 2. doi: 10.2202/1557-4679.1104
Purif 2008; 26: 231–237 44. Strasak AM, Umlauf N, Pfeiffer RM et al. Comparing penalized splines and
36. Wald R, Sarnak MJ, Tighiouart H et al. Disordered mineral metabolism in fractional polynomials for flexible modelling of the effects of continuous
hemodialysis patients: an analysis of cumulative effects in the Hemodialysis predictor variables. Comput Stat Data Anal 2011; 55: 1540–1551
(HEMO) Study. Am J Kidney Dis 2008; 52: 531–540 45. Tentori F, Wang M, Bieber BA et al. Recent changes in therapeutic ap-
37. Lacson E, Jr, Wang W, Hakim RM et al. Associates of mortality and hospi- proaches and association with outcomes among patients with secondary
talization in hemodialysis: potentially actionable laboratory variables and hyperparathyroidism on chronic hemodialysis: the DOPPS study. Clin J
vascular access. Am J Kidney Dis 2009; 53: 79–90 Am Soc Nephrol 2015; 10: 98–109
38. Robinson BM, Port FK. International hemodialysis patient outcomes com- 46. Torres A, Lorenzo V, Hernandez D et al. Bone disease in predialysis, hemo-
parisons revisited: the role of practice patterns and other factors. Clin J Am dialysis, and CAPD patients: evidence of a better bone response to PTH.
Soc Nephrol 2009; 4 Suppl 1: S12–S17 Kidney Int 1995; 47: 1434–1442
39. Larsson TE, Olauson H, Hagstrom E et al. Conjoint effects of serum calcium
and phosphate on risk of total, cardiovascular, and noncardiovascular mor-
Received for publication: 18.12.2014; Accepted in revised form: 17.3.2015
tality in the community. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 2010; 30: 333–339

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Nephrol Dial Transplant (2015) 30: 1551–1559
doi: 10.1093/ndt/gfv213
Advance Access publication 11 June 2015

Effect of a single dialysis session on cognitive function in CKD5D


patients: a prospective clinical study

Sabrina M. Schneider1,2, Anne K. Malecki2,3, Katrin Müller3, Robby Schönfeld3, Matthias Girndt4,
Peter Mohr4, Marcus Hiss2, Heike Kielstein5, Kristin Jäger5 and Jan T. Kielstein2
1
Department of Neurology, Philipps-University Marburg, Marburg, Germany, 2Department of Nephrology and Hypertension Hannover Medical
School, Hannover, Germany, 3Department of Psychology, Martin-Luther University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany, 4Department of
Internal Medicine II, Martin-Luther University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany and 5Department of Anatomy, Martin-Luther
University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany

Correspondence and offprint requests to: Jan T. Kielstein; E-mail: kielstein@yahoo.com

dialysis (CKD5D)]. Aside from structural permanent damage,


A B S T R AC T there seems to be a reversible part of low cognitive performance.
The potential effect of a single dialysis session on cognitive
Background. Cognitive function declines in parallel to the function remains still elusive. The aim of the study was to assess
decrease in glomerular filtration rate, best epitomized by the cognitive function using a widespread test battery and avoiding
markedly reduced cerebral performance in patients undergoing excluding effects of circadian variations.
maintenance haemodialysis [chronic kidney disease stage 5

© The Author 2015. Published by Oxford University Press 1551


on behalf
Downloaded from of ERA-EDTA. All rights reserved.
https://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056
by guest
on 03 January 2018
Methods. Twenty-eight medically stable CKD5D patients Other authors reported that the removal of uraemic toxins by
(age: 54.9 ± 13.2 years, dialysis vintage: 46.2 ± 51.0 month) haemodialysis leads to an improvement in cognitive processing,
at two tertiary care centres with outpatient dialysis units but mainly if the severely impaired cognitive function 1 h after
were enrolled. Cognitive testing was always performed twice dialysis is taken as a comparator [5]. Also, the potential effect of
within 24 h, 1 h prior to haemodialysis (T1pre-dialysis) as well an increase in dialysis intensity on cognitive function is contro-
as 19 h after the end of dialysis (T2post-dialysis) including as- versially discussed. While one pilot study in 12 patients showed
sessment of memory, attention and concentration, executive improved general cognitive efficiency 6 months after switching
functioning, word fluency and psychomotor speed by using from thrice weekly to nocturnal haemodialysis [6], the results
a well-validated neuropsychological test battery. Patients from the frequent haemodialysis network nocturnal trial
were randomized into two groups. One group was examined showed no effect of an increased dialysis intensity on cognitive
before (T1pre-dialysis) and after (T2post-dialysis) Dialysis Session performance, measured by the Trail Making Test (TMT) B only
1. The other group was first examined the day after Dialysis [7]. Hence, the aim of our study was to investigate the effect of
Session 1 (T2post-dialysis) and then before Dialysis Session 2 a single dialysis session on a large variety of neurocognitive
(T1pre-dialysis) in order to exclude potential learning effects. functions in CKD5D patients using a large, state-of-the-art
Twenty age-matched subjects with normal excretory renal test battery instead of simple screening tests.
function were used for comparison.
Results. Neuropsychological testing found that the CKD5D
performed significantly worse on measures of alertness, atten- METHODS
tion, working memory, logical and visual memory, word flu-
ency and executive functions compared with non-CKD Study participants
subjects. No differences in short-term memory, selective atten- The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee (ref-
tion, as well as problem-solving and planning were found be- erence # 2012-30). All patients provided informed consent.
tween CKD5D patients and non-CKD subjects. A single Patients were treated at the dialysis units of the two participat-
haemodialysis session led to a significant improvement in logic- ing tertiary care centres in Hannover and Halle. Eligible
al (Rivermead Behaviour Memory Test story: P < 0.001) and CKD5D patients were approached if they met the following cri-
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

visual memories [Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test teria: (i) aged 18 years or more, (ii) no clinical history of neuro-
(RCFT) memory quotient: P < 0.001], psychomotor speed logical disorders (e.g. stroke and Parkinson’s disease) and (iii)
[Trail Making Test (TMT) B: P = 0.020], activity planning (ex- being fluent in spoken German. Of the 43 approached patients,
ecutive functions) (RCFT copy/points deduction: P < 0.001) 31 agreed to take part in the study. Out of those, three declined
and concentration (TMT A: P < 0.001). to perform the second testing session. All dialysis patients (n =
Conclusion. Our data demonstrate improvements in memory 28) were dialysed thrice weekly. The non-CKD group consisted
functions, executive functions and psychomotor abilities after a of 20 subjects recruited from the community, with normal renal
single dialysis session, pointing to a reversible component of function and self-reported health, confirmed by laboratory data
low cognitive performance in CKD5D. obtained from the primary care physician within 12 months of
testing, who also met the same inclusion criteria.
Keywords: neuropsychological tests, uraemic toxins
Measures
Sociodemographic characteristics and clinical measures.
INTRODUCTION Demographic, clinical data and medication details were
collected from patients’ medical records as well as during
Treatment of uraemia by chronic haemodialysis can lead to a the assessment. Furthermore, routine clinical chemistry
marked improvement in neurocognitive function as already and complete blood count results were recorded on the day
reported by Scriber et al. in 1960 [1]. The first two chronic of T1pre-dialysis.
haemodialysis [chronic kidney disease stage 5 dialysis
(CKD5D)] patients who showed ‘increased fatigability, muscle Neuropsychological assessments. To assess various cogni-
cramps, irritability and lethargy’ before the initiation of treat- tive domains, we designed a neuropsychological test battery
ment improved to a point that ‘neither patient has yet shown measuring memory, attention, executive functions and psycho-
the relentless loss of weight and the mental deterioration’ [1]. motor functions. All participants were tested twice. Cognitive
Since this first description, a burgeoning body of evidence has testing was always performed twice within 24 h, 1 h prior to
accumulated that cognitive function is indeed severely impaired haemodialysis (T1pre-dialysis) as well as 19 h after the end of dia-
in patients with CKD5D as recently reviewed by Bugnicourt lysis (T2post-dialysis) including assessment of memory, attention
et al. [2]. Rare cases of severe uraemia, which is fortunately and concentration, executive functioning, word fluency and
only rarely seen in industrialized countries, vividly illustrate psychomotor speed by using a well-validated neuropsycho-
the reversibility of decreased cognitive performance by dialysis logical test battery. Patients were randomized into two groups.
[3]. In the setting of maintenance dialysis, the effect of a single One group was examined before (T1pre-dialysis) and after
haemodialysis on cognitive function is controversially dis- (T2post-dialysis) Dialysis Session 1. The other group was first ex-
cussed. One study could not find any effect of a single dialysis amined the day after Dialysis Session 1 (T2post-dialysis) and then
on an insensitive test like mini mental state examination [4]. before Dialysis Session 2 (T1pre-dialysis) in order to exclude

Downloaded from1552
https://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056 S.M. Schneider et al.
by guest
on 03 January 2018
potential learning effects that have been well knwon [8, 9]. of variance. The statistical analyses were performed using
(Supplementary data, Table S1). Tests were performed in a sep- SPSS version 17.0 for Windows. All statistical tests were two
arate room and quiet environment. All assessments were con- sided. Differences were considered statistically significant at
ducted by researchers who had received training on P < 0.05.
administering these tests. With all participants, we used the
standardized and validated test versions in German. The neuro-
psychological test battery is summarized in Table 1. A more ex-
R E S U LT S
tensive description can be found in Supplementary data, File 1.
Subject characteristics
Statistical analyses Twenty-eight CKD5D patients were recruited for the study.
The mean age was 54.9 ± 13.1 years (range = 27–79). The non-
Continuous variables were characterized using mean ± SD
CKD group of 20 participants had a mean age of 54.6 ± 14.0
or percentages and compared using t-tests. Independent sam-
years (range = 32–80). Eleven (39.3%) of the CKD5D patients
ple t-tests were used to evaluate the differences between
and 13 (65%) non-CKD subjects were female. Socio-
non-CKD subjects and CKD5D patients. Frequencies are pre-
demographic and medical characteristics of the study partici-
sented for categorical variables. Pearson’s chi-square tests
pants are shown in Tables 2 and 3.
were performed to compare sociodemographic characteristics
between CKD5D patients and non-CKD cohort. For each
cognitive test, raw scores were converted to age- and Baseline test results of CKD5D patients compared with
education-standardized T-scores (the equivalent to Z-scores healthy non-CKDs
in samples sizes of <30 with an arbitrarily assigned mean Baseline scores (T1pre-dialysis) identified significant differ-
and SD) obtained from the test manuals, which were used ences between non-CKD subjects and CKD5D patients
to evaluate performance. For Rivermead Behaviour Memory (Table 4a). Patients showed a poorer performance in tests re-
Test (RBMT) story, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test garding attention, long-term memory and working memory
(RCFT) points deduction T-scores were not available, so we functions.
used raw scores for statistical analyses. A subject’s individual T-scores visualizing the time needed to complete TMT B

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
test performance was considered impaired if it was >1 SD (≙ were significantly higher in the CKD5D group, which reflects
T-scores <40) below the mean of the norms. The influence of a poorer performance. For TMTs A and B, we found 18
gender on the test results was calculated by using an analysis CKD5D patients (64.3%) for each of the tests, who received a

Table 1. Neuropsychological assessments used in this study

Test Description Task Parameter


Attention TAP [10] Subtests: –Reaction (pushing a button) to a –Reaction time (in ms)
–Alertness→measures every day attention visual stimulus
–Go/NoGo→measures ability to suppress undesired
responses
–Computer-based test battery
TMT A [11] –Measures attention, visual scanning and processing –Connections of randomly arranged –Processing time (in s)
speed numbers from 1 to 25
Memory Wechsler Memory Subtests: –Memorization of a set of numbers –Number of correct
Scale-Revised (WMS-R) –Digit span forward→measures short-term memory in the correct serial order sets of numbers
[12] –Digit span backward→measures working memory
RBMT [13] –Subtest: story measures logical memory –Retrieval of a short story that has –Number of correct
–Alternate forms for T2post-dialysis been read out to the subjects facts
–Immediate and delayed recall
(30 min)
RCFT [14] –Measures visual memory (and executive function) –Copy and recall of a complex figure –Number of correct
–Quality of drawing = RCFT copy (delayed recall after 3 and 30 min) items of the figure
–Visual memory function = RCFT memory quotient
–Logical activity planning = RCFT points deduction
–Alternate forms for T2post-dialysis
Executive TMT B [11] –Measures mental flexibility and psychomotor speed –Connecting numbers and letters in –Processing time (in s)
function an alternate sequence
BADS [13] –Subtests key search and zoo map measure the –Key search: creating logical –Points for logical
capability to plan behaviour and logical activity strategies to search an imaginative strategies
planning key on a huge meadow
–Zoo map: finding the way through a
labyrinth
RWT [15] –Measures verbal fluency –Retrieval of words from a certain –Number of words in
–Categories: semantic, divergent semantic, lexical and category within 1 min 1 min
divergent lexical

N e uhttps://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056
Downloaded from rocognitive improvement by a single dialys is 1553
by guest
on 03 January 2018
Table 2. Characteristics of study sample: Part 1
CKD5D patients (n = 28) Non-CKD subjects (n = 20) P

M ± SD M ± SD
Age (years) (M ± SD) 54.9 ± 13.2 54.6 ± 14.0 0.941
Gender (F/M) 11/17 13/7 0.079
Dialysis vintage (months) 46.2 ± 51.0
Education 0.040
9th grade (%) 35.7 5
10th grade (%) 35.7 60
>12th grade (%) 28.6 35
Laboratory results Reference | KDIGOa
Haemoglobin (g/dL) 10.02 ± 2.02 15.02 ± 1.79 12.0–16.0 | <10.0 to <11.5a
Leukocytes (10³/μL) 7.75 ± 4.21 7.35 ± 3.88 4.4–11.3
Albumin (g/dL) 33.00 ± 5.83 35–52
Ferritin (μg/L) 774 ± 635 27–365 | >500
MCV (fl) 91.80 ± 8.50 83.27 ± 5.79 80–100
MCHC (g/dL) 31.84 ± 4.13 31.37 ± 3.75 31–37
PTH (ng/L) 425.15 ± 587.22 15–65 | 150–300a
TSH (mU/L) 2.67 ± 4.19 0.27–4.2
S_Potassium (mmol/L) 5.27 ± 0.84 4.17 ± 1.25 3.6–5.4
S_Sodium (mmol/L) 138.1 ± 4.1 135–145
S_Calcium (mmol/L) 2.22 ± 0.21 2.15–2.60
Phosphate (mmol/L) 1.50 ± 0.47 0.83–1.67
Creatinine (μmol/L) 640.6 ± 300.1 82.7 59–104
eGFR mL/min CKD-EPI 98.5 ± 17.3 >60
Urea (mmol/L) 16.63 ± 9.04 2.8–7.2
S_Uric acid (μmol/L) 351.3 ± 116.2 200–420
S_GOT (U/L) 20.61 ± 10.51 <35
S_GPT (U/L) 22.77 ± 32.22 <45
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

GGT (U/L) 58.49 ± 59.62 <55


S-Protein 61.39 ± 9.14 65–80
Iron (μmol/L) 8.10 ± 3.81 11–25
Transferrin satur. (%) 15.47 15–45
a
KDIGO (Kidney Disease Global Outcome Initiative) recommendation if applicable.
MCV, mean corpuscular/cell volume; MCHC, mean corpuscular/cellular hemoglobin concentration; PTH, Parathyroid hormone; TSH, Thyroid stimulating hormone.

score of >1 SD below the mean, meeting the definition for cog- Pre-dialysis (T1pre-dialysis)/and post-dialysis
nitive impairment. In the non-CKD cohort, there were only (T2post-dialysis) tests
three (15%) participants (TMT A) and four (20%) participants Assessing the effect of a single dialysis session by a pre–post-
(TMT B) with impairment in both tests. Other significant dif- comparison of test results showed an improvement in attention
ferences between the groups are discovered on Tests of Atten- ( processing speed/visual scanning) and long-term memory
tional Performance (TAP) (attention). Fourteen (50%) CKD5D functions (Table 4b). This was best epitomized by criteria for
patients and four (20%) non-CKD subjects were cognitively mild cognitive impairment in the TMT A test. While at pre-
impaired. While 14 (50%) CKD5D patients had an impaired dialysis (T1pre-dialysis) 64% fulfilled the criteria for cognitive im-
performance on digit span backwards (working memory), pairment, this was only true for 25% of CKD5D patients at
there was only 1 (5%) individual in the non-CKD cohort. Mea- T2post-dialysis, i.e. the day after dialysis. Other functions of atten-
sures of short-term memory (forward digit span) and selective tion (like alertness and selective attention), short-term and
attention (TAP Go/NoGo) did not differ between groups (Fig- working memory did, however, not differ between T1pre-dialysis
ure 1). Comparisons between non-CKD subjects and CKD5D and T2post-dialysis (Figure 2).
patients also revealed significant differences regarding particu- Cognitive shifting (TMT B) improved significantly after a
lar results in executive performance. Regensburg Word Fluency single dialysis session regarding normed T-scores. Interestingly,
Test (RWT) produced a consistent effect between the groups the same was true for the non-CKD group. Results of the TMT
regarding every subtest T-score. The strongest impairment B were the only test results that also improved in the non-CKD
was found in the subtest divergent lexical with 16 (57%) group, pointing to a potential learning effect in this test. To ac-
CKD5D patients, who received a score clearly more than 1 count for this potential learning effect, the study design of the
SD below the mean, compared with 5 (25%) of the non-CKD CKD5D patients was tailored accordingly (see above). While
group. Subtests from Behavioural Assessment of Dysexecutive 64% of patients fulfilled the impaired performance before dia-
Syndrome (BADS) inventory did not differentiate between the lysis (T1pre-dialysis), this decreased to 46% at T2post-dialysis, i.e. the
groups. The executive assessment being included in the RCFT day after dialysis. In contrast to attention and cognitive shifting,
could mark a significant difference between our two groups. neither call of semantic and lexical words in an alternating way
The results of the independent t-test are presented in Table 4a. nor productive thinking (RWT) improved after dialysis. The

Downloaded from1554
https://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056 S.M. Schneider et al.
by guest
on 03 January 2018
Table 3. Characteristics of study sample—Part 2
DISCUSSION
CKD5D patients
(n = 28)
The aim of the study was a detailed examination of the cognitive
M ± SD performance level of adults with CKD5D. The neuropsychologic-
Weight (kg) 75.6 ± 19.1 al test battery consisted of tests measuring memory, attention and
Ultrafiltration (mL) 1608 ± 1346 executive functions. Compared with the non-CKD group, we
Dialysis time (h) 4.07 ± 0.51 found a poorer performance level in CKD5D patients. More spe-
Dialysis vintage (months) 46.2 ± 51.0
Heart rate (bpm) 81.9 ± 15.7 cifically, it should be noted that significant differences in perform-
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 80.3 ± 14.4 ance regarding logical and visual long-term memories (tested
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 130.0 ± 20.8 with RBMT story or RCFT), working memory (digit span back-
Kt/Vurea 1.34 ± 0.12 wards), processing speed (TMT A) and alertness (TAP), as well as
Pre-dialysis HCO3/dialysis bath 22.2 ± 2.7/33.9 ± 2.8
HCO3
executive functions such as cognitive shifting (TMT B), activity
Pre-dialysis K+/dialysis bath K+ 5.27 ± 0.84/3.77 ± 0.37 planning (RCFT points deduction) and verbal fluency (RWT)
N %
could be detected between the two study groups at T1pre-dialysis.
Medication Besides the cross-sectional comparison between CKD5D pa-
AT1-blocker 14 50 tients before dialysis and non-CKD subjects, we aimed to iden-
Beta-blocker 17 60.7 tify the effect of a single dialysis session on cognitive functions.
EPO 16 57.1 After dialysis session (T2post-dialysis), patients showed an im-
Diuretics 11 39.3
Vitamin D 13 46.4
proved performance in logical and visual long-term memories
Statins 4 14.3 (RBMT story and RCFT), processing speed/visual scanning
PPI 20 71.4 (TMT A), cognitive shifting (TMT B) and activity planning
Calcium antagonists 12 42.9 (RCFT points deduction) when compared with the test results
Phosphate binders w/Ca 5 17.9 before a dialysis session (T1pre-dialysis). There was neither a per-
Phosphate binders w/o Ca 10 35.7
Intravenous iron 12 42.9
formance change regarding short-term and working memories

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Benzodiazepines 4 14.3 (forward and backward digit span) and attention (TAP) nor in
Antidepressants 6 21.4 verbal fluency and planning behaviour (BADS). Despite the im-
Total number of tablets per day 28 9.29 ± 2.84 provements in some cognitive areas, it should be noted that the
level of cognitive function in CKD5D was still below the per-
formance level of the non-CKD group. With the exception of
the TMT B, there was no difference between the two time points
slight improvement of T-scores regarding working memory was of testing (which were 24 h apart) in non-CKD subjects. The
not significant comparing normed T-scores within both results in TMT B did improve significantly pointing to a pos-
groups. sible learning effect, underlining the importance of the study
Capability to plan behaviour and to refer to logical activity design we did choose for the CKD5D patients to minimize
planning, examined by the subtests key search and zoo map, the impact of learning effects.
was not influenced by dialysis. Patients’ ability to visual plan- CKD5D patients showed a stable performance on the for-
ning was assessed by the quality of the complex figure they had ward digit span, which is thought to reflect unimpaired short-
to copy. Interestingly, the objective scoring improved signifi- term memory functions. The available alertness of dialysis pa-
cantly comparing pre-dialysis with post-dialysis (RCFT copy tients lasts to keep up with those short-term memory functions.
and RCFT points deduction). All patients showed an average Therefore, the performance in these tests remains on an average
to above average copy performance at T2post-dialysis. Consider- level. However, learning and memorizing information over a
ing strong ceiling effects for these test results, a chi-square test longer period of time require more than short activation of at-
was conducted in order to reveal correlation between quality of tention. Higher brain actions are involved, which are apparently
copying and group membership. Chi-square test shows a rela- reduced in patients with CKD. Some specific abilities are more
tionship between group membership and test results during vulnerable to moderate CKD than others. Our findings support
the first point of testing [RCFT (copy): χ 2 = −3.15, P = 0.003]. the hypothesis that cognitive impairment in CKD5D patients is
For the second point of testing, this correlation is much weaker reversible by haemodialysis. Patients are more likely able to
[RCFT (copy): χ 2 = −1.88, P = 0.07], supporting the notion memorize, concentrate and manage situations a day after
that executive functioning in CKD5D is improved after a single dialysis.
dialysis session (Figure 3). Similar results could be found in the A chronic disease, like CKD, requires a patient’s adherence to a
deduction of points during figure copying [T1pre-dialysis: RCFT complex pharmacological regimen and to specific dietary restric-
( points deduction): χ 2 = 2.37, P = 0.02/T2post-dialysis: RCFT tions. A lower cognitive function level is a major barrier as it im-
( points deduction): χ 2 = 1.59, P = 0.118]. All results of the de- pairs self-management, which is even more important in patients
pendent t-tests are summarized in Table 4b. Gender did not undergoing dialysis treatment. In CKD5D, errors in medication
influence the increase in performance comparing pre- and and diet due to decreased cognitive performance can have dele-
post-dialysis cognitive function in the CKD5D patients (data terious consequences. As pointed out by Elias et al., the measures
not shown). of everyday cognitive abilities relevant to patient understanding of

N e uhttps://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056
Downloaded from rocognitive improvement by a single dialys is 1555
by guest
on 03 January 2018
Table 4. Mean and SD of T scores before dialysis (T1pre-dialysis) and the morning after dialysis (T2post-dialysis)

Group a) before dialysis (T1pre-dialysis) b) follow-up—the morning after


dialysis (T2post-dialysis)

M ± SD P valuea M ± SD P valueb
Memory
RBMT—storyc CKD5D 1.71 ± 0.50 0.004 1.93 ± 0.18 <0.001
Non-CKD 1.96 ± 0.11 1.90 ± 0.35 0.210
RCFT—MQ CKD5D 49.82 ± 7.80 0.001 57.93 ± 4.87 <0.001
Non-CKD 58.00 ± 8.09 60.10 ± 6.95 0.131
Digit span forward CKD5D 53.39 ± 10.14 0.474 55.57 ± 12.63 0.141
Non-CKD 55.55 ± 10.28 56.50 ± 9.70 0.712
Digit span backward CKD5D 45.32 ± 12.21 0.002 46.68 ± 11.64 0.540
Non-CKD 55.55 ± 8.15 53.80 ± 9.84 0.229
Attention
TMT A CKD5D 38.89 ± 5.94 0.001 43.86 ± 8.13 <0.001
Non-CKD 50.45 ± 8.00 52.20 ± 8.33 0.090
TAP alertness
With signal CKD5D 40.50 ± 7.21 0.001 41.57 ± 8.01 0.307
Non-CKD 48.05 ± 8.19 50.25 ± 7.30 0.214
Without signal CKD5D 40.68 ± 8.29 <0.001 42.96 ± 9.51 0.083
Non-CKD 50.55 ± 7.86 50.80 ± 7.63 0.868
TAP Go/NoGo CKD5D 47.71 ± 12.82 0.111 49.89 ± 11.78 0.225
Non-CKD 53.05 ± 8.48 52.85 ± 8.02 0.851
Executive functions
TMT B CKD5D 38.79 ± 7.59 <0.001 41.54 ± 9.25 0.020
Non-CKD 49.35 ± 9.52 55.30 ± 10.44 0.001
RCFT copy CKD5D 58.04 ± 13.60 0.003 66.25 ± 8.99 0.001
Non-CKD 70 ± 12.09 73.90 ± 8.64 0.175
RCFT points deductionc CKD5D 2 ± 2.48 0.022 0.25 ± 0.70 <0.001
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Non-CKD 0.55 ± 1.36 0.00 ± 0.00 0.086


BADS key searchc CKD5D 12.32 ± 4.09 0.599 12.57 ± 3.70 0.588
Non-CKD 12.90 ± 3.16 13.50 ± 2.82 0.280
BADS zoo mapc CKD5D 14.14 ± 1.88 0.991 14.71 ± 1.30 0.062
Non-CKD 14.15 ± 2.28 14.75 ± 2.92 0.209
RWT lexical CKD5D 41.29 ± 7.88 0.005 43.57 ± 10.68 0.133
Non-CKD 48.20 ± 8.33 48.65 ± 10.54 0.767
RWT divergent lexical CKD5D 38.61 ± 7.85 0.001 41.21 ± 12.31 0.119
Non-CKD 47.50 ± 9.78 49.20 ± 11.80 0.427
RWT semantic CKD5D 43.46 ± 11.40 0.005 47.36 ± 10.02 0.030
Non-CKD 52.70 ± 9.42 54.10 ± 8.73 0.504
RWT divergent semantic CKD5D 44.25 ± 9.22 <0.001 47.11 ± 8.43 0.122
Non-CKD 54.15 ± 7.65 55.20 ± 8.29 0.581
a
P value of independent t-tests between the groups (patient group versus non-CKD).
b
P value of dependent t-tests within the groups (Tpre versus Tpost).
c
absolute numbers (raw scores)

the disease and treatments need to be outlined, studied and im- patients, using a state-of-the-art test battery to examine learn-
plemented [16]. Our data indicate that the best time for doctor– ing and memory functions, yet in this study detailed tests of at-
patient communication is the day after a dialysis session. Patients tention were not performed.
seem to be more able to listen carefully, comprehend, memorize All studies point to a cognitive improvement after dialysis, or
and to follow instructions of nurses, dieticians and physicians. In more specifically both studies as well as our study have found an
real life, this time point, which would require an additional trip to increase in psychomotor performance, executive functions,
the dialysis unit, seems difficult to implement. learning and memory.
Our results support earlier studies that have found an asso- We wish to point out several important limitations of our
ciation between decreased kidney function and lower cognitive study. First, the recruitment of patients underlies a positive se-
performance over a whole range of renal function [17]. More- lection bias. Clinically stable patients voluntarily participated in
over, only a few studies have examined the influence of a single the study. Some of the envisaged patients did not consider
dialysis session on cognitive functioning. Among these, Har- themselves ‘good’ enough to participate because of the fear of
ciarek et al. [18] and Lux et al. [19] have been the only two stud- performing badly. Furthermore, patients with severe cognitive
ies, which have tested specific cognitive areas. Those studies impairment were less likely to participate. These facts lead to
were different from our study in the following respects. The the assumption of an underestimation of the true severity
sample size in the study of Lux was quite small, only including of cognitive decline in CKD5D patients. Second, the generaliz-
12 participants. Harciarek and colleagues tested 20 CKD5D ability of our study is limited by the small sample size. Although

Downloaded from1556
https://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056 S.M. Schneider et al.
by guest
on 03 January 2018
F I G U R E 1 : Memory tested pre- (T1pre-dialysis) and post-dialysis (T2post-dialysis) with the following tests: (A) forward digit span, (B) backward digit

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
span, (C) RMBT and (D) RCFT. Data are visualized as box and whisker plots (horizontal bars indicate median values).

F I G U R E 2 : Attention tested pre- (T1pre-dialysis) and post-dialysis (T2post-dialysis) with the following tests: (A) TMT A, (B) TAP Go/NoGo, (C) TAP
Alertness with signal and (D) TAP Alertness without signal. Data are visualized as box and whisker plots (horizontal bars indicate median values).

N e uhttps://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056
Downloaded from rocognitive improvement by a single dialys is 1557
by guest
on 03 January 2018
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

F I G U R E 3 : Executive function tested pre- (T1pre-dialysis) and post-dialysis (T2post-dialysis) with the following tests: (A) TMT A, (B) RCFT copy, (C)
RCFT points deduction, (D) BADS—key search, (E) BADS—zoo map and (F–I) RWT. Data are visualized as box and whisker plots (horizontal
bars indicate median values).

patients underwent an extensive test battery, we limited the tests cognitive impairment could be detected, which means that the
for specific cognitive areas to executive functions like the BADS results were at least 1 SD below the mean for age- and education-
(key search and zoo map) for performance of complex executive matched controls. In addition, this study showed that a single
functions. Third, controls exhibited a higher education than dialysis session improves several aspects of cognitive function,
CKD5D patients, which had, however, no effect on the evalu- pointing to a reversible component of decreased cognitive per-
ation of the influence of a single dialysis on the applied test bat- formance in CKD5D. These data support that the knowledge
tery, which was the main study aim. of cognitive function level and the temporary improvement of
Does dialysis result in transitory improvement or longer-term cognitive functions are exceedingly important for the progress
improvement? If so, is this related to the dose of renal replacement of the disease. Treatment of cognitive deficits should start early
therapy? Are changes in blood pressure (during dialysis) related to in the beginning of a kidney disease to ensure the best psycho-
cognitive function decline, as described for patients with uncon- logical and nephrological care geared to specific patient needs.
trolled hypertension [20]? Can early intervention in patients with
mild to moderate CKD reduce the decline in cognitive perform-
ance? Should a rapid decline in cognitive performance prompt the
start of renal replacement therapy? These remain open questions
that urgently need to be studied. FUNDING

The study was funded by intramural support.

CONCLUSION

In summary, our findings demonstrate that among adults with


CKD5D, lower cognitive performance is common. Compared S U P P L E M E N TA R Y D ATA
with non-renal subjects, we found a decreased performance in
long-term memory. Even more severely affected were executive Supplementary data are available online at http://ndt.oxford
functions, as well as attention. In some of the tests, even a mild journals.org.

Downloaded from1558
https://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056 S.M. Schneider et al.
by guest
on 03 January 2018
7. Rocco MV, Lockridge RS, Jr, Beck GJ et al. The effects of frequent nocturnal
C O N F L I C T O F I N T E R E S T S TAT E M E N T home hemodialysis: the Frequent Hemodialysis Network Nocturnal Trial.
Kidney Int 2011; 80: 1080–1091
None declared. 8. Falleti MG, Maruff P, Collie A et al. Practice effects associated with the
repeated assessment of cognitive function using the CogState battery at
10-minute, one week and one month test-retest intervals. J Clin Exp
Neuropsychol 2006; 28: 1095–1112
AUTHORS’ CONTRIBUTIONS 9. Pyc MA, Rawson KA. Why testing improves memory: mediator effective-
ness hypothesis. Science 2010; 330: 335
S.S., A.M. and K.M. conducted the neurocognitive testing. J.T. 10. Zimmermann P, Sprengelmeyer R, Fimm B et al. Cognitive slowing in
K. was the treating physician of the patient reported. S.S., A.M., decision tasks in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn
1992; 18: 60–69
K.M., H.K., J.T.K. and R.S. evaluated the test results. All of the 11. Reitan RM. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage.
authors have participated in the discussion and in writing of the J Consult Psychol 1955; 19: 393–394
submitted manuscript. All authors read and approved the final 12. Cohen J. Wechsler Memory Scale performance of psychoneurotic, organic,
manuscript. and schizophrenic groups. J Consult Psychol 1950; 14: 371–375
13. Baddeley A. Measuring memory. Psychopharmacol Ser 1988; 6: 12–22
14. Tupler LA, Welsh KA, Asare-Aboagye Y et al. Reliability of the Rey-
(See related article by Elias et al. Improved cognitive perform- Osterrieth Complex Figure in use with memory-impaired patients. J Clin
ance after a single dialysis session: where do we go from here? Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17: 566–579
Nephrol Dial Transplant 2015; 30: 1414–1417.) 15. Wutzler A, Becker R, Lammler G et al. The anticipatory proportion as an
indicator of language impairment in early-stage cognitive disorder in the
elderly. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2013; 36: 300–309
16. Elias MF, Dore GA, Davey A. Kidney disease and cognitive function.
REFERENCES Contrib Nephrol 2013; 179: 42–57
17. Davey A, Elias MF, Robbins MA et al. Decline in renal functioning is asso-
1. Scribner BH, Buri R, Caner JE et al. The treatment of chronic uremia by
ciated with longitudinal decline in global cognitive functioning, abstract
means of intermittent hemodialysis: a preliminary report. Trans Am Soc
reasoning and verbal memory. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2013; 28:
Artif Intern Organs 1960; 6: 114–122
1810–1819
2. Bugnicourt JM, Godefroy O, Chillon JM et al. Cognitive disorders and de-
18. Harciarek M, Biedunkiewicz B, Lichodziejewska-Niemierko M et al. Cog-
mentia in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. J Am Soc Nephrol 2013;

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
nitive performance before and after kidney transplantation: a prospective
24: 353–363
controlled study of adequately dialyzed patients with end-stage renal dis-
3. Schneider S, Malecki AK, Boenisch O et al. Cognitive function at 2443
ease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009; 15: 684–694
mumol/l creatinine. BMC Nephrol 2012; 13: 86
19. Lux S, Mirzazade S, Kuzmanovic B et al. Differential activation of
4. Dahbour SS, Wahbeh AM, Hamdan MZ. Mini mental status examination
memory-relevant brain regions during a dialysis cycle. Kidney Int 2010;
(MMSE) in stable chronic renal failure patients on hemodialysis: the effects
78: 794–802
of hemodialysis on the MMSE score. A prospective study. Hemodial Int
20. Crichton GE, Elias MF, Dore GA et al. Measurement-to-measurement
2009; 13: 80–85
blood pressure variability is related to cognitive performance: the Maine
5. Williams MA, Sklar AH, Burright RG et al. Temporal effects of dialysis on cog-
Syracuse study. Hypertension 2014; 64: 1094–1101
nitive functioning in patients with ESRD. Am J Kidney Dis 2004; 43: 705–711
6. Jassal SV, Devins GM, Chan CT et al. Improvements in cognition in pa-
tients converting from thrice weekly hemodialysis to nocturnal hemodialy- Received for publication: 29.8.2014; Accepted in revised form: 10.4.2015
sis: a longitudinal pilot study. Kidney Int 2006; 70: 956–962

N e uhttps://academic.oup.com/ndt/article-abstract/30/9/1551/2460056
Downloaded from rocognitive improvement by a single dialys is 1559
by guest
on 03 January 2018

You might also like